Abstract
Rationale
The majority of drug abusers are incapable of sustaining abstinence over any length of time. Accumulating evidence has linked intense and involuntary craving, Impulsive decision-making and mood disturbances to risk for relapse. However, little is known about temporal changes of these neuropsychological functions in methamphetamine (METH)-dependent individuals.
Objectives
To investigate the effect of length of abstinence on decision-making, craving (baseline and cue-induced), and emotional state in METH-addicted individuals.
Methods
In this cross-sectional study, 183 adult METH-dependent patients at an addiction rehabilitation center who were abstinent for 6 days (n = 37), 14 days (n = 33), 1 month (n = 31), 3 months (n = 30), 6 months (n = 26), or 1 year (n = 30) and 39 healthy subjects were administered the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) to assess decision-making performance. Depression, anxiety, and impulsivity were also examined. One hundred thirty-nine METH abusers who were abstinent for the aforementioned times then underwent a cue session, and subjective and physiological measures were assessed.
Results
METH dependent individuals who were abstinent for longer periods of time exhibited better decision-making than those who were abstinent for shorter periods of time. And self-reported emotional symptoms improved with abstinence. METH abusers’ ratings of craving decreased with the duration of abstinence, while cue-induced craving increased until 3 months of abstinence and decreased at 6 months and 1 year of abstinence.
Conclusions
We present time-dependent alterations in decision-making, emotional state, and the incubation of cue-induced craving in METH-dependent individuals, which might have significant clinical implications for the prevention of relapse.
Introduction
Methamphetamine (METH) abuse is a worldwide problem that imposes substantial global public health and costly social burdens [1], [2]. Relapse rates following psychosocial and pharmacological treatments are high in METH-addicted individuals and remains the major problem [3], [4], [5]. A better understanding of the mechanisms that increase relapse susceptibility could contribute to the development of more effective relapse prevention treatments in METH dependence.
There is a growing evidence of potential psychological problems and cognitive impairments as a result of prolonged METH use in humans [6]. Individuals with substance dependence frequently show signs of impaired emotion and neurocognitive functions, even after prolonged abstinence from drug use [7], [8], [9]. But few have examined the role of abstinence on neurocognitive deficits, and the results is mixed [10].
Decision-making
Neuropsychological and personality research indicated that addiction is associated with elevated scores on questionnaires that measure impulsivity and impaired performance on laboratory tasks of impulsivity [11], [12], [13], [14]. The behavioral analysis of impulsivity in humans has predominantly used three paradigms: decision-making, response inhibition, and reflection impulsivity [14]. The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) was specially designed to measure decision-making impairment in the laboratory, resembling the decisions made in real life in terms of reward, punishment, and the uncertainty of outcomes [15]. An impairment of decision-making has been found in nearly all substance-dependent populations, including people addicted to alcohol [16], cannabis [17], METH [18], cocaine [19], and heroin [13]. Our previous work have shown that the short-term abstinent heroin-dependent patients performed worse on the IGT compared with the long-term abstinence groups [13]. However, whether the length of abstinence affects decision-making ability in METH-addicted individuals is unknown.
Craving
Theoretical perspectives and clinical survey reports indicated that drug-related cues are a key factor that increases the risk of relapse in substance-dependent subjects [5], [20]. Abstinent heroin-addicted patients who relapsed exhibited greater drug cue-induced reactivity compared with nonrelapsers and controls [21]. Cue-induced drug seeking increases with longer periods of abstinence, termed the “incubation of drug craving” [22], which was used to account for the persistent relapse after prolonged abstinence. The incubation of craving has been reported in rats that were trained to self-administer cocaine [22], heroin [23], METH [24], nicotine [25], and oral sucrose [26] and in human smokers [9]. Whether the incubation of cue-induced craving occurs in METH abusers and how long it remains require further studies.
Emotional Symptoms
Chronic METH abuse can cause mood disturbances. Symptoms of depression and anxiety are common among METH-dependent and -withdrawn individuals [27], [28] and associated with craving for the drug, which may exacerbate relapse to METH use [29]. Previous studies found that depressive symptoms varied considerably in intensity and duration, which resolved within 2 to 3 weeks [30], [31], [32].
In the present study, we employed cross-sectional and experimental approaches to investigate decision-making performance, emotional state, and the desire for drug confronted with METH-related cues or not in METH-dependent subjects at different periods of abstinence.
Materials and Methods
The study was approved by the Peking University Research Ethics Board. The participants were given detailed study descriptions, provided written informed consent, and received monetary compensation.
Subjects
This was a cross-sectional study conducted at Zhongshan Compulsory Addiction Rehabilitation Center, Guangdong province. One hundred eighty-three male inpatients who were formerly addicted to METH and drug-free for 6 days (6 d, n = 37), 14 days (14 d, n = 33), 1 month (1 m, n = 31), 3 months (3 m, n = 30), 6 months (6 m, n = 26) and 1 year (1 y, n = 30) were administered the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) to assess decision-making performance. And 139 METH abusers who were abstinent for the aforementioned times (6 d: n = 24; 14 d: n = 26; 1 m: n = 19; 3 m: n = 20; 6 m: n = 20; 1 y: n = 29) underwent cue sessions. The last use of METH of drug abusers was on the day before study entry and urine sample for kind of drugs abused verification were provided before admitting. In the center, METH abusers have no chance to find and use METH. So the abstinence period can be strictly controlled. Prior to enrollment, all of the subjects were screened using the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) and had to provide answers to questions regarding their METH abuse, which was based on when they were actively using. All of the subjects had a history of a DSM-IV diagnosis of METH dependence and did not have any current medical illness that required pharmacological treatment or any other substance use disorder with the exception of nicotine dependence. Other exclusion criteria included a history of head injury, severe mental illness, psychosis and neurological disorders. All of the METH participants identified smoking as the primary route of METH administration. Each participant was permitted to be in only one abstinence-period test. Thirty-nine education-matched male healthy controls were recruited through advertisements. All participants are aged between 18 and 40 years.
Neuropsychological Measures
Iowa Gambling Task
Decision-making was assessed by the original version of the IGT. The subjects were asked to choose among four decks of cards and accumulate as much money as possible by picking one card at a time. To win money, the subjects must discover which are the most advantageous decks and preferentially select cards from those decks. Deck A and B are associated with high immediate wins but larger future penalties that results in a net loss over time (i.e., the disadvantageous decks). Deck C and D yields lower immediate wins but smaller future penalties, such that the participants gradually accumulate a profit by choosing these decks (i.e., the advantageous decks). The outcome measure for this task is the net score, which is calculated as the number of cards from the disadvantageous decks subtracted from the advantageous decks. Therefore, a positive score reflects a tendency to make good decisions.
Cue Sessions
The main design was between-subjects. During the cue sessions, neutral cues or METH cues were presented first randomly. Baseline physiological and subjective measurements were collected prior to the first cue exposure. After baseline measures, participants were exposed to the first cues. Immediately after exposure, cardiovascular (CV) and subjective measurements were collected. There was a 20 min of free time between the two cues sessions. Physiological and subjective measurements were again collected just before participants started the second cue set, which was followed by CV and subjective measurements. Subjects who reported craving after METH cue exposure were debriefed at the end of assessments and remained in the research clinic until their levels of craving returned to baseline levels. For METH cues, the subjects viewed (1) 32 photographs of individuals procuring and using METH, with each presented for 7 s in a slide show, (2) a 5 min video in which METH abusers made drug paraphernalia and used METH, and (3) paraphernalia placed directly on the table in front of the subjects. Neutral cues consisted of 32 neutral pictures without explicit METH cues and a 5 min video of a natural landscape. Subjective measures included a 10-point visual analog scale (VAS), in which the participants marked a score of 1 (not at all) to 10 (extremely high) in response to the question “How much do you feel the urge to use METH?”.
Questionnaires and Neuropsychological Measures
Hamilton Anxiety Scale (HAMA)
This scale consisted of 14 items, each defined by a series of symptoms. Each item is rated on a five-point scale, ranging from 0 (not present) to 4 (severe) [33]. It was cited from Chinese classification and diagnostic criteria of mental disorder-3rd edition (CCMD-3).
Beck Depression Inventory–2nd edition (BDI-II)
This scale comprises 13 multiple-choice items that assess the presence and severity of depressive symptoms in clinical and nonclinical samples. It was cited from Handbook of Behavioral Medical Scales (2005).
Barratt impulsiveness scale
The subjects were administered the Chinese version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, version 11 (BIS-11), a validated 30-item questionnaire that conceptualizes impulsiveness in three main dimensions [34]: attention impulsiveness, motor impulsiveness, and non-planning impulsiveness. The Chinese version of the BIS-11 was shown to have good internal consistency and test-retest reliability by the Medical Psychological Research Centre of the Second Xiangya Hospital, China [13].
d2 test of attention/psychomotor speed
From a series of “d” and “p” letters with one or two dashes above and/or below each letter, the participants were asked to mark the “d” accompanied by two dashes as quickly and accurately as possible. A summary test score represents the total number of correctly marked “d” letters minus the number of errors [35]. The Germany version of d2 test was used.
Working memory
The digit span task derived form the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III was used to evaluate working memory. The experimenter reads a string of digits aloud and the subject is instructed to immediately recall those digits either in the forward position or in the backward position. The series of digits were incrementally increased by one digit every time the subject performs the task correctly. The task was stopped when the subjects failed to recall two trials with the same number of digits. Score was defined as the total number of digits the subject could correctly recall in the exact order they were presented or in reverse order.
Cardiovascular Measures
Systolic and diastolic blood pressure was measured with an arm cuff connected to a 9062D monitor (Baozhong Biotechnology, Beijing, China). Heart rate was continuously measured with a fingertip pulse sensor connected to an SD-700 monitor.
Statistical Analysis
All of the data were analyzed using SPSS 17.0 software. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used for between-group comparisons of demographic characteristics (i.e., age, years of education, duration of METH use, typical dose and frequency of use every month), neuropsychological task performance (i.e., working memory and d2 test of attention), psychiatric symptoms (including anxiety, depression, and impulsivity), baseline craving scores and physiological measures. IGT performance was analyzed using one-way Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) models with age and education as covariates. To assess whether METH cues were effective, we used t-tests to compare within-session changes in the craving scores (postcue - precue) for METH cues vs. neutral cues. Only those METH abusers whose craving ratings increased after the METH-related cures exposure were included in the main analyses. Group differences were assessed using an ANOVA. We collected subjective measurement and CV as baseline before each type of cues was exposed, and the dependent variables were within-session changes in the craving scores, with METH and neutral cues analyzed separately. A similar approach tested group differences in baseline cue session craving [9]. Values of p<0.05 were considered statistically significant.
Results
Effects of Length of Abstinence on Decision-making in METH addicts
Demographic and clinical characteristics
The demographic and clinical characteristics of the healthy control and METH-dependent subjects are shown in Table 1. No significant between-group differences were found with regard to age, level of education, drug use history (i.e., duration, dose, and frequency per month), and working memory.
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of subjects.
Characteristic | Control(n = 39) | 6 day(n = 37) | 14 day(n = 33) | 1 month (n = 31) | 3 month (n = 30) | 6 month (n = 26) | 1 year (n = 30) | p value |
Age (years) | 30.00±1.40 | 27.11±1.06 | 26.76±1.04 | 29.93±1.17 | 26.06±1.11 | 30.00±1.16 | 28.87±1.12 | 0.06 |
Education (years) | 9.77±0.41 | 8.71±0.33 | 9.18±0.41 | 8.10±0.42 | 8.62±0.40 | 8.62±0.45 | 8.47±0.37 | 0.065 |
Duration of METH use (months) | – | 18.9±4.20 | 21.38±2.77 | 25.93±3.61 | 20.93±3.84 | 26.77±4.72 | 17.87±2.64 | 0.46 |
Average dose (g) | – | 0.71±0.13 | 0.47±0.07 | 0.58±0.09 | 0.60±0.08 | 0.63±0.10 | 0.67±0.11 | 0.60 |
Frequency per month | – | 18.31±1.59 | 18.35±1.62 | 18.47±1.83 | 18.21±1.93 | 17.11±2.21 | 16.87±1.85 | 0.98 |
Decision-making ability
Since the age and education are almost statistically different between the METH and control groups (p = 0.06 and p = 0.065), the age and education was used as covariate in the statistical analysis. Fig. 1 shows the IGT net scores in former METH dependent individuals and healthy control subjects. One-way ANCOVA revealed significant differences in decision-making ability between the seven groups (F 6,215 = 2.356, p<0.05). Post hoc tests showed that the 6 d and 14 d abstinence groups selected significantly more cards from the disadvantageous decks than healthy control subjects (p = 0.001, p = 0.033), while 1 m and 3 m abstinence groups selected almost statistically more disadvantageous cards than healthy subjects (p = 0.063, p = 0.081). Moreover, 6 d abstinence group differed from 12 m abstinence in IGT score (p = 0.007).
Effects of Length of Abstinence on Emotional Ratings, Impulsivity, and Neuropsychological Task Performance
Anxiety, depression, working memory, and attention
One-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of group on anxiety (F 6,215 = 4.53, p<0.0005), depression (F 6,215 = 5.12, p<0.0001), and attention (F 6,215 = 2.67, p<0.05). Post hoc tests showed that the 14 d, 1 m, and 3 m groups had more anxiety symptoms, while the 6 d, 14 d, 1 m, and 3 m abstinence groups had less depressive symptoms. And the 6 d abstinence group had an impaired attention level compared with healthy controls (all p<0.05). No significant differences were found in working memory (data not shown; Fig. 2).
Impulsivity
The BIS-11 scores are presented in Fig. 3. A statistically significant difference was found in self-reported total impulsiveness (F 6,215 = 5.55, p<0.0001), motor impulsiveness (F 6,215 = 5.71, p<0.0001), and attention impulsiveness (F 6,215 = 3.39, p<0.01) between the METH abusers and controls. Post hoc tests revealed that the 6 d, 14 d, 1 m, 3 m, and 6 m abstinence groups had higher total scores, while the 6 d, 14 d, 1 m, and 3 m abstinence groups had higher attention scores. And all of the METH groups had higher motor scores than controls.
Effects of Length of Abstinence on Craving in METH-dependent Subjects
Demographic and clinical characteristics
Table 2 presents the demographic and drug use characteristics of the METH addicts enrolled in the cue-exposure experiment. The subjects were well-matched with regard to age, years of education, duration of METH use, average dose and frequency of METH use.
Table 2. Demographic characteristics of METH-dependent subjects.
Characteristic | 6 day (n = 24) | 14 day (n = 26) | 1 month (n = 19) | 3 month (n = 20) | 6 month (n = 20) | 1 year (n = 29) | p value |
Age (years) | 28.43±1.41 | 27.38±1.27 | 29.95±1.63 | 26.7±1.38 | 30.9±1.43 | 29.67±1.26 | 0.28 |
Education (years) | 7.65±0.48 | 9.23±0.50 | 7.79±0.43 | 8.40±0.55 | 7.89±0.70 | 8.27±0.50 | 0.29 |
Duration of METH use (months) | 24.83±6.86 | 27.77±3.84 | 20.06±4.04 | 25.30±4.03 | 22.45±5.54 | 17.23±2.63 | 0.55 |
Average dose (g) | 0.65±0.13 | 0.52±0.08 | 0.58±0.12 | 0.53±0.09 | 0.72±0.24 | 0.72±0.11 | 0.81 |
Frequency per month | 19.6±2.23 | 19.69±1.88 | 20.53±2.43 | 16.7±2.06 | 16.25±2.46 | 17.80±1.88 | 0.68 |
Baseline craving
Ratings of craving in METH-dependent subjects at different abstinence periods before cue presentations are shown in Fig. 4. The participants’ baseline craving decreased with abstinence (F 5,135 = 4.06, p<0.005). Post hoc tests revealed that the 1 m, 3 m, 6 m, and 1 y abstinent METH abusers had lower craving than the 6-day group (1 m, 3 m, and 6 m: p<0.05; 1 y: p<0.01). The 6 m and 1 y groups had lower craving than the 14 d group (6 m: p<0.05; 1 y: p<0.01).
Cue-induced craving
As shown in Fig. 5, the effects of length of abstinence on cue-induced craving in METH-dependent subjects were significantly different (F 5,133 = 2.62, p<0.05). Within 3 m of METH abstinence, cues-induced craving increased as the length of abstinence increased. The 3 m group had higher responsivity to the cues than the 6 d and 14 d groups (p<0.01), and the responsivity decreased with 6 m and 1 y of abstinence (p<0.05).
Effects of cues on physiological measures in METH abusers
The effects of length of abstinence on cue-induced physiological measures, including heart rate, systolic blood pressure and diastolic blood pressure, were not significantly different (data not shown).
Discussion
The present study presented three characteristics of the effects of length of abstinence on METH abusers: impaired impulse control, craving, and emotion. METH dependent individuals who were abstinent for longer periods of time exhibited better decision-making and emotions than those who were abstinent for shorter periods of time. Cue-induced craving increased with increased abstinence up to 3 months.
Some degree of recovery after protracted abstinence has been shown in neuroimaging studies in abstinent METH abusers [36], [37], [38], [39]. However, previous studies on relationship of cognitive function with sustained abstinence from METH use in METH-dependent individuals are inconsistent [10]. Two of three cross-sectional studies of abstinence suggested that some cognitive functions improve with abstinence [38], [40], and a longitudinal study that assessed 1 year of abstinence found METH-dependent individuals who were neuropsychologically impaired at baseline demonstrating significant improvement with abstinence [41]. These suggested that partial recovery may be possible in some cognitive domains, other METH-associated cognitive deficits may be irreversible. Our previous study have shown that stress can exacerbate an already existing impairment of decision-making or unmask a latent one in individuals recovering from heroin dependence [13], which suggested the recovery of decision-making in normal circumstances may not represent the overall recovery of function. No differences were found in working memory among the groups, suggesting that decision-making and other cognitive functions may show differential recovery trajectories. Attention is an important prerequisite for learning, and IGT involves exploratory learning via rewards and penalties. So the impaired attention of METH at 6 days abstinence may exacerbate the impulsive decision-making.
We found that cue-induced craving in METH abusers increased with prolonged abstinence up to 3 months, even as baseline craving declined. This finding is broadly consistent with nonhuman incubation studies that used lever-pressing to measure drug seeking and craving [42] and human smokers [9]. Cue-induced craving decreased at 6 months and 1 year of abstinence, which is also consistent with a rat study in which the incubation phenomenon was long-lasting but not permanent [43]. Recent research found that heroin relapsers exhibited greater drug cue-induced subjective responses compared with both nonrelapsers and control subjects [21]. So the present study suggested that the risk for relapse may increase with an increased length of abstinence up to 3 months. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and glial cell line-derived neurotrophic factor in the ventral tegmental area, neuronal activation of the ventral medial prefrontal cortex, extracellular signal-regulated kinase activation in the central nucleus of the amygdala, and glutamate receptor 2-lacking 2-amino-3-(5-methyl-3-oxo-1,2- oxazol-4-yl) propionic acid receptors in the nucleus accumbens have been shown to play important roles in the incubation of cocaine craving [42], but the neurobiological mechanisms of METH incubation need further investigation.
Depressive symptoms after the cessation of stimulant abuse have been associated with reduced treatment retention [44]. One study in Filipino male METH users found that negative affect and craving predicted relapse [45], while the antidepressant sertraline delayed relapse in recently abstinent cocaine-dependent patients with depressive symptoms [46]. Diagnosing and monitoring depression in drug abusers and the timely remediation of post-detoxification depression symptoms may help in the prevention of drug relapse. In METH abusers, self-reports of depressive symptoms covaried positively with relative glucose metabolism in limbic regions, and ratings of state anxiety covaried negatively with relative activity in the anterior cingulate cortex and left insula [27], suggesting potential targets for therapeutic intervention. Nakama et al. found that depression and anxiety were significantly correlated with METH craving [29]. Therefore, METH abusers may benefit from treatment of their psychiatric symptoms to minimize craving and subsequent relapse to drug use.
The limitation of the present study should be noted. First, the design did not include a group that was enrolled for a within-subjects component (i.e., IGT performance and cue-induced craving were tested in only one group at different abstinence times). Thus we can not resolve conclusively the pre-existing versus acquired nature of the observed alterations. Second, longitudinal designs would enhance our ability to provide general conclusions regarding the associations between neurocognitive function and abstinence times. However, a previous study found that the incubation of cue-induced craving in smokers was weaker in a within-subjects design than in a between-subjects design, which may be attributable to some extinction of the conditioned response in the laboratory in within-subjects assessments [9].
Conclusion
In summary, we found that impulsive decision-making, craving, and psychiatric symptoms in METH abusers abated with a longer duration of abstinence, but cue-induced craving did not decrease over an extended period of abstinence and actually increased with 3 months of abstinence. These findings suggest that the risk of relapse in drug abusers does not decline with abstinence, and concurrent interventions that mitigate stress and cue reactivity should be extended to a longer duration.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the Natural Science Foundation of China (number 81000571), National Science foundation for Distinguished Young Scholars of China (number 81225009), National Basic Program of China (numbers 2009CB522000 and 91132719). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Darke S, Kaye S, McKetin R, Duflou J (2008) Major physical and psychological harms of methamphetamine use. Drug Alcohol Rev 27: 253–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Buxton JA, Dove NA (2008) The burden and management of crystal meth use. Cmaj 178: 1537–1539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Elkashef A, Vocci F, Hanson G, White J, Wickes W, et al. (2008) Pharmacotherapy of methamphetamine addiction: an update. Subst Abus 29: 31–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Rawson RA, Marinelli-Casey P, Anglin MD, Dickow A, Frazier Y, et al. (2004) A multi-site comparison of psychosocial approaches for the treatment of methamphetamine dependence. Addiction 99: 708–717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. O’Brien CP, Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R (1992) Classical conditioning in drug-dependent humans. Ann N Y Acad Sci 654: 400–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Scott JC, Woods SP, Matt GE, Meyer RA, Heaton RK, et al. (2007) Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: a critical review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 17: 275–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Verdejo-Garcia A, Rivas-Perez C, Vilar-Lopez R, Perez-Garcia M (2007) Strategic self-regulation, decision-making and emotion processing in poly-substance abusers in their first year of abstinence. Drug Alcohol Depend 86: 139–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Woicik PA, Moeller SJ, Alia-Klein N, Maloney T, Lukasik TM, et al. (2009) The neuropsychology of cocaine addiction: recent cocaine use masks impairment. Neuropsychopharmacology 34: 1112–1122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Bedi G, Preston KL, Epstein DH, Heishman SJ, Marrone GF, et al. (2010) Incubation of cue-induced cigarette craving during abstinence in human smokers. Biol Psychiatry 69: 708–711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Dean AC, Groman SM, Morales AM, London ED (2013) An Evaluation of the Evidence that Methamphetamine Abuse Causes Cognitive Decline in Humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 38: 259–274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Fillmore MT, Rush CR (2002) Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend 66: 265–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Barratt ES, Oderinde V, Mathias CW, et al. (2002) Increased impulsivity in cocaine dependent subjects independent of antisocial personality disorder and aggression. Drug Alcohol Depend 68: 105–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Zhang XL, Shi J, Zhao LY, Sun LL, Wang J, et al. (2011) Effects of stress on decision-making deficits in formerly heroin-dependent patients after different durations of abstinence. Am J Psychiatry 168: 610–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Clark L, Robbins TW, Ersche KD, Sahakian BJ (2006) Reflection impulsivity in current and former substance users. Biol Psychiatry 60: 515–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50: 7–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Goudriaan AE, Grekin ER, Sher KJ (2007) Decision making and binge drinking: a longitudinal study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 31: 928–938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, Matochik JA, Cadet JL (2005) Neural substrates of faulty decision-making in abstinent marijuana users. Neuroimage 26: 480–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Gonzalez R, Bechara A, Martin EM (2007) Executive functions among individuals with methamphetamine or alcohol as drugs of choice: preliminary observations. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 29: 155–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Verdejo-Garcia AJ, Perales JC, Perez-Garcia M (2007) Cognitive impulsivity in cocaine and heroin polysubstance abusers. Addict Behav 32: 950–966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Sinha R (2001) How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 158: 343–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Fatseas M, Denis C, Massida Z, Verger M, Franques-Reneric P, et al. (2011) Cue-induced reactivity, cortisol response and substance use outcome in treated heroin dependent individuals. Biol Psychiatry 70: 720–727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y (2001) Neuroadaptation. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature 412: 141–142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Shalev U, Morales M, Hope B, Yap J, Shaham Y (2001) Time-dependent changes in extinction behavior and stress-induced reinstatement of drug seeking following withdrawal from heroin in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 156: 98–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Shepard JD, Bossert JM, Liu SY, Shaham Y (2004) The anxiogenic drug yohimbine reinstates methamphetamine seeking in a rat model of drug relapse. Biol Psychiatry 55: 1082–1089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Abdolahi A, Acosta G, Breslin FJ, Hemby SE, Lynch WJ (2010) Incubation of nicotine seeking is associated with enhanced protein kinase A-regulated signaling of dopamine- and cAMP-regulated phosphoprotein of 32 kDa in the insular cortex. Eur J Neurosci 31: 733–741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Grimm JW, Fyall AM, Osincup DP (2005) Incubation of sucrose craving: effects of reduced training and sucrose pre-loading. Physiol Behav 84: 73–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. London ED, Simon SL, Berman SM, Mandelkern MA, Lichtman AM, et al. (2004) Mood disturbances and regional cerebral metabolic abnormalities in recently abstinent methamphetamine abusers. Arch Gen Psychiatry 61: 73–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Melega WP, Jorgensen MJ, Lacan G, Way BM, Pham J, et al. (2008) Long-term methamphetamine administration in the vervet monkey models aspects of a human exposure: brain neurotoxicity and behavioral profiles. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 1441–1452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Nakama H, Chang L, Cloak C, Jiang C, Alicata D, et al. (2008) Association between psychiatric symptoms and craving in methamphetamine users. Am J Addict 17: 441–446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Newton TF, Kalechstein AD, Duran S, Vansluis N, Ling W (2004) Methamphetamine abstinence syndrome: preliminary findings. Am J Addict 13: 248–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. McGregor C, Srisurapanont M, Jittiwutikarn J, Laobhripatr S, Wongtan T, et al. (2005) The nature, time course and severity of methamphetamine withdrawal. Addiction 100: 1320–1329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Zorick T, Nestor L, Miotto K, Sugar C, Hellemann G, et al. (2010) Withdrawal symptoms in abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects. Addiction 105: 1809–1818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Hamilton M (1959) The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br J Med Psychol 32: 50–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES (1995) Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol 51: 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Brickenkamp R, Zillmer E (1998) The d2 Test of Attention. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.
- 36. Hwang J, Lyoo IK, Kim SJ, Sung YH, Bae S, et al. (2006) Decreased cerebral blood flow of the right anterior cingulate cortex in long-term and short-term abstinent methamphetamine users. Drug Alcohol Depend 82: 177–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Volkow ND, Chang L, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Franceschi D, et al. (2001) Loss of dopamine transporters in methamphetamine abusers recovers with protracted abstinence. J Neurosci 21: 9414–9418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Kim SJ, Lyoo IK, Hwang J, Chung A, Hoon Sung Y, et al. (2006) Prefrontal grey-matter changes in short-term and long-term abstinent methamphetamine abusers. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 9: 221–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Chang L, Miller E, Sedler M, et al. (2004) Partial recovery of brain metabolism in methamphetamine abusers after protracted abstinence. Am J Psychiatry 161: 242–248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Salo R, Nordahl TE, Galloway GP, Moore CD, Waters C, et al. (2009) Drug abstinence and cognitive control in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. J Subst Abuse Treat 37: 292–297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Iudicello JE, Woods SP, Vigil O, Scott JC, Cherner M, et al. (2010) Longer term improvement in neurocognitive functioning and affective distress among methamphetamine users who achieve stable abstinence. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 32: 704–718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Pickens CL, Airavaara M, Theberge F, Fanous S, Hope BT, et al. (2011) Neurobiology of the incubation of drug craving. Trends Neurosci 34: 411–420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Lu L, Grimm JW, Hope BT, Shaham Y (2004) Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal: a review of preclinical data. Neuropharmacology 47 Suppl 1214–226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Leventhal AM, Kahler CW, Ray LA, Stone K, Young D, et al. (2008) Anhedonia and amotivation in psychiatric outpatients with fully remitted stimulant use disorder. Am J Addict 17: 218–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Tuliao AP, Liwag ME (2011) Predictors of relapse in Filipino male methamphetamine users: a mixed methods approach. J Ethn Subst Abuse 10: 162–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Oliveto A, Poling J, Mancino MJ, Williams DK, Thostenson J, et al. (2012) Sertraline delays relapse in recently abstinent cocaine-dependent patients with depressive symptoms. Addiction 107: 131–141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]