Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jul 16.
Published in final edited form as: Br J Psychiatry. 2009 Jul;195(1):7–14. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051672

Prenatal Risk Factors for Autism: A Comprehensive Meta-analysis

Hannah Gardener 1,*, Donna Spiegelman 2, Stephen L Buka 3
PMCID: PMC3712619  NIHMSID: NIHMS480288  PMID: 19567888

Abstract

Background

The etiology of autism is unknown, although prenatal exposures have been the focus of epidemiologic research for over 40 years.

Aims

To provide the first quantitative review and meta-analysis of the association between maternal pregnancy complications and pregnancy-related factors and risk of autism.

Methods

PubMed, Embase, and PsycInfo databases were searched for epidemiologic studies that examined the association between pregnancy-related factors and autism. Forty studies were eligible for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Summary effect estimates were calculated for factors examined in multiple studies.

Results

Over 50 prenatal factors have been examined. The factors associated with autism risk in the meta-analysis were advanced parental age at birth, maternal prenatal medication use, bleeding, gestational diabetes, being first born vs. third or later, and having a mother born abroad. The factors with the strongest evidence against a role in autism risk included previous fetal loss and maternal hypertension, proteinuria, preeclampsia, and swelling.

Conclusions

There is insufficient evidence to implicate any one prenatal factor in autism aetiology, although there is some evidence to suggest that exposure to pregnancy complications may increase the risk.

Introduction

Autism is a developmental disorder characterized by deficits in social interaction and communication, and restricted, repetitive interests and behaviors beginning in infancy and toddler years1,2. The prevalence of autism has been estimated at 13/10,000 and is believed to be rising3. The aetiology is unknown. Although the estimated 60-92% concordance rate in monozygotic twins as compared to 0-10% in dizygotic twins underscores the importance of genetic influences, the incomplete concordance in monozygotic twins also indicates a role of environmental factors4,5. It is now believed that the mechanism underlying autism aetiology is most likely polygenic and potentially epistatic, and that environmental factors may interact with genetic factors to increase risk6,7.

Although the distinctive neuropathology remains elusive, studies have shown macroscopic, microscopic and functional brain abnormalities6,8. These brain abnormalities suggest that the aetiologically relevant period may be in utero because the pathogenesis may begin during the prenatal period6.

Pregnancy-related exposures have been the focus of a significant amount of epidemiologic research on possible risk factors for autism. While many studies support the hypothesis that obstetrical complications may increase the risk of autism9, the specific complications, magnitude of effect, and overall conclusions of these studies are inconsistent. These inconsistencies may be due to methodological variations including diagnostic criteria, comparison groups, sample size, and exposure assessment methods.

The purpose of this study is to provide a systematic review and meta-analysis of the epidemiologic literature on the relationship between prenatal complications/exposures and autism. A review article by Kolevson and colleagues discussed seven studies on this topic9. Our study expands upon this review by providing the first formal meta-analysis as well as a quantitative review of all 64 studies of prenatal risk factors for autism published through March, 2007. We review the evidence for all prenatal factors examined in the literature, and provide a summary effect estimate for all factors examined in two or more studies. The scope of literature reviewed allows for meta-regression analyses to examine whether study design characteristics explain the heterogeneity in results across studies.

Methods

Data Sources and Review Methods

The PubMed, Embase, and PsycInfo databases were searched using the keywords “autism” in combination with “prenatal” or “perinatal” or “pregnancy” or “neonatal,” limited to peer-reviewed studies published in any language through March, 2007. The search identified 698 studies in PubMed, 176 in Embase, and 416 in PsycInfo. The literature search sought to identify all epidemiologic studies that have examined the association of pregnancy and delivery factors and neonatal complications to the risk of autism. Based on a review of all abstracts, 83 papers were identified as potentially relevant and reviewed further. Those studies that were not reviewed included case series, animal studies, autism prevalence studies, medical hypotheses, studies of other psychiatric diseases (e.g. schizophrenia), and studies of unrelated exposures (e.g. demographics, familial psychiatric diseases, genetics, infant behaviors). Forty-one additional potential papers were identified after screening the reference lists of original and review articles. Among the 124 studies that were reviewed, we excluded those that did not include a comparison group (n=13) or any formal statistical analyses (n=3), did not examine exposures during pregnancy or the first month of life (n=10), grouped their autism cases with other childhood psychotic disorders (n=15), and were review or commentary articles (n=18). The control group had to be non-autistic but could be otherwise abnormal. In total, 65 studies were eligible for inclusion5,10-73 in the quantitative review. Two studies15,30 reporting on the same data set were considered together, resulting in 64 studies for review.

Although the literature search covered the scope of prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal factors, the current report reviews the pregnancy-related factors only, and a future publication will address factors related to labor and delivery as well as neonatal complications in relation to autism. However, it is important to recognize that prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal complications are inter-related, and are therefore difficult to disentangle and reliably categorize. Many perinatal and neonatal complications are often the result of both observed and unobserved prenatal insults and compromises to fetal development. This report focuses on those potential risk factors that were commonly identified as being specifically related to the prenatal period in the extant literature.

The first author abstracted each article on two separate occasions spaced one year apart. For each study the following information was recorded: 1. study design (cohort, case-control); 2. sample size and description (e.g. clinic-based, population-based); 3. comparison group description (e.g. matching criteria, sibling controls, healthy vs. abnormal controls, diagnoses of abnormal controls); 4. autism diagnostic criteria and mode of reporting (e.g. DSM-III vs. DSM-IV, parental report vs. medical record review vs. study physician assessment, diagnostic measures used); 5. risk factors examined and mode of reporting (e.g. parental interview, medical record review); 6. covariates included in multivariate models; 7. study results, including indicators of statistical significance, prevalence of exposures among cases and controls, rates or risks of autism across exposure levels, relative risks (RR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI). Studies were classified as prospective vs. retrospective if exposures were assessed and recorded before or after the onset of autism, regardless of when they were analyzed for the purposes of the given study. For the quantitative review, we counted the number of studies that examined each prenatal factor in relation to the risk of autism, and the number of null findings, significant and marginally significant positive findings, significant and marginally significant negative findings.

Statistical Analysis

Meta-analysis

Of the 64 studies reviewed, 40 were appropriate for inclusion in the meta-analysis10-49. Twenty-four studies were excluded from the meta-analysis because they did not report relative risks and confidence intervals, or did not provide information needed to calculate them. A separate meta-analysis was conducted for each exposure variable that was examined in two or more studies. For each exposure, a summary effect estimate was calculated using a random effects model74. Because power to detect heterogeneity is low in meta-analyses such as these75, we took a conservative approach and used random effects models to form confidence intervals, because random effects models account for any observed heterogeneity regardless of whether the heterogeneity is statistically significant. When available, the estimate used for each study was the multivariate estimate controlling for the maximum number of covariates.

If an effect estimate was reported without the corresponding 95% CI, the confidence bounds were derived from the p-value provided. If no p-value was provided, then a p-value of 0.05 or 0.50 was assumed for factors that did and did not reach statistical significance, respectively.

Several studies included autism spectrum disorders in their case definition. Five studies reported results for both the broader phenotype and for narrowly-defined autism22,25,26,27,29, in which case the study-specific exposure effect estimates using the narrowest diagnostic criteria were recorded.

The relationships between autism and maternal/paternal age at birth as well as birth order were assessed categorically and meta-analytic tests of trend76,77 were conducted using ordinal categorical variables with the score of each category equal to the mid-point of the exposure range, using SAS version 9 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). These trend tests were restricted to studies that provided information on the number of cases and participants at each exposure level.

Due to the rarity of many of the exposures and small sample sizes there were tables in some (<5%) of the meta-analyses with zero cell counts. In these instances, 0.5 was added to each cell of the 2×2 table78.

Several studies used multiple control groups (e.g. mentally retarded and healthy controls). In these studies, the comparison groups were pooled and compared to the cases as a single group.

Some studies classified the exposures of interest into distinct subcategories (e.g. bleeding by trimester). In addition to providing a summary estimate for the primary exposure of interest (e.g. pregnancy bleeding), we also calculated summary estimates for each subcategory. If only the crude estimates were provided then the exposures were pooled by simply adding the cases/controls who experienced each subcategory type. If multivariate adjusted estimates were provided then the adjusted estimates for each exposure subcategory were combined using the method proposed by Greenland and Longnecker76 to adjust the variance of the summary estimate by accounting for the covariance due to the inclusion of overlapping comparison groups across exposure subcategories.

Meta-Regression

For each risk factor assessed in multiple studies we examined the heterogeneity in the relative risks estimated across studies using the Q statistic74,79. Due to the limited power of this test75 a liberal p-value of <0.10 was used to identify meta-analyses that required further examination to assess potential sources of heterogeneity. If we found evidence of suggested heterogeneity a meta-regression80,81 was conducted to identify measured methodological factors that could explain the between-study variability (i.e. between-study effect modification).

The analyses of effect modification were conducted using the “metareg” command in STATA880. The study characteristics that were examined included: diagnostic criteria (inclusion of spectrum disorders: yes vs. no); exposure information quality (0=retrospective exposure assessment, 1=mix of retrospective and prospective exposure assessment, 2=prospective exposure assessment); control for confounding (0=univariate analysis, 1=control for select demographic factors, birth order, or IQ, 2=full multivariate analysis or matching with sibling controls); normal vs. abnormal controls; and case selection (clinic-based vs. population-based). If effect modification was suggested for a given study characteristic (p<0.10), then a stratified analysis was performed.

Publication bias was assessed for each factor by conducting tests for funnel plot asymmetry82 using the “metabias” command in STATA8. Two statistical approaches were used to examine the association between study size and the effect of the exposure: the Begg test83 and the Egger test84.

Results

Table 1 lists the prenatal factors that were not included in the meta-analysis due to unavailability of two or more effect estimates and 95% CI’s, as well as an indication of whether they were associated with autism in the studies in which they were examined. Table 2 lists the prenatal factors included in the meta-analyses, as well the number of null findings, significant and marginally significant positive findings, significant and marginally significant negative findings (protective association). For each factor that was examined in the meta-analysis, Table 2 reports the summary effect estimate and 95% CI from the random effects model, and the p-value for the test of heterogeneity.

Table 1. Review of prenatal risk factors not eligible for meta-analysis.

Pregnancy-related risk factors examined in only 1 study
No association with autism Chronic maternal disease, maternal cytomegalovirus,
autoimmune disease, severe cholecystitis, endocrine
diseases, venous thrombosis, infertility requiring
medical intervention, previous live births now dead,
frequency of intercourse during pregnancy, irregular
menstrual periods, maternal immunization, maternal
transfusions, previous x-rays, chorionic villi sampling,
amniocentesis, pre-pregnancy BMI, drug use during
pregnancy, fetal oxygenation, maternal age at first birth
30+, father with foreign citizenship,
Positive association with autism Maternal asthma, allergies, maternal toxemia or
bleeding, prenatal stressors, month prenatal care began,
urbanization of birth place
Negative association with autism Maternal alcohol use during pregnancy
Pregnancy-related risk factors examined in multiple studiesa
Prenatal Factor (# studies) Results across studiesb
 Maternal depression (2) 2 ↑
 Maternal emotional strain (3) 2 ↑, 1 ?
 Maternal psychiatric care (2) 2 -
 Contraception use prior to pregnancy (2) 1 -, 1 ↓
a

Although these factors were examined in multiple studies, effect estimates and confidence intervals were available for fewer than 2 studies.

b

Total number of studies included in the review: 64

Null results: -

Significant positive results (p<0.05): ↑

Significant negative results (p<0.05): ↓

Marginally significant positive results (0.10<p<0.05): ?

Table 2. Meta-analysis of prenatal risk factors for autism.

Prenatal Factors (# studies) Results across
studiesa
Summary Effect
Estimate (95% CI)c
Heterogeneity
(p-value)
Parental demographics
Maternal age (30) 20 -, 8 ↑, 2 ↓ p-value test for
trend=0.02
 5-year increase (9) 1.07(1.01-1.13) <0.001
 <20 vs. 25-29 (6) 0.86 (0.51-1.43) <0.001
 20-24 vs. 25-29 (7) 0.94 (0.71-1.23) <0.001
 30-34 vs. 25-29 (7) 1.27 (1.11-1.44) 0.03
 35+ vs. 25-29 (7) 1.42 (1.17-1.72) 0.002
 40+ vs. 25-29 (3) 1.43 (1.05-1.96) 0.63
 <20 vs. 20-34 (6) 0.68 (0.39-1.20) <0.001
 35+ vs. 20-34 (5) 1.53 (1.32-1.77) 0.11
 <20 or >30 (6) 1.43 (1.30-1.57) 0.27
 30+ vs. <30 (7) 1.73 (1.36-2.19) <0.001
 35+ vs. <35 (7) 1.60 (1.32-1.95) <0.001
 40+ vs. <30 (4) 2.06 (1.48-2.86) 0.92
Paternal age (9) 4-, 4 ↑, 1 ↓ p-value test for
trend=0.004
 5-year increase (4) 1.04 (1.01-1.06) 0.12
 <25 vs. 25-29 (3) 0.74 (0.59-0.92) 0.49
 30-34 vs. 25-29 (3) 1.07 (0.94-1.21) 0.85
 35+ vs. 25-29 (3) 1.34 (1.16-1.54) 0.74
 40+ vs. 25-29 (2) 1.44 (1.17-1.77) 0.70
 30-39 vs. <30 (3) 1.24 (1.09-1.41) 0.55
 40+ vs. <30 (2) 3.10 (0.95-9.49) 0.01
Mother born in another country (5) 1 -, 3 ↑, 1 ↓ 1.28 (0.99-1.65) <0.001
 Nordic studies (3) 1.58 (1.14-2.19) 0.05
Maternal obstetrical history
Previous fetal loss (abortion, miscarriage,
stillbirth) (13)
8 -, 5 ↑ 1.11 (0.75-1.64) 0.26
Birth order/Parity/Gravidity (20) 11 -, 6 Mb, 1 ↑, 2 ↓ p-value test for
trend=0.18
 1 pregnancy increase (8) 0.95 (0.89-1.02) <0.001
 1st vs. not 1st (11) 1.14 (0.97-1.35) <0.001
 1st vs. 2nd (4) 1.20 (0.85-1.71) <0.001
 1st vs. 2nd or 3rd (6) 1.20 (0.90-1.59) <0.001
 1st vs. 3rd+ (4) 1.61 (1.42-1.82) 0.27
 1st vs. 4th+ (6) 0.95 (0.63-1.42) <0.001
 1st or 4th vs. 2nd or 3rd (5) 1.20 (0.95-1.52) <0.01
 4th vs. 2nd or 3rd (5) 1.02 (0.79-1.32) 0.19
Maternal Illness During Pregnancy
Proteinuria/Albuminuria (3) 3 - 0.77 (0.34-1.73) 0.85
Anaemia (4) 4 - 0.54 (0.14-2.15) 0.26
Diabetes (6) 5 -, 1 ↑ 2.07 (1.24-3.47) 0.96
Infections (15) 10 -, 4 ↑, 1 ↓ 1.18 (0.76-1.83) 0.09
 Rubella (3) 1.66 (0.84-3.29) 0.92
 Vaginal infections (2) 0.49 (0.22-1.09) 0.36
Fever (4) 3 -, 1 ↑ 1.24 (0.76-2.04) 0.27
Nausea/Vomiting (6) 5 -, 1 ↓ 1.16(0.65-2.09) 0.05
Any illness during pregnancy (5) 4 -, 1 ↑ 1.23 (0.93-1.62) 0.67
Physical injury/accident (4) 3 -, 1 ↑ 3.24 (0.70-15.03) 0.99
Medical treatment during pregnancy
Medication use (15) 10 -, 5 ↑ 1.46 (1.08-1.96) 0.15
 Antiepileptic/anticinvulsant drug use (2) 2 - 1.87 (0.65-5.37) 0.28
 Psychoactive or antidepressant drug use (2) 1 ?, 1 - 1.68 (1.09-2.60) 0.34
Prenatal visits (2) 2 - 0.60 (0.17-2.14) 0.18
Bleeding and Toxemia
Bleeding (19) 12 -, 6 ↑, 1? 1.81 (1.14-2.86) <0.001
 1st trimester (2) 1.16 (0.45-3.01) 0.38
 2nd trimester (2) 0.91 (0.25-3.34) 0.36
 3rd trimester (2) 0.48 (0.10-2.18) 0.78
Toxemia/preeclampsia, hypertension, swelling
(25)
21 -, 2 ↑, 2 ↓ 1.01 (0.80-1.27) 0.07
Placental abnormalities (8) 7-, 1 ↑ 1.40 (0.93-2.12) 0.40
 Placenta previa (2) 1.04 (0.21-5.22) 0.29
 Placenta abruptio (2) 0.90 (0.39-2.08) 0.81
 Placental infarcts (2) 1.49 (0.78-2.83) 0.59
Other
High maternal weight gain during pregnancy (5) 3 -, 1 ↑, 1 ?/↓ 0.90 (0.49-1.63) 0.03
Smoking during pregnancy (6) 4 -, 1 ↑, 1 ↓ 1.00 (0.75-1.36) 0.05
Threatened abortion (3) 1 -, 2 ↑ 1.13 (0.12-10.95) 0.12
1+ prenatal complications (2) 1 -, 1 ↑ 1.17 (0.71-1.92) 0.02
a

Total number of studies included in the review: 64

Null results: -

Significant positive results (p<0.05): ↑

Significant negative results (p<0.05): ↓

Marginally significant positive results (0.10<p<0.05): ?

Marginally significant negative results (0.10<p<0.05): ?/↓

b

“M” indicates significant findings with a mixed trend (e.g. elevated risk among those born first or 3rd or later)

c

Total number of studies included in the meta-analysis: 40

The meta-analysis found few statistically significant risk factors. Maternal gestational diabetes was associated with a two-fold increased risk of autism. In addition, a significant 81% elevated risk was observed in relation to maternal bleeding during pregnancy. Maternal medication use was also associated with a 46% increased risk. Although 15 studies examined the relationship between prenatal medication use and risk of autism, the majority studied the general use of any medications during pregnancy while only a few examined the association with specific classes of medications. A meta-analysis of the two studies that looked specifically at psychiatric medication use during pregnancy suggested a significant positive association with the risk of autism (RR=1.68).

Maternal age at birth over 30 was associated with an increased risk with effect estimates ranging from a 27% increased risk (30-34 vs. 25-29) to a 106% increase in risk (40+ vs. <30). Thirteen studies were included in the meta-analyses of maternal age at birth. The trend test included nine studies and indicated a significant increase in risk of autism with increasing maternal age at birth (p-value test for trend=0.02). A five-year increase in maternal age was associated with a 7% increase in risk.

Increased paternal age at birth was also found to be a significant risk factor (p-value test for trend=0.004), with a five-year increase in paternal age associated with a 3.6% increase in risk. Individual exposure category effect estimates ranged from 1.24 (30-39 vs. <30) to 1.44 (40+ vs. 25-29). In addition, the three studies that examined the effect of young paternal age at birth indicated a 26% decrease in risk for paternal age < 25 vs. 25-29. Only four studies were included in the meta-analyses of paternal age.

Of the nine studies that indicated a significant relationship between birth order/parity and risk of autism, six indicated a mixed trend. Specifically, autism was associated with being first or later born (≥3rd), often depending on the size of the sibship. The meta-analysis found a statistically significant 61% increase in risk for first born children compared to children born third or later. This meta-analysis included four studies. No significant associations were observed in the comparisons of other birth order categories, and the trend test did not indicate a linear relationship between birth order and autism risk.

Maternal birth abroad was marginally associated with risk of autism. In the five studies included in the meta-analysis, maternal birth abroad was associated with a 28% increased risk (p=0.06). However, the definition of “abroad” varied as the studies were conducted in different countries and areas of the world. In the studies conducted in Nordic countries, a statistically significant 58% increased risk of autism was observed among the offspring of mothers born abroad.

Heterogeneity in effect estimates across studies was observed for the following factors (p<0.10): infections during pregnancy, nausea/vomiting, bleeding, weight gain, maternal age at birth, paternal age at birth (40+ vs. <30), birth order, smoking during pregnancy, mother born abroad, and preeclampsia. Table 3 shows the results of the regression analyses that examined the potential between-study sources of heterogeneity.

Table 3. Analysis of effect modification by study characteristics: Prenatal risk factors with heterogeneity (p<0.10).

Prenatal Risk Factors Significant Sources of Between-Study
Heterogeneity: Study Characteristics (p<0.10) a
Summary Effect
Estimate
(95% CI)
Infections during pregnancy 1.18 (0.76-1.83)
Multivariate vs. univariate analysis (p=0.09)
 4 studies: controlled for multiple covariates 1.82 (1.01-3.30)
 7 studies: no control for covariates 0.89 (0.56-1.42)
Nausea/Vomiting 1.16 (0.65-2.09)
Exposure data collection (p=0.004)
 3 studies: prospective 1.48 (1.03-2.14)
 3 studies: retrospective 0.55 (0.31-0.98)
Maternal age: linear trend none 1.07 (1.01-1.13)
Birth order: linear trend none 0.95 (0.89-1.02)
Smoking during pregnancy 1.00 (0.75-1.36)
Population-based (p=0.06)
 3 studies: population-based 1.15 (0.90-1.47)
 2 studies: clinic-based 0.63 (0.37-1.08)
Mother born in another country none 1.28 (0.99-1.65)
Bleeding none 1.81 (1.14-2.86)
Toxemia/Preeclampsia,
hypertension, swelling
none 1.01 (0.80-1.27)
a

exposure data collection= effect modification by exposure measurement (prospective vs. retrospective) diagnostic criteria = effect modification by diagnostic criteria (narrow vs. broad) multivariate vs. univariate analysis = effect modification by the degree of control for covariates population-based = effect modification by population-based vs. clinic-based sample abnormal = effect modification by use of normal comparison group vs. abnormal comparison group none= no effect modification (p<0.10) by any of the above study characteristics

The analysis of infections during pregnancy indicated significant effect modification based on control for covariates. Exposure to intra-uterine infections was associated with a significant increase in risk for autism in the analysis limited to the four studies that controlled for multiple covariates or used sibling controls. However, there was no relationship between infections during pregnancy and autism in the studies that did not control for covariates or use sibling controls. For nausea/vomiting, there was significant effect modification based on whether the exposure was assessed prospectively or retrospectively. The positive relationship between nausea/vomiting and autism was only significant among prospective studies (RR=1.48, 95% CI 1.03 to 2.14). In fact, the meta-analysis restricted to the three retrospective studies that examined nausea/vomiting in relation to autism suggested a protective association (RR=0.55, 95% CI 0.31 to 0.98).

The test for linear trend in birth order indicated significant heterogeneity across studies that could not be explained by variation in any of the study characteristics examined. The analyses of several maternal age at birth comparisons as well as the linear trend test also indicated heterogeneity in the effect estimates across studies. Variation in the methodological characteristics could not explain the heterogeneity in the trend estimates. However, heterogeneity in the effect estimates for the maternal age categorical comparisons may have been due to the control for covariates. In general, the elevation in risk observed in relation to older maternal age at birth was slightly attenuated in the studies that controlled for multiple covariates.

Heterogeneity in the effect estimates for maternal smoking during pregnancy may have been due to the study base (population-based or clinic-based). No significant relationship with autism was observed overall or within strata, although only five studies were included in this meta-analysis.

Lastly, for the analyses of toxemia/preeclampsia (17 studies), maternal birth abroad (five studies), and bleeding (13 studies), the heterogeneity of effect estimates across studies could not be explained by any of the study characteristics investigated.

Publication bias was assessed for all factors examined in three or more studies. Significant publication bias was only suggested for smoking during pregnancy (Begg’s test p=0.03, Egger’s test p=0.04). The test for publication bias for prenatal smoking in fact indicated a potential bias in the direction of publishing inverse associations, as suggested by the fact that the three (out of five) smaller studies in the meta-analysis all reported relative risks that were below the null. Both of the tests for publication bias lacked power because of the small number of studies included in each meta-analysis85. However, due to the many tests of publication bias performed it is likely that we would observe one or more significant results due to chance alone.

Several studies examined the relationship between compromised prenatal health in general and risk of autism, although none provided the necessary data for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Specifically, six studies utilized prenatal optimality scales to assess the number of prenatal complications experienced in cases and controls (Gillberg Optimality Scale55,61, modified Gillberg Optimality Scale41, 53, Lewis-Murray Scale44, Rochester Research Obstetrical Scale60). Four of these studies reported a significant association between reduced prenatal optimality and risk of autism53,55,60,61.

Discussion

This study is the first meta-analysis of the relationship between prenatal factors and risk of autism. Over 50 prenatal factors have been studied in relation to autism in 64 epidemiologic studies, of which 40 studies were eligible for meta-analysis. However, few factors have been examined in multiple well-conducted studies. Therefore, attempted replication in methodologically strong studies remains necessary. While the majority of factors examined in multiple studies have given inconsistent results, the preponderance of findings overall have not been statistically significant. The factors with the strongest evidence for an association with autism risk included advanced maternal and paternal age at birth, maternal gestational bleeding, gestational diabetes, being first born vs. third or later, maternal prenatal medication use, and maternal birth abroad. The factors with the strongest evidence against a role in autism risk included previous fetal loss and maternal preeclampsia, proteinuria, hypertension, and swelling.

Although there is insufficient evidence to implicate any one prenatal factor in autism aetiology, the studies using prenatal optimality scales provide some evidence to suggest that exposure to pregnancy complications in general may increase the risk of autism. It is also important to note that the etiologic importance of the prenatal period may not be fully captured by examining only those complications and characteristics that are manifested and observed during the period of gestation. Many perinatal and neonatal complications also reflect what was occurring during pregnancy, and it may be that only those compromises to the prenatal environment that are manifested in labor and delivery as well as neonatal health complications are etiologically relevant. The potential effects of a non-optimal prenatal environment as manifested in perinatal and neonatal complications will be addressed in our subsequent manuscript on this topic.

The current meta-analysis shows that increased maternal and paternal age at birth are both associated with an elevated risk of autism. The biological mechanisms underlying these relationships are not known. Maternal age may be associated with autism due to the increased risk of chromosomal abnormalities in ova of increased age, or due to unstable trinucleotide repeats9. While advanced maternal age has been shown to be associated with an increased risk of obstetrical complications86,87, it is unknown which, if any, of these complications may affect the risk of autism. Reichenberg et al.42 suggested that the relationship between paternal age and autism may be due to imprinted genes, de novo spontaneous mutations that accumulate with advancing age in spermatagonia, or confounding by sociocultural environmental factors. Maternal and paternal age at birth are likely correlated88,89 and many of the studies included did not adjust paternal age for maternal age and vice versa. It is possible that advanced age of both parents plays a role in the susceptibility to autism, or perhaps only maternal age or paternal age is aetiologically relevant. There is evidence to suggest that paternal age may be more important. Of the four studies that controlled for the age of the co-parent, three found only a significant association for paternal age at birth33,34,42, and one found only a significant association for maternal age38. When the analysis of maternal age was restricted to the four studies that controlled for paternal age the RR for a five-year increase in maternal age was 1.06, p=0.08. All studies of paternal age included in the meta-analysis were adjusted for maternal age.

Perhaps the factor that was most commonly associated with the risk of autism in the literature was birth order. Nine studies reported a significant relationship between birth order/parity and autism. However, the nature of the relationship was inconsistent across studies and was generally not found to be linear. The difficulty in elucidating the relationship between birth order/parity and autism may be due to potential effect modification by sibship size, as autistic cases are more likely to be first-born in sibship sizes of two and later-born in families with larger sibship sizes61,69. The latter trend has been attributed to parents deciding not to have additional children after one has developed autism90.

Maternal immigration has also been highlighted as a potential risk factor for autism9. In the meta-analysis, the elevated risk of autism among the offspring of women born abroad was just shy of statistical significance. In the three studies conducted in Nordic countries there was a significant 58% increased risk among the offspring of mothers born abroad, although the definition and categorization of “abroad” differed across the studies. The strength of the association in the Nordic studies may be due to an unknown mechanism particular to this area, or, perhaps more likely, may have been due to the methodological strengths of these three studies.

Several hypotheses have been postulated, including the idea that fathers with social disability potentially due to a genetic mechanism associated with autism may be less able to find a spouse from their own country and may therefore find a wife from a foreign country with whom to have children91. More likely, Gillberg et al.91 suggested that women born in another country may not be immunized against the common infectious agents in the country in which she gives birth and may therefore be more susceptible to relatively innocuous infections which may increase the risk for autism. Other possible explanations include a potential role of maternal stress due to the demands of residing in a new country, particularly with limited social support, or stress resulting from the experience of emigrating, perhaps due to economic or social factors. These hypotheses do not explain the relationship with maternal place of birth seen in a cohort study of children born in California between 1989-199416, which showed a 40% decreased risk of autism among the children of women born in Mexico as compared to California. The association between maternal immigration and autism risk requires further examination in other areas of the world to examine whether the relationship can truly be generalized.

Fetal hypoxia may underlie a potential relationship between gestational bleeding and autism. Maternal bleeding is one of several complications believed to be associated with fetal hypoxia9. Fetal distress, maternal hypertension, prolonged labor, cord complications, low Apgar score, and Cesarean delivery are other pregnancy-related factors that are believed to be related to hypoxia and have been associated with autism risk in some, but not all, studies. While some brain abnormalities observed in individuals with autism may reflect a potential role of oxygen deprivation during development, this possibility requires further examination. Hypoxia has also been shown to increase dopaminergic activity, and there is evidence for dopamine overactivation in autism91.

Bleeding in the second half of pregnancy in particular may reflect severe complications including placenta previa or abruptio placenta29. Although the analyses stratified by trimester did not produce significant associations, only two studies were available to calculate the trimester-specific estimates.

A biological mechanism underlying the potential elevated risk of autism associated with gestational diabetes is unknown. Gestational diabetes has been associated with various adverse pregnancy outcomes93-95, and the hormonal and metabolic abnormalities and oxidative stress due to gestational diabetes may have lasting consequences for offspring health and development93,96. It is possible that the reported increasing maternal and paternal age at birth and rate of gestational diabetes may be contributing factors to the rising prevalence of autism97.

The mechanism underlying the suggested association with maternal medication use is also unclear, due to the variety of medications consumed during pregnancy and assessed in these studies. While many medications may cross the placenta and affect fetal development, the current analysis cannot indicate which medications may be detrimental. However, the meta-analysis of two studies that looked at psychiatric medication use suggested a significant 68% increased risk of autism, and one small Croatian study32 suggested a higher frequency of hormone use among the mothers of autistic cases than among the mothers of mentally retarded controls. Maimburg and Vaeth38 found a 50% increased risk of autism associated with maternal use of medicine in a population-based case-control study using Danish national registries. Although they observed no significant association for antiepileptics, antihypertensives, cardiovascular drugs, tocolytics, nor use of steroids, a significant 60% increased risk of autism was observed in relation to use of psychoactive drugs. The association with maternal use of psychoactive drugs may reflect either an effect of the medication exposure, an adverse effect of the actual treated condition itself on fetal development (confounding by indication), or transmission of genetic traits possibly shared between autism and other psychiatric disorders.

Investigators have questioned the causal nature of the observed relationship between prenatal complications and autism. Confounding by birth order has been suggested, as an increased risk of autism and obstetrical complications are often observed in first-, fourth- and later-born offspring52,73. Although some studies have shown that associations were attenuated and no longer significant after adjusting for parity41,61, other studies have shown that the positive relationship persists52,73. A second noncausal hypothesis is that obstetrical complications occur as a result of the autistic condition in the offspring or as a consequence of other factors (e.g. genetic factors) that are the true causal determinants of autism52. In this epiphenomena explanation, pregnancy complications simply reflect the abnormalities of autistic fetal development, or the same familial factors cause both autism and obstetrical complications. The study conducted by Bolton52 provided strong evidence in support of the shared risk hypothesis, as there was an association between obstetric suboptimality and measures of autism severity and familiality, and the obstetric suboptimality scores in the cases were highly correlated with that of their affected siblings. In addition, probands with increased obstetric complications had more extended family members with the broader autism phenotype, although this finding was not replicated in a second study by Zwaigenbaum73. The shared risk hypothesis was also supported by the findings in the Zwaigenbaum study that indicated more obstetric adversity among unaffected siblings of children with pervasive developmental disorders that had high familial loading for the broader autism phenotype73.

Methodological limitations that have impaired the precision and validity of results include small sample size, non-normal control groups (e.g. Down’s syndrome), broad disease definition, and retrospective parental recall of exposures. Of the 64 studies included in the review, only 19 had over 80% power to detect a relative risk of 2 for an exposure with 10% prevalence. Nineteen of the studies used broad diagnostic criteria resulting in the possible inclusion of cases with other autism spectrum disorders, which may limit the ability to detect associations due to aetiologic heterogeneity. Twenty-one studies assessed the exposure variables retrospectively resulting in the high possibility of recall bias. However, the use of medical records also has the limitation of being incomplete. Lastly, the majority of studies included only univariate analyses and did not assess potential confounding. These methodological weaknesses were also likely sources of heterogeneity of effects across studies. Although significant heterogeneity was observed for few factors, the test of heterogeneity lacked power because the majority of the meta-analyses conducted were able to include fewer than six studies and therefore variability in study characteristics was lacking.

This meta-analysis has a few limitations. First, only published data were used. Second, of the 64 studies reviewed, only 40 reported the data necessary for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Within these 40 studies the investigators did not report the necessary data for a meta-analysis on all factors examined. And although 40 studies were included in the meta-analysis overall, for each factor there were generally fewer than six studies included, limiting the statistical power to detect heterogeneity across studies and potential effect modification by study characteristics. Third, due to the rarity of many of the exposures examined and the small sample sizes in many studies, there were instances of 0 cell counts within studies. The relatively small addition of 0.5 to the cell counts may have had an impact on the overall results due to the small sample sizes. Fourth, a few studies only reported an effect estimate and an indication of whether the results were statistically significant. In these cases, the confidence intervals were estimated based on assumptions regarding the actual p-value (p=0.05 if significant, p=0.50 if not significant). In the case of statistically significant findings, these assumptions resulted in conservative estimates of the true confidence intervals. Fifth, the tests of publication bias were under-powered due to the limited number of studies in each meta-analysis. Lastly, many studies simply examined all available prenatal data using designs with methodological weaknesses and without a priori hypotheses or knowledge about reproductive epidemiology. As a result, significant associations observed due to chance are possible in this meta-analysis.

The current review and meta-analysis was not restricted to studies with particular methodological strengths. In addition, individual study characteristics were examined in meta-regressions rather than assigning studies aggregate quality scores. These strategies are consistent with the recommendations proposed by the “Meta-Analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology Group” which advocated the use of broad inclusion criteria for studies along with regression analyses to relate specific study design characteristics to outcome98. This maximizes the amount of data available for review. In addition, different methodological considerations are relevant for each exposure. However, the increased probability for heterogeneity of results using the broad inclusion criteria is important to note.

Twin studies and family aggregation studies have provided clear evidence for the important role of genetics in autism aetiology6. The difficulty in identifying environmental risk factors is likely due to the complex interactions between these factors and genetics in determining disease susceptibility and the methodologic considerations detailed above. Future investigations of prenatal exposures should also collect DNA to study potential gene-environment interactions. Autism is a devastating condition with no known cure. The rising prevalence, coupled with the severe emotional and financial impact on the families, underscores the need for large, prospective, population-based studies with the goal of elucidating the modifiable risk factors, particularly those during the prenatal period.

Acknowledgments

We thank Ruifeng Li, MS (Harvard School of Public Health, Department of Biostatistics) for providing additional statistical and programming support. We also thank Alberto Ascherio, MD, DrPH (Harvard School of Public Health, Departments of Epidemiology and Nutrition; Channing Laboratory, Department of Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Harvard Medical School) and Janet Rich-Edwards, ScD (Harvard School of Public Health Department of Epidemiology; Harvard Medical School) for their help and guidance in reviewing the results and manuscript. We thank Dr. Tatjana Rundek, Yueh-Hsiu Mathilda Chiu, and Handan Wand for their translations of articles published in languages other than English. No compensation was received by the individuals acknowledged and listed above.

Funding: Gardener received a National Research Service Award grant from the Training Program in Psychiatric Epidemiology and Biostatistics (T32 MH17119). Partial funding was provided by the Stanley Medical Research Institute.

Footnotes

Disclosures: None of the authors have conflicts of interest, including financial interests and relationships and affiliations relevant to the subject matter or materials discussed in the manuscript.

Author Contributions:

Study concept and design: Buka, Gardener, Spiegelman.

Literature search and acquisition of data: Gardener.

Analysis and interpretation of data: Gardener, Spiegelman, Buka.

Drafting of the manuscript: Gardener.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Buka, Spiegelman.

Statistical analysis: Gardener, Spiegelman.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Gardener.

Study supervision: Buka, Spiegelman.

Dr. Gardener had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Contributor Information

Hannah Gardener, Department of Epidemiology, Harvard School of Public Health, 677 Huntington Ave, Boston, MA 02115.

Donna Spiegelman, Departments of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Harvard School of Public Health, 677 Huntington Ave, Boston, MA 02115.

Stephen L. Buka, Department of Community Health, Brown University, 121 South Main Street, Providence, RI 02912.

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ozonoff S, Goodlin-Jones BL, Solomon M. Evidence-based assessment of autism spectrum disorders in children and adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2005;34:523–540. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fombonne E. The changing epidemiology of autism. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2005;18:281–294. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Klauck SM. Genetics of autism spectrum disorder. Eur J Hum Genet. 2006;14:714–720. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, Bolton P, Simonoff E, Yuzda E, Rutter M. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med. 1995;25:63–77. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700028099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Newschaffer CJ, Fallin D, Lee NL. Heritable and noninheritable risk factors for autism spectrum disorders. Epidemiol Rev. 2002;24:137–153. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxf010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Santangelo SL, Tsatsanis K. What is known about autism: genes, brain, and behavior. Am J Pharmacogenomics. 2005;5:71–92. doi: 10.2165/00129785-200505020-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.DiCicco-Bloom E, Lord C, Zwaigenbaum L, Courchesne E, Dager SR, Schmitz C, Schultz RT, Crawley J, Young LJ. The developmental neurobiology of autism spectrum disorder. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6897–6906. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1712-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kolevson A, Gross R, Reichenberg A. Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism: A review and integration of findings. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161:326–333. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.4.326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Akçakin M, Polat S. A comparison of demographic and birth-related characteristics of autistic and intellectually handicapped children. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi. 1993;4:39–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Badawi N, Dixon G, Felix JF, Keogh JM, Petterson B, Stanley FJ, Kurinczuk JJ. Autism following a history of newborn encephalopathy: more than a coincidence? Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:85–89. doi: 10.1017/S001216220600020X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Barak Y, Ring A, Sulkes J, Gabbay U, Elizur A. Season of birth and autistic disorder in Israel. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152:798–800. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.5.798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bolton P, Pickles A, Harrington R, Macdonald H, Rutter M. Season of birth: issues, approaches and findings for autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33:509–530. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00888.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Brimacombe M, Ming X, Lamendola M. Prenatal and birth complications in autism. Matern Child Health J. 2007;11:73–39. doi: 10.1007/s10995-006-0142-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Burd L, Severud R, Kerbeshian J, Klug MG. Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism. J Perinat Med. 1999;27:441–450. doi: 10.1515/JPM.1999.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Croen L, Grether J, Selvin S. Descriptive epidemiology of autism in a California population: who is at risk? J Autism Dev Disord. 2002;32:217–224. doi: 10.1023/a:1015405914950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Croen LA, Grether JK, Yoshida CK, Odouli R, Van de Water J. Maternal autoimmune diseases, asthma and allergies, and childhood autism spectrum disorders: A case control study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159:151–157. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.2.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Croen LA, Yoshida CK, Odouli R, Newman TB. Neonatal hyperbilirubinemia and risk of autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics. 2005;115:e135–e138. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cryan E, Byrne M, O’Donovan A, O’Callaghan E. Brief report: A case-control study of obstetric complications and later autistic disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 1996;26:453–460. doi: 10.1007/BF02172829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Deykin EY, MacMahon B. Viral exposure and autism. Am J Epidemiol. 1979;109:628–638. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Deykin EY, MacMahon B. Pregnancy, delivery, and neonatal complications among autistic children. Am J Dis Child. 1980;134:860–864. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1980.02130210044012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Eaton WW, Mortensen PB, Thomsen PH, Frydenberg M. Obstetric complications and risk for severe psychopathology in childhood. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:279–285. doi: 10.1023/a:1010743203048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Finegan J, Quarrington B. Pre-, peri-, and neonatal factors and infantile autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1979;20:119–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1979.tb00492.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gillberg C. Maternal age and infantile autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1980;10:293–297. doi: 10.1007/BF02408288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gillberg C. Do children with autism have March birthdays? Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1990;82:152–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb01373.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Glasson EJ, Bower C, Petterson B, de Klerk N, Chaney G, Hallmayer JC. Perinatal factors and the development of autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:618–627. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.6.618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Guillem P, Cans C, Guinchat V, Ratel M, Jouk P. Trends, perinatal characteristics, and medical conditions in pervasive developmental disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:896–900. doi: 10.1017/S0012162206001964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hultman CM, Sparen P, Cnattingius S. Perinatal risk factors for infantile autism. Epidemiology. 2002;13:417–423. doi: 10.1097/00001648-200207000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Juul-Dam N, Townsend J, Courchesne E. Prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal factors in autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, and the general population. Pediatrics. 2001;107:63–68. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.4.e63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Klug MG, Burd L, Kerbeshian J, Benz B, Martsolf JT. A comparison of the effects of parental risk markers on pre- and perinatal variables in multiple patient cohorts with fetal alcohol syndrome, autism, Tourette syndrome, and sudden infant death syndrome: an enviromic analysis. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2003;25:707–717. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2003.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Knobloch H, Pasamanick B. Some etiologic and prognostic factors in early infantile autism and psychosis. Pediatrics. 1975;55:182–191. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kocijan-Hercigonja D, Remeta D, Orehovac M, Brkljacic D. Prenatal, perinatal and neonatal factors in infantile autism. Acta Med Croatica. 1991;45:357–62. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Larsson HJ, Eaton WW, Madsen KM, Vestergaard M, Olesen AV, Agerbo E, Schendel D, Thorsen P, Mortensen PB. Risk factors for autism: Perinatal factors, parental psychiatric history, and socioeconomic status. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161:916–925. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwi123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lauritsen MB, Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB. Effects of familial risk factors and place of birth on the risk of autism: a nationwide register-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:963–971. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00391.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Laxer G, Rey M, Ritvo ER. A comparison of potentially pathologic factors in European children with autism, Down Syndrome, and multiple physical handicaps. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1988;18:308–314. doi: 10.1007/BF02211955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Levy S, Zoltak B, Saelens T. A comparison of obstetrical records of autistic and nonautistic referrals for psychoeducational evaluations. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1988;18:573–581. doi: 10.1007/BF02211875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lobascher ME, Kingerlee PE, Gubbay SS. Childhood autism: An investigation of aetiological factors in twenty-five cases. Br J Psychiatry. 1970;117:525–529. doi: 10.1192/bjp.117.540.525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Maimburg RD, Vaeth M. Perinatal risk factors and infantile autism. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006;114:257–264. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2006.00805.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mason-Brothers A, Ritvo ER, Pingree C, Petersen PB, Jenson WR, McMahon WM, Freeman BJ, Jorde LB, Spencer MJ, Mo A, Ritvo A. The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: Prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal factors. Pediatrics. 1990;86:514–519. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Matsuishi T, Yamashita Y, Ohtani Y, Ornitz E, Kuriya N, Murakami Y, Fukuda S, Hashimoto T, Yamashita F. Brief report: incidence of and risk factors for autistic disorder in neonatal intensive care unit survivors. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1999;29:161–166. doi: 10.1023/a:1023048812202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Piven J, Simon J, Chase GA, Wzorek M, Landa R, Gayle J, Folstein S. The etiology of autism: pre-, peri-, and neonatal factors. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;32:1256–1263. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199311000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Reichenberg A, Bresnahan M, Rabinowitz J, Lubin G, Davidson M. Adancing paternal age and autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:1026–1032. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.9.1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Soldin OP, Lai S, Lamm SH, Mosee S. Lack of a relation between human neonatal thyroxine and pediatric neurobehavioral disorders. Thyroid. 2003;13:193–198. doi: 10.1089/105072503321319503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Stein D, Weizman A, Ring A, Barak Y. Obstetric complications in individuals diagnosed with autism and in healthy controls. Compr Psychiatry. 2006;47:69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2005.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sugie Y, Sugie H, Fukuda T, Ito M. Neonatal factors in infants with Autistic Disorder and typically developing infants. Autism. 2005;9:487–494. doi: 10.1177/1362361305057877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tanoue Y, Oda S, Asano F, Kawashima K. Epidemiology of infantile autism in Southern Ibaraki, Japan: Differences in prevalence in birth cohorts. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1988;18:155–166. doi: 10.1007/BF02211943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ward AJ. A comparison and analysis of the presence of family problems during pregnancy of mothers of “autistic” children and mothers of normal children. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1990;20:279–288. doi: 10.1007/BF00706020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wier ML, Yoshida CK, Odouli R, Grether JK, Croen LA. Congenital anomalies associated with autism spectrum disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:500–507. doi: 10.1017/S001216220600106X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Williams G, Oliver JM, Allard A, Sears L. Autism and associated medical and familial factors: A case control study. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2003;15:335–349. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Atladottir HO, Parner ET, Schendel D, Dalsgaard S, Thomsen PH, Thorsen P. Variation in incidence of neurodevelopmental disorders with season of birth. Epidemiology. 2007;18:240–245. doi: 10.1097/01.ede.0000254064.92806.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Beversdorf DQ, Manning SE, Hillier A, Anderson SL, Nordgren RE, Walters SE, Nagaraja HN, Cooley WC, Gaelic SE, Bauman ML. Timing of prenatal stressors and autism. J Autism Dev Disorder. 2005;35:471–478. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-5037-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Bolton PF, Murphy M, Macdonald H, Whitlock B, Pickles A, Rutter M. Obstetric complications in autism: Consequences or causes of the condition? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:272–281. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199702000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bryson SE, Smith IM, Eastwood D. Obstetrical suboptimality in autistic children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1988;27:418–422. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198807000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Deb S, Prasad KBG, Seth H, Eagles JM. A comparison of obstetric and neonatal complications between children with autistic disorder and their siblings. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1997;41:81–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1997.tb00680.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Gillberg C, Gillberg IC. Infantile autism: A total population study of reduced optimality in the pre-, peri-, and neonatal period. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1983;13:153–166. doi: 10.1007/BF01531816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Harper J, Williams S. Early environmental stress and infantile autism. Med J Aus. 1974;1:341–346. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.1974.tb47736.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hazlett HC, Poe M, Gerig G, Smith RG, Provenzale J, Ross A, Gilmore J, Piven J. Magnetic resonance imaging and head circumference study of brain size in autism: birth through age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:1366–1376. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.12.1366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Konstantareas MM, Hauser P, Lennox C, Homatidis S. Season of birth in infantile autism. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1986;17:53–65. doi: 10.1007/BF00707913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Li S, Yang X, Jia M. A study of perinatal risk factors in patients with autism. Chin J Psychiatry. 1998;31:178–180. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Links PS, Stockwell M, Abichandani F, Simeon J. Minor physical anomalies in childhood autism, part I: Their relationship to pre-and perinatal complications. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1980;10:273–285. doi: 10.1007/BF02408286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lord C, Mulloy C, Wendelboe M, Schopler E. Pre- and perinatal factors in high-functioning females and males with autism. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1991;21:197–209. doi: 10.1007/BF02284760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Mouridsen SE, Nielsen S, Rich B, Isager T. Season of birth in infantile autism and other types of childhood psychoses. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1994;25:31–43. doi: 10.1007/BF02251098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Mouridsen SE, Rich B, Isager T. Brief report: Parental age in infantile autism, autistic-like conditions, and borderline childhood psychosis. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1993;23:387–396. doi: 10.1007/BF01046227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Park RJ, Bolton PF. Pervasive developmental disorder and obstetric complications in children and adolescents with tuberous sclerosis. Autism. 2001;5:237–248. doi: 10.1177/1362361301005003002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Steffenberg S, Gillberg C, Hellgren L, Andersson L, Gillberg IC, Jakobsson G, Bohman M l. A twin study of autism in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway, and Sweden. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989;30:405–416. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00254.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Stevens MC, Fein DH, Waterhouse LH. Season of birth effects in autism. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22:399–407. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200006)22:3;1-V;FT399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Torrey EF, Dhavale D, Lawlor JP, Yolken RH. Autism and head circumference in the first year of life. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56:892–894. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Torrey EF, Hersh SP, McCabe KD. Early childhood psychosis and bleeding during pregnancy: A prospective study of gravid women and their offspring. J Autism Child Schizophr. 1975;5:287–297. doi: 10.1007/BF01540676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Tsai LY, Stewart MA. Etiological implications of maternal age and birth order in infantile autism. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1983;13:57–65. doi: 10.1007/BF01531359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Wilkerson DS, Volpe AG, Dean RS, Titus JB. Perinatal complications as predictors of infantile autism. Int J Neurosci. 2002;112:1085–1098. doi: 10.1080/00207450290026076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Yeates-Frederikx MH, Nijman H, Logher E, Merckelbach HL. Birth patterns in mentally retarded autistic patients. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:257–262. doi: 10.1023/a:1005500803764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Zambrino CA, Balottin U, Bettaglio E, Gerardo A, Lanzi G. Obstetrical suboptimality in autistic children: An Italian sample. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1997;27:493–494. doi: 10.1023/a:1025817707914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Zwaigenbaum L, Szatmari P, Jones MB, Bryson SE, MacLean JE, Mahoney WJ. Pregnancy and birth complications in autism and liability to the broader autism phenotype. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41:572–579. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200205000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1986;7:177–188. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Takkouche B, Cadarso-Suarez C, Spiegelman D. Evaluation of old and new tests of heterogeneity in epidemiologic meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;150:206–215. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Greenland S, Longnecker MP. Methods for trend estimation from summarized dose-response data, with applications to meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;135:1301–1309. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Orisni N, Li R, Wolk A, Spiegelman D. (draft) Meta-analysis for dose-response relationships: examples, an evaluation of approximations, and software. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Sterne JAC, Bradburn MJ, Egger M. Meta-analysis in stata. In: Egger M, Smith GD, Altman DG, editors. Systematic Reviews in Health Care: Meta-analysis in Context. BMJ Publishing Group; 2006. pp. 86–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Br Med J. 2003;327:557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Sharp S. Meta-analysis regression. Stata Technical Bulletin. 1998;42:16–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Stram DO. Meta-analysis of published data using a linear mixed-effects model. Biometrics. 1996;52:536–544. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Khoshdel A, Attia J, Carney SL. Basic concepts in meta-analysis: A primer for clinicians. Int J Clin Pract. 2006;60:1287–1294. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-1241.2006.01078.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Begg CB, Mazumdar M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics. 1994;50:1088–1101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br Med J. 1997;315:629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Sterne JAC, Gavaghan D, Egger M. Publication and related bias in meta-analysis: power of statistical tests and prevalence in the literature. J Clin Epidemiol. 2000;53:1119–1129. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(00)00242-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Rosenthal AN, Paterson-Brown S. Is there an incremental rise in the risk of obstetric intervention with increasing maternal age? Br J Obstet and Gynaecol. 1998;105:1064–1069. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-0528.1998.tb09937.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Ezra Y, McParland P, Farine D. High delivery intervention rates in nulliparous women over age 35. Eur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol. 1995;62:203–207. doi: 10.1016/0301-2115(95)02201-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Kazmaura MR, Lie RT. Down’s syndrome and paternal age in Norway. Paedaitr Perinat Epidemiol. 2002;16:314–319. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3016.2002.00446.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Tang C, Wu M, Liu J, Lin H, Hsu C. Delayed parenthood and the risk of Cesarean delivery – Is paternal age an independent risk factor? Birth. 2006;33:18–26. doi: 10.1111/j.0730-7659.2006.00070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Jones MB, Szatmari P. Stoppage rules and genetic studies of autism. J Autism Dev Disorder. 1988;18:31–40. doi: 10.1007/BF02211816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Gillberg C, Schaumann H, Gillberg IC. Autism in immigrants: children born to in Sweden to mothers born in Uganda. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1995;39:141–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1995.tb00482.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Previc FH. Prenatal influenced on brain dopamine and their relevance to the rising incidence of autism. Med Hypotheses. 2007;68:46–60. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.06.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Ben-Haroush A, Yogev Y, Hod M. Epidemiology of gestational diabetes mellitus and its association with type 2 diabetes. Diabet Med. 2004;21:103–113. doi: 10.1046/j.1464-5491.2003.00985.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Catalano PM, Kirwan JP, Haugel-de Mouzon S, King J. Gestational diabetes and insulin resistance: Role in short- and long-term implications for mother and fetus. J Nutr. 2003;133:1674S–1683S. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.5.1674S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Eidelman AI, Samueloff A. The pathophysiology of the fetus of the diabetic mother. Semin Perinatol. 2002;26:232–236. doi: 10.1053/sper.2002.34215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Biri A, Onan A, Devrim E, Babacan F, Kavutcu M, Durak I. Oxidant status in maternal and cord plasma and placental tissue in gestational diabetes. Placenta. 2006;27:327–332. doi: 10.1016/j.placenta.2005.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Martin JA, Hamilton BE, Ventura SJ, Menacker F, Park MM. Births: Final data for 2000. National Vital Statistics Reports. 2002;50:1–104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, Olkin I, Williamson GD, Rennie D, Moher D, Becker BJ, Sipe TA, Thacker SB. Meta-analysis of observational studies for epidemiology: A proposal for reporting. JAMA. 2000;283:2008–2012. doi: 10.1001/jama.283.15.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES