Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jul 22.
Published in final edited form as: Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010 Feb;67(2):187–196. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.185

Escitalopram and Enhancement of Cognitive Recovery Following Stroke

Ricardo E Jorge 1, Laura Acion 1, David Moser 1, Harold P Adams Jr 1, Robert G Robinson 1
PMCID: PMC3718472  NIHMSID: NIHMS405805  PMID: 20124118

Abstract

Context

Adjunctive restorative therapies administered during the first few months after stroke, the period with the greatest degree of spontaneous recovery, reduce the number of stroke patients with significant disability.

Objective

To examine the effect of escitalopram on cognitive outcome. We hypothesized that patients who received escitalopram would show improved performance in neuropsychological tests assessing memory and executive functions than patients who received placebo or underwent Problem Solving Therapy.

Design

Randomized trial.

Setting

Stroke center.

Participants

One hundred twenty-nine patients were treated within 3 months following stroke. The 12-month trial included 3 arms: a double-blind placebo-controlled comparison of escitalopram (n=43) with placebo (n=45), and a nonblinded arm of Problem Solving Therapy (n=41).

Outcome Measures

Change in scores from baseline to the end of treatment for the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) and Trail-Making, Controlled Oral Word Association, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–III Similarities, and Stroop tests.

Results

We found a difference among the 3 treatment groups in change in RBANS total score (P<.01) and RBANS delayed memory score (P<.01). After adjusting for possible confounders, there was a significant effect of escitalopram treatment on the change in RBANS total score (P<.01, adjusted mean change in score: escitalopram group, 10.0; nonescitalopram group, 3.1) and the change in RBANS delayed memory score (P<.01, adjusted mean change in score: escitalopram group, 11.3; nonescitalopram group, 2.5). We did not observe treatment effects in other neuropsychological measures.

Conclusions

When compared with patients who received placebo or underwent Problem Solving Therapy, stroke patients who received escitalopram showed improvement in global cognitive functioning, specifically in verbal and visual memory functions. This beneficial effect of escitalopram was independent of its effect on depression. The utility of antidepressants in the process of poststroke recovery should be further investigated.

Trial Registration

clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT00071643


Although there has been a significant decline in stroke-related mortality during the past 50 years, stroke remains a major health care problem, the third greatest cause of death in developed countries.1 Worldwide, 5.5 million people die from stroke yearly.1 The World Health Organization estimates that 60% of those in developed countries who have a stroke die or become dependent.1 The World Health Organization has projected stroke burden to rise from around 38 million disability-adjusted life-years globally in 1990 to 61 million disability-adjusted life-years in 2020.1 Thus, it is not surprising that the direct and indirect costs of stroke were estimated to be $65.5 billion per year in the United States.2

There have been significant advances in the treatment of acute stroke. The most significant of them are the implementation of organized stroke care and the use of acute thrombolytic therapy.3-7 However, the latter needs to be administered within a narrow therapeutic window (ie, the first few hours after the onset of symptoms), which limits the number of patients effectively treated. Consequently, besides the efforts currently undertaken to increase the number of patients treated with thrombolytic agents, there is growing interest in restorative therapies that can be administered during the first few months after stroke, the period within which we observe the greatest degree of spontaneous recovery of initial motor and cognitive deficits. Restorative therapies include those based on the synergistic effect of a medication and a specific rehabilitation intervention, such as physical therapy or speech therapy8-12 and those based on a more generalized effect of pharmacological agents on widely distributed aminergic or cholinergic projection systems.13-19 It is hypothesized that an intervention of this kind will improve stroke’s physical and cognitive outcomes, reducing the number of patients who experience significant disability.20

Another line of research strongly suggests that antidepressants exert their therapeutic effects through complex signaling cascades that result in the increased expression of neurotrophic factors, the proliferation of neural and glial cell precursors, increased axonal sprouting, and the development of new synapses.21-24 Furthermore, serotonin plays a role in neuroplastic changes associated with brain development,25 and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) increase hippocampal neurogenesis, probably through serotonin1A receptor–mediated effects.26-29 Overall, the neurotrophic effects of antidepressants and the fact that they are widely used medications of proven safety make them excellent candidates for use as restorative agents in the subacute phase of stroke and other forms of brain damage.30

We previously reported preliminary evidence that treatment with antidepressants in the subacute phase of stroke is associated with significant improvement in activities of daily living and cognitive functioning among patients who recovered from poststroke depression.31-33 Other investigators have also observed the beneficial effects of antidepressants on motor and cognitive outcomes.8,34 In addition, we reported that early antidepressant treatment was significantly associated with greater long-term survival rates among stroke patients both with and without depression.35 Finally, we demonstrated that escitalopram oxalate, a widely prescribed SSRI, prevents the onset of depressive disorders among patients with an index stroke.36 In the present study, we examine the effect of escitalopram on cognitive outcome. We hypothesized that patients treated with escitalopram would show more improved performance on neuropsychological tests assessing memory and executive functions than patients who received placebo or underwent Problem Solving Therapy (PST). In addition, we hypothesized that this improvement would be related to an effect of antidepressants on neural circuits that process cognitive information and, thus, would occur independendy of a specific rehabilitation program or changes in depressive symptoms.

METHODS

PATIENTS

The 129 patients included in the present study were examined from July 9, 2003, to October 1, 2007, at the University of Iowa Stroke Center. These individuals represent most patients (571 of 799 [71.5%]) assessed for eligibility in our recent multicenter trial investigating the efficacy of escitalopram and PST to prevent the onset of poststroke depression. The characteristics of this trial have been extensively described elsewhere.36 The participants whose data are reported in this article correspond to the Iowan subsample of that study. Patients recruited at the University of Iowa constitute our study group because they had a rigorous longitudinal follow-up of their cognitive status using complete neuropsychological evaluations performed by the same research assistant and supervised by the same neuropsychologist. Thus, these data correspond to the most complete and reliable information on cognitive outcome available for analysis.

All patients were randomized within 3 months of an index stroke (Figure 1). The protocol was approved by the University of Iowa institutional review board, and written informed consent was obtained from cach participant. Inclusion criteria included age older than 50 and younger than 90 years and clinical and imaging findings consistent with cerebral hemisphere, brainstem, or cerebellar stroke. Participants with both ischemic and hemorrhagic strokes were included. Patients were excluded if they met DSM-IV37 diagnostic criteria for major or minor (research criteria) depressive disorder or had a 17-item Hamilton Scale for Depression (HAM-D) score greater than 11.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Patient flowchart. PST indicates Problem-Solving Therapy.

Patients with severe comprehension deficits, as demonstrated by an inability to complete part 1 of the Token Test38 or patients with neuropsychological testing showing impaired decision-making capacity were excluded. Other exclusion criteria included strokes secondary to complications from an intracranial aneurysm, arteriovenous malformation, or neoplastic process and stroke during the course of myocardial infarction or revascularization surgery. General exclusionary criteria included life-threatening organ failure, severely disabling musculoskeletal disorder, cancer, and neurodegenerative disorders, such as idiopathic Parkinson or Alzheimer diseases. In addition, patients were excluded if they met DSM-IV criteria for alcohol or substance abuse or dependence within the 12 months prior to enrollment or if they met DSM-IV criteria for a depressive disorder at the time of the index stroke.

NEUROLOGICAL AND NEURORADIOLOGICAL EVALUATIONS

A complete neurological examination was performed at the time of entry. We classified lesion location as right and left hemispheres or brainstem/cerebellar. Mechanism of ischemic stroke was categorized on the basis of the Trial of Org 10172 in Acute Stroke Treatment classification.39 Therefore, stroke type was classified as hemorrhagic or ischemic, and the ischemic strokes were further categorized as large-artery atherosclerosis, cardioembolic, small-artery occlusion, or an undetermined or other cause. Severity of stroke was recorded using the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale.40 Neuroimaging scans were obtained from the treating hospital for analysis by a radiologist who was masked to the treatment assignment. There was no standardized imaging protocol.

EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN AND TREATMENT

Patients were randomized by 1 team member not involved in any evaluation. Specifically, we randomly assigned the 3 treatments using computer-generated random numbers of 1,2, or 3 to escitalopram (10 mg, once daily in the morning, for patients <65 years and 5 mg for patients ≥65 years), placebo (all pills were identical), or PST. The PST used in this study was a manual-based intervention originally developed in the United Kingdom for treating medical patients with depression41 that has been applied in the United States.42 A rater masked to drug assignment and not involved in the administration of PST evaluated patients.

ASSESSMENT INSTRUMENTS

Patients were administered the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV43 at the initial evaluation and at 3-, 6-, 9-, and 12-month follow-ups. The diagnoses of depression due to stroke with major depressive-like episode and minor depression (research criteria) were based on symptoms elicited by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV and DSM-IV-TR criteria.37,43 The HAM-D, Hamilton Anxiety Gating Scale,44 and Functional Independence Measure (FIM)45 were administered at the initial and each follow-up interviews. Socioeconomic status was determined using the Hollingshead and Redlich classification.46 Adverse effects were recorded using a detailed ad hoc instrument listing the described adverse effects of escitalopram.

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING

The test battery used in this study had an emphasis on memory and executive functioning. Neuropsychological testing was performed at baseline and at the end of the intervention phase for 70 to 85 minutes by a trained technician masked to the patient treatment status. Most of the tests had alternate forms to minimize practice effects during repeated testing, and administration of alternate forms were counterbalanced to prevent order effects. All tests were standardized instruments with published norms appropriate for use with the subjects in this study. The neuropsychological tests included:

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS).47 This 25- to 30-minute battery assesses functioning in 5 domains (immediate memory, visuospatial/constructional, language, attention, and delayed memory). These domains are evaluated by 12 individual subtests, including List Learning, List Recall, List Recognition, Story Memory, Story Recall, Figure Copy, Figure Recall, Line Orientation, Picture Naming, Semantic Fluency, Digit Span, and Coding. This battery provides age-corrected norms for overall performance (total scale score) and scores in each domain.

Trail-Making Test Parts A and B.48 This test assesses psychomotor processing speed and cognitive flexibility.

Controlled Oral Word Association.49 This verbal fluency task assesses one’s ability to initiate and maintain effort and requires the subject to rapidly produce words beginning with specified letters.

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Similarities.50 This test of abstract thinking requires the subjects to identify commonalities between word pairs.

Stroop Test.51 This test of selective attention and response inhibition has 3 trials lasting 45 seconds each: (1) color naming, (2) color reading, and (3) color-word interference, also known as the Stroop effect.

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

We conducted univariate analyses using 2 independent sample t tests or 1-way analysis of variance. Mann-Whitney or Kruskal-Wallis tests were used when data did not follow the statistical assumptions of the t test or analysis of variance, respectively. Categorical data were analyzed using the Fisher exact test or logistic or multinomial regression.

We assessed whether treatment predicted the change in neuropsychological scores between baseline and end of treatment evaluations after controlling for possible confounders. The confounders investigated were age, education, change in HAM-D scores, time elapsed between index stroke and start of treatment, baseline FIM score, and the stroke mechanism. The rationale for considering each of these variables as a confounder include the following: the change in HAM-D scores sought to control for the effect of depression on neuropsychological performance, particularly in the case of executive functions; the time from stroke to initiation of treatment sought to control for the spontaneous recovery of function that is observed during the subacute phase of stroke; stroke mechanism is related to the type and severity of cerebrovascular illness; and baseline FIM scores provide a measure of the severity of stroke when starting escitalopram treatment. We built several multiple linear regression models for each outcome of interest, including the aforementioned covariates. The models with the smallest corrected Akaike information criterion52 were preferred for each of the neuropsychological outcomes. Models’ assumptions were assessed with residual and influence analyses. Regression analyses were performed using the intent-to-treat sample. To this end, we assumed data were missing at random and imputed missing data 5 times using the Markov chain Monte Carlo method.53 All regression results reported are adjusted according to the methods recommended by Rubin.54

All analyses were performed using SAS, version 9.1.3 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, North Carolina). All P values reported are 2-tailed. Significance level was set at P≤.05.

RESULTS

PARTICIPANTS

A total of 129 patients were randomized and 117 began treatment (Figure 1). Twelve (9.3%) patients dropped out before receiving the first drug dose or PST session for the following reasons: 5 participants did not want to commit the time, 4 patients’ families withdrew consent, 2 developed depression, and 1 developed an illness. Of the total 117 patients who began treatment, 7 patients (6.0%) dropped out of the study, 3 within 90 days, 2 between 91 and 180 days, and 2 between 181 and 365 days. Of these dropouts, 2 patients died (sepsis and lung cancer), 2 developed an intercurrent illness judged to be incompatible with their continuation, 2 could not be contacted, and 1 violated the protocol. We compared demographic and baseline variables (eg, age, sex, and baseline HAM-D and FIM scores) in the group of patients who were randomized to treatment with those who dropped out. A logistic regression analysis found no demographic or baseline impairment variables related to post-randomization drop-out.

Demographic characteristics of the intent-to-treat sample (n= 129) are shown in Table 1. There were 45 patients randomized to placebo, 43 to escitalopram, and 41 to PST.

Table 1.

Characteristics of Poststroke Patients Randomized to Receive Placebo, Escitalopram, or Problem-Solving Therapy

N (%)
Placebo
n = 45
Escitalopram
n = 43
Problem-
Solving
Therapy
n = 41
Statistic, df p
value
Age, mean (SD), years 64.2 (13.9) 60.8 (14.4) 68.9 (11.7) F(2,126) = 3.7 0.03
Male 29 (64.4) 26 (60.5) 21 (51.2) Fisher’s Exact 0.78
Married 27 (60.0) 23 (53.5) 19 (46.3) Fisher’s Exact 0.45
Education, mean (SD), years 13.0 (2.9) 13.4 (3.1) 14.4 (2.9) K-W χ2(2) = 4.1 0.13
Right handed 42 (93.3) 35 (81.4) 32 (78.0) Fisher’s Exact 0.64
Socioeconomic class IV-V 13 (28.9) 12 (27.9) 10 (24.4) Fisher’s Exact 0.91
Previous history of mood disorder 2 (4.4) 3 (7.0) 2 (4.9) Fisher’s Exact 0.47
Baseline HDRS score, mean (SD) 6.9 (3.5) 7.3 (3.9) 7.4 (3.4) K-W χ2(2) = 2.1 0.36
Close out HDRS score, mean (SD) 7.1 (6.1) 7.4 (5.0) 6.1 (5.2) K-W χ2(2) = 1.8 0.40
Medical comorbidity
 Systolic blood pressure, mean (SD),
 mm Hg
144.0 (23.1) 137.8 (26.2) 146.8 (20.6) F(2,126) = 2.0 0.14
 Hypertension 34 (75.6) 27 (62.8) 34 (82.9) Fisher’s Exact 0.04
 Low-density lipoprotein cholesterol,
 mean (SD), mg/dL
121.5 (48.1) 118.1 (41.1) 112.1 (42.8) K-W χ2(2) = 0.7 0.70
 Hypercholesterolemia 13 (28.9) 11 (25.6) 10 (24.4) Fisher’s Exact 0.94
 Diabetes mellitus 11 (24.4) 17 (39.5) 8 (19.5) Fisher’s Exact 0.14
 Coronary artery disease 10 (22.2) 13 (30.2) 14 (34.1) Fisher’s Exact 0.39
 Congestive heart failure 2 (4.4) 8 (18.6) 6 (14.6) Fisher’s Exact 0.09
 Atrial fibrillation 6 (13.3) 7 (16.3) 9 (22.0) Fisher’s Exact 0.49
 Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease 3 (6.7) 7 (16.3) 4 (9.8) Fisher’s Exact 0.35
Cumulative illness rating scale total score,
mean (SD)
1.8 (0.5) 1.9 (0.5) 1.8 (0.4) K-W χ2(2) = 0.8 0.68
Stroke characteristics and functional impairment
 Hemorrhagic Stroke 5 (11.1) 2 (4.7) 4 (9.8) Likelihood
Ratio χ2(8) =
10.77
0.22
 Ischemic Stroke
  Large Artery Atherosclerosis 10 (22.2) 13 (30.2) 9 (22.0)
  Cardioembolic 8 (17.8) 13 (30.2) 6 (14.6)
  Small Artery Occlusion 16 (35.6) 5 (11.6) 10 (24.4)
  Unidentified/Other 6 (13.3) 10 (23.3) 12 (29.3)
 Supratentorial lesions 39 (86.7) 33 (76.7) 30 (73.2) Fisher’s Exact 0.27
 Left side lesions 23 (51.1) 25 (58.1) 16 (39.0) Fisher’s Exact 0.21
 Lacunar infarctions 18 (40.0) 12 (27.9) 13 (31.7) Fisher’s Exact 0.49
NIHSS score, mean (SD) 6.2 (2.1) 7.1 (3.0) 5.8 (2.4) F(2,126) = 3.0 0.06
Baseline FIM total score, mean (SD) 120.8 (5.6) 118.9 (12.7) 116.2 (12.4) K-W χ2(2) = 3.0 0.23
Time from index stroke to treatment start,
median (q1, q3) days
25.0
(14.0, 43.0)
30.0
(19.0, 60.0)
32.5
(23.5, 53.5)
K-W χ2(2) = 3.6 0.17

K-W = Kruskal-Wallis Test. q1 = first quartile, q3 = third quartile

BACKGROUND CHARACTERISTICS

Descriptive statistics of baseline demographic, neurological, psychiatric, and functional variables are presented in Table 1. Patients who received PST were older than patients randomized to escitalopram or placebo and had hypertension more often than patients randomized to escitalopram. There were no differences between the 3 groups in the mechanism, type, severity, or location of stroke or baseline FIM scores. Overall, this group of patients had mild to moderate strokes.

One patient (2.2%) in the placebo group, 3 (7.0%) in the escitalopram group, and 2 (4.9%) in the PST group were receiving benzodiazepines. No patient in the placebo group, 1 (2.3%) in the escitalopram group, and 1 (2.4%) in the PST group were receiving cholinesterase inhibitors. We found no evidence of a difference between the 3 groups and the proportion of subjects receiving medications that could affect cognition.

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL VARIABLES

There were no differences between the 3 groups in baseline scores of neuropsychological tests probing memory and executive functioning (Table 2). We looked for differences between treatment groups in change in RBANS total score as well as in its attention, memory, language, and visuospatial/constructional domains. In addition, we examined the difference in Trail-Making Test B and Trail-Making Test A execution times, the Controlled Oral Word Association total score, the Stroop Color-Word interference score, and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Similarities score.

Table 2.

Baseline and Closeout Neuropsychological Scores of Poststroke Patients Randomized to Receive Placebo, Escitalopram, or Problem-Solving Therapy

Placebo Escitalopram Problem-Solving
Therapy
Statistic, df,
p-value for
change scores
Age-corrected RBANS Scores mean (SD, %ile)
Total Scale Score
 Baseline 85.3 (14.4, 16) 80.7 (16.8, 10) 88.2 (16.8, 21) K-W χ2(2) = 10.6
p < 0.01
 Close Out 91.0 (17.8, 27) 89.8 (15.1, 25) 89.1 (15.7, 23)
Delayed Memory
 Baseline 88.1 (17.7, 21) 84.2 (17.7, 14) 88.6 (19.8, 23) K-W χ2(2) = 13.4
p = 0.001
 Close Out 94.2 (20.7, 34) 96.6 (14.3, 42) 89.1 (20.7, 23)
Immediate Memory
 Baseline 91.1 (19.8, 27) 83.4 (20.3, 13) 93.2 (17.2, 32) K-W χ2(2) = 8.4
P = 0.01
 Close Out 98.5 (19.3, 47) 95.1 (17.3, 37) 94.9 (18.0, 37)
Attention
 Baseline 86.2 (17.2, 18) 82.6 (18.4, 13) 90.0 (17.9, 25) K-W χ2(2) = 1.6
P = 0.44
 Close Out 91.6 (16.0, 30) 89.2 (19.6, 23) 91.7 (16.4, 30)
Language
 Baseline 91.4 (9.9, 27) 89.3 (16.3, 23) 94.7 (14.0, 37) K-W χ2(2) = 1.0
P = 0.61
 Close Out 93.0 (10.3, 32) 93.8 (13.4, 34) 97.7 (11.9, 45)
Visuospatial/Constructional
 Baseline 87.4 (18.2, 19) 83.4 (20.7, 13) 88.2 (17.2, 21) K-W χ2(2) = 3.8
p = 0.15
 Close Out 89.0 (19.8, 23) 86.9 (18.0, 19) 85.7 (16.9, 18)
Trail Making Test (TMT)
TMT B Time
 Baseline 146.1 (113.0, 9) 113.6 (76.3, 16) 147.2 (106.8, 16) -
 Close Out 120.6 (75.1, 16) 122.0 (97.4, 9) 141.3 (95.1, 9)
TMT B time - TMT A time, mean (SD)
 Baseline 82.5 (76.0) 58.9 (65.0) 87.2 (86.1) K-W χ2(2) = 1.7
p = 0.42
 Close Out 60.9 (51.6) 65.4 (74.7) 94.5 (84.3)
COWAT Raw Scores
 Baseline 28.8 (13.6, 25) 28.2 (14.4, 16) 28.9 (12.2, 37) K-W χ2(2) = 2.4
p = 0.30
 Close Out 31.8 (13.8, 37) 33.6 (13.6, 37) 30.6 (12.5, 37)
STROOP Color-Word Trial Raw Scores
 Baseline 26.1 (12.9, 8) 29.7 (14.4, 16) 24.3 (12.7, 27) K-W χ2(2) = 0.1
p = 0.96
 Close Out 28.9 (12.5, 14) 30.4 (13.8, 16) 26.6 (11.3, 39)
Age-corrected WAIS-III Similarities Scores
 Baseline 9.8 (3.2, 50) 9.2 (3.0, 37) 10.0 (3.4, 50) K-W χ2(2) = 2.8
p = 0.24
 Close Out 10.3 (3.1, 50) 9.6 (2.9, 50) 10.7(2.9, 63)

K-W = Kruskal-Wallis Test.

RBANS Total Score

Before controlling for other covariates, we found a difference in the change in RBANS total score between the 5 groups (Kruskal-Wallis χ22=10.6, P<.01). The change in score was higher among participants who received escitalopram compared with the participants in the other 2 groups. After controlling for change in HAM-D and the stroke mechanism, we found that the escitalopram group presented a higher change in score compared with the other 2 groups (adjusted mean change in score: escitalopram group=9.9; PST group = 1.9, P<.01; placebo group=4.0, P=.02). Because the objective of this study was to determine the effect of escitalopram on the longitudinal course of cognitive variables, we collapsed the placebo and PST groups into a single group of patients not receiving escitalopram (referred to as the nonescitalopram group) and compared it with the group receiving escitalopram. After controlling for change in HAM-D score and stroke mechanism, there was an effect of escitalopram on the change in RBANS total score (adjusted mean change in score: escitalopram group= 10.0; nonescitalopram group=3.1, P<.01) (Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) scores by patients receiving and not receiving escitaiopram. Error bars indicate SDs.

RBANS Delayed Memory Score

Before controlling for other covariates, we found a difference in the change in RBANS delayed memory score between the 3 groups (Kruskal-Wallis χ22=13.4, P<.01). The change in score was higher among participants receiving escitalopram compared with the participants in the other 2 groups. After controlling for time elapsed between the index stroke and treatment start, change in HAM-D score, stroke mechanism, and the interaction between treatment and change in HAM-D score (not significant), the group receiving escitalopram had a greater change in score than the other 2 groups (adjusted mean change in score; escitalopram group=11.2, PST group=−0.7, P<.001; placebo group=3.9, P=.02). When comparing the escitalopram group with the nonescitalopram group, we observed a significant effect of escitalopram on the change in RBANS delayed memory score after controlling for time elapsed between the index stroke and treatment start, change in HAM-D score, and stroke mechanism (adjusted mean change in score; escitalopram group=11.3, nonescitalopram group=2.5, P<.01) (Figure 2).

RBANS Immediate Memory Score

Before controlling for other covariates, we found a difference in the change in RBANS immediate memory score between the 3 groups (Kruskal-Wallis χ22=8.4, P=.01). The change in score was higher among participants who received escitalopram compared with the participants in the other 2 groups. After controlling for time elapsed between the index stroke and treatment start, change in HAM-D score, stroke mechanism, and the interaction between treatment and change in HAM-D score (not significant), the group receiving escitalopram presented a higher change in score than the other 2 groups (adjusted mean change in score: escitalopram group = 13.4, PST group=2.0, P<.001; placebo group=7.2, P=.04). We also observed an effect of escitalopram on the change in RBANS immediate memory score after controlling for time elapsed between the index stroke and treatment start, change in HAM-D score, and stroke mechanism (adjusted mean change in score: escitalopram group= 13.5, nonescitalopram group=5.3, P=.01) (Figure 2).

We did not find an effect of treatment in the change in RBANS attention, language, or visuospatial/constructional domains, Trail-Making Test, Controlled Oral Word Association, Stroop Color-Word Trial, or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Similarities scores before or after controlling for other covariates.

There was not an effect of age, lesion location, or type or mechanism of stroke on these outcome measures. There were no significant correlations of change in RBANS score with change in HAM-D or Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale scores.

We examined if the reported changes in neuropsychological function had an impact on the functional status of stroke patients as measured by the FIM cognitive domain. We found a difference in changes in the FIM cognitive domain scores between the participants receiving escitalopram and those not receiving it (Mann-Whitney U= 2181, P=.05). Similarly, participants who received escitalopram showed greater changes in FIM memory scores than those receiving placebo or PST (Kruskal-Wallis χ22=6.83, P=.03). When change in FIM memory scores of those receiving escitalopram are compared with those not receiving it, the difference persists (Mann-Whitney U=2291, P=.01). Furthermore, there was a positive correlation between change in RBANS total scores and change in FIM total scores (Spearman ρ=0.25, P=.02). Thus, the reported changes in neuropsychological test scores result in a measurable improvement in activities of daily living related to these changes.

We recorded living conditions before and after stroke for all participants. We also recorded occupational level data before and after the stroke. Most patients (92.2%) did not change living conditions after their stroke. Four (10.3%) participants receiving placebo, 2 (5.6%) participants receiving PST, and 1 (2.9%) participant receiving escitalopram started living in a more dependent environment after their stroke. Despite having slightly less severe strokes, patients receiving placebo were almost 4 times more likely to be in a more structured environment than patients receiving escitalopram after stroke, a difference that was not statistically significant.

There was no difference between groups in the change in occupational level before and after stroke. Eight (20.5%) patients in the placebo group, 5 (13.9%) in the escitalopram group, and 5 (14.3%) in the PST group had worse occupational levels after stroke, while 2 (5.6%) of the subjects receiving escitalopram augmented their occupational level after stroke.

ADVERSE EVENTS AND EFFECTS

The frequency of adverse events or medication adverse effects is shown in Table 3 for all patients who received at least the first drug dose or PST session. There were no differences between groups in the frequency of any of these events. In addition, there were no differences between the groups in the frequency of hospital admissions resulting from gastrointestinal bleeding or falls, which have been previously described as complications of SSRI treatment.

Table 3.

Adverse Events in Poststroke Patients Receiving at Least the First Drug Dose or Problem-Solving Therapy Session

No. (%)
Placebo
n = 41
Escitalopram
n = 39
Problem-
Solving
Therapy
n = 37
Gastrointestinal adverse effects
 Dry mouth 22 (53.7) 14 (35.9) 17 (45.9)
 Constipation 11 (26.8) 13 (33.3) 13 (35.1)
 Indigestion 9 (22.0) 11 (28.2) 15 (40.5)
 Anorexia 5 (12.2) 12 (30.8) 7 (18.9)
 Diarrhea 8 (19.5) 7 (17.9) 13 (35.1)
 Abdominal pain 7 (17.1) 5 (12.8) 9 (24.3)
 Nausea 4 (9.8) 4 (10.3) 8 (21.6)
 Bleeding 1 (2.4) 1 (2.6) 1 (2.7)
Cardiovascular adverse effects
 Tachycardia 32 (78.0) 34 (87.2) 32 (86.5)
 Chest pain 5 (12.2) 8 (20.5) 9 (24.3)
 Palpitations 3 (7.3) 3 (7.7) 4 (10.8)
Other adverse effects
 Dizziness 31 (75.6) 33 (84.6) 31 (83.8)
 Fatigue 18 (43.9) 22 (56.4) 15 (40.5)
 Increased sweating 6 (14.6) 12 (30.8) 11 (29.7)
 Falls 1 (2.4) 1 (2.6) 1 (2.7)
Sexual adverse effects
 Decreased libido 9 (22.0) 11 (28.2) 12 (32.4)
No. (% females)
 Anorgasmia 2 (14.3) 2 (13.3) 2 (11.1)
No. (% males)
 Ejaculation disorders 2 (7.4) 2 (8.3) 3 (15.8)
 Impotence 2 (7.4) 2 (8.3) 3 (15.8)

COMMENT

We examined the effect of escitalopram treatment on cognitive recovery following stroke. Compared with patients receiving placebo or PST, patients receiving escitalopram showed higher scores in neuropsychological tests assessing global cognitive functioning and, specifically, neuropsychological measures probing verbal and visual memory. Importantly, the reported changes in neuropsychological performance resulted in an improvement in related activities of daily living. While one could argue that the findings are due to chance given the number of outcomes tested, most findings represent large effects that translate into P values that would remain significant even after applying a conservative approach of adjusting for multiple comparisons (eg, Bonferroni method).

The beneficial effect of escitalopram on cognitive recovery was independent of its effect on depressive symptoms and was not influenced by stroke type or mechanism of ischemic stroke. In addition, escitalopram was well tolerated and the frequency of adverse effects related to its administration was not different than that observed among patients receiving placebo.

Before discussing the implications of these findings, we must mention the limitations of the present study. First, we obtained reliable neuropsychological data on nondepressed patients with mild to moderate strokes recruited in a specialized unit; thus, the results cannot be extrapolated to other stroke patient groups with different characteristics. Second, patients receiving escitalopram were younger than patients in the other groups. However, age was not a significant factor in our statistical models, suggesting that it did not influence the results. Finally, though we examined initial clinical computed tomography and magnetic resonance imaging scans, we did not perform research studies that would have allowed us to examine the effect of important baseline neuroimaging variables (eg, hippocampal and white matter hyperintensity volumes) on cognitive outcomes.

Given these limitations, what are the implications of the present findings? Cognitive deficits are a major contributor to disability and decreased quality of life after stroke.20 The presence of cognitive impairment has been associated with institutionalization and increased caregiver burden and health care costs.55-58 Furthermore, it has been shown that dementia is a strong predictor of mortality after stroke.59,60 A therapeutic intervention that fosters cognitive recovery of stroke patients has the potential to significantly reduce the burden of this disease.

There are several questions that require clarification before considering SSRI antidepressants as valuable candidates for restorative intervention. Is the cognitive enhancement effect of escitalopram a phenomenon observed only among stroke patients or should we expect a similar effect among other elderly populations? Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors have a beneficial effect on cognitive functions among elderly patients with major depression.61,62 It has also been reported that previous antidepressant use is associated with increased frontal gray matter volumes among elderly patients with depression.63 Serotonin might have a regulatory effect on memory functioning among healthy controls, particularly in what relates to storage and consolidation of new information.64 For instance, acute tryptophan depletion, an experimental paradigm that produces a reduction in serotonergic neurotransmission, has been associated with long-term memory impairment, an effect that is independent of mood or attentional changes.64,65 On the other hand, acute intravenous administration of 10 mg of citalopram was shown to enhance verbal memory consolidation.66 However, these acute effects might be qualitatively different than those related to the chronic administration of SSRIs among stroke patients. The early stages of recovery from stroke are characterized by increased expression of neurotrophic factors, increased expression of proteins involved in synaptogenesis and axonal sprouting, increased release of inflammatory cytokines, and proliferation of neural stem cells.67-69 These changes might provide a unique biological environment that may enhance the effect of antidepressants.

We have hypothesized that escitalopram would have a beneficial effect on memory and executive functions. However, an effect was only observed in immediate memory and delayed recall. This is consistent with the literature on serotonergic modulation of cognitive functioning among healthy subjects. Acute tryptophan depletion impairs memory consolidation but does not affect executive functions associated with dorsolateral prefrontal circuits.70-73 Conversely, tryptophan supplementation has been associated with improved performance in memory tests.74,75 Although there is some evidence of serotonergic modulation of reversal learning and decision-making tasks related to ventromedial prefrontal circuits,76,77 these types of tasks were not included in the neuropsychological battery used in the present study.

Previous studies show that drugs modulating aminergic transmission have a beneficial effect on stroke recovery.16 However, a meta-analysis of the efficacy of amphetamine to enhance recovery after stroke could not reach a definitive conclusion on this issue and recommended further research.78 As demonstrated in patients with neurodegenerative conditions, cholinesterase inhibitors improve their performance in memory tests. A similar beneficial effect has also been reported in patients with cerebrovascular disease.14,15,18,19 Cholinergic agents, such as donepezil, may also improve fluency and comprehension among aphasic patients.13

The present findings suggest that chronic administration of SSRIs may also improve cognitive function in patients with stroke. The magnitude of this change appears to be greater for escitalopram than cholinesterase inhibitors (ie, 0.15-0.22 standard deviation [SD] units in the case of cholinesterase inhibitors vs 0.44-0.74 SD units for escitalopram).79 We must acknowledge, however, that the efficacy of cholinesterase inhibitors was tested on multicenter trials, which usually present smaller effect sizes than single-center studies. In addition, SSRIs have a more benign adverse effect profile than cholinesterase inhibitors. For instance, mortality was significantly greater among stroke patients who received donepezil compared with patients who received placebo (odds ratio, 4.57; 95% confidence interval, 1.3-16.1).79 Antidepressants, on the other hand, might increase survival after stroke.35 Thus, long-term administration of SSRIs appears to be an effective and safe treatment option to improve cognitive outcomes among patients with cerebrovascular disease.

Another important question relates to the mechanisms involved in this effect of escitalopram. Cognitive function recovery following stroke is a complex process mediated by multiple mechanisms, including enhanced regional metabolism, resolution of diaschisis, and neuroplastic changes.80 Serotonergic agents may exert their effects at different levels of the brain, interacting with other neurotransmitter systems (eg, modulation of cholinergic transmission in forebrain structures81 or modulation of prefrontal function).76 However, there is growing evidence that SSRIs produce neuroplastic changes in the hippocampus and the cerebral cortex. For instance, fluoxetine increases the proliferation of neuronal precursors in the subgranular zone of the dentate gyrus.24 A recent study has also shown that fluoxetine restores cortical plasticity in the visual system of adult amblyopic rats, fostering the recovery of visual function. This effect is probably mediated by a decrease in intracortical inhibition and increased expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor.82 Similarly, sertraline appears to enhance plastic changes in the visual cortex of human subjects.83 Furthermore, sertraline may prevent brain atrophy, improve motor performance, and delay mortality in a rat model of Huntington disease.84 Although a causal relationship between structural changes and behavioral performance has not been established, we can speculate that the improvement in verbal and visual memory observed in the escitalopram group is probably related to increased dentate gyrus neurogenesis and remodeling of the hippocampal circuitry. In this sense, Dupret et al85 reported that selective ablation of newly generated hippocampal neurons of transgenic mice results in severe deficits in spatial relational memory, suggesting that hippocampal neurogenesis is necessary for complex forms of learning.

Overall, whatever may be the mechanism of improved cognitive recovery, this study has shown, for the first time, that escitalopram, an SSRI, is associated with improved cognitive recovery following stroke compared with placebo and PST. The utility of antidepressants in the process of poststroke recovery deserves to be further investigated.

Acknowledgments

Funding/Support: This work was supported solely by grant RO1 MH-65134 from the National Institute of Mental Health.

Role of the Sponsor: The National Institute of Mental Health played no role in the design or conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript. All of the study medications were purchased using National Institute of Mental Health grant funds.

All of the authors received salary contributions from the grant supporting this study. During the past 5 years, Dr Jorge received 2 travel awards to participate in national meetings from the former Hamilton Pharmaceutical Company and Avanir Pharmaceutical Company. Dr Adams received research, speaking, or consultation support from Merck, NMT Medical, Eli Lilly and Company, Centocor, Sanofi-Bristol-Meyers-Squibb, Boerhringer-Ingelheim, and Schering-Plough and served on data safety and monitoring boards of trials funded by AstraZeneca and Glaxo-Smith-Kline. Dr Robinson served as a consultant to the former Hamilton Pharmaceutical Company and Avanir Pharmaceutical Company; received support or honoraria from Lubeck, Forest Laboratories, and Pfizer; and was on the speakers’ bureaus for Forest Laboratories and Pfizer. Although Forest Laboratories provided honoraria and expenses through their speakers’ bureau to Dr Robinson, none of the design, analysis, or expenses (including the cost of medications) of this study were supported by monies, materials, or any intellectual input from Forest Laboratories.

Footnotes

Author Contributions: Dr Jorge had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Ms Acion performed the statistical analysis for this study.

Financial Disclosures: None of the other aforementioned pharmaceutical companies had any financial interest in this prevention study.

Additional Contributions: The authors thank Stephanie Rosazza, our study coordinator, and Teresa Kopel, who is certified in the administration of PST.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Mackay J, et al. The Atlas of Heart Disease and Stroke. Switzerland: World Health Organization; Geneva: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Rosamond W, Flegal K, Furie K, Go A, Greenlund K, Haase N, Hallpem SM, Ho M, Howard V, Kissela B, Kittner S, Lloyd-Jones D, McDermott M, Meigs J, Moy C, Nichol G, O’Donnell C, Roger V, Sorlie P, Steinberger J, Thom T, Wilson M, Hong Y. American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee. Heart disease and stroke statistics–2008 update: a report from the American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee. Circulation. 2008;117(4):e25–e146. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.187998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Klijn CJ, Hankey GJ. American Stroke Association and European Stroke Initiative. Management of acute ischaemic stroke: new guidelines from the American Stroke Association and European Stroke Initiative. Lancet Neurol. 2003;2(11):698–701. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(03)00558-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Norrving B, Adams RJ. Organized stroke care. Stroke. 2006;37(2):326–32B. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000200554.95094.09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Leira EC, Adams HP., Jr Management of acute ischemic stroke. Clin Geriatr Med. 1999;15(4):701–720. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Tissue plasminogen activator for acute ischemic stroke: The National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke rt-PA Stroke Study Group. N Engl J Med. 1995;333(24):1581–1587. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199512143332401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Caplan LR. Tissue plasminogen activator for acute Ischemic stroke. N Engl J Med. 1999;341(16):1240–1241. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199910143411618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dam M, Tonin P, De Boni A, Pizzolato G, Casson S, Ermani M, Freo U, Plron L, Battistin L. Effects of fluoxetine and maprotiline on functional recovery in post-stroke hemiplegic patients undergoing rehabilitation therapy. Stroke. 1996;27(7):1211–1214. doi: 10.1161/01.str.27.7.1211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Stroemer RP, Kent TA, Hulsebosch CE. Enhanced neocortical neural sprouting, synaptogenesls, and behavioral recovery with D-amphetamine therapy after neocortical infarction in rats. Stroke. 1998;29(11):2381–2395. doi: 10.1161/01.str.29.11.2381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Scheidtmann K. Advances in adjuvant pharmacotherapy for motor rehabilitation: effects of levodopa. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2004;22(3-5):393–398. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gladstone DJ, Danells CJ, Armesto A, Mcllroy WE, Staines WR, Graham SJ, Herrmann N, Szalal JP. Black 5E: Subacute Therapy with Amphetamine and Rehabilitation for Stroke Study Investigators. Physiotherapy coupled with dextroamphetamine for rehabilitation after hemiparetic stroke: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Stroke. 2006;37(1):179–185. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000195169.42447.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sonde L, Lokk J. Effects of amphetamine and/or L-dopa and physiotherapy after stroke: a blinded randomized study. Acta Neurol Scand. 2007;115(1):55–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2006.00728.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Berthier ML, Green C, Higueras C, Fernández I, Hlnojosa J, Martin MC. A randomized, placebo-controlled study of donepezil in poststroke aphasia. Neurology. 2006;67(9):1687–1689. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000242626.69666.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Black S, Román GC, Geldmacher DS, Salloway S, Hecker J, Burns A, Perdomo C, Kumar D, Pratt R, Donepezil 307 Vascular Dementia Study Group Efficacy and tolerabillty of donepezil in vascular dementia: positive results of a 24-week, multi-center, international, randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Stroke. 2003;34(10):2323–2330. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000091396.95360.E1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Erkinjuntti T, Kurz A, Gauthier S, Bullock R, Lillenfeld S, Damaraju CV. Efficacy of galantamine in probable vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease combined with cerebrovascular disease: a randomised trial. Lancet. 2002;359(9314):1283–1290. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)08267-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Goldstein LB. Effects of amphetamines and small related molecules on recovery after stroke in animals and man. Neuropharmacology. 2000;39(5):852–859. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(99)00249-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Goldstein LB. Amphetamines and related drugs in motor recovery after stroke. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am. 2003;14(1):S125–S134. x. doi: 10.1016/s1047-9651(02)00060-8. (suppl) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Román GC, Wilkinson DG, Doody RS, Black SE, Salloway SP, Schlndler RJ. Donepezil in vascular dementia: combined analysis of two large-scale clinical trials. Dement Getiatr Cogn Disord. 2005;20(61):338–344. doi: 10.1159/000088494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wilkinson D, Doody R, Helme R, Taubman K, Mintzer J, Kertesz A, Pratt RD. Donepezil 308 Study Group. Donepezil in vascular dementia: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. Neurology. 2003;61(4):479–486. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000078943.50032.fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hachinskl V. The 2005 Thomas Willis Lecture: stroke and vascular cognitive impairment: a transdisciplinary, translational and transactional approach. Stroke. 2007;38(4):1396. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000260101.08944.e9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Duman RS, Heninger GR, Nestier EJ. A molecular and cellular theory of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54(7):597–606. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830190015002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Duman RS, Monteggia LM. A neurotrophic model for stress-related mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(12):1116–1127. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Manji HK, Duman RS. Impairments of neuroplastlclty and cellular resilience in severe mood disorders: Implications for the development of novel therapeutics. Psychopharmacol Bull. 2001;35(2):5–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Santarelli L, Saxe M, Gross C, Surget A, Battaglia F, Dulawa S, Welsstaub N, Lee J, Duman R, Arancio O, Belzung C, Hen R. Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science. 2003;301(5634):805–809. doi: 10.1126/science.1083328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gaspar P, Cases O, Maroteaux L. The developmental role of serotonin: news from mouse molecular genetics. Nat Rev Neuroscl. 2003;4(12):1002–1012. doi: 10.1038/nrn1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Jacobs BL, Praag H, Gage FH. Adult brain neurogenesis and psychiatry: a novel theory of depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5(3):262–269. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Radley JJ, Jacobs BL. 5-HT1A receptor antagonist administration decreases cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus. Brain Res. 2002;955(1-2):264–267. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03477-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Banasr M, Hery M, Printemps R, Daszuta A. Serotonin-induced increases in adult cell proliferation and neurogenesis are mediated through different and common 5-HT receptor subtypes in the dentate gyrus and the subventricular zone. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(3):450–460. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Encinas JM, Vaahtokarl A, Enikolopov G. Fluoxetine targets early progenitor cells in the adult brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103(21):8233–8238. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601992103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cramer SC. Repairing the human brain after stroke, II: restorative therapies. Ann Neuol. 2008;63(5):549–560. doi: 10.1002/ana.21412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Chemerlnski E, Robinson RG, Kosier JT. Improved recovery in activities of daily living associated with remission of post-stroke depression. Stroke. 2001;32(1):113–117. doi: 10.1161/01.str.32.1.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kimura M, Robinson RG, Kosier T. Treatment of cognitive impairment after post-stroke depression. Stroke. 2000;31(7):1482–1486. doi: 10.1161/01.str.31.7.1482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Murata Y, Kimura M, Robinson RG. Does cognitive impairment cause post-stroke depression? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;8(4):310–317. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Reding MJ, Orto LA, Winter SW, Fortuna IM, Di Ponte P, McDowell FH. Antidepressant therapy after stroke: a double-blind trial. Arch Neurol. 1986;43(8):763–765. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1986.00520080011011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Jorge RE, Robinson RG, Arndt S, Starkstein S. Mortality and post-stroke depression: a placebo controlled trial of antidepressants. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(10):1823–1829. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Robinson RG, Jorge RE, Moser DJ, Acion L, Solodkin A, Small SL, Fonzetti P, Hegel M, Arndt S. Escitalopram and problem-solving therapy for prevention of poststroke depression: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2008;299(20):2391–2400. doi: 10.1001/jama.299.20.2391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 2000. text revision. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.De Renzi E, Vignolo LA. The token test: a sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain. 1962;65:665–678. doi: 10.1093/brain/85.4.665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Adams HP, Jr, Bendixen BH, Kappelle LJ, Biller J, Love BB, Gordon DL, Marsh EE., III Classification of subtype of acute ischemic stroke: definitions for use in a multicenter clinical trial. TOAST. Trial of Org 10172 in Acute Stroke Treatment. Stroke. 1993;24(1):35–41. doi: 10.1161/01.str.24.1.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kunitz SC, Gross CR, Heyman A, Kase CS, Mohr JP, Price TR, Wolf PA. The pilot stroke data bank: Definition, design and data. Stroke. 1984;15(4):740–746. doi: 10.1161/01.str.15.4.740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Mynors-Wallis LM, Gath DH, Lloyd-Thomas AR, Tomlinson D. Randomised controlled trial comparing problem solving treatment with amitryptyline and placebo for major depression in primary care. BMJ. 1995;310(6977):441–445. doi: 10.1136/bmj.310.6977.441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hegel MT, Barrett JE, Cornell JE, Oxman TE. Predictors of response to problem-solving treatment of depression in primary care. Behavior Therapy. 2002;33(4):511–527. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) Biometric Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York, NY: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety state of rating. Br J Med Psychol. 1959;32(1):50–55. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Forer S, Granger CV. Functional Independence Measure. The Buffalo General Hospital State University of New York at Buffalo; Buffalo, NY: 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hollingshead AB, Redlich FC. Social Class and Mental Illness: A Community Study. Wiley; New York, NY: 1958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Randolph C. Repeatabie Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status. The Psychological Corporation; New York, NY: 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Army Individual Test Battery . Manual ot Directions and Scoring. War Department, Adjutant General’s Office; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Benton AL, Hamsher K, Slvan AB. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. AJA Associates; Iowa City, IA: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale. 3rd ed Harcourt Brace and Co; San Antonio, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Golden CJ. Stroop Color and Word Test. Stoelting; Chicago, IL: 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sugiura N. Further analysis of the data by Akaike’s information criterion and the finite corrections. Comm Statist Theory Methods. 1978;7(1):13–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Schafer JL. Analysis of Incomplete Multivariate Data: Monographs on Statistics and Applied Probability 72. Chapman & Hall; New York. NY: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Rubin DB. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. Wiley; New York, NY: 1987. Wilsy Series in Probability and Mathematical Statistics. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hachlnskl V, Ladecola C, Petersen RC, Breteler MM, Nyenhuis DL, Black SE, Powers WJ, DeCarll C, Merino JG, Kalarla RN, Vinters HV, Holtzman DM, Rosenberg GA, Wallin A, Dichgans M, Marler JR, Leblanc GG. National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke-Canadian Stroke Network vascular cognitive impairment harmonization standards. Stroke. 2006;37(9):2220–2241. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000237236.88823.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.King P, Song X, Rockwood K. CIVIC Investigators. Cognitive impairment of acute onset in the Consortium to Investigate Vascular Impairment of Cognition (CIVIC) study: occurrence, correlates, and outcomes. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(10):893–896. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000224603.52403.dc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Moorhouse P, Rockwood K. Vascular cognitive impairment: current concepts and clinical developments. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7(3):246–255. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70040-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rockwood K, Brown M, Merry H, Sketris I, Fisk J. Vascular Cognitive Impairment Investigators of the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Societal costs of vascular cognitive Impairment In older adults. Stroke. 2002;33(6):1605–1609. doi: 10.1161/01.str.0000017878.85274.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wolfson C, Wolfson DB, Asgharian M, M’Lan CE, Ostbye T, Rockwood K, Hogan DB. Clinical Progression of Dementia Study Group. A revaluation of the duration of survival after the onset of dementia. N Engl J Med. 2001;344(15):1111–1116. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200104123441501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Knopman DS, Rocca WA, Cha RH, Edland SD, Kokmen E. Survival study of vascular dementia in Rochester, Minnesota. Arch Neurol. 2003;60(1):85–90. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.1.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bondareff W, Alpert M, Friedhoff AJ, Richter EM, Clary CM, Batzar E. Comparison of sertraline and nortriptyline in the treatment o1 major depressive disorder in late life. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(5):729–736. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Finkel SI, Richter EM, Clary CM. Comparative efficacy and safety of sertraline versus nortriptyline in major depression in patients 70 and older. Int Psychogeriatr. 1999;11(1):85–99. doi: 10.1017/s104161029900561x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Lavretsky H, Roybal DJ, Ballmaier M, Toga AW, Kumar A. Antidepressant exposure may protect against decrement in frontal gray matter volumes in geriatric depression. J Clin Psychiatry. 2005;66(8):964–967. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v66n0801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Schmitt JA, Wingen M, Ramaekers JG, Evers EA, Riedel WJ. Serotonin and human cognitive performance. Curr Pharm Des. 2006;12(20):2473–2486. doi: 10.2174/138161206777698909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Sambeth A, Riedel WJ, Tlllle DE, Blokland A, Postma A, Schmitt JA. Memory impairments in humans after acute tryptophan depletion using a novel gelatin-based protein drink. J Psychopharmacol. 2009;23(1):56–64. doi: 10.1177/0269881108089577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Harmer CJ, Bhagwagar Z, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Acute administration of citalopram facilitates memory consolidation in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;163(1):106–110. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1151-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Vandenbosch R, Borgs L, Beukelaers P, Belachew S, Moonen G, Nguyen L, Malgrange B. Adult neurogenesis and the diseased brain. Curr Med Chem. 2009;16(6):652–666. doi: 10.2174/092986709787458371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Macas J, Nern C, Plate KH, Momma S. Increased generation of neuronal progenitors after ischemic injury in the aged adult human forebrain. JNeurosci. 2006;26(50):13114–13119. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4667-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Jin K, Wang X, Xie L, Mao XO, Zhu W, Wang Y, Shen J, Mao Y, Banwait S, Greenberg DA. Evidence for stroke-induced neurogenesis in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103(35):13198–13202. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0603512103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Merens W, Willem Van der Does AJ, Spinhoven P. The effects of serotonin manipulations on emotional Information processing and mood. J Affect Disord. 2007;103(1-3):43–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.01.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Harrison BJ, Olver JS, Norman TR, Burrows GD, Wesnes KA, Nathan PJ. Selective effects of acute serotonin and catecholamine depletion on memory in healthy women. J Psychopharmacol. 2004;18(1):32–40. doi: 10.1177/0269881104040225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Evers EA, Tillie DE, van dar Veen FM, Lieben CK, Jolles J, Deutz NE, Schmitt JA. Effects of a novel method of acute tryptophan depletion on plasma tryptophan and cognitive performance in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;178(1):92–99. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2141-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Riedel WJ, Klaassen T, Deutz NE, van Someren A, van Praag HM. Tryptophan depletion in normal volunteers produces selective impairment in memory consolidation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;141(4):362–369. doi: 10.1007/s002130050845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Booij L, Merens W, Markus CR, Van der Does AJ. Diet rich in alpha-lactalbumin improves memory in unmedicated recovered depressed patients and matched controls. J Psychopharmacol. 2006;20(4):526–535. doi: 10.1177/0269881105057466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Markus CR, Olivier B, de Haan EH. Whey protein rich in alpha-lactalbumin increases the ratio of plasma tryptophan fo the sum of the other large neutral amino acids and improves cognitive performance in stress-vulnerable subjects. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002;75(6):1051–1056. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/75.6.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Robbins TW. Chemistry of the mind: neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cortical function. J Comp Neurol. 2005;493(1):140–146. doi: 10.1002/cne.20717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, Blackshaw AJ, Swainson R, Wynne K, Baker NB, Hunter J, Carthy T, Booker E, London M, Deakin JF, Sahaklan BJ, Robbins TW. Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;20(4):322–339. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00091-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Martinsson L, Hardemark H, Eksborg S. Amphetamines for improving recovery after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2007;(1):CD002090. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002090.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Kavlrajan H, Schneider LS. Efficacy and adverse effects of cholinesterase inhibitors and memantine in vascular dementia: a meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6(9):782–792. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70195-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Feeney DM, De Smet AM, Rai S. Noradrenergic modulation of hemiplegia: facilitation and maintenance of recovery. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2004;22(3-5):175–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Ogren SO, Eriksson TM, Elvander-Tottie E, D’Addario C, Ekström JC, Svenningsson P, Melster B, Kehr J, Stiedl O. The role of 5-HT(1A) receptors in learning and memory. Behav Brain Res. 2008;195(1):54–77. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Maya Vetencourt JF, Sale A, Visgl A, Baroncelli L, De Pasquale R, O’Leary OF, Castrén E, Maffei L. The antidepressant fluoxetine restores plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science. 2008;320(5874):385–388. doi: 10.1126/science.1150516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Normann C, Schmllz D, Fürmaier A, Döing C, Bach M. Long-term plasticity of visually evoked potentials in humans is altered in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(5):373–380. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Peng Q, Masuda N, Jiang M, Li Q, Zhao M, Ross CA, Duan W. The antidepressant sertraline improves the phenotype, promotes neurogenesis and increases BDNF levels in the R6/2 Huntington’s disease mouse model. Exp Neurol. 2008;210(1):154–163. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2007.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Dupret D, Revest JM, Koehl M, Ichas F, De Giorgl F, Costert P, Abrous DN, Piazza PV. Spatial relational memory requires hippocampal adult neurogenesis. PLoS One. 2008;3(4):e1959. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES