Abstract
Background
Down’s syndrome (DS) is caused by triplication of all or part of human chromosome 21 and is characterized by a decrease in the overall size of the brain. One of the brain regions most affected is the cerebellum, in which the number of granule cells (GCs) is markedly decreased. GCs process sensory information entering the cerebellum via mossy fibres and pass it on to Purkinje cells and inhibitory interneurons. How GCs transform incoming signals depends on their input–output relationship, which is adjusted by tonically active GABAA receptor channels.
Results
We report that in the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS, in which cerebellar volume and GC number are decreased as in DS, the tonic GABAA receptor current in GCs is smaller than in wild-type mice and is less effective in moderating input resistance and raising the minimum current required for action potential firing. We also find that tonically active GABAA receptors curb the height and broaden the width of action potentials in wild-type GCs but not in Ts65Dn GCs. Single-cell real-time quantitative PCR reveals that these electrical differences are accompanied by decreased expression of the gene encoding the GABAA receptor β3 subunit but not genes coding for some of the other GABAA receptor subunits expressed in GCs (α1, α6, β2 and δ).
Conclusions
Weaker moderation of excitability and action potential waveform in GCs of the Ts65Dn mouse by tonically active GABAA receptors is likely to contribute to atypical transfer of information through the cerebellum. Similar changes may occur in DS.
Keywords: Down syndrome, Down’s syndrome, Cerebellum, Ts65Dn, Granule cell, GABAA receptor, Tonic inhibition, Shunting, qPCR, Single-cell RT-PCR
Background
Down’s syndrome (DS) occurs in different populations in 1 in 450 to 2200 live births [1]. It is caused by the presence of a third copy of all or part of human chromosome 21 (trisomy Hsa21; OMIM ID: 190685) [2], which results in a range of neurological, behavioural and physical phenotypes that vary in occurrence and expressivity between individuals [3]. Characteristics that all individuals with DS display include a smaller brain, a pronounced decrease in neuron number and distorted neuronal morphology in the cerebellum, hippocampus and cerebral cortex [4]. They also show impaired learning, memory and language, delays in the acquisition of motor skills, poor fine motor skills, altered balance and gait, and unclear speech [4,5]. These changes in brain structure and function are accompanied by altered expression of genes on the triplicated Hsa21 as well as on non-trisomic chromosomes [2,3,6]. How information processing in the DS brain is affected to produce the cognitive and motor deficits is incompletely understood [7]. Recent studies in mouse models of DS have made progress in delineating the modifications in hippocampal synaptic transmission and plasticity that contribute to deficits in specific types of memory [8-19]. In comparison, there have been surprisingly few electrophysiological investigations of cerebellar function in mouse models of DS [20,21].
The cerebellum is a key brain structure in the control of movement. The altered gait, posture, stride length, speech production, acquisition of motor skills and quality of fine motor skills observed in DS implicate cerebellar dysfunction [4,5]. This inference is supported by the finding that in individuals with DS, the volume of the cerebellum and the density of cerebellar GCs are reduced by one third and one quarter respectively, through impaired proliferation of precursor cells [22-28]. Increasing evidence suggests that the cerebellum also plays a role in various cognitive functions [29,30] and that cerebellar dysfunction may contribute to some of the cognitive deficits in DS [5]. As GCs process signals transmitted to the cerebellum by mossy fibres and transmit them to Purkinje cells (PCs) and inhibitory interneurons [31], changes in their number, intrinsic electrical properties or synaptic transmission are likely to distort cerebellar processing.
Postnatal maturation of cerebellar GCs in rodents entails increased expression of specific GABAA receptor (GABAAR) subunits and the development of a tonic current generated by repeated opening of extrasynaptic GABAAR channels [32-35]. Inhibition of the tonic current with a competitive GABAAR antagonist demonstrates that it is caused by the activation of GABAARs by ambient GABA [32,33,35-37]. The tonic current dampens GC excitability through shunting inhibition and so modulates information flow through the cerebellar cortex [33,35,37]. In this study, we made whole-cell patch-clamp recordings of GCs in the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS to determine whether the properties of the tonically active GABAAR channels are modified. We assessed whether their impact on the electrical properties of GCs is altered, as this could contribute to the increased excitability and changed action potential (AP) waveform observed previously in Ts65Dn GCs [21]. We also investigated whether expression of GABAAR subunit genes is modified in Ts65Dn GCs, by means of reverse transcription and real-time quantitative PCR (qPCR) of single GCs extracted from slices of cerebellum. The Ts65Dn mouse is the most widely investigated model of DS and is generated by triplication of a region of mouse chromosome 16 (Mmu16), which makes it trisomic for approximately half of the orthologous protein-coding genes and a subset of non-protein coding RNAs located on the long arm of Hsa21 [17,28,38]. The Ts65Dn mouse replicates the drop in GC number and density that typifies DS [26,39,40]. It also shows a decrease in the number of cerebellar PCs and displays morphological abnormalities in PC axons [26,39], changes that may be indicative of alterations in DS [28]. The structural changes in the Ts65Dn cerebellum are accompanied by variable changes in cerebellar expression of genes located in the triplicated region of Mmu16 and on non-trisomic chromosomes [41,42].
We describe previously unknown properties of the tonic current and profile gene expression levels for the major GABAAR subunits (α1, α6, β2, β3, γ2 and δ) expressed in wild-type GCs. Our recordings indicate that in the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS, the control of the electrical properties of cerebellar GCs by tonically active GABAARs is weaker. Single-cell qPCR analyses demonstrate a decrease in expression of the GABAAR β3 subunit gene in Ts65Dn GCs, but not of most of the other GABAAR subunit genes investigated. These differences are likely to affect information flow through the Ts65Dn cerebellum.
Results
To investigate if the properties of tonically active GABAARs in cerebellar GCs are altered in the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS, we made whole-cell patch-clamp recordings from GCs of mature Ts65Dn animals and their euploid (wild-type) littermates, aged between postnatal day (P)40 and P60. We used 10 μM SR95531, a competitive antagonist at GABAARs, to inhibit GABAARs activated by ambient GABA. This is expected to block all GABAARs activated by ambient GABA because 10 μM SR95531 reduces currents evoked by exogenous 10 μM GABA in P7-20 rat GCs by more than 99% [36]. Furthermore, the concentration of GABA surrounding GCs is estimated to be less than 200 nM in P21-40 wild-type rats [43] and more than 80% of the tonic current in these cells is inhibited by 200 nM SR95531 [43]. In previous studies, 10 μM SR95531 has been used to inhibit tonic currents or tonic inhibition in P35-45 rodent GCs under respectively voltage-clamp or current-clamp [35,44]. We also compared expression of genes encoding GABAAR subunits by means of single-cell qPCR in P42-69 GCs. The data presented were obtained from slices derived from 68 Ts65Dn mice and 95 wild-type mice.
Tonic GABAAR-mediated current-density is reduced in Ts65Dn GCs
Application of SR95531 (10 μM) to cells in voltage-clamp (held at −70 mV with a pipette containing a high Cl− concentration; chloride reversal potential, ECl, ~0 mV) caused a positive shift in mean current and a decrease in the amplitude of current fluctuations (variance) in both wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs (Figure 1A), indicating that GABAAR channels are continuously, or tonically, opening in both cell-types. However, the tonic GABAAR current-density (current divided by cell input capacitance in order to correct for variation in cell surface area) was significantly lower in Ts65Dn than in wild-type GCs (Figure 1B) (median and quartile values, wild-type, 10.6 pA/pF (14.8, 5.9), n = 54; Ts65Dn, 5.8 pA/pF (11.5, 3.8), n = 38; equivalent to chord conductances of 172 pS/pF and 97 pS/pF). The reduced tonic current-density did not reflect smaller current flow through individual channels, as there was no difference between wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs in the mean single-channel current amplitude, calculated as the slope of plots of current variance against current mean [45], for whole-cell currents inhibited by SR95531 (Figure 1C, −1.39 pA at −70 mV; corresponds to a mean unitary chord conductance of ~23 pS).
We compared the amplitudes of whole-cell tonic currents inhibited by SR95531 at different membrane potentials in some cells, in order to determine if there was a difference in voltage-dependence of the tonic GABAAR current, as such a difference could potentially contribute to different electrical properties of wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs [46]. Figure 1D shows that the dependence of current-density on voltage was non-linear in both wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs; alterations in membrane potential below ~−40 mV caused smaller changes in current-density than changes in membrane potential above ~−40 mV. The slope conductance (calculated from plots of absolute current-density against voltage) increased from 23 ± 14 pS/pF at −100 mV to 283 ± 98 pS/pF at −10 mV in wild-type GCs (n = 5), and from 16 ± 8 pS/pF at −100 mV to 216 ± 112 pS/pF at −10 mV in Ts65Dn GCs (n = 5). Therefore, the tonic GABAAR current showed outward rectification in both wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs.
During postnatal development of wild-type cerebellar GCs, spontaneously-occurring discrete phasic postsynaptic GABAergic currents decrease in size and frequency, while the tonic GABAergic current appears at ~ P7 and increases in magnitude, so that in mature GCs most of the spontaneous GABAAR-mediated charge transfer occurs via tonically active GABAARs [32,33,37]. We found that phasic postsynaptic currents that were identified as GABAAR-mediated by their sensitivity to SR95531 also occur at low frequency in Ts65Dn GCs (Figure 1A; median and quartile values, wild-type 0.28 Hz (0.12, 0.75), n = 67; Ts65Dn, 0.50 Hz (0.12, 1.01), n = 47; p = 0.1577, Mann Whitney U test) and that there was no change in their mean amplitude (median and quartile values, wild-type, -29.2 pA (−22.7, -35.78), n = 59; Ts65Dn, -26.9 pA (−21.2, -37.3), n = 41; p = 0.8835 ) or decay time course (median and quartile values of weighted decay time constant, wild-type, 7.8 ms (4.4, 10.9); Ts65Dn, 5.9 ms (4.5, 8.0); p = 0.1095, Mann Whitney U test). They carried a minor percentage of the total charge transferred by spontaneously active GABAARs, which did not differ from that in wild-type GCs (median and quartile values, wild-type, 3.3% (1.8, 5.0); Ts65Dn, 3.9% (2.0, 6.7), p = 0.2068, Mann Whitney U test).
Unchanged relative contributions of δ and α6 subunits to GABAARs mediating tonic currents in Ts65Dn GCs
The lower tonic current-density in Ts65Dn GCs could potentially reflect altered subunit composition of the underlying GABAARs, as subunit composition is a key determinant of the probability of channel opening and the extent and speed of receptor desensitisation upon activation by GABA [47]. In wild-type GCs, the tonic current develops postnatally [32,33] in parallel with increasing expression of δ and α6 subunits in extrasynaptic GABAARs [34,35]. These receptors have high affinity for GABA and are continually activated by submicromolar ambient GABA [48]. We investigated if the contribution of δ or α6 subunits to extrasynaptic receptors was modified in Ts65Dn GCs, by testing the effects of subunit-selective drugs on the tonic current blocked by SR95531. Figure 2A and 2B show that application of the agonist THIP (gaboxadol), at a δ-selective concentration of 300 nM [49], enhanced the current in both wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs. There was no difference in the magnitude of the effect (Figure 2C). Figure 2D - F show that furosemide, at a concentration (100 μM) at which it is selective for the α6–containing GABAARs in GCs [50,51], inhibited the tonic current by a similar degree in wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs. Furosemide also binds to α4-containing GABAARs but with lower affinity [52] and α4-receptors are absent from cerebellar GCs [53]. The changes in the size of the current produced by THIP or furosemide were accompanied by proportionate changes in variance, as the drugs did not alter the linear relationship between SR95531-sensitive variance and mean current (Figure 3). This indicates that there were no differences in mean unitary current amplitude of drug-sensitive and drug-resistant tonically active GABAARs in Ts65Dn and wild-type GCs. These pharmacological investigations show that the relative contributions of δ and α6 subunit-containing GABAARs to the tonic current and the unitary conductances of these receptors do not differ between wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs.
GABAAR subunit mRNAs in wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs
To test the possibility that the lower tonic current-density in Ts65Dn GCs was associated with weaker expression of GABAAR genes, real-time qPCR was used to quantify GABAAR subunit transcripts in individual granule cells. Single-cell mRNA was reverse-transcribed into cDNA and cDNAs encoding δ, α6, β2 and β3 subunits were quantified with real-time PCR, as these encode the extrasynaptic GABAARs mediating the tonic current in mature wild-type GCs [35,37,43,54-57]. We also measured cDNAs encoding GABAAR α1 and γ2 subunits, which together with β2 and β3 subunits form synaptic GABAARs that mediate phasic currents when transiently activated [54,55,58], because the relative synaptic and extrasynaptic distributions of these subunits are not known for Ts65Dn GCs. Moreover, in wild-type GCs some extrasynaptic receptors may contain α1 subunits and some synaptic receptors contain α6 subunits [43,55,59-62]. For this analysis, individual cells were ‘harvested’ from cerebellar slices into the tip of a blunt pipette, as illustrated in the sequence of photographs in Figure 4A. The pipette tip contained a solution that prevented mRNA degradation during harvesting and processing of the cell prior to reverse-transcription of mRNA into cDNA, all of which were carried out immediately after cell harvesting [63]. The single-cell cDNAs were stored at −20°C until real-time qPCR, which was performed under conditions that had been optimised on GABAAR cDNAs generated from wild-type mouse cerebellum (see methods for more details). Only one subunit cDNA was measured per cell.
Figure 4B summarises the percentages of GCs in which expression of a gene encoding one of six GABAAR subunits was detected. Considering firstly wild-type GCs, Figure 4B reports wide diversity in the proportions of cells in which the different subunit cDNAs were detected (α6, α1 and β2, 86 - 93% cells; δ, ~59% cells; β3, ~42% cells; γ2, ~35% cells). This denotes disparity in transcription dynamics of GABAAR genes, as lower detection rates mirror higher percentages of cells without transcripts at the time of cell sampling. The disparity was also present in Ts65Dn GCs and the detection frequencies for α6, α1, β2, δ and β3 cDNAs in these cells were the same as in wild-type GCs (Figure 4B). Therefore, there were no major alterations to transcription dynamics of α6, α1, β2, δ and β3 subunit genes in Ts65Dn GCs, such as a complete shut down or a several-fold augmentation of transcriptional activity. However, γ2 cDNAs were detected in only half the percentage of Ts65Dn GCs (17%) as in wild-type GCs (35%), although this difference was not statistically different (Fisher’s exact test, p = 0.202), possibly because of the relatively small number of Ts65Dn GCs examined for the expression of γ2 mRNA.
The numbers of cDNA copies in the cDNA-positive cells (determined from standard curves, see methods) are summarised in Figure 4C. As expected from the stochastic nature and bursting characteristics of gene transcription [63,64], the values are widely and non-normally distributed. Figure 4C shows marked variation in the numbers of cDNAs encoding different subunits, which parallels the variation in the percentages of cDNA-positive cells shown in Figure 4B. It suggests that relative levels of mRNA expression can be roughly approximated in wild-type GCs by the sequence α6 ≥ α1 ≥ β2 > δ ≈ β3 ≈ γ2. The α6, α1, β2 and δ cDNA copy numbers were not different between wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs, but the β3 cDNA copy number was reduced in Ts65Dn GCs. In addition, the number of γ2 cDNAs appeared to be reduced, but the low number of γ2-positive Ts65Dn GCs precluded a statistical analysis (Figure 4C). These single-cell qPCR data indicate that reduced β3 mRNA expression accompanies the reduced tonic current-density in Ts65Dn GCs. More work is required to confirm the suggested decrease in γ2 mRNA expression.
Weaker influence of tonic GABAAR conductance on input resistance of Ts65Dn GCs
Previous studies have established that a tonic GABAAR current dampens the excitability of mature cerebellar GCs because it lowers cell input resistance, which results in smaller changes in voltage in response to current input [35,37]. This shunting inhibition raises rheobase (the minimum current input required to initiate firing of APs) [33,35,37]. Notably, moderation of GC input resistance by tonically active GABAARs is not accompanied by clear moderation of the resting membrane potential at P18-22 [33] or P30-62 [35,65]. To investigate if shunting inhibition is altered in Ts65Dn GCs, in which we recorded a smaller tonic GABAAR current-density in voltage-clamp, we made whole-cell current-clamp recordings before and during block of GABAARs with 10 μM SR95531. The composition of the pipette solution was similar to that previously used in current-clamp recordings of wild-type cerebellar GCs [44,66,67]. It established an ECl (~−69 mV) that was more positive than the mean resting potential (~ − 80 mV) of wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs [21], in keeping with the difference between the resting potential and the equilibrium potential for GABAAR currents in wild-type GCs measured with the non-invasive perforated patch technique [33,65]. As our voltage-clamp recordings indicated that the charge carried by the tonic current is far in excess of that carried by phasic currents in Ts65Dn GCs as well as in wild-type GCs, the predominant effect of the application of SR95531 on both types of cells in current-clamp is block of tonic inhibition rather than block of phasic inhibition [35,44,67].
Recordings of voltage-changes evoked by current injections showed that before block of tonically active GABAARs, subthreshold voltage–current relationships were non-linear in both types of cells (Figure 5A - C), but depolarising currents caused greater changes in voltage in Ts65Dn GCs at potentials above resting potential (Figure 5C). Therefore, input resistance changed with voltage in both cell-types but Ts65Dn GCs had higher input resistance at potentials approaching the voltage threshold for AP firing, as reported previously [21]. This difference is clearer in plots of mean input resistance against membrane potential (Figure 5D), derived from the mean voltage–current relationship in Figure 5C. Resting membrane potentials did not differ (Figure 1B, wild-type, −80.21 ± 0.4 mV, n = 24; Ts65Dn, −79.76 ± 0.7 mV, n = 13; p = 0.5137, Student’s unpaired t-test). By contrast, after block of tonically active GABAARs with SR95531, the subthreshold voltage–current relationships in wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs were indistinguishable (Figure 5A - C). Furthermore, they were much steeper indicating that in SR95531 the input resistance was markedly elevated in both cell-types (Figure 5D). In addition, the voltage-dependence of input resistance was weaker (Figure 5D). These changes induced by SR95531 were not accompanied by a change in resting membrane potential in either type of cell (Figure 5C; wild-type: control, −80.21 ± 0.4 mV, SR95531, −80.87 ± 0.8 mV, n = 24, p = 0.3563 Student’s paired t-test; Ts65Dn: control, −79.96 ± 0.7 mV, SR95531, −79.67 ± 1.0 mV, n = 13; p = 0.9047 Student’s paired t-test). Our finding that the impact of the tonic inhibition was weaker on the current–voltage relationship of the Ts65Dn GCs while its lack of effect on the resting potential was unaltered, lends further support to other observations that the predominant effect of tonically active GABAARs in mature GCs is shunting inhibition rather than alteration of membrane potential [33,35,37]. The lack of a clear effect on the resting potential is thought to reflect the small magnitude of the outward driving force on the Cl− ions [33,65].
The transformations of the subthreshold voltage–current relationships revealed that the tonic GABAAR conductance lowers input resistance in a voltage-dependent manner in both wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs, but this decrease was weaker in Ts65Dn GCs at potentials approaching the voltage-threshold for AP firing. The vertical differences between the curves in Figure 5D show that this effect accounts for a mean decrease of ~1.6 GΩ at −85 mV and ~1.1 GΩ at −75 mV in wild-type GCs, and ~1.7 GΩ at −85 mV and ~0.7 GΩ at −75 mV in Ts65Dn GCs. Elimination of the difference between the subthreshold voltage–current relationships of wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs in SR95531 (Figure 5C) demonstrates that the higher input resistance of Ts65Dn GCs at membrane potentials approaching AP threshold under control conditions (Figure 5D) is due to weaker moderation of input resistance by the tonic GABAAR conductance. This is consistent with the smaller tonic current-density recorded in Ts65Dn GCs in voltage-clamp.
Weaker inhibition of Ts65Dn GC excitability by tonic activation of GABAARs
In agreement with previous studies of wild-type GCs [35,37,67], the increase in input resistance caused by blockade of GABAARs with SR95531 was accompanied by a decrease in rheobase (minimum current input required to trigger AP firing) in wild-type GCs. This is illustrated by voltage recordings in Figure 5A and plots summarising rheobase values in Figure 6A. In Ts65Dn GCs, block of GABAARs also reduced rheobase (Figure 5B and 6A), but the decrease was smaller (Figure 6B), in parallel with the smaller increase in input resistance near AP threshold (Figure 5D). Furthermore, after block of GABAARs, there was no difference between the rheobase of wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs (median and quartile values, wild-type, 1.9 (1.6, 3.4) pA/pF, n = 24; Ts65Dn, 2.4 (1.6, 3.1) pA/pF, n = 13, p = 0.8056, Mann Whitney U test), whereas before the application of SR95531, rheobase was lower in Ts65Dn GCs (wild-type, 5.0 (4.1, 6.5) pA/pF, n = 24; Ts65Dn, 3.8 (3.3, 5) pA/pF, n = 13, p = 0.0211, Mann Whitney U test) [21]. The decrease in rheobase upon application of SR95531 was not accompanied by a change in the sensitivity of cell firing to changes in suprathreshold current input in either wild-type or Ts65Dn mice (Figure 6C). It was also not accompanied by changes in latency to first AP at rheobase (wild-type, p = 0.3716, Student’s paired t test; n = 24; Ts65Dn, p = 0.2922, Student’s paired t test; n = 13), which did not differ between wild-type and Ts65Dn before (p = 0.9700, Student’s unpaired t test) or after (p = 0.1358, Student’s unpaired t test) application of SR95531. In summary, our finding that rheobase was lower in Ts65Dn than in wild-type GCs before but not after block of GABAARs, demonstrates that the tonic conductance exerts weaker inhibition of cell-excitability in Ts65Dn GCs.
Weaker control of action potential waveform in Ts65Dn GCs by tonic activation of GABAARs
Under control conditions, the height of APs (measured between peak and afterhyperpolarisation) was greater in Ts65Dn GCs than in wild-type GCs (Figure 7A and B) [21]. We investigated the possibility that this was also due to a weaker influence of the tonically active GABAARs in Ts65Dn GCs, although previous studies in wild-type GCs had not reported a moderating effect on AP waveform [35,37]. We found that block of GABAARs with SR95531 increased AP height in wild-type (by ~4%) but not Ts65Dn GCs, thus eliminating the difference in height present in control conditions (Figure 7A and B). SR95531 also hyperpolarised AP voltage threshold, as illustrated by the vertical displacement of the superimposed traces in Figure 7A (from −47.93 ± 0.63 mV to −52.34 ± 0.66 mV in 24 wild-type GCs, p < 0.0001; from −46.51 ± 0.69 mV to −50.13 ± 0.92 mV in 13 Ts65Dn GCs, p = 0.0001, Student’s paired t tests). This negative shift was similar in magnitude in wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs (wild-type, 4.41 ± 0.64 mV; Ts65Dn GCs, 3.62 ± 0.49 mV, p = 0.4040, Student’s unpaired t test) unlike the differential effect of SR95531 on AP height. Vertical alignment of APs recorded before and during application of SR95531 on their voltage-thresholds (Figure 7C) revealed that the increase in AP height in wild-type GCs was mainly due to an increase in peak amplitude relative to threshold, which was unchanged in Ts65Dn GCs (Figure 7D). In addition, there was a small decrease in afterhyperpolarisation relative to threshold (wild-type, 1.3 ± 0.7 mV, n = 24; Ts65Dn GCs, 1.5 ± 0.5 mV, n = 13) that did not differ between cell-type (Figure 7D, p = 0.8514, Student’s unpaired t test). As well as increasing AP height in wild-type GCs, block of GABAARs accelerated maximum rates of rise and fall of APs (Figure 7E and F) by a similar percentage (~16 and ~18%, p = 0.7156, Student’s unpaired t test). This resulted in a shortening of AP duration (Figure 7G) and elimination of the difference in AP width between wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs under control conditions (control: p = 0.0473, n = 24; SR95531: p = 0.9334, n = 13, Student’s unpaired t test). In contrast, the kinetics and duration of APs in Ts65Dn GCs were not significantly altered in SR95531 (Figure 7E - G). Therefore, tonically active GABAARs modulate AP waveform in wild-type GCs but have a much weaker effect on AP waveform in Ts65Dn GCs.
Our finding that the changes in AP waveform in wild-type GCs upon inhibition of GABAARs occurred in the absence of a change in mean resting potential does not support the possibility that the increase in amplitude and narrowing of the APs reflects removal of inactivation of voltage-gated ion channels by reversal of GABAAR-induced membrane depolarisation. However, it was possible that a relationship between a change in AP waveform and hyperpolarisation was obscured in our comparison of average values, which masked variation in the effect of SR95531 in different cells between a small decrease, no effect or a small increase in resting potential or AP amplitude. Therefore, we plotted the change in AP amplitude against the change in resting potential for individual wild-type cells. This revealed a weak correlation (r(22) = −0.317, p = 0.1494), suggesting that a change in membrane potential is unlikely to be the main factor underlying the increase and narrowing of APs caused by inhibition of tonically active GABAARs.
Discussion
Our study demonstrates that in wild-type cerebellar GCs, tonic activation of GABAARs moderates input resistance in a voltage-dependent manner and modifies the waveform of APs. Our study also profiles wide variation in expression of genes encoding α1, α6, δ, γ2, β2 and β3 subunits of GABAARs in wild-type GCs. In GCs of the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS, tonically active GABAARs exert a weaker control over input resistance and AP waveform, which results in increased excitability and the firing of APs with increased amplitude. These differences in electrical properties are accompanied by decreased transcription of the Gabrb3 gene encoding the GABAAR β3 subunit, in the absence of changes in expression of genes encoding GABAAR α1, α6, δ or β2 subunits, while a suggested difference in γ2 gene expression deserves further exploration.
Tonically active GABAARs moderate input resistance and action potential waveform
We confirmed that tonically active GABAARs control input resistance and rheobase in mature wild-type GCs [35,37,67] without a detectable effect on resting membrane potential, as reported previously for mature GCs [35,65], but also uncovered previously unknown characteristics. We find that the tonic GABAAR conductance is not constant, as sometimes assumed, but increases in size at voltages approaching threshold, like the outwardly rectifying tonic GABAAR current in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons [46]. We also find that tonic activation of GABAARs slows the kinetics and curbs the height of APs in wild-type GCs. In contrast, tonic activation of GABAARs has minimal effect on AP shape in Ts65Dn GCs, and APs in wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs are the same height after block of GABAARs. Our finding that SR95531 had almost no impact on AP waveform in Ts65Dn GCs counteracts the possibility that the effects of SR95531 on APs in wild-type GCs reflect off-target binding to the ion channels that generate the AP (unless these are different in wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs). Further evidence that the effects of SR95531 are mediated by inhibition of GABAARs, without effects on other channels, is the previously reported lack of an effect of SR95531 on input resistance and rheobase in cerebellar GCs lacking extrasynaptic GABAARs [35].
It is not clear how tonic activation of GABAARs modifies AP waveform in wild-type GCs. In hippocampal mossy fibre boutons, GABAARs tonically activated by ambient GABA also reduce AP amplitude and make APs wider [68]. These changes are thought to reflect enhanced inactivation of voltage-gated sodium channels secondary to depolarisation of the membrane by tonically active GABAARs [68]. However, such a mechanism seems unlikely to account for the effects of tonically active GABAARs on APs in wild-type GCs, because the increase in AP amplitude and decrease in AP duration upon inhibition of GABAARs with SR95531 were not accompanied by detectable changes in membrane potential or in the duration of the subthreshold depolarisation preceding the AP (which might change the fraction of inactivated voltage-gated channels). An alternative explanation is that the outwardly-rectifying tonic conductance generated by the repeatedly opening GABAAR channels is able to shunt the currents that generate the AP. Shunting of APs by a GABAAR conductance has been previously observed in primary afferents [69] and in the soma of dentate granule neurons [70], and has been demonstrated in a modelling study [71].
Decreased expression of Gabrb3 in Ts65Dn GCs
Our results indicate that the smaller tonic GABAAR current in GCs of Ts65Dn mice does not arise because of a reduced single-channel conductance or differences in the relative contributions of α6 and δ subunits, which are important determinants of GABA potency and receptor desensitisation [43,47,56,72,73]. A contributing factor to the smaller current might be the decreased transcription of Gabrb3 that we detected with single cell qPCR. This could result in fewer receptors at the cell surface because β3 subunits control trafficking of GABAARs [74]. This possibility has yet to be explored experimentally by measurement of cell surface expression of the receptors or currents evoked by exogenous GABA. Further contributing factors to the smaller tonic current in Ts65Dn GCs may be changes in ambient GABA concentration. A decrease would result in the activation of fewer channels while an increase might also reduce the mean number of channels open if it enhances desensitisation. The GABA concentration is reported to be reduced in foetal DS brains [75] but unchanged in post-mortem samples of cerebellum from adults with DS [76].
Decreased expression of GABAAR β3 mRNA or β2/3 protein has been detected before in adult Ts65Dn mouse brain, in the fascia dentata of Ts65Dn hippocampus at 3 months but not at 8 months, in DS brain cells (cultured neural progentior cells derived from foetal DS cortex) but not in the hippocampus of Ts65Dn mice at P15 [13,77-79]. Why transcription of Gabrb3 might be down-regulated is unclear. This cannot be due to a change in gene dosage because Gabrb3 is located on Mmu7 and not on the partially trisomic Mmu16. Likewise, in humans, Gabrb3 is located on Hsa15 and not on Hsa21, which is trisomic in DS. It is possible that in DS, disomic Gabrb3 is modified by overexpressed trisomic microRNAs (miRNAs) or transcription factors located on Hsa21 [2,3,28,80-82]. The trisomic region of Mmu16 in Ts65Dn mice harbours two of the miRNAs located on Hsa21, mir-155 and mir-802, [2,80,81,83,84] and these are overexpressed in Ts65Dn hippocampus and prefrontal cortex [80]. However, they are not predicted to target human or mouse Gabrb3 mRNAs [85-87]. Gabrb3 expression might also be modified by disomic miRNAs or transcription factors, whose expression is altered by trisomic miRNAs or transcription factors or whose properties are postranslationally modified by interactions with trisomic proteins [2,6,78,80-82]. A further possibility is that downregulation of Gabrb3 expression in Ts65Dn GCs is secondary to changes in the ambient concentration of GABA.
GABAergic transmission is altered in Ts65Dn mice
The smaller tonic GABAAR current and its weaker effects in Ts65Dn GCs are in contrast with augmented GABAergic inhibition in dentate granule cells and CA1 pyramidal neurons of Ts65Dn hippocampus [8,10-18,88], which like the cerebellum is markedly reduced in volume and cell-number in individuals with DS and in Ts65Dn mice [4,5,17,28,89]. The enhanced inhibition impairs long term potentiation (LTP) and learning and memory in Ts65Dn mice and is suggested to be due to up regulation of fast or slow phasic transmission by, respectively, GABAA or GABAB receptors [8-18,88]. The increase in GABAAR-mediated synaptic transmission is cell and age specific. It does not occur in all subtypes of CA1 neurons or at all ages [13,90], whereas hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons of P13-16 Ts65Dn mice show a reduction in inhibitory GABAAR-mediated synaptic input and no impairment of LTP [91]. The molecular basis of the enhanced GABABR-mediated inhibition is cell-type specific over expression of the GABAB effector, GIRK2, which occurs because the encoding Kcnj6 gene is triplicated in Ts65Dn mice, as it is in individuals with DS [13,14,92]. In adult Ts65Dn cerebellum, GIRK2 expression is elevated in unipolar brush cells rather than GCs [93]. Our study did not detect differences in the frequency or time course of spontaneous phasic GABAAR-mediated postsynaptic currents in cerebellar GCs, but evoked phasic GABAAR-mediated transmission has yet to be investigated.
The possibility that increased inhibition in Ts65Dn CA1 hippocampal neurons is due not only to changes in phasic inhibition but also to upregulation of tonic GABAAR currents has not been addressed directly, by measuring the tonic GABAAR currents that are known to be expressed in these and other types of hippocampal cells [46,56,94-97]. Indirect evidence for their augmentation in the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS is alleviation of cognitive deficits by a selective inverse agonist (α5IA) [98] or a negative allosteric modulator (RO4938581) [99] of α5-containing GABAARs, which is the class of GABAAR that generates a tonic current in hippocampal CA1 and CA3 pyramidal neurons [96,97]. It has also been suggested that the beneficial effects of chronic treatment with α5IA involve changes in gene expression [100].
Significance of weaker control of electrical properties in Ts65Dn cerebellar GCs by tonically active GABAARs
The weaker moderation of excitability and AP waveform by tonically active GABAARs in Ts65Dn GCs predicts that detection and transfer of incoming signals from mossy fibres and hence sensory information processing by the cerebellum is not the same as in wild-type mice. In vivo recordings from GCs of young rats (P18-22) show that block of tonically active GABAARs increases firing of GCs in response to incoming sensory signals. The increase is moderate relative to the greater increase in spontaneous firing [44,67] and hence sensory signals are less likely to be discriminated from the enhanced background activity, resulting in impaired fidelity of sensory transmission through the GC layer. However, it is uncertain how closely these observations predict the effect of tonically active GABAARs on adult GCs, because the electrical properties of P18-22 and mature GCs are not identical [21,35,44,65,67]. Information transfer by Ts65Dn GCs could also be affected by the change in AP shape, as it is becoming apparent that neural information is encoded not only in the frequency and pattern of firing but also in the shape of APs [101]. For example, the altered AP waveform could distort the time course of glutamate release from GC axons on to Purkinje cell dendrites and inhibitory interneurons. If the changes in GC excitability and AP waveform also occur in DS, reinstatement of the tonic current to an optimal level with agonists or positive allosteric modulators selective for the underlying α6-containing GABAARs may help correct cerebellar dysfunction. Such drugs should not exacerbate the cognitive deficits in Ts65Dn mice arising from increased inhibition in the hippocampus because the hippocampus, like most brain structures external to the cerebellum, is devoid of α6 GABAARs [53,57]. On the other hand, if the decrease in tonic current is a compensatory mechanism for the fall in GC number that aims to maintain information flow through the cerebellum, drugs that enhance the function of α6-containing GABAARs may exacerbate cerebellar dysfunction in DS, since augmentation of tonic inhibition above an optimum level can also impair transfer of sensory transmission through the GC layer of the cerebellar cortex [44].
Drugs that inhibit GABAAR function have been suggested as potential treatments for the improvement of mental capacity in DS because they counteract excess inhibition in the hippocampus [5,11,15-17,38,88,98,99]. Since we find that the tonic GABAAR current is decreased in cerebellar GCs of the Ts65Dn mouse, such drugs should ideally target GABAARs in the hippocampus without affecting extrasynaptic GABAARs in cerebellar GCs. Examples of such drugs are the α5-selective compounds α5IA [98] and RO4938581 [99], because α5-containing GABAARs are highly expressed in the hippocampus but not the cerebellum [53,57]. In contrast, the interaction of the antagonist pentylenetetrazole with a broader range of GABAARs may explain why it not only improves memory function in Ts65Dn mice but may also exacerbate their impaired ability to maintain equilibrium, a behaviour that requires the proper operation of the cerebellum [88].
The effects of upregulating the tonic current in cerebellar GCs with drugs selective for α6- containing GABAARs, could be investigated on cognitive deficits in Ts65Dn mice with standard tests of learning and memory [17,38]. Evaluation of the potential effects of these drugs on speech and language, both of which rely on the correct operation of the cerebellum and are markedly affected in DS [5,30,102,103], might be possible in Ts65Dn mice through the analysis of ultrasonic vocalisations [104], the development of which is delayed in Ts65Dn mice [105]. However, appraisal of the potential of these drugs to improve motor dysfunction in DS by testing their effects on Ts65Dn mice would not be straightforward, because despite the marked drop in cerebellar volume and GC number and density, and the weaker influence of the tonic-active GABAARs on electrical properties of GCs described in the current study, changes in sensorimotor performance of Ts65Dn mice have not been consistently observed. Alterations in equilibrium, motor coordination, locomotor activity or gait dynamics have been detected in some studies or in a subset of tests [88,105-111] but not in other studies [26,112-114]. It is not known if the tonic GABAAR current is altered in cerebellar GCs of other mouse models of DS.
In summary, we report that GCs in the hypogranular cerebellum of the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS have a smaller tonic GABAAR current and weaker expression of the GABAAR β3 subunit-gene than GCs in wild-type animals. The smaller tonic current contributes to increased excitability and the firing of APs with increase amplitude and faster kinetics. Similar changes may accompany the decrease in cerebellar GC number in DS and modify cerebellar function.
Methods
Animals
Mice were generated by crossing female B6EiC3Sn a/A-Ts(1716)65Dn (Ts65Dn) mice, carrying a partial trisomy of chromosome 16 [115], with C57BL/6JEi × C3H/HeSnJ (B6EiC3Sn) F1 males, at the University of Bristol, in accordance with the United Kingdom Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and with the University of Bristol Ethical Review Group. Parental generations of all three mice strains were obtained from The Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, Maine, USA). To distinguish trisomic Ts65Dn animals from euploid animals (wild-type, littermates of the trisomic animals), quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction of tail-tip genomic DNA [116] was used to measure expression of the App gene (present in three copies in Ts65Dn and two copies in wild-type animals) relative to expression of the Apob gene (present in two copies in both Ts65Dn and wild-type animals; The Jackson Laboratory Protocols) [117].
Cerebellar slices
Parasagittal slices of cerebellar vermis (200 μm) were prepared from 50 male Ts65Dn mice and 70 male wild-type mice (littermates of Ts65Dn mice) aged P40-60, on a Leica VT1000S vibrating microtome (Leica Microsystems, Nussloch, Germany). Animals were culled in accordance with the United Kingdom Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and the University of Bristol Ethical Review Group. Slices were cut in ice-cold sucrose-based solution (in mM: 248 sucrose, 1.3 MgSO4, 5 KCl, 2.4 CaCl2, 1.2 KH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 D-glucose, pH 7.4, bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2) and stored in standard Krebs-Henseleit solution (in mM: 124 NaCl, 1.3 MgSO4, 5 KCl, 2.4 CaCl2, 1.2 KH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 D-glucose, pH 7.4, bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2) prior to patch-clamp recording or harvesting of cells.
Patch-clamp recording and analysis
Individual slices were viewed on a Zeiss FS Axioskop microscope (Carl Zeiss Ltd., Welwyn Garden City, UK). Patch-clamp recordings were made with pipettes (thick-walled borosilicate glass, coated with Sylgard 184, fire-polished) and an Axopatch 200A or 200B amplifier (Axon Instruments, Union City, CA), from slices superfused with standard Krebs-Henseleit solution (~1.5 ml/min) at ~23°C, as described previously [32] and in keeping with previous patch-clamp studies of granule cells at a similar temperature [35,118-121]. Pipettes were filled with, in mM: 135 CsCl, 10 HEPES, 2 MgATP, 10 EGTA (pH 7.2 with TEA-OH) for voltage-clamp recording, or 126 KCH3SO3, 4 KCl, 10 HEPES, 4 MgATP, 5 EGTA, 4 NaCl, 0.5 CaCl2 (pH 7.2 with K-OH) for current-clamp recording, and had resistances of 3.5 – 10 MΩ. Input capacitance measurements of cells recorded in voltage-clamp were taken from amplifier settings used to cancel current transients generated by 5 mV jumps, as in several previous patch-clamp studies of granule cells [32,35,36,119]. Median and quartile values: wild-type, 1.9 (1.5, 2.3) pF, n = 58; Ts65Dn, 2.7 (2.0, 3.2) pF, n = 38. When cells were recorded in current-clamp, input capacitance was calculated from the time-constant of a single exponential function fitted to the voltage deflection generated by a negative current injection (−10 or −8 pA) [122]. Median and quartile values: wild-type, 3.0 (2.4, 4.1) pF, n = 43; Ts65Dn, 3.7 (3.0, 4.0), n = 25. GCs of all ages are known to behave as single electrical compartments and the measured input capacitance encompasses capacitances of the soma and dendrites [119].
Current recordings were low-pass filtered by the filter in the Axopatch 200A amplifier (10 kHz, 4 pole Bessel) and then passed through a second filter (2 kHz, 8 pole low-pass Bessel filter, Frequency Devices, Haverhill, MA, USA), or they were filtered by the amplifier-filter alone (2 or 5 kHz), before being digitised on-line at 10 or 25 kHz with a Cambridge Electronic Design (CED) power 1401 A/D interface using Spike2 software (v. 5.13) (CED, Cambridge, UK). Voltage recordings were low-pass filtered (4 pole Bessel filter in the Axopatch 200B amplifier) at 10 kHz and acquired at 62.5 kHz with Signal (v. 3 or 4) or Spike2 software. They were analysed with CED Signal or Spike2 software and with Origin software v.6 or 7 (Microcal, Northampton, MA).
Currents were recorded before and after blockade of GABAARs by SR95531 (10 μM) at a holding potential of −70 mV. In a few cells, tonic current–voltage relationships were measured by holding the membrane potential at different values between −100 mV and 0 mV for 2–4 s before and during the application of SR95531. The amplitude and variance of the tonic current generated by the activation of GABAARs by ambient GABA were calculated as the difference between the mean amplitude and variance around the mean of ~5 s periods of digitised data recorded before and during application of SR95531. Sections of data without discrete synaptic currents were chosen for measurement, but if these were rare, multiple periods lacking discrete events adding up to 5 s were measured. Slope conductances at different potentials were calculated from sigmoidal curves fitted to plots of mean current-density against voltage. Plots of SR95531-sensitive variance against SR95531-sensitive mean current measured in many cells were used to derive the mean unitary current of the GABAARs [45]. Whole-cell and single-channel chord conductances were calculated using the reversal potentials of the mean whole-cell current–voltage curves in wild-type and Ts65Dn GCs. The relative charge transfer during tonic and phasic synaptic currents was calculated from 20–30 s periods of digitised data recorded before and during the application of SR99531 [123]. The decay time course of the phasic currents was determined by fitting a double exponential function or, rarely, a single exponential function to average currents constructed for each recording by aligning the phasic currents on their rising phase (CED Spike 2 software). The zero time point was defined as the time at the peak of the current. The weighted mean time constant was calculated as A1.τ1 + A2.τ2, where A1 and A2 are the fractional amplitudes of the fast and slow components, and τ1 and τ2 are the time constants of the two components.
Membrane potentials recorded in current-clamp were corrected for a calculated junction potential of 8.8 mV. AP parameters were measured for the first three APs elicited near rheobase (current injection threshold) using Signal or a supplementary Signal script that generated ‘phase-plane plots’ for the measurement of voltage threshold and maximum rates of rise and fall (Steven Clifford, CED), and then averaged. AP height was measured between the peak and the afterhyperpolarisation [124]. Increments in the size of currents injected result in unequal increments in current-density (pA/pF) in different cells, because of cell-to-cell variation in input capacitance. To enable averaging of plots of voltage or AP frequency against current-density, the plot for each cell was interpolated using equally-spaced points (0.5 or 0.1 pA/pF interval) and interpolated values were averaged.
Stock solutions were made of the following drugs: SR95531 (10 mM in filtered Milli-Q water, Tocris Bioscience, Bristol, UK or Ascent Scientific, Bristol, UK), THIP (30 mM in filtered Milli-Q water, Tocris Bioscience), furosemide (100 or 300 mM in 100% DMSO, Sigma). They were stored as aliquots at −20°C and added to the external solution when required. The effects of the drugs THIP and furosemide were measured 11–14 mins after application.
Single-cell reverse transcription real-time PCR
GABAAR subunit mRNAs were quantified in individual GCs harvested from cerebellar slices prepared from 18 male Ts65Dn mice and 25 male euploid littermates (wild-type). These animals were similar in age (P42 – 69) to the animals from which slices were prepared for patch-clamp recording (P40-60). The procedures described below are based on methods developed previously [63]. The study reports absolute quantification of mRNAs in individual cells derived from comparison of real-time data to standard curves, rather than relative expression normalised by expression levels of reference genes.
Cell harvesting
On the day of cell harvesting, pipettes were pulled from thin-walled borosilicate glass capillaries (Harvard Apparatus, Kent, UK) and fire-polished to a tip size of 1.5 – 3 μm. These had been made RNase-free by one rinse with 0.5 M NaOH, two rinses with absolute ethanol, three or four rinses with sterile ultra-pure Elgastat water (0.2 μm filtered) and baking at 160°C for 2 × 4 hours. In order to harvest a single cell, a pipette was filled with 4 μl of ‘RNA-protecting solution’, which contained 15 U RNasin® Plus or RNasin® ribonuclease inhibitor (Promega, N2515 or N2615, Southampton, UK) and 25 mM DTT (Clontech, accompanies PowerScript reverse transcriptase 639501, Saint-Germain-en-Laye, France). The two components were combined on each day of harvesting and stored on ice. The pipette was placed in the bath solution under positive pressure and moved towards the cell of interest. Once the tip was at the cell, the positive pressure was released and gentle suction was applied to the pipette via a mouthpiece and a length of tubing, so as to draw the cell into the pipette. Suction was stopped as soon as the majority of the cell was in the tip. This procedure was observed on a monitor using AxioVision software and an AxioCam HRm camera attached to the Zeiss FS Axioskop microscope (Carl Zeiss Ltd.). Much effort was put into optimising the size of the pipette tips, by iteratively changing puller settings and fire-polishing. (If the tips were too large, it was difficult to prevent the cell and bath solution from rushing far up the pipette. If they were too small, the cell stuck to the outside of the pipette tip.) After the cell was just inside the pipette, the outside of the pipette was inspected for any extraneous tissue that may have become attached. Such material was blown away with a second pipette containing extracellular solution. If the unwanted material could not be removed, the pipette containing the harvested cell was discarded. We found that it was important to maintain the positive pressure on the pipette until it touched the cell so as to prevent entry of bath solution into the pipette.
Reverse transcription
In order to release mRNA from the cell, the pipette containing the harvested-cell and 4 μl of ‘RNA-protecting solution’ (see above) was quickly positioned inside a thin-walled 0.2 ml PCR tube (sometimes a 0.5 ml tube) containing 2 μl of a solution that included the detergent Nonidet P40 (0.2%, Roche 11754599001, Mannheim, Germany), plus some reagents necessary for reverse transcription (10 mM Tris–HCl pH 8, Ambion Life Technologies, AM9855G, Paisley UK; 25 μM random hexamers, Roche 11034731001; 2.5 mM of each dNTP, Bioline 39025 or 39028, London, UK). The tip was broken by pushing it against the inside wall of the tube, and the pipette contents were expelled into the mixture by applying pressure (12 psi for 100 ms) via a filtered (0.2 μm) tube. This procedure was observed under a dissecting microscope. The tube was placed in a water bath at 65°C for 5 min and then on ice for 5 min. Additional reagents necessary for reverse transcription were then added. Their final concentrations in a total volume of 10 μl were 10 mM DTT (Clontech, comes with PowerScript reverse transcriptase), 2 U RNasin® or RNasin® Plus (Promega, N2515 or N2615), 1× first strand buffer (Clontech, accompanies PowerScript reverse transcriptase). (Taking into account the reagents present in the harvesting-pipette solution, the solution into which the cell was ejected and the added reagents, the final concentrations of the various components in a total volume of 10 μl were 20 mM DTT, 0.04% Nonidet P40, 5 μM random hexamers, 0.5 mM of each dNTP, 2 mM Tris–HCl, 17U Rnasin® or RNasin® Plus). Reverse transcriptase was then added (PowerScript, 0.5 or 0.75 μl, Clontech 639501, concentration undisclosed) and the tube was incubated at 42°C overnight in a thermal cycler (usually a PTC-200 DNA engine, Bio-Rad, Hemel Hempstead, UK). The reaction was stopped at 75°C for 15 minutes. The tube containing the single-cell cDNA was stored at −20°C.
Single-cell real-time quantitative PCR
For amplification of cDNA encoding a single type of GABAAR subunit in a single harvested granule cell, the mix containing single-cell cDNA was combined with a pair of forward and reverse primers (final concentrations, 400 nM, except for the reverse primer for the Gabrg2 gene, which was 300 nM) (OliGold® purified on a reverse-phase cartridge, Eurogentec, Southampton, UK), 25 μl of a SYBR-Green I mix (QuantiTect SYBR Green PCR kit, Qiagen 204143, Crawley, UK) and water to bring the final reaction volume up to 50 μl. The SYBR-Green I mix contained HotStarTaq DNA polymerase, PCR buffer (composed of Tris–HCl, KCl, (NH4)2SO4, MgCl2, pH8.7), a dNTP mix, SYBR Green I and the passive reference dye ROX (concentrations undisclosed by supplier except for MgCl2). The final concentration of MgCl2 was 2.5 mM. The forward and reverse primers were designed to be located in different exons and to amplify multiple splice variants of a gene of interest, as indicated in Table 1, with the aid of BLAST, Primer-BLAST and Beacon Designer software (version 4, PREMIER Biosoft, Palo Alto, CA, USA). To activate the HotStarTaq DNA polymerase, the tube was incubated on a Stratagene Mx3000P qPCR machine at 95°C for 15 min (Agilent Technologies, Stockport, UK). PCR was executed by cycling the temperature 50 times according to the sequence: 95°C, 15 s; 60 or 61°C, 60 s; 72°C 30 s. This was followed by generation of a melt curve between 55°C and 95°C to confirm the amplification of a single product and to check for the occurrence of primer dimers. cDNA encoding only one type of GABAAR subunit was measured per cell.
Table 1.
Gene | NCBI mRNA reference sequence(s) | Primers (5′-3′) | Position | Amplicon length (bp) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Gabra1 |
NM_010250.4 |
F: GAGCAGAAGTTGTCTATGAGTGG |
1349, exon 7 |
164 |
|
|
R: GTGGAAGTGAGTCGTCATAACC |
1512, exon 8 |
|
Gabra6 |
NM_001099641.1 |
F: CTACTCTGAAAATGTCAGTCGGATTC |
364, exon 2 |
123 |
(variant 1) |
R: CCAAAGCTGGTCACATAGATGTCT |
486, exon 3 |
|
|
NM_008068.2 |
|
364, exon 2 |
123 |
|
|
(variant 2) |
|
486, exon 3 |
|
Gabrb2 |
NM_008070.3 |
F: AGGGGCTACTTTGGGATTTGG |
446, exon 2 |
108 |
|
|
R: TCTGTCCACCGTCTCTTTAACC |
553, exon 3 |
|
Gabrb3 |
NM_008071.3 |
F: TTGCGGAGAAGACAGCCAAG |
1128, exon 9 |
100 |
(variant 1) |
R: TGAACATCCATCGGTGCTAATAGG |
1227, exon 10 |
|
|
NM_001038701.1 |
|
1060, exon 9 |
100 |
|
|
(variant 2) |
|
1159, exon 10 |
|
Gabrd |
NM_008072.2 |
F: GCCAGCATTGACCATATCTCAG |
324, exon 3 |
169 |
|
|
R: CATTCACGATGAAGGTGTCAGG |
492, exon 4 |
|
Gabrg2 |
NM_008073.2 |
F: CTTACATTCCCTGCACACTCATC |
1181, exon 7 |
140 |
(variant 1) |
R: AGATTTTCTGGCTATGGTGCTT |
1320, exon 8 |
|
|
NM_177408.5 |
|
822, exon 7 |
140 |
|
(variant 2) | 961, exon 8 |
The PCR reaction volume was relatively large (50 μl) so as to dilute components of the reverse-transcription mixture and thus minimise any inhibition they might have on the efficiency of the PCR assay. Reactions were carried out in strips or plates of 0.2 ml tubes sealed with optically-clear caps (Stratagene 410088; 401425; Fisher Scientific FB68750, Loughborough, UK). In order to minimise variation and allow comparison between wild-type and Ts65Dn cells, PCR of cDNA encoding a single type of GABAAR subunit was carried out simultaneously on cells from both wild-type and Ts65Dn mice. For each assay on a batch of cells, the PCR reaction and melt analysis were also concurrently carried out on a non-template control (containing water instead of a cell sample) and on a serial-dilution of known numbers of PCR products in duplicate (9 concentrations expressed as a number of single-stranded copies, spanning ~3.5 orders of magnitude), from which a gene-specific standard curve was generated (Table 2). The lowest points on the curves were 4, 6, 9 or 15 single-stranded copies. Copy numbers of GABAAR subunit transcripts in each cell were determined as the number of single-stranded copies relative to the regression lines fitted to the standard curves. Detection of a GABAAR subunit cDNA in a cell was defined as an estimated copy number of more than 2.
Table 2.
Gene | Slope | Y-intercept | Efficiency (%) | Regression | Cq of no-template controls |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gabra1 |
−3.21 |
41.22 |
105 |
0.995 |
48.9 (primer dimer) |
(Cq for the lowest unknown concentration was 38.3) | |||||
−3.42 |
41.72 |
96 |
0.993 |
No signal |
|
−3.19 |
41.57 |
106 |
0.999 |
No signal |
|
|
−3.30 |
40.65 |
101 |
0.998 |
No signal |
Gabra6 |
−3.39 |
41.55 |
97 |
0.995 |
No signal |
−3.48 |
41.59 |
94 |
0.987 |
No signal |
|
−3.45 |
41.91 |
95 |
0.997 |
No signal |
|
|
−3.39 |
41.52 |
97 |
0.999 |
No signal |
Gabrb2 |
−3.22 |
39.62 |
104 |
0.998 |
No signal |
|
−3.35 |
39.35 |
99 |
0.991 |
No signal |
Gabrb3 |
−3.44 |
40.22 |
95 |
0.999 |
No signal |
|
−3.37 |
40.99 |
98 |
0.999 |
No signal |
Gabrd |
−3.11 |
40.68 |
110 |
0.998 |
No signal |
−3.18 |
40.44 |
106 |
0.999 |
No signal |
|
−3.33 |
39.49 |
100 |
0.996 |
No signal |
|
|
−3.33 |
39.66 |
100 |
0.999 |
No signal |
Gabrg2 |
−3.28 |
39.03 |
102 |
0.999 |
No signal |
−3.34 | 39.70 | 99 | 0.996 | No signal |
Standard curves for quantification of single-cell copy numbers
For construction of standard curves that were run in parallel with cell samples, PCR products were serially-diluted in 1 mM Tris–HCl pH 8 and 1.25 ng/μl sonicated DNA from salmon testes (included as a DNA carrier, incubated before use at 95°C for 10 min to inactivate potential DNases, Sigma D9156). Each reaction was carried out in duplicate in a volume of 50 μl, containing the same concentrations of primers and SYBR-Green mix as the reactions on single-cell cDNA. So as to mimic the environment in which single-cell cDNA was amplified, the reaction mix was supplemented with reagents used during cell harvesting and reverse transcription (2 μl of the nucleotide/detergent solution (final concentration: 0.4 mM Tris–HCl, 0.1 mM each dNTP, 0.008% Nonidet P40), 2 μl of 5× first strand buffer (final concentration: 0.2×) and 4 mM DTT). A constant threshold fluorescence value was selected from the exponential phase of PCR and the cycle number at this threshold (the quantification cycle, Cq) was plotted against the log of the number of copies in each serial dilution. The parameters of the standard curves are shown in Table 2.
Generation of PCR products for standard curves
PCR products used to generate gene-specific standard curves that were run alongside cell-samples, were made by conventional PCR of cDNA that we reverse-transcribed from RNA isolated from wild-type mouse cerebellar vermis (see below), using the forward and reverse primers listed in Table 1. PCR reactions were carried out in a volume of 50 μl, which contained 0.5 U Biotaq Red DNA polymerase (Bioline 21038), NH4 buffer (composed of 67 mM Tris–HCl (pH 8.7), 16 mM (NH4)2SO4, 10 mM KCl), 2 mM MgCl2 (accompanies Biotaq), 0.2 mM each dNTP; 200 nM each primer (Eurogentec); 625 pg or 10 ng mouse cerebellar cDNA. The generation of a single product of expected size was verified by agarose gel electrophoresis. Each amplicon was extracted from an agarose gel (Qiaex II, Qiagen 20021) and the concentration of the product recovered was quantified by running an aliquot on an agarose gel, measuring the intensity of the band (Kodak EDAS 290 imaging system, Carestream, CT) and comparing this intensity to the intensities of quantitative ladders run on the same gel (EZ Load precision Molecular Mass ruler, Bio-Rad 170–8356, Hemel Hempstead, UK). Concentrations were converted to numbers of moles (Promega BioMath calculator; http://www.promega.com/biomath/Default.htm) and then to numbers of double-stranded DNA molecules using the Avogadro constant. Quantified PCR products were stored at −20°C at two concentrations (108 and 106 double-stranded copies/μl) in low-binding 0.5 ml tubes, precoated with 0.1 mg/ml BSA (New England BioLabs, B9001S) in 10 mM Tris–HCl (pH 8, Ambion, Life Techonolgies, Paisley, UK) to prevent binding of the amplicons to the tubes.
Cerebellar cDNA and initial optimisation of real-time PCR
Prior to amplification of single-cell cDNA with SYBR-Green I, reaction conditions for real-time PCR were optimised by amplification of adult mouse cerebellar cDNA that was made and quantified using standard protocols. Briefly, total RNA was extracted from cerebellar vermis (frozen in liquid nitrogen immediately upon dissection and stored at −80°C) of wild-type mice using TRIzol® Reagent (Invitrogen 15596–026, Life Technologies, Paisley UK), quantified with the Quant-iT RNA Assay Kit (Invitrogen Q32852) on the Stratagene Mx3000P machine and stored at −80°C. The integrity of the RNA was assessed on a denaturing formaldehyde agarose gel. Samples of RNA (1 μg) were treated with DNase I (Ambion AM1906) and reverse-transcribed at 42°C with oligodT primers (Ambion AM5730G) and Powerscript reverse transcriptase (Clontech, as above). The cDNA was purified, concentrated (QIAEX II resin, Qiagen) and quantified at 260 nm (GeneQuant RNA/DNA calculator (Amersham Biosciences, Little Chalfont, UK). PCR reactions (SYBR-Green I) were run in triplicate on 5-fold serial dilutions of cerebellar cDNA covering a range of 128 – 80000 double-stranded copies. The specificity of the PCR products was verified with melting curve analysis and agarose gel electrophoresis. Primer concentrations and the annealing temperature were adjusted until the PCR efficiency was above 90% and the coefficient of variation of the replicate Cq values was below 1. The same conditions were then applied to more dilute serial dilutions of PCR products which were expected to correspond more closely to numbers of transcripts in individual cells (9 dilutions spanning ~3.5 orders of magnitude, the lowest points on the curves were 4, 6, 9 or 15 single-stranded copies). The conditions were altered as necessary, before being used in single-cell PCR (see above and Table 2).
Statistical tests
Statistical tests on electrophysiological data were performed using Origin (v. 6 or 7), GraphPad Prism (v.4, La Jolla, CA) or PASW Statistics (v. 18, IBM SPSS, Portsmouth, UK) and considered significant at p < 0.05. PCR data were analysed with MxPro (v.3, Stratagene), Origin, GraphPad Prism, Microsoft Excel and PASW. Differences were examined using the Student’s paired or unpaired t test (when normally distributed as assessed by the Shapiro-Wilk test) or the Mann–Whitney U test (when data were not normally distributed). Slopes of fitted linear regression lines were compared using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). The chi-squared test and Fisher’s exact tests were used to compare the frequency of detection of cDNAs in individual cells. Kruskal Wallis non-parametric ANOVA was used to compare the numbers of different cDNAs. Data are summarized as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) or median and quartile values (in parentheses), with n denoting numbers of cells.
Abbreviations
AP: Action potential; DS: Down’s syndrome; GC: Granule cell; GABAAR: GABA type A receptor; P: Postnatal; THIP: 4,5,6,7-tetrahydroisoxazolo [4,5-c] pyridine-3-ol.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors’ contributions
MS designed and optimised primers and real-time PCR assays, performed single-cell reverse transcription and real-time PCR and analysed the results. RD optimised real-time PCR assays, carried out single-cell reverse transcription and real-time PCR and analysed the results. CG established and supervised genotyping by real-time PCR and helped draft the manuscript. MU conceived the study, made the electrophysiological recordings, harvested cells, analysed electrophysiological and PCR data and wrote the manuscript. All authors critically revised the manuscript and approved the final version.
Contributor Information
Marianna Szemes, Email: M.Szemes@bris.ac.uk.
Rachel L Davies, Email: Rachel.Davies@bris.ac.uk.
Claire LP Garden, Email: C.Garden@napier.ac.uk.
Maria M Usowicz, Email: M.M.Usowicz@bris.ac.uk.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Ewan Basterfield and other members of the Bristol University Animals Services Unit for breeding and care of the animals. We thank John Crabtree and Jon Brown for helpful comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust [075235/Z/04/Z to MMU]. MMU was in receipt of a Wellcome Trust Research Leave Award.
References
- Khoshnood B, Greenlees R, Loane M, Dolk H. on behalf of the EUROCAT Project Management Committee. Paper 2: EUROCAT public health indicators for congenital anomalies in Europe. Birth Defects Res A Clin Mol Teratol. 2011;91:S16–S22. doi: 10.1002/bdra.20776. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sturgeon X, Le T, Ahmed MM, Gardiner KJ. Pathways to cognitive deficits in Down syndrome. Prog Brain Res. 2012;197:73–100. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-54299-1.00005-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Letourneau A, Antonarakis SE. Genomic determinants in the phenotypic variability of Down syndrome. Prog Brain Res. 2012;197:15–28. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-54299-1.00002-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lott IT. Neurological phenotypes for Down syndrome across the life span. Prog Brain Res. 2012;197:101–121. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-54299-1.00006-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edgin JO, Mason GM, Spano G, Fernandez A, Nadel L. Human and mouse model cognitive phenotypes in Down syndrome: implications for assessment. Prog Brain Res. 2012;197:123–151. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-54299-1.00007-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilardell M, Rasche A, Thormann A, Maschke-Dutz E, Perez-Jurado LA, Lehrach H, Herwig R. Meta-analysis of heterogeneous Down syndrome data reveals consistent genome-wide dosage effects related to neurological processes. BMC Genomics. 2011;12:229. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-12-229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gotti S, Caricati E, Panzica G. Alterations of brain circuits in Down syndrome murine models. J Chem Neuroanat. 2011;42:317–326. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleschevnikov AM, Belichenko PV, Villar AJ, Epstein CJ, Malenka RC, Mobley WC. Hippocampal long-term potentiation suppressed by increased inhibition in the Ts65Dn mouse, a genetic model of Down syndrome. J Neurosci. 2004;24:8153–8160. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1766-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siarey RJ, Stoll J, Rapoport SI, Galdzicki Z. Altered long-term potentiation in the young and old Ts65Dn mouse, a model for down syndrome. Neuropharmacology. 1997;36:1549–1554. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(97)00157-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siarey RJ, Carlson EJ, Epstein CJ, Balbo A, Rapoport SI, Galdzicki Z. Increased synaptic depression in the Ts65Dn mouse, a model for mental retardation in Down syndrome. Neuropharmacology. 1999;38:1917–1920. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(99)00083-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez F, Morishita W, Zuniga E, Nguyen J, Blank M, Malenka RC, Garner CC. Pharmacotherapy for cognitive impairment in a mouse model of Down syndrome. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:411–413. doi: 10.1038/nn1860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa ACS, Grybko MJ. Deficits in hippocampal CA1 LTP induced by TBS but not HFS in the Ts65Dn mouse: a model of Down syndrome. Neurosci Lett. 2005;382:317–322. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.03.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Best TK, Cramer NP, Chakrabarti L, Haydar TF, Galdzicki Z. Dysfunctional hippocampal inhibition in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Exp Neurol. 2012;233:749–757. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.11.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleschevnikov AM, Belichenko PV, Gall J, George L, Nosheny R, Maloney MT, Salehi A, Mobley WC. Increased efficiency of the GABAA and GABAB receptor-mediated neurotransmission in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Neurobiol Dis. 2012;45:683–691. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleschevnikov AM, Belichenko PV, Faizi M, Jacobs LF, Htun K, Shamloo M, Mobley WC. Deficits in cognition and synaptic plasticity in a mouse model of Down syndrome ameliorated by GABAB receptor antagonists. J Neurosci. 2012;32:9217–9227. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1673-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleschevnikov AM, Belichenko PV, Salehi A, Wu C. Discoveries in Down syndrome: moving basic science to clinical care. Prog Brain Res. 2012;197:199–221. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-54299-1.00010-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rueda N, Florez J, Martinez-Cue C. Mouse models of Down syndrome as a tool to unravel the causes of mental disabilities. Neural Plast. 2012;2012:584071. doi: 10.1155/2012/584071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chakrabarti L, Best TK, Cramer NP, Carney RSE, Isaac JTR, Galdzicki Z, Haydar TF. Olig1 and Olig2 triplication causes developmental brain defects in Down syndrome. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:927–934. doi: 10.1038/nn.2600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cramer N, Galdzicki Z. From abnormal hippocampal synaptic plasticity in down syndrome mouse models to cognitive disability in down syndrome. Neural Plast. 2012;2012:101542. doi: 10.1155/2012/101542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galante M, Jani H, Vanes L, Daniel H, Fisher EMC, Tybulewicz VLJ, Bliss TVP, Morice E. Impairments in motor coordination without major changes in cerebellar plasticity in the Tc1 mouse model of Down syndrome. Hum Mol Genet. 2009;18:1449–1463. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Usowicz MM, Garden CLP. Increased excitability and altered action potential waveform in cerebellar granule neurons of the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Brain Res. 2012;1465:10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.05.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raz N, Torres IJ, Briggs SD, Spencer WD, Thornton AE, Loken WJ, Gunning FM, McQuain JD, Driesen NR, Acker JD. Selective neuroanatomic abnormalities in Down's syndrome and their cognitive correlates: evidence from MRI morphometry. Neurol. 1995;45:356–366. doi: 10.1212/WNL.45.2.356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aylward EH, Habbak R, Warren AC, Pulsifer MB, Barta PE, Jerram M, Pearlson GD. Cerebellar volume in adults with Down syndrome. Arch Neurol. 1997;54:209–212. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1997.00550140077016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jernigan TL, Bellugi U. Anomalous brain morphology on magnetic resonance images in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Arch Neurol. 1990;47:529–533. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1990.00530050049011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinter JD, Eliez S, Schmitt JE, Capone GT, Reiss AL. Neuroanatomy of Down's syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1659–1665. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baxter LL, Moran TH, Richtsmeier JT, Troncoso J, Reeves RH. Discovery and genetic localization of Down syndrome cerebellar phenotypes using the Ts65Dn mouse. Hum Mol Genet. 2000;9:195–202. doi: 10.1093/hmg/9.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guidi S, Ciani E, Bonasoni P, Santini D, Bartesaghi R. Widespread proliferation impairment and hypocellularity in the cerebellum of fetuses with Down syndrome. Brain Pathol. 2011;21:361–373. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2010.00459.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haydar TF, Reeves RH. Trisomy 21 and early brain development. Trends Neurosci. 2011;35:81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography in the human cerebellum: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage. 2009;44:489–501. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoodley C. The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum. 2011;11:1–14. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arenz A, Bracey EF, Margrie TW. Sensory representations in cerebellar granule cells. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009;19:445–451. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wall MJ, Usowicz MM. Development of action potential-dependent and independent spontaneous GABAA receptor-mediated currents in granule cells of postnatal rat cerebellum. Eur J Neurosci. 1997;9:533–548. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01630.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brickley SG, Cull-Candy SG, Farrant M. Development of a tonic form of synaptic inhibition in rat cerebellar granule cells resulting from persistent activation of GABAA receptors. J Physiol. 1996;497:753–759. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson BX, Elster L, Schousboe A. Pharmacological and functional implications of developmentally-regulated changes in GABAA receptor subunit expression in the cerebellum. Eur J Pharmacol. 1998;352:1–14. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(98)00355-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brickley SG, Revilla V, Cull-Candy SG, Wisden W, Farrant M. Adaptive regulation of neuronal excitability by a voltage-independent potassium conductance. Nature. 2001;409:88–92. doi: 10.1038/35051086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaneda M, Farrant M, Cull-Candy SG. Whole-cell and single-channel currents activated by GABA and glycine in granule cells of the rat cerebellum. J Physiol. 1995;485:419–435. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamann M, Rossi DJ, Attwell D. Tonic and spillover inhibition of granule cells control information flow through cerebellar cortex. Neuron. 2002;33:625–633. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00593-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Das I, Reeves RH. The use of mouse models to understand and improve cognitive deficits in Down syndrome. Dis Model Mech. 2011;4:596–606. doi: 10.1242/dmm.007716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Necchi D, Lomoio S, Scherini E. Axonal abnormalities in cerebellar Purkinje cells of the Ts65Dn mouse. Brain Res. 2008;1238:181–188. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herault Y, Duchon A, Velot E, Marechal D, Brault V. The in vivo Down syndrome genomic library in mouse. Prog Brain Res. 2012;197:169–197. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-54299-1.00009-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moldrich RX, Dauphinot L, Laffaire J, Rossier J, Potier MC. Down syndrome gene dosage imbalance on cerebellum development. Prog Neurobiol. 2007;82:87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saran NG, Pletcher MT, Natale JE, Cheng Y, Reeves RH. Global disruption of the cerebellar transcriptome in a Down syndrome mouse model. Hum Mol Genet. 2003;12:2013–2019. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddg217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santhakumar V, Hanchar HJ, Wallner M, Olsen RW, Otis TS. Contributions of the GABAA receptor α6 subunit to phasic and tonic inhibition revealed by a naturally occurring polymorphism in the a6 gene. J Neurosci. 2006;26:3357–3364. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4799-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duguid I, Branco T, London M, Chadderton P, Haüsser M. Tonic inhibition enhances fidelity of sensory information transmission in the cerebellar cortex. J Neurosci. 2012;32:11132–11143. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0460-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinemann SH. In: Single-channel Recording. 2. Sakmann B, Neher E, editor. Springer; 2009. Guide to data acquisition and analysis; pp. 53–94. [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov I, Savtchenko LP, Kullmann DM, Semyanov A, Walker MC. Outwardly rectifying tonically active GABAA receptors in pyramidal cells modulate neuronal offset, not gain. J Neurosci. 2009;29:15341–15350. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2747-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mortensen M, Patel B, Smart TG. GABA potency at GABA(A) receptors found in synaptic and extrasynaptic Zones. Front Cell Neurosci. 2011;6:1. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2012.00001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brickley S, Mody I. Extrasynaptic GABAA receptors: their function in the CNS and implications for disease. Neuron. 2012;73:23–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meera P, Wallner M, Otis TS. Molecular basis for the high THIP/gaboxadol sensitivity of extrasynaptic GABAA receptors. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:2057–2064. doi: 10.1152/jn.00450.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korpi ER, Luddens H. Furosemide interactions with brain GABAA receptors. Br J Pharmacol. 1997;120:741–748. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0700922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korpi ER, Kuner T, Seeburg PH, Luddens H. Selective antagonist for the cerebellar granule cell-specific gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptor. Mol Pharmacol. 1995;47:283–289. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wafford KA, Thompson SA, Thomas D, Sikela J, Wilcox AS, Whiting PJ. Functional characterization of human gamma-aminobutyric acidA receptors containing the alpha 4 subunit. Mol Pharmacol. 1996;50:670–678. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hörtnagl H, Tasan RO, Wieselthaler A, Kirchmair E, Sieghart W, Sperk G. Patterns of mRNA and protein expression for 12 GABAA receptor subunits in the mouse brain. Neuroscience. 2013;236:347–372. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nusser Z, Roberts JD, Baude A, Richards JG, Somogyi P. Relative densities of synaptic and extrasynaptic GABAA receptors on cerebellar granule cells as determined by a quantitative immunogold method. J Neurosci. 1995;15:2948–2960. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-04-02948.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nusser Z, Sieghart W, Somogyi P. Segregation of different GABAA receptors to synaptic and extrasynaptic membranes of cerebellar granule cells. J Neurosci. 1998;18:1693–1703. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-05-01693.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stell BM, Brickley SG, Tang CY, Farrant M, Mody I. Neuroactive steroids reduce neuronal excitability by selectively enhancing tonic inhibition mediated by δ subunit-containing GABAA receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:14439–14444. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2435457100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pirker S, Schwarzer C, Wieselthaler A, Sieghart W, Sperk G. GABA(A) receptors: immunocytochemical distribution of 13 subunits in the adult rat brain. Neuroscience. 2000;101:815–850. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00442-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somogyi P, Fritschy JM, Benke D, Roberts JDB, Sieghart W. The γ2 Subunit of the GABAA receptor is concentrated in synaptic junctions containing the α1 and β2/3 subunits in hippocampus, cerebellum and globus pallidus. Neuropharmacology. 1996;35:1425–1444. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(96)00086-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bright DP, Renzi M, Bartram J, McGee TP, MacKenzie G, Hosie AM, Farrant M, Brickley SG. Profound desensitization by ambient GABA limits activation of delta-containing GABAA receptors during spillover. J Neurosci. 2011;31:753–763. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2996-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wall MJ. Competitive GABAA receptor antagonists increase the proportion of functional high-affinity α6 subunit-containing receptors in granule cells of adult rat cerebellum. Neuropharmacology. 2003;44:56–69. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(02)00331-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi DJ, Hamann M. Spillover-mediated transmission at inhibitory synapses promoted by high affinity alpha6 subunit GABA(A) receptors and glomerular geometry. Neuron. 1998;20:783–795. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81016-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tia S, Wang JF, Kotchabhakdi N, Vicini S. Developmental changes of inhibitory synaptic currents in cerebellar granule geurons: role of GABAA receptor α6 subunit. J Neurosci. 1996;16:3630–3640. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-11-03630.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durand G, Marandi N, Herberger S, Blum R, Konnerth A. Quantitative single-cell RT-PCR and Ca2+ imaging in brain slices. Pflugers Arch. 2006;451:716–726. doi: 10.1007/s00424-005-1514-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lionnet T, Singer RH. Transcription goes digital. EMBO Rep. 2012;13:313–321. doi: 10.1038/embor.2012.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seja P, Schonewille M, Spitzmaul G, Badura A, Klein I, Rudhard Y, Wisden W, Hubner CA, De Zeeuw CI, Jentsch TJ. Raising cytosolic Cl- in cerebellar granule cells affects their excitability and vestibulo-ocular learning. EMBO J. 2012;31:1217–1230. doi: 10.1038/emboj.2011.488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brickley SG, Farrant M, Swanson GT, Cull-Candy SG. CNQX increases GABA-mediated synaptic transmission in the cerebellum by an AMPA/kainate receptor-independent mechanism. Neuropharmacology. 2001;41:730–736. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(01)00135-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chadderton P, Margrie TW, Haüsser M. Integration of quanta in cerebellar granule cells during sensory processing. Nature. 2004;428:856–860. doi: 10.1038/nature02442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruiz A, Campanac E, Scott RS, Rusakov DA, Kullmann DM. Presynaptic GABAA receptors enhance transmission and LTP induction at hippocampal mossy fiber synapses. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:431–438. doi: 10.1038/nn.2512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cattaert D, El Manira A. Shunting versus inactivation: analysis of presynaptic inhibitory mechanisms in primary afferents of the crayfish. J Neurosci. 1999;19:6079–6089. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-14-06079.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rojas P, Akrouh A, Eisenman LN, Mennerick S. Differential effects of axon initial segment and somatodendritic GABAA receptors on excitability measures in rat dentate granule neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2011;105:366–379. doi: 10.1152/jn.00165.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- French AS, Panek I, Torkkeli PH. Shunting versus inactivation: Simulation of GABAergic inhibition in spider mechanoreceptors suggests that either is sufficient. Neurosci Res. 2006;55:189–196. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2006.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxena NC, Macdonald RL. Properties of putative cerebellar gamma-aminobutyric acid A receptor isoforms. Mol Pharmacol. 1996;49:567–579. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baur R, Kaur KH, Sigel E. Diversity of structure and function of alpha1alpha6beta3delta GABAA receptors: comparison with alpha1beta3delta and alpha6beta3delta receptors. J Biol Chem. 2010;285:17398–17405. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M110.108670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacob TC, Moss SJ, Jurd R. GABAA receptor trafficking and its role in the dynamic modulation of neuronal inhibition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:331–343. doi: 10.1038/nrn2370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittle N, Sartori SB, Dierssen M, Lubec G, Singewald N. Fetal Down syndrome brains exhibit aberrant levels of neurotransmitters critical for normal brain development. Pediatr. 2007;120:e1465–e1471. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidl R, Cairns N, Singewald N, Kaehler ST, Lubec G. Differences between GABA levels in Alzheimer's disease and Down syndrome with Alzheimer-like neuropathology. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol. 2001;363:139–145. doi: 10.1007/s002100000346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vink J, Incerti M, Toso L, Roberson R, Abebe D, Spong CY. Prenatal NAP + SAL prevents developmental delay in a mouse model of Down syndrome through effects on N-methyl-D-aspartic acid and g-aminobutyric acid receptors. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2009;200:524. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2009.01.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhattacharyya A, McMillan E, Chen SI, Wallace K, Svendsen CN. A critical period in cortical interneuron neurogenesis in Down syndrome revealed by human neural progenitor cells. Dev Neurosci. 2009;31:497–510. doi: 10.1159/000236899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belichenko PV, Kleschevnikov AM, Masliah E, Wu C, Takimoto-Kimura R, Salehi A, Mobley WC. Excitatory-inhibitory relationship in the fascia dentata in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. J Comp Neurol. 2009;512:453–466. doi: 10.1002/cne.21895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn DE, Nuovo GJ, Terry AV Jr, Martin MM, Malana GE, Sansom SE, Pleister AP, Beck WD, Head E, Feldman DS. et al. Chromosome 21-derived microRNAs provide an etiological basis for aberrant protein expression in human Down syndrome brains. J Biol Chem. 2010;285:1529–1543. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M109.033407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
- Keck-Wherley J, Grover D, Bhattacharyya S, Xu X, Holman D, Lombardini ED, Verma R, Biswas R, Galdzicki Z. Abnormal microRNA expression in Ts65Dn hippocampus and whole blood: contributions to Down syndrome phenotypes. Dev Neurosci. 2011;33:451–467. doi: 10.1159/000330884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elton TS, Sansom SE, Martin MM. Trisomy-21 gene dosage over-expression of miRNAs results in the haploinsufficiency of specific target proteins. RNA Biol. 2010;7:540–547. doi: 10.4161/rna.7.5.12685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinholdt L, Ding Y, Gilbert G, Czechanski A, Solzak J, Roper R, Johnson M, Donahue L, Lutz C, Davisson M. Molecular characterization of the translocation breakpoints in the Down syndrome mouse model Ts65Dn. Mamm Genome. 2011;22:685–691. doi: 10.1007/s00335-011-9357-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duchon A, Raveau M, Chevalier C, Nalesso V, Sharp A, Herault Y. Identification of the translocation breakpoints in the Ts65Dn and Ts1Cje mouse lines: relevance for modeling Down syndrome. Mamm Genome. 2011;22:674–684. doi: 10.1007/s00335-011-9356-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X, El Naqa IM. Prediction of both conserved and nonconserved microRNA targets in animals. Bioinformatics. 2008;24:325–332. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btm595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kozomara A, Griffiths-Jones S. miRBase: integrating microRNA annotation and deep-sequencing data. Nucl Acids Res. 2011;39:D152–D157. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkq1027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman RC, Farh KK-H, Burge CB, Bartel DP. Most mammalian mRNAs are conserved targets of microRNAs. Genome Res. 2009;19:92–105. doi: 10.1101/gr.082701.108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rueda N, Florez J, Martinez-Cue C. Chronic pentylenetetrazole but not donepezil treatment rescues spatial cognition in Ts65Dn mice, a model for Down syndrome. Neurosci Lett. 2008;433:22–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.12.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartesaghi R, Guidi S, Ciani E. Is it possible to improve neurodevelopmental abnormalities in Down syndrome? Rev Neurosci. 2011;22:419–455. doi: 10.1515/RNS.2011.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitra A, Blank M, Madison DV. Developmentally altered inhibition in Ts65Dn, a mouse model of Down syndrome. Brain Res. 2012;1440:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanson JE, Blank M, Valenzuela RA, Garner CC, Madison DV. The functional nature of synaptic circuitry is altered in area CA3 of the hippocampus in a mouse model of Down's syndrome. J Physiol (Lond) 2007;579:53–67. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.114868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harashima C, Jacobowitz DM, Witta J, Borke RC, Best TK, Siarey RJ, Galdzicki Z. Abnormal expression of the G-protein-activated inwardly rectifying potassium channel 2 (GIRK2) in hippocampus, frontal cortex, and substantia nigra of Ts65Dn mouse: a model of Down syndrome. J Comp Neurol. 2006;494:815–833. doi: 10.1002/cne.20844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harashima C, Jacobowitz D, Stoffel M, Chakrabarti L, Haydar T, Siarey R, Galdzicki Z. Elevated expression of the G-protein-activated inwardly rectifying potassium channel 2 (GIRK2) in cerebellar unipolar brush cells of a Down Syndrome mouse model. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2006;26:1–16. doi: 10.1007/s10571-006-8565-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nusser Z, Mody I. Selective modulation of tonic and phasic inhibitions in dentate gyrus granule cells. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:2624–2628. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.87.5.2624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semyanov A, Walker MC, Kullmann DM. GABA uptake regulates cortical excitability via cell type-specific tonic inhibition. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:484–490. doi: 10.1038/nn1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glykys J, Mody I. Hippocampal network hyperactivity after selective reduction of tonic inhibition in GABAA receptor α5 subunit deficient mice. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:2796–2807. doi: 10.1152/jn.01122.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caraiscos VB, Elliott EM, You-Ten KE, Cheng VY, Belelli D, Newell JG, Jackson MF, Lambert JJ, Rosahl TW, Wafford KA. et al. Tonic inhibition in mouse hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons is mediated by α5 subunit-containing γ-aminobutyric acid type A receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:3662–3667. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0307231101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braudeau J, Delatour B, Duchon A, Lopes-Pereira P, Dauphinot L, de Chaumont F, Olivo-Marin JC, Dodd RH, Hérault Y, Potier MC. Specific targeting of the GABA-A receptor α5 subtype by a selective inverse agonist restores cognitive deficits in Down syndrome mice. J Psychopharmacol. 2011;25:1030–1042. doi: 10.1177/0269881111405366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martínez-Cué C, Martínez P, Rueda N, Vidal R, García S, Vidal V, Corrales A, Montero JA, Pazos Á, Flórez J. et al. Reducing GABAA α5 receptor-mediated inhibition rescues functional and neuromorphological deficits in a mouse model of Down syndrome. J Neurosci. 2013;33:3953–3966. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1203-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braudeau J, Dauphinot L, Duchon A, Loistron A, Dodd RH, Herault Y, Delatour B, Potier MC. Chronic treatment with a promnesiant GABA-A alpha5-selective inverse agonist increases immediate early genes expression during memory processing in mice and rectifies their expression levels in a Down syndrome mouse model. Adv Pharmacol Sci. 2011;2011:153218. doi: 10.1155/2011/153218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juusola M, Robinson HP, de Polavieja GG. Coding with spike shapes and graded potentials in cortical networks. Bioessays. 2007;29:178–187. doi: 10.1002/bies.20532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes EF, Roberts J, Mirrett P, Sideris J, Misenheimer J. A comparison of oral structure and oral-motor function in young males with Fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49:903–917. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/065). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ackermann H. Cerebellar contributions to speech production and speech perception: psycholinguistic and neurobiological perspectives. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:265–272. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scattoni ML, Crawley J, Ricceri L. Ultrasonic vocalizations: a tool for behavioural phenotyping of mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:508–515. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtzman DM, Santucci D, Kilbridge J, Chua-Couzens J, Fontana DJ, Daniels SE, Johnson RM, Chen K, Sun Y, Carlson E. et al. Developmental abnormalities and age-related neurodegeneration in a mouse model of Down syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:13333–13338. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.23.13333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa AC, Walsh K, Davisson MT. Motor dysfunction in a mouse model for Down syndrome. Physiol Behav. 1999;68:211–220. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00178-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kida E, Rabe A, Walus M, Albertini G, Golabek AA. Long-term running alleviates some behavioral and molecular abnormalities in Down syndrome mouse model Ts65Dn. Exp Neurol. 2013;240:178–189. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner CA, Presti MF, Newman HA, Bugenhagen P, Crnic L, Lewis MH. Spontaneous stereotypy in an animal model of Down syndrome: Ts65Dn mice. Behav Genet. 2001;31:393–400. doi: 10.1023/A:1012226603255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampton TG, Stasko MR, Kale A, Amende I, Costa AC. Gait dynamics in trisomic mice: quantitative neurological traits of Down syndrome. Physiol Behav. 2004;82:381–389. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vidal V, García S, Martínez P, Corrales A, Flórez J, Rueda N, Sharma A, Martínez-Cué C. Lack of behavioral and cognitive effects of chronic ethosuximide and gabapentin treatment in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Neuroscience. 2012;220:158–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.06.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faizi M, Bader PL, Tun C, Encarnacion A, Kleschevnikov A, Belichenko P, Saw N, Priestley M, Tsien RW, Mobley WC. et al. Comprehensive behavioral phenotyping of Ts65Dn mouse model of Down Syndrome: activation of b1-adrenergic receptor by xamoterol as a potential cognitive enhancer. Neurobiol Dis. 2011;43:397–413. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyde LA, Crnic LS, Pollock A, Bickford PC. Motor learning in Ts65Dn mice, a model for Down syndrome. Dev Psychobiol. 2001;38:33–45. doi: 10.1002/1098-2302(2001)38:1<33::AID-DEV3>3.0.CO;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein SL, Kriegsfeld LJ, Hairston JE, Rau V, Nelson RJ, Yarowsky PJ. Characterization of sensorimotor performance, reproductive and aggressive behaviors in segmental trisomic 16 (Ts65Dn) mice. Physiol Behav. 1996;60:1159–1164. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(96)00218-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Escorihuela RM, Fernandez-Teruel A, Vallina IF, Baamonde C, Lumbreras MA, Dierssen M, Tobena A, Florez J. A behavioral assessment of Ts65Dn mice: a putative Down syndrome model. Neurosci Lett. 1995;199:143–146. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)12052-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reeves RH, Irving NG, Moran TH, Wohn A, Kitt C, Sisodia SS, Schmidt C, Bronson RT, Davisson MT. A mouse model for Down syndrome exhibits learning and behaviour deficits. Nat Genet. 1995;11:177–184. doi: 10.1038/ng1095-177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Truett GE, Heeger P, Mynatt RL, Truett AA, Walker JA, Warman ML. Preparation of PCR-quality mouse genomic DNA with hot sodium hydroxide and tris (HotSHOT) Biotechniques. 2000;29:52–54. doi: 10.2144/00291bm09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu DP, Schmidt C, Billings T, Davisson MT. Quantitative PCR genotyping assay for the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Biotechniques. 2003;35:1170–1174. doi: 10.2144/03356st02. 1176, 1178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pugh JR, Jahr CE. Axonal GABAA receptors increase cerebellar granule cell excitability and synaptic activity. J Neurosci. 2011;31:565–574. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4506-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cathala L, Brickley S, Cull-Candy S, Farrant M. Maturation of EPSCs and intrinsic membrane properties enhances precision at a cerebellar synapse. J Neurosci. 2003;23:6074–6085. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-14-06074.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brickley SG, Aller MI, Sandu C, Veale EL, Alder FG, Sambi H, Mathie A, Wisden W. TASK-3 two-pore domain potassium channels enable sustained high-frequency firing in cerebellar granule neurons. J Neurosci. 2007;27:9329–9340. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1427-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osorio N, Cathala L, Meisler MH, Crest M, Magistretti J, Delmas P. Persistent Nav1.6 current at axon initial segments tunes spike timing of cerebellar granule cells. J Physiol. 2010;588:651–670. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.183798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Angelo E, De Filippi G, Rossi P, Taglietti V. Synaptic excitation of individual rat cerebellar granule cells in situ: evidence for the role of NMDA receptors. J Physiol. 1995;484:397–413. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glykys J, Mody I. The main source of ambient GABA responsible for tonic inhibition in the mouse hippocampus. J Physiol. 2007;582:1163–1178. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.134460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bean BP. The action potential in mammalian central neurons. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:451–465. doi: 10.1038/nrn2148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]