Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jul 22.
Published in final edited form as: Res Soc Work Pract. 2010 Aug 18;21(1):32–42. doi: 10.1177/1049731509355812

Effects of Cognitive Enhancement Therapy on Employment Outcomes in Early Schizophrenia: Results From a Two-Year Randomized Trial

Shaun M Eack 1,2,5, Gerard E Hogarty 2,3, Deborah P Greenwald 2, Susan S Hogarty 2, Matcheri S Keshavan 2,4
PMCID: PMC3718562  NIHMSID: NIHMS486517  PMID: 23885163

Abstract

Objective

To examine the effects of psychosocial cognitive rehabilitation on employment outcomes in a randomized controlled trial for individuals with early course schizophrenia.

Method

Early course schizophrenia outpatients (N = 58) were randomly assigned to Cognitive Enhancement Therapy (CET) or an Enriched Supportive Therapy (EST) control and treated for two years. Comprehensive data on cognition and employment were collected annually.

Results

Individuals treated with CET were significantly more likely to be competitively employed, had greater earnings from employment, and were more satisfied with their employment status by the end of treatment compared to EST recipients. Mediator analyses revealed that improvements in both social and non-social cognition mediated the CET effects on employment.

Conclusion

CET can help facilitate employment in early schizophrenia, by addressing the cognitive impairments that limit functioning in the disorder. Inclusion of cognitive rehabilitation in social work practice can support more optimal functional recovery from schizophrenia.


Schizophrenia is a chronic and disabling mental disorder that places significant burden on the individuals who suffer from it, their families, and society (Thaker & Carpenter, 2001). Although the advent of antipsychotic medications has helped people with schizophrenia move beyond the confines of the mental hospital and experience a greater control over hallucinations, delusions, and paranoia, evidence indicates that such individuals continue to experience significant disability in the community even when the cardinal symptoms of psychosis have been well controlled (Swartz et al., 2007). The limitations of antipsychotic therapy highlight the importance of providing adjunctive psychosocial treatment to facilitate a greater level of functional recovery among this population (Hogarty, Goldberg, Schooler, & the Collaborative Study Group, 1974).

Social work has and continues to play a major role in the development, testing, and implementation of psychosocial treatments for people with schizophrenia. Some notable efforts led by social workers include Major Role Therapy (one of the first rigorously tested forms of social casework among this population; Hogarty, Goldberg, Schooler, Ulrich, & the Collaborative Study Group, 1974), Assertive Community Treatment (Stein & Test, 1980), Family Psychoeducation (Anderson, Hogarty, & Reiss, 1980; Hogarty et al., 1986), Strengths-Based Case Management (Rapp, 1998), and Personal Therapy (Hogarty, 2002). All of these interventions have been shown to be highly effective at reducing psychotic relapse and hospitalization, and increasing community tenure when used in conjunction with antipsychotic medication. Most of these psychosocial treatments focus on increasing the availability of community supports and resources through assertive outreach, or buffering the impact of stress through applied coping techniques and family alliance. In addition to treatment development and evaluation efforts, social workers continue to be the primary providers of psychosocial services to individuals with schizophrenia (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2001).

Although previous psychosocial treatment efforts have been met with success by further reducing the need for hospitalization and increasing community tenure, a great deal of residual disability remains in the majority of individuals, even after the implementation of effective psychosocial treatment. Hogarty, Greenwald, et al. (1997) noted at the end of their 3-year trials of Personal Therapy and Family Psychoeducation, that while adjustment had significantly improved and psychotic relapse had reached its likely floor (13% over 3 years; Hogarty, Kornblith, et al., 1997), “these clinically meaningful but relative improvements would not qualify as optimal recovery from schizophrenia (p. 1523).” Residual disability in employment has been particularly problematic for individuals with schizophrenia (Rosenheck et al., 2006). While the majority of people with the disorder demonstrate a desire to work, studies estimate that only approximately 20% are able to achieve competitive employment (Mechanic, Blider, & McAlpine, 2002). Supported employment programs have helped to remediate this problem to a degree (Bond, Drake, & Becker, 2008), however, the achievement and maintenance of independent competitive employment remains a serious problem. Helping individuals return to work after the development of schizophrenia is a high priority for both policy makers and practitioners, and there is an urgent need to identify novel treatment targets for the disorder and apply effective new approaches as early as possible to enhance employment and other functional outcomes.

Cognition as a Critical Target for Psychosocial Intervention in Schizophrenia

Cognition has emerged as a promising new target for addressing the residual functional limitations that many people with schizophrenia experience, including employment difficulties (Green et al., 2004). While cognitive impairments have been observed and studied in schizophrenia for many years (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, 1998), only recently have concerted efforts begun to develop methods of improving cognition in the disorder. To date, research has shown that individuals with schizophrenia experience a wide range of cognitive impairments that have been broadly separated into two categories: neurocognition and social cognition. Neurocognitive impairments represent difficulties in basic cognitive processes, such as attention, memory, and problem-solving skills. Individuals with chronic schizophrenia have repeatedly been shown to perform nearly 1 SD below the mean of healthy individuals on a variety of neurocognitive tests (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, 1998), although no single domain appears to be particularly affected, indicating a generalized impairment in basic cognitive processing.

Social cognition refers to those higher-order cognitive processes that specifically support interpersonal interactions. Definitions of social cognition vary, but most consider the processing and interpretation of socio-emotional information in oneself and others to be central (Wyer & Srull, 1994). Under this rubric falls such widely studied constructs as perspective-taking (Taylor, 1988), theory of mind (Premack & Woodruff, 1978), emotion perception (Ekman, 1993), social context appraisal (Hilton, 1995), and emotion regulation (Gross, 1998). Social cognition is known to develop over the lifespan (Selman & Schultz, 1990), with adolescence being a particularly critical time period for the refinement of various social-cognitive abilities. It has been proposed that the onset of schizophrenia during late adolescence disrupts the development of critical aspects of social cognition that are needed for effective transition to adulthood (Keshavan & Hogarty, 1999). Although research on social-cognitive impairments in schizophrenia is only recently emerging, a variety of studies exist showing that people with this disorder have substantial difficulty with taking the perspective of others (Brune, 2005), recognizing emotional and social cues (Corrigan & Green, 1993), and regulating stress and negative emotions (Zubin & Spring, 1977).

Related impairments in both neurocognition and social cognition have emerged as potentially critical targets for intervention in schizophrenia for a variety of reasons. As discussed above, difficulties are observed in many of these cognitive domains among persons with schizophrenia. In addition, evidence indicates that these impairments in cognition begin early (Saykin et al., 1994), persist throughout the course of the disorder (Hoff, Svetina, Shields, Stewart, & DeLisi, 2005), and have a limited response to antipsychotic medications (Keefe et al., 2007; Sergi et al., 2007). Most importantly, however, research has repeatedly shown that these difficulties in cognitive processing are among the strongest predictors of functional recovery from the disorder, even more so than the symptoms of psychosis (Brekke, Kay, Lee, & Green, 2005; Couture, Penn, & Roberts, 2006; Green, Kern, Braff, & Mintz, 2000). Vocational impairments, specifically, have shown stronger relations with cognitive difficulties (Eack et al., in press; Nuechterlein et al., 2008), and a large study by Rosenheck and colleagues (2006) found that individuals with schizophrenia who demonstrated better cognitive function were significantly more likely to be employed. This is not surprising given that limited attentional capacity, poor working memory, and an inability to understand the perspective of others and identify important cues from the social context makes it exceedingly difficult for a person with schizophrenia to succeed at work. Although efforts to develop novel pharmacotherapeutics to address cognitive impairments are underway, thus far improvements from molecular cognitive enhancers have been limited (Buchanan et al., 2007; Buchanan et al., 2008). A psychosocial approach that addresses these disabling and untreated impairments in cognition would be of substantial benefit for individuals with schizophrenia, and perhaps support the achievement of competitive employment in this population.

Cognitive Enhancement Therapy: A Psychosocial Treatment for Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenia

The recognition of cognitive impairments as a significant barrier to functional recovery from schizophrenia has hastened the development and application of psychosocial cognitive rehabilitation methods in an attempt to address these problems. The underlying assumption behind cognitive rehabilitation is that environmental experiences can affect how the brain processes information, and that the provision of enriched environmental experiences can improve brain function and associated cognitive abilities (Keshavan & Hogarty, 1999). Such enriched experiences usually consist of novel cognitive exercises, sometimes computer-based, that resemble exercise programs for the brain. For many years, researchers in the field of traumatic brain injury have observed the beneficial effects of cognitive rehabilitation on people with focal brain injuries (Ben-Yishay et al., 1985), but these treatments have only recently been extended to individuals with psychiatric disorders.

Cognitive Enhancement Therapy (CET) is a comprehensive cognitive rehabilitation approach that was developed by the late social work researcher, G. E. Hogarty and his colleagues for addressing cognitive impairments in schizophrenia (Hogarty et al., 2004; Hogarty & Greenwald, 2006). CET was designed for individuals with schizophrenia in the recovery phase of their illness (i.e., individuals stabilized on antipsychotic medication). Unlike the majority of cognitive rehabilitation methods, CET is uniquely integrated and explicitly targets problems in both neurocognition and social cognition. Neurocognitive impairments are addressed through 60 hours of computer-based exercises designed to improve attention, memory, and problem-solving abilities. Impairments in social cognition are targeted during the course of 45, 1.5 hour social-cognitive group therapy sessions, where participants learn various social-cognitive abilities (e.g., perspective-taking, social context appraisal, recognizing emotional cues) and apply these abilities during in vivo cognitive exercises, homework, and unrehearsed social situations in the group setting (see Methods for more details regarding CET).

To date, CET has been tested in two NIMH-funded randomized controlled trials. The initial study was conducted with 121 individuals with long-term schizophrenia (average illness duration = 15.70 years). Participants were randomly assigned to two years of treatment with CET or an active Enriched Supportive Therapy (EST) control condition. Results indicated that by the end of treatment, participants in CET demonstrated significant and large improvements in both neurocognitive and social-cognitive domains. Most importantly, CET effects were not limited to improved cognitive testing scores, but as hypothesized, generalized to significant improvement in social adjustment, particularly work readiness, instrumental task performance, and social and major role functioning (Hogarty et al., 2004). A 1-year post-treatment follow-up study of 106 of the 121 individuals who completed the trial demonstrated that both the cognitive and functional gains made by CET participants were maintained even a year after treatment had ended (Hogarty, Greenwald, & Eack, 2006). To date, this remains the largest and longest study of cognitive rehabilitation for schizophrenia, and highlights the promise of psychosocial interventions for addressing persistent cognitive impairments in the disorder.

We recently completed a second study of CET with 58 individuals in the early course of schizophrenia (average illness duration = 3.19 years). It was hypothesized that early intervention with CET might capitalize on cognitive resources that were more preserved during the initial phases of the disorder, and that the functional benefits of CET might be even greater when applied to a less chronic sample. Preliminary 1-year effects from this trial comparing CET to EST again showed strong improvements in social cognition favoring CET (Eack, Hogarty, Greenwald, Hogarty, & Keshavan, 2007). After two years of treatment, highly significant and robust differential effects were observed in neurocognition, social cognition, symptomatology, and social adjustment. As suspected, functional improvement was even larger in this early course trial, with individuals treated with CET demonstrating over 50% improvement in functioning, with strong effects on social functioning, global adjustment, activities of daily living, and even competitive employment (Eack et al., in press).

Strong effects on social functioning and global adjustment were expected in this study, given previous results (Hogarty et al., 2004); however, improvements in competitive employment were surprising, given that CET is not an employment program. The broad number of domains investigated in this trial precluded us from more closely examining the effects of CET on employment outcomes, beyond a cursory analysis of the proportion of individuals who gained competitive employment (Eack et al., in press). Of particular importance, it also remains unclear whether improvements in the primary target of CET (cognition) served as a mechanism for these important vocational gains. Using data collected during this early course trial, we now seek to more closely assess the effects of CET on employment by examining several critical employment outcomes in the same participants and evaluating the degree to which specific gains in neurocognition and social cognition contributed to improved employment in CET recipients.

Method

Study Design and Setting

This research consisted of a two-year randomized-controlled trial comparing CET to EST. Each treatment condition was provided for a total of two years, and participants were assessed on a variety of employment and cognitive measures prior to participation and every year following the initiation of psychosocial treatment. Participants were randomized to CET or EST using a 1:1 ratio by way of computer-generated random numbers. The study was located in a specialty outpatient clinic in the comprehensive care service at the University of Pittsburgh Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, which serves the community mental health needs of the majority of schizophrenia population in Pittsburgh, PA. All treatments and data collection efforts occurred in this specialty outpatient research clinic. This research was conducted between August, 2001 and September, 2007, with recruitment ending in November, 2005.

Participants

Individuals in the early course of schizophrenia were recruited to participate in a two-year study of CET versus EST. Eligible participants were those with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, schizoaffective, or schizophreniform disorder, confirmed by the Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002), who were stabilized on antipsychotic medications, and had experienced the onset of their first psychotic symptom within the past 8 years, an IQ ≥ 80, an absence of significant substance abuse within the past 2 months prior to study enrollment, and exhibited significant social and cognitive disability, as assessed using the Cognitive Styles and Social Cognition Eligibility Interview (Hogarty et al., 2004). The resulting sample consisted of 58 individuals with schizophrenia (n = 38) or schizoaffective disorder (n = 20) who were randomized and had some treatment exposure. Individuals were young, with an average age of 25.92 (SD = 6.31) years, the majority were male (n = 40) and Caucasian (n = 40), and although most had attended some college (n = 39), few were employed at baseline (n = 15). Treated participants had been ill an average of 3.19 (SD = 2.24) years since their first psychotic symptom.

Measures

Employment outcomes

Employment characteristics were assessed using the Major Role Adjustment Inventory (Hogarty, Goldberg, et al., 1974; MRAI), a clinician-rated interview-based assessment of social, role, and vocational functioning developed for people with schizophrenia. The MRAI consists of 32 items covering the domains of vocational, role, and social functioning. Items are rated on variable scales depending on the domain assessed, with some rated on Likert scales ranging from 1 (functioning well) to 5 (functioning poorly) and others rated using nominal or ordinal categorical scales. For the purposes of this research, 5 employment items covering employment status (working [e.g., returned to former employer, working for new employer], pre-vocational I [e.g., involved in ≥ 10 hours of volunteer work], pre-vocational II [e.g. involved in vocational rehabilitation], and pre-vocational III [e.g., too ill to work]), weekly wages earned, weekly hours worked (“35+”, “20–34”, “1–19”, “0”, “homemaker”), job category (“professional”, “clerical/sales”, “skilled manual labor”, “semi-skilled manual labor”, “unskilled labor”, “not employed”, “student/homemaker”), and satisfaction with employment (rated from 1 [very satisfied] to 7 [very dissatisfied]) were used. Categories for employment status were collapsed into competitive vs. non-competitive/no employment; job categories were collapsed into professional, clerical/sales, skilled manual labor, and unskilled labor; and weekly hours worked were collapsed into full time (≥ 35 hours/week) or part time (< 35 hours/week) work for the purposes of analysis. The MRAI has been used as a core measure of role functioning among individuals with schizophrenia for many years, including the first controlled trial of social casework and antipsychotic medication (Hogarty, Goldberg, et al., 1974). Previous research has found the MRAI to have adequate test-retest reliability and to converge with other measures of functional outcome in schizophrenia, including the Social Adjustment Scale-II, the Global Assessment Scale, and the Social Security Administration’s work disability criteria (Hogarty et al., 2004; Eack et al., in press).

Cognitive assessments

A comprehensive battery of social and non-social cognitive assessments was collected as part of the larger study upon which this investigation is based to examine the effects of CET on various cognitive domains. The non-social cognitive or neurocognitive domains of attention, memory, and executive functioning were assessed using items from the Revised Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1987), California Verbal Learning Test (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1987), Revised Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Wechsler, 1981), Trails B (Reitan & Waltson, 1985), Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Heaton, Chelune, Talley, Kay, & Curtiss, 1993), Tower of London (Culbertson & Zillmer, 1996), and Neurological Evaluation Scale (Buchanan & Heinrichs, 1989). Social-cognitive domains of emotional (e.g., perception of emotional cues, emotion management) and social processing (e.g., reciprocity, foresightfulness, social context appraisal) were assessed using the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, & Sitarenios, 2003), Social Cognition Profile (Hogarty et al., 2004), and components of the Cognitive Styles and Social Cognition Eligibility Interview (Hogarty et al., 2004). All of these measures have been shown to have adequate psychometric properties among individuals with schizophrenia, and their use in this study has been described in detail in previous reports (Eack et al., 2007; Eack et al., in press). Due to the large number of cognitive measures used in this research, two composite indexes of neurocognition and social cognition were constructed by averaging across measures of social and non-social cognition after placing them on a standardized (z) metric. The internal consistency of both the neurocognition (α = .87) and social cognition composites was adequate (α = .72).

Treatments

Medications

All participants were maintained on antipsychotic medications approved by the Food and Drug Administration for the treatment of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and prescribed by a study psychiatrist. Participants also met at least biweekly with a clinical nurse specialist to assess efficacy, tolerability, and adherence to pharmacotherapy. Medication changes were allowed throughout the course of the study, although every effort was made to stabilize participants on an efficacious and acceptable antipsychotic regime before the initiation of psychosocial treatment. The majority (> 98%) of participants received second-generation antipsychotic medications, and there were no significant differences in chlorpromazine equivalent dosages or clinician-estimated adherence between study groups at baseline, 1-, or 2-years of treatment (Eack et al., in press).

Cognitive Enhancement Therapy

Cognitive Enhancement Therapy (CET; Hogarty & Greenwald, 2006) is a comprehensive, integrated psychosocial treatment for addressing the social and non-social cognitive impairments that limit functional recovery from schizophrenia. Over the course of two years, CET provides 60 hours of computer-based neurocognitive rehabilitation designed to improve attention, memory, and problem solving; which is integrated with 45 social-cognitive group therapy sessions that employ in vivo learning experiences to foster the development of social wisdom and success in interpersonal interactions. Neurocognitive rehabilitation relies on the use of standard personal computers utilizing Ben-Yishay’s Orientation Remediation Module (Ben-Yishay, Piasetsky, & Rattok, 1985) and Bracy’s memory and problem-solving software (Bracy, 1994), and is administered in participant pairs with facilitation by a CET therapist/coach. Based on developmental theories of social cognition (Hogarty & Flesher, 1999a), a broad range of social-cognitive abilities are targeted in the CET groups, such as learning how to take the perspective of others, appraising the social context, managing emotions in oneself and others, and gleaning the main point or “gist” of social situations. Participants actively engage in the social-cognitive groups by responding to unrehearsed social exchanges, presenting homework, participating in cognitive exercises that focus on experiential learning, providing feedback to peers, and chairing homework sessions. Unlike cognitive-behavior therapy, which seeks to correct faulty self-schemas through cognitive restructuring and behavioral techniques, CET focuses on addressing incorrect or incomplete schemas about others and the social world. In addition, CET does not attempt to provide individuals with strict behavioral rules for social interactions, but rather attempts to facilitate effective interpersonal behavior in unrehearsed social situations through the acquisition and application of higher-order social-cognitive abilities (e.g., perspective-taking, social context appraisal, giving support). CET begins with approximately 3 months of weekly 1-hour computer-based neurocognitive rehabilitation in attention, after which individuals begin the weekly 1.5-hour social-cognitive group therapy sessions, which proceed concurrently with neurocognitive training throughout the duration of the treatment. No part of CET is explicitly focused on gaining competitive employment and CET therapists do not provide supported employment services. However, if a CET participant has the goal of employment, the individual’s treatment plan is tailored to address the cognitive problems that limit his/her ability to reach that goal. A complete description of the theory, practice principles, and methods of CET has been provided elsewhere (Hogarty & Greenwald, 2006).

Enriched Supportive Therapy

Enriched Support Therapy (EST) is an illness management and psychoeducation approach based on components of the basic and intermediate phases of the demonstrably effective Personal Therapy (Hogarty, 2002). The treatment consists of individual therapy sessions where people with schizophrenia learn about and practice stress management techniques designed to enhance adjustment and forestall late post-discharge relapse. EST consists of two phases, which are individually tailored to the person’s level of recovery. In Phase I, the treatment focuses on basic psychoeducation about schizophrenia, the role of stress in the disorder, and ways to avoid/minimize stress. In Phase II, individuals learn more about the identification and management of stressors that pose particular challenges to their own social and role functioning. Some therapeutic techniques used in EST include the identification of early warning signs of stress and psychotic relapse, diaphragmatic breathing, healthy passive and active relaxation strategies, pro-social behavior and conflict avoidance. No aspect of EST is specifically focused on employment, although methods of managing stress that might interfere with employment were taught to individuals who expressed employment as a goal. Although individuals move through the two phases of EST at their own pace, each phase is typically provided for a year, with Phase I provided on a weekly basis and Phase II on a biweekly basis. No attempt was made to match CET and EST approaches with regard to hours of treatment. EST was chosen as the active control for this trial, in part, to account for the potential effects of illness management and education interventions on outcome (Hogarty, Greenwald, et al., 1997, Hogarty, Kornblith, et al., 1997), which is provided in both CET and EST. In addition, a state-of-the-art manualized approach was judged to be a more desirable and beneficial control than treatment as usual.

All individuals in who expressed interest in employment were referred to the Office of Vocational Rehabilitation, regardless of whether they were assigned to CET or EST. The administration of psychosocial treatments was performed by masters-level practitioners who were trained and supervised by the treatment developers in both CET and EST. The use of treatment manuals, weekly supervision, and audio/video recording of treatment sessions were employed to ensure treatment adherence and prevent diffusion between treatment groups. Study clinicians carried an approximately equal number of cases assigned CET or EST at random.

Procedures

Participants were recruited from inpatient and outpatient services at Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, Pittsburgh, PA and several nearby community clinics. After recruitment, participants were screened for eligibility criteria in consensus conferences based on video-taped interviews. Eligible participants were then randomly assigned to CET or EST by a project statistician using computer-generated random number sequences that were implemented by the study clinician assigned the patient and overseen by the principal investigator. Following randomization, participants were assessed annually using the aforementioned measures of employment and cognition, and treated for up to two years. Neurocognitive and some social-cognitive assessments were administered via computer-based tests or by trained neuropsychologists, and the remaining measures were collected by master’s-level study clinicians who had been extensively trained in their use and were not blind to treatment assignment. In total, 67 participants were randomized, 58 of whom received some exposure to CET (N = 31) or EST (N = 27). After 1 year, 49 (85%; 25 in CET, 24 in EST) participants remained in treatment, and 46 (79%; 85%; 24 in CET, 22 in EST) completed the entire two years of the study. The most frequent reasons for attrition included relocation, the emergence of clinical and medication non-adherence issues, and withdraw of consent for participation. There were no significant differences between treatment groups with regard to attrition, demographics, employment status, or baseline symptom characteristics (Eack et al., in press). Participants were reimbursed by the study for the time needed to complete research assessments, but no monetary incentives were provided for participation CET or EST. For individuals who could not afford transportation costs, transportation reimbursement was provided regardless of treatment assignment. All individuals provided written informed consent prior to participation, and this research was reviewed and approved annually by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board. No significant adverse events were reported during the course of this research.

Data Analysis

Analyses for this investigation focused on characterizing differences in employment outcomes among the 46 individuals who completed two years of CET or EST. Critical outcomes included the number of individuals who were competitively employed, weekly earnings, whether the work was full- or part-time, type of occupation engaged in, and satisfaction with one’s employment status. Significant differences in these outcomes were tested using likelihood-ratio independent t-tests where appropriate, and Mann-Whitney U tests in the presence of highly skewed continuous (i.e., weekly earnings) data. In addition, secondary analyses were conducted with all 58 participants who received any treatment in the study to identify the degree to which cognitive improvement during CET mediated any potential effects of the treatment on obtaining competitive employment. These analyses used linear mixed-effects models and Kraemer, Wilson, Fairburn, and Agras’ (2002) mediator analytic framework for clinical trials with the intent-to-treat sample of 58 individuals who received any exposure to the two psychosocial treatments. The focus was to characterize the associations between improved neurocognition, social cognition, and movement toward competitive employment, and to examine the degree to which improvements in these cognitive domains served as mechanisms of CET effects on increased engagement in employment. Mixed-effects models of cognitive growth adjusted for the confounding effects of age, sex, IQ, illness duration, and medication dose. In addition, error terms were allowed to correlate across time periods with descending magnitude via an first-order autoregressive error structure most appropriate for longitudinal models (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Mixed models of movement toward competitive employment used the binary competitive employment variable from the MRAI as the dependent variable, made use of penalized quasi-likelihood estimation to model binary outcomes (Breslow & Clayton, 1993), and did not adjust for demographic, illness, or medication characteristics, as they were not associated with employment. Penalized quasi-likelihood estimation is a method of maximum-likelihood (ML) estimation used to construct ML estimates for non-normal generalized linear mixed-effects models. In such models, ML estimation based on normal theory can lead to substantially biased parameter estimates, and thus an alternative “quasi-likelihood” estimation is needed. Continuous variables were placed on a z-metric for the computation of odds rations for these models. The size and significance of indirect effects was tested using the products of coefficients approach, with asymptotic tests for statistical significance (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002; MacKinnon, Lockwood, Brown, Wang, & Hoffman, 2007).

Results

Effects of Cognitive Enhancement Therapy on Employment Outcomes

We began our analyses by examining the effects of two years of CET treatment on a variety of employment outcomes compared to individuals who received two years of EST. As can be seen in Table 1, consistent with our previous report (Eack et al., in press), a greater proportion of individuals receiving CET were competitively employed in paid jobs after two years of treatment compared to those receiving EST. Further, individuals receiving CET earned significantly more money per week and were more satisfied with their employment status at the end of treatment. No significant differences emerged between treatment groups with regard to full-time employment, but CET participants were more likely to work on a part-time basis. Analysis of work patterns over the two years of study indicated that a significantly greater number of individuals receiving CET obtained new employment compared to EST, whereas the majority of participants treated with EST who were working were not able to maintain employment. In addition, a greater proportion of individuals receiving CET engaged in more skilled work, particularly clerical/sales jobs, although this difference with EST participants was only significant at a trend level (p = .110). Together, these findings suggest that not only did CET improve the likelihood that early course individuals with schizophrenia would engage in employment, but participation in CET also resulted in greater income and satisfaction with employment.

Table 1.

Employment Characteristics Among Individuals with Early Schizophrenia Treated For Two Years with Cognitive Enhancement Therapy or Enriched Supportive Therapy.

Variable CET (N = 24)
EST (N = 22)
Test p ES
N % N %
Competitively Employed 13 54% 4 18% LR = 6.34 .009 φ = .37
Job Status
 Full time 6 25% 3 14% LR = .96 .327 φ = .14
 Part time 7 29% 1 5% LR = 5.40 .020 φ = .33
Employment Pattern LR = 14.30 .003 V = .50
 Achieved employment 10 42% 2 9%
 Maintained employment 3 12% 2 9%
 Lost employment 0 0% 6 27%
Job Categorya LR = 7.53 .110 V = .38
 Professional 1 5% 0 0%
 Clerical/sales 5 23% 1 5%
 Skilled manual labor 1 5% 0 0%
 Unskilled labor 4 18% 3 14%

M SD M SD

Weekly Earnings 207.92 337.97 69.77 156.47 U = 1044 .044 d = .53
Satisfaction with Employment Statusb 2.46 1.56 3.77 1.69 t = −2.74 .009 d = −.81

Note. CET = Cognitive Enhancement Therapy, EST = Enriched Supportive Therapy, ES = Effect Size

a

Job category data were missing for two employed individuals who received CET

b

1 = Very satisfied, 2 = Moderately satisfied, 3 = Mildly satisfied, 4 = Neutral, 5 = Mildly dissatisfied, 6 = Moderately dissatisfied, 7 = Very dissatisfied

Cognitive Mechanisms of Improvement in Employment Outcomes

After finding that receiving CET produced significant benefits on a variety of employment outcomes, we proceeded to examine the degree to which the social-cognitive and neurocognitive effects of the treatment contributed to the beneficial effects of CET on employment. Table 2 presents direct and indirect relations between changes in cognition and movement to competitive employment across the two years of treatment. As expected, there was little overall growth in employment for the group of participants as a whole, but a significant treatment × time interaction was present favoring CET with regard to the probability of becoming competitively employed. When examining the effects of changes in social cognition on employment, a significant relationship emerged indicating that increases in social cognition were predictive of a greater probability of becoming employed, with individuals who experienced large (1 SD) improvements in social cognition being over 3 times more likely to engage in competitive employment. Analyses of the indirect effect of social-cognitive enhancement with respect to CET effects on employment indicated that improved social cognition significantly mediated the beneficial effects of CET on employment status, and when taken into account, the direct effect of CET on employment was reduced to marginal levels.

Table 2.

Direct and Indirect Effects of Cognitive Improvement on Probability of Employment (N = 58).

Variable B OR SE t pa
Base Model
Direct Effects
 Treatment Assignment (−1 = EST, 1 = CET) −.65 .52 .55 −1.17 .245
 Time .66 1.93 .38 1.74 .086
 Treatment Assignment × Time .99 2.69 .38 2.62 .010

Social Cognition Mediator Model
Direct Effects
 Treatment Assignment (−1 = EST, 1 = CET) −.74 .48 .57 −1.31 .196
 Time −.11 .90 .50 −.21 .832
ΔSocial Cognition 1.12 3.06 .48 2.34 .022
 Treatment Assignment × Time .69 1.99 .38 1.80 .075
Indirect Effect
ΔSocial Cognition .27 - .13 - .018

Neurocognition Mediator Model
Direct Effects
 Treatment Assignment (−1 = EST, 1 = CET) −.68 .51 .62 −1.08 .283
 Time .44 1.55 .43 1.03 .307
ΔNeurocognition 1.22 3.39 .43 2.84 .006
 Treatment Assignment × Time .93 2.53 .42 2.20 .030
Indirect Effect
ΔNeurocognition .16 - .09 - .017

Note. Mixed-effects models are based on an intent-to-treat analysis with all 58 individuals who received either CET or EST, regardless of treatment exposure.

CET = Cognitive Enhancement Therapy, EST = Enriched Supportive Therapy

a

p-values for indirect effects are based on asymptotic distribution tests outlined by MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets (2002)

With regard to neurocognitive change, which was also robust but more modest than social-cognitive improvement during this trial (Eack et al., in press), significant relations were observed between improvement in the overall neurocognitive composite and competitive employment. Inspection of model odds ratios indicated that individuals who experienced 1 SD of improvement in neurocognition were 3 times more likely to become competitively employed. Further, analyses of the indirect effects of CET on employment through neurocognitive improvement, indicated that changes in neurocognition significantly mediated CET effects on employment status. However, this neurocognitive mediational effect appeared smaller than the effects of improved social cognition, which was evidenced by a more modest indirect effect size and a continued significant direct effect of CET on employment after adjusting for improved neurocognition. These results point to improvements in both social and non-social cognition as potential treatment mechanisms for movement toward competitive employment among individuals in the early course of schizophrenia.

Discussion and Applications to Practice

Cognition has emerged as a critical treatment target for individuals with schizophrenia. Numerous studies have shown that the cognitive difficulties experienced by this population place significant limitations on functional recovery from the disorder (Couture, Penn, & Roberts, 2006; Green et al., 2002). Unfortunately, antipsychotic medications and newer pharmacotherapies have little effect on these persistent cognitive impairments (Buchanan et al., 2007; Buchanan et al., 2008). At the same time, social workers and allied professionals have begun developing and adapting psychosocial cognitive rehabilitation methods to address this important unmet therapeutic need (Hogarty et al., 2004). In this study, we examined the effects of Cognitive Enhancement Therapy (CET) when applied in the early course of schizophrenia on a critical domain of functioning - employment. Results indicated that participants who received CET were significantly more likely to be competitively employed, earned more wages, and were more satisfied with their employment status by the end of two years of treatment. Effects on employment and wages earned were of medium magnitude, and effects on employment satisfaction were large, supporting the clinical significance of these findings for CET participants. Furthermore, these improvements in employment were attributable, in part, to the cognitive gains made by such individuals during CET treatment. Such findings suggest that addressing impairments in both neurocognition and social cognition may serve as critical mechanism for greater functional recovery from schizophrenia, and that social work practitioners and treatment developers need to attend to these problems with their clients. In addition, these results continue to support the use of social work interventions as key adjuncts to pharmacotherapy and the ability of psychosocial treatment to affect the biopsychosocial aspects of the schizophrenia.

To date, this is the first study to point to active cognitive improvements as a mechanism for enhancing employment outcomes among individuals with schizophrenia. It is particularly important to note, that both neurocognitive and social-cognitive improvements contributed to improved employment in this study, which suggests both domains of cognition need to be targeted and integrated into treatment. Previous studies of short-term rehabilitation programs that only address problems in neurocognition have yielded little effects on functioning in this population (Fisher, Holland, Merzenich, & Vinogradov, 2009; Wykes et al., 2007), and CET remains the only cognitive rehabilitation approach that currently integrates the treatment of social and non-social cognition. Given the importance of the social context and social information processing to community adjustment, it is not surprising that social cognition plays a key role in helping individuals secure and maintain work. As such, social work rehabilitation programs seeking to optimize work outcomes with their clients might profitably incorporate treatments, such as CET, to address difficulties in social cognition.

Although this work has important implications for social work practice, several limitations should be noted. First, the sample size included in this research was modest. The a priori objective sample size for this research was 30 participants per treatment group, based on the large effect sizes observed in our previous study of CET (Hogarty et al., 2004), and with 58 participants studied, sufficient power was available to detect differences in a variety of employment outcomes. Second, no information was available regarding on-the-job functioning, so while CET was able to help individuals become employed and more satisfied with their employment status, it is unclear whether such individuals were succeeding in their work. However, it seems like that the large improvements in social adjustment observed in this study would have also contributed to enhanced work performance in this sample (Eack et al., in press). Third, the durability of these effects is not addressed in this research, and it is unknown whether CET can improve not only the attainment of, but also tenure in competitive employment. Previous studies of supported employment and individual place and train programs have demonstrated very high rates of obtaining initial employment, only to later find short job tenure and frequent turnover. In this study, individuals who received CET and became employed mid-treatment were all employed at the end of treatment one year later. Perhaps by addressing a core psychological impairment in schizophrenia, CET can better facilitate the maintenance of employment gains. Currently, a 1-year post-treatment follow-up is being completed with individuals who participated in this trial to examine this possibility. Fourth, assessments were not conducted by raters who were blind to treatment assignment. Although this could bias functional ratings, employment outcomes such as hours worked and weekly earnings are relatively objective measures that leave little room for rater bias. Fifth, inclusion in this trial precluded individuals who used substances and had comorbid developmental disabilities, which limits the applicability of these findings to those populations. Finally, it is important to remember that CET is not an employment intervention. Although the treatment appears to have had beneficial effects on several employment outcomes, 46% of the people who received CET were still unemployed at the end of treatment. The primary focus of CET is cognition, and improving cognitive functioning is likely to be necessary but not sufficient for helping some individuals return to work. The integration of CET with other vocational rehabilitation programs might serve as a particularly effective treatment package for improving employment outcomes among people with schizophrenia.

In summary, this research found significant improvements in competitive employment, wages earned, and satisfaction with employment status among outpatients in the early course of schizophrenia treated with CET compared to an active supportive control. Mediator analyses showed that improvements in cognition served as potential mechanisms for achieving greater rates of competitive employment among individuals treated with CET. These findings highlight the beneficial effects of cognitive rehabilitation in early schizophrenia and begin to identify its active mechanisms. Social workers seeking to enhance functional recovery among the individuals with schizophrenia that they treat should collaborate with colleagues and administrators in their agencies to incorporate such methods into their practice. Future research will need to determine whether the effects of CET on employment are durable, generalize to work performance, and result in longer job tenure in this population.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NIMH grants MH 60902 (MSK) and MH 79537 (SME). We thank the late Gerard E. Hogarty, M.S.W. for his leadership and direction as Co-Principal Investigator of this study, and Susan Cooley M.N.Ed., Anne Louise DiBarry, M.S.N., Konasale Prasad, M.D., Haranath Parepally, M.D., Debra Montrose, Ph.D., Diana Dworakowski, M.S., Mary Carter, Ph.D., and Sara Fleet, M.S. for their help in various aspects of the study.

References

  1. Anderson CM, Hogarty GE, Reiss DJ. Family treatment of adult schizophrenic patients: a psycho-educational approach. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1980;6(3):490–505. doi: 10.1093/schbul/6.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ben-Yishay Y, Piasetsky EB, Rattok J. A systematic method for ameliorating disorders in basic attention. In: Meir MJ, Benton AL, Diller L, editors. Neuropsychological rehabilitation. New York: Guilford Press; 1985. pp. 165–181. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ben-Yishay Y, Rattok J, Lakin P, Piasetsky E, Ross B, Silver S, Ezrachi O. Neuropsychological rehabilitation, quest for a holistic approach. Seminars in Neurology. 1985;5:252–259. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bond GR, Drake RE, Becker DR. An update on randomized controlled trials of evidence-based supported employment. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal. 2008;31(4):280–290. doi: 10.2975/31.4.2008.280.290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bracy OL. PSSCogRehab [computer software] Indianapolis, IN: Psychological Software Services Inc; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  6. Brekke J, Kay DD, Lee KS, Green MF. Biosocial pathways to functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2005;80(2–3):213–225. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Breslow NE, Clayton DG. Approximate inference in generalized linear mixed models. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1993;88(421):9–25. [Google Scholar]
  8. Brune M. Theory of Mind in Schizophrenia: A Review of the Literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2005;31(1):21–42. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Buchanan RW, Heinrichs DW. The Neurological Evaluation Scale (NES): a structured instrument for the assessment of neurological signs in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. 1989;27(3):335–350. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90148-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buchanan RW, Conley RR, Dickinson D, Ball MP, Feldman S, Gold JM, McMahon RP. Galantamine for the treatment of cognitive impairments in people with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165(1):82–89. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07050724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buchanan RW, Javitt DC, Marder SR, Schooler NR, Gold JM, McMahon RP, Carpenter WT. The Cognitive and Negative Symptoms in Schizophrenia Trial (CONSIST): The Efficacy of Glutamatergic Agents for Negative Symptoms and Cognitive Impairments. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164(10):1593–1602. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06081358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Corrigan PW, Green MF. Schizophrenic patients’ sensitivity to social cues: The role of abstraction. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;150(4):589–594. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.4.589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Couture SM, Penn DL, Roberts DL. The Functional Significance of Social Cognition in Schizophrenia: A Review. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2006;32(Suppl1):S44–63. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Culbertson WC, Zillmer EA. Tower of London-DX manual. 1996. Unpublished manuscript. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  16. Eack SM, Greeno CG, Pogue-Geile MF, Newhill CE, Hogarty GE, Keshavan MS. Assessing social-cognitive deficits in schizophrenia with the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test. Schizophrenia Bulletin. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn091. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Eack SM, Hogarty GE, Cooley SJ, DiBarry AL, Hogarty SS, Greenwald DP, Keshavan MS. Cognitive Enhancement Therapy for early course schizophrenia: Effects of a two-year randomized controlled trial. Psychiatric Services. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.60.11.1468. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Eack SM, Hogarty GE, Greenwald DP, Hogarty SS, Keshavan MS. Cognitive Enhancement Therapy improves Emotional Intelligence in early course schizophrenia: Preliminary effects. Schizophrenia Research. 2007;89(1–3):308–311. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ekman P. Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist. 1993;48(4):384–392. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.48.4.384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview For DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  21. Fisher M, Holland C, Merzenich MM, Vinogradov S. Using Neuroplasticity-Based Auditory Training to Improve Verbal Memory in Schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166(7):805–811. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08050757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: Are we measuring the right stuff. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2000;26(1):119–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Gold JM, Barch DM, Cohen J, Essock S, Marder SR. Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrenia: The NIMH-MATRICS conference to select cognitive domains and test criteria. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56(5):301–307. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology. 1998;2(3):271–299. [Google Scholar]
  25. Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual: Revised and Expanded. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  26. Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12(3):426–445. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hilton DJ. The social context of reasoning: Conversational inference and rational judgment. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118(2):248–248. [Google Scholar]
  28. Hoff AL, Svetina C, Shields G, Stewart J, DeLisi LE. Ten year longitudinal study of neuropsychological functioning subsequent to a first episode of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2005;78(1):27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hogarty GE. Personal Therapy for schizophrenia and related disorders: A guide to individualized treatment. New York: Guilford; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hogarty GE, Flesher S. Developmental theory for a cognitive enhancement therapy of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1999a;25(4):677–692. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hogarty GE, Greenwald DP. Cognitive Enhancement Therapy: The Training Manual. University of Pittsburgh Medical Center: Authors; 2006. Available through www.CognitiveEnhancementTherapy.com. [Google Scholar]
  32. Hogarty GE, Anderson CM, Reiss DJ, Kornblith SJ, Greenwald DP, Javna CD Environmental/Personal Indicators in the Course of Schizophrenia Research Group. Family psychoeducation, social skills training, and maintenance chemotherapy in the aftercare treatment of schizophrenia: I. One-year effects of a controlled study on relapse and expressed emotion. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1986;43(7):633–642. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800070019003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hogarty GE, Flesher S, Ulrich R, Carter M, Greenwald D, Pogue-Geile, Zoretich R. Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizophrenia. Effects of a 2-year randomized trial on cognition and behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61(9):866–876. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hogarty GE, Goldberg SC, Schooler NR the Collaborative Study Group. Drug and sociotherapy in the aftercare of schizophrenic patients: III. Adjustment of nonrelapsed patients. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1974;31(5):609–618. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1974.01760170011002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hogarty GE, Goldberg SC, Schooler NR, Ulrich RF the Collaborative Study Group. Drug and sociotherapy in the aftercare of schizophrenic patients. II. Two-year relapse rates. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1974;31(5):603–608. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1974.01760170005001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hogarty GE, Greenwald DP, Eack SM. Durability and mechanism of effects of Cognitive Enhancement Therapy. Psychiatric Services. 2006;57(12):1751–1757. doi: 10.1176/ps.2006.57.12.1751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hogarty GE, Greenwald D, Ulrich RF, Kornblith SJ, Dibarry AL, Cooley S, Flesher S. Three-year trials of personal therapy among schizophrenic patients living with or independent of family: II. Effects of adjustment of patients. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154(11):1514–1524. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.11.1514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hogarty GE, Kornblith SJ, Greenwald D, Dibarry AL, Cooley S, Ulrich RF, Flesher S. Three-year trials of personal therapy among schizophrenic patients living with or independent of family: I. Description of study and effects of relapse rates. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154(11):1504–1513. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.11.1504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Keefe RSE, Bilder RM, Davis SM, Harvey PD, Palmer BW, Gold JM, Meltzer HY, Lieberman JA. Neurocognitive Effects of Antipsychotic Medications in Patients With Chronic Schizophrenia in the CATIE Trial. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64(6):633–647. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Keshavan MS, Hogarty GE. Brain maturational processes and delayed onset in schizophrenia. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11(3):525–543. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kraemer HC, Wilson G, Fairburn CG, Agras W. Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2002;59(10):877–884. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.10.877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Brown CH, Wang W, Hoffman JM. The intermediate endpoint effect in logistic and probit regression. Clinical Trials. 2007;4(5):499–513. doi: 10.1177/1740774507083434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods. 2002;7(1):83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mayer JD, Salovey P, Caruso DR, Sitarenios G. Measuring emotional intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion. 2003;3(1):97–105. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mechanic D, Blider S, McAlpine DD. Employing persons with serious mental illness. Health Affairs. 2002;21(5):242–253. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.21.5.242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, Baade LE, Barch DM, Cohen JD, Marder SR. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, Part 1: Test Selection, Reliability, and Validity. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165(2):203–213. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1978;1(4):515–526. [Google Scholar]
  48. Rapp CA. The Strengths Model: Case Management with People Suffering from Severe and Persistent Mental Illness. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  49. Raudenbush DSW, Bryk DAS. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and data analysis methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  50. Reitan RM, Waltson D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  51. Rosenheck R, Leslie D, Keefe R, McEvoy J, Swartz M, Perkins D, Lieberman J. Barriers to employment for people with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163(3):411–417. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Saykin AJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RE, Kester DB, Mozley LH, Stafiniak P, Gur RC. Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51(2):124–131. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950020048005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Selman RL, Schultz LH. Making a Friend in Youth. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  54. Sergi MJ, Green MF, Widmark C, Reist C, Erhart S, Braff DL, Mintz J. Cognition and Neurocognition: Effects of Risperidone, Olanzapine, and Haloperidol. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164(10):1585–1592. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06091515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Stein LI, Test MA. Alternative to mental hospital treatment: I. Conceptual model, treatment program, and clinical evaluation. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1980;37(4):392–397. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1980.01780170034003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Mental health, United States: 2000. Washington, DC: Author; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  57. Swartz MS, Perkins DO, Stroup TS, Davis SM, Capuano G, Rosenheck RA, Lieberman JA. Effects of Antipsychotic Medications on Psychosocial Functioning in Patients With Chronic Schizophrenia: Findings From the NIMH CATIE Study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164(3):428–436. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.3.428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Taylor M. Conceptual perspective taking: Children’s ability to distinguish what they know from what they see. Child Development. 1988;59(3):703–718. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Thaker GK, Carpenter WT. Advances in schizophrenia. Nature Medicine. 2001;7(6):667–671. doi: 10.1038/89040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  61. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corp; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  62. Wyer RS, Srull TK, editors. Basic Processes. Vol. 1. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Association; 1994. Handbook of social cognition. [Google Scholar]
  63. Wykes T, Newton E, Landau S, Rice C, Thompson N, Frangou S. Cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) for young early onset patients with schizophrenia: An exploratory randomized controlled trial. Schizophrenia Research. 2007;94(1–3):221–230. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Zubin J, Spring B. Vulnerability: A new view of schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1977;86(2):103–126. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.86.2.103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES