Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Aging. 2012 Jun 18;27(4):989–997. doi: 10.1037/a0028557

Intraindividual Variability in Psychometrically Defined Mild Cognitive Impairment Status in Older African Americans

Alyssa A Gamaldo 1, Jason C Allaire 2, Keith E Whitfield 3
PMCID: PMC3730493  NIHMSID: NIHMS493881  PMID: 22708537

Abstract

The current study examines day to day variability in psychometrically defined MCI status and potential predictors of changes in MCI status in an independent-living sample of urban dwelling older adults in Baltimore, Maryland. The participant sample consisted of 50 older adults ranging in age from 50 to 80 years. Participants completed health and cognitive measures (i.e. executive function, language, memory, and global cognition) over 8 occasions within a 2–3 week period. After each testing occasion, a post-hoc classification of MCI status was determined using psychometrically defined criteria based upon cognitive performance. Participants who classified as MCI after one assessment often did not meet MCI criteria at subsequent occasions. Daily fluctuations in sleep duration were associated with an increased risk for MCI classification. These results demonstrate that changes in sleep may explain changes in MCI status, particularly for African Americans.

Keywords: mild cognitive impairment, intraindividual variability, sleep, blood pressure


A diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), the transitional period between normal cognitive functioning and impaired cognitive functioning, is often unstable and changes over the course of several years (Larrieu et al., 2002; Ritchie et al., 2001). The unstable MCI diagnosis can vary depending on the diagnostic criteria used (Ganguli, Chang, Snitz, Saxton, Vanderbilt, and Lee, 2010). Specifically, MCI status is partially determined by objective cognitive assessments (Petersen, 2004), which tend to demonstrate day to day variability. For example, within-person daily fluctuations have been previously observed on psychometric and neuropsychological assessments of memory, reasoning, perceptual speed, executive function, and language (Gamaldo, Allaire, Sims, & Whitfield, 2010; Gamaldo, Weatherbee, & Allaire, 2008; Weatherbee, Gamaldo, & Allaire, 2009). Thus, this paper sought expand upon the previous literature suggesting that MCI classification varies across a macro-level time course (i.e. yearly) by examining whether an MCI classification would vary across a micro-level time course (i.e. daily). In addition, the current study will examine whether short-term changes in MCI classification may be dependent upon short-term changes in potential factors (i.e. blood pressure and sleep disturbance) contributing to health disparities in African Americans..

Studies have found that variability in cognitive performance is significantly related to other domains such as blood pressure (Gamaldo et al., 2008), vision (Weatherbee et al., 2009), stress (Neupert, Almeida, Mroczek, & Sprio, 2006), emotion, and physical functioning (Strauss, MacDonald, Hunter, Moll, & Hultsch, 2002). For example, individuals with pre-hypertension appear to exhibit worse reasoning performance, especially when their systolic blood pressure (SBP) fluctuated above their mean SBP (Gamaldo et al., 2008). Given the covariation between the intraindividual variability of non-cognitive and cognitive domains, intraindividual variability of non-cognitive domains may also be associated with intraindividual variability in MCI status.

Complaints of sleep problems have shown to be associated with cognitive functioning in older adults (Gamaldo, Allaire, & Whitfield, 2008; Nebes, Buysse, Halligan, Houck, & Monk, 2009). Self-reported complaints of trouble falling asleep have shown to be associated with worse short-term and working memory, which is often used to determine MCI status (Gamaldo et al., 2008). Sleep disturbances have also been shown to be associated with MCI status (Gauthier & Touchon, 2005). MCI cases appear to have a higher frequency of disturbances in sleep than other symptoms (i.e. irritability and depression; Lyketsos, Lopez, Jones, Fitzpatrick, Breitner, & DeKosky, 2002). Thus, an older adult might have a bad night of sleep prior to a cognitive assessment and score poorly due to the lack of sleep, which, in turn, may cause that individual to be classified as MCI.

Within African Americans, elevated blood pressure and sleep disturbances appear to be common health issues (Durrence & Lichstein, 2006; Sundquist, Winkleby, & Pudaric, 2001), and both appear to be associated with worse cognition (Gamaldo et al., 2008; Whitfield, Allaire, Aiken-Morgan, Gamaldo, Sims, & Edwards, 2008). The current study is particularly interested in exploring whether these factors may explain fluctuations in MCI status.

Although the current study is not designed to assess whether variability in cognitive performance is a precursor to cognitive impairment, this study attempts to assess whether changes in MCI status, using an ad hoc approach to MCI classification, might be a proxy for fluctuations on cognitive variables.

The current study had three primary aims. First, determine whether there is variability or consistency in MCI classification across testing occasions. Second, examine whether blood pressure was a predictor of MCI status across the testing occasions. Third, examine whether sleep was a predictor of MCI status across the testing occasions.

Method

Participants

The current study was a supplemental project of the Baltimore Study of Black Aging: Patterns of Cognitive Aging (BSBA: PCA; NIA#24108), which was designed to examine cognition, health, and other critical factors in older African Americans (Whitfield, Baker-Thomas, Heyward, Gatto, & Williams, 1999). The current study recruited 50 community-dwelling African Americans 50 years of age or older. Participants were recruited from the senior housing facilities in Baltimore, Maryland.

Demographics and Health Measures

The current analyses included the demographic variables age, education, and gender. Participants were asked to report whether they had been diagnosed with any of a series of specified illnesses or diseases. A composite score, labeled cardiovascular risk factors (CVRFS), was estimated by summing the self-report of diabetes, cardiovascular disease, high blood pressure, stroke, heart attack, angina, and circulation problems. The CVRFS variable had scores that ranged from 0 (no risk factors) to 7 or more risk factors. A second composite score, labeled other comorbid health illnesses (Non-CVRFS), summed the report of health illnesses that included arthritis, broken hip, asthma, gout, gallbladder problems, stomach ulcers, thyroid trouble, tuberculosis, kidney trouble, and cancer. Non-CVRFS had scores that ranged from 0 (no comorbid health illnesses) to 10 (high comorbid health illnesses). Both the CVRFS and Non-CVRFS variables were included in the analyses as covariates.

The Elderly Life Stress Inventory (ELSI; Aldwin, 1990) was used to assess self-reported stress at baseline testing. This questionnaire asked participants to indicate on a scale of 0 (did not occur) to 5 (extremely stressful) the extent to which they have experienced a number of different stressful events (i.e. death of spouse, major personal injury, and retirement) during the past year.

The Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression (CES-D) scale was used to measure depressive symptoms at the baseline testing session (Radloff, 1977). The CES-D is commonly used in detecting depressive symptoms in older adults across diverse populations (Foley, Reed, Mutran, & DeVellis, 2002). The total CES-D score was included in the analyses.

The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman, & Kupfer, 1988) was used to assess participants’ typical sleep habits within the last month at the baseline testing session. Questions were designed to assess seven components, including sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, sleep medications, and daytime dysfunction due to sleep habits. Global sleep quality scores can range from 0 (good sleeper) to 21 (poor sleeper). In addition to including the global sleep quality score in the analyses, the analyses included the item regarding the use of sleep medications.

At baseline and each follow-up testing session, participants were asked the number of hours slept the previous night at each testing session. Responses for this question were categorized as sleep duration.

Systolic and diastolic blood pressures were monitored at the end of each testing occasion using an oscillometric automated device (A & D model UA-767; Milpitas California). Following a five minute rest period, a trained research assistant placed a blood pressure cuff of appropriate size on participants’ bare arms to record blood pressure while the participant was sitting. Three readings were taken to assure accuracy and consistency of the blood pressure measures. The average of these three readings was included in the current study’s analyses.

Cognitive Measurements

A large cognitive battery was administered to assess a broad range of cognitive abilities. Executive function was assessed using The Stroop Task (Trenerry et al., 1989) and the Clock Drawing Test (CDT), which measures a participant’s ability to draw a clock to a specified time (i.e. 10 after 11, 3:25, 10 after 9, 6:55, 10 after 6, 1:45, 5 after 4, and 9:40). Clocks were scored based on a 10-point scoring system (Manos & Wu, 1994; Shulman et al., 1986). Decalarative memory was assessed using the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (AVLT; Rey, 1941). Perceptual speed was assessed using the Number Comparison test (Ekstrom et al., 1976). Inductive reasoning was assessed using the Letter Series test (Thurstone, 1962), a measure of a participant’s ability to identify novel relationships in over learned material. Constructional praxis or visual-motor integration was assessed by asking participants to copy two constructions (i.e. circle, cube, diamond, two interlocking rectangles, triangle, circle touching a diamond, sideways diamond, and/or interlocking large and small squares (Beery, Buktenica, & Beery, 1997). Language was assessed using the Boston Naming Task (BNT; Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983). Finally, Verbal Fluency (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983) was also used to assess language as well as semantic memory and executive function.

Procedure

Participants met with a research assistant for eight testing sessions over a 2–3 week period. At baseline testing, participants were randomly assigned a testing booklet associated with specific color, which consisted of the assessments as described above. At follow-up assessments (i.e. the daily sessions), participants were administered a battery of measures similar to the baseline assessment. To reduce practice effects, alternate versions were created for a majority of the measures. Testing booklets included eight alternate versions of the AVLT, Number Comparison, Letter Series, and CDT as well as four alternate versions of BNT, Letter Fluency, and Semantic Fluency. The order of each measure’s alternate versions varied across the participants and occasions. The color of the testing booklet assigned at baseline testing for each participant assisted in identifying the specific order that the alternate versions for each measure were administered across the entire testing sessions. Alternate versions of the Stroop task were not included in the testing booklets. Given preliminary analyses revealed some significant differences in performance across the alternate measure versions, each measure’s version was standardized to reduce the fluctuation from occasion to occasion. Initially, a mean and standard deviation was calculated for each of the measurement versions across occasions and participants. Using the version’s mean and standard deviation, each version was then standardized to a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10. Furthermore, the testing booklet color assigned at baseline was included as a covariate in the analyses.

Initial testing session lasted approximately 2 – 2 ½ hours, but subsequent testing sessions lasted roughly 1 ½ – 2 hours due to the administration of a shorter assessment battery. Participants were compensated a total of $120 for the completion of all 8 sessions within the two-week time period.

MCI status was psychometrically determined using a post-hoc approach in which a computer algorithm was conducted to identify mildly impaired individuals based upon their collected cognitive data at each testing assessment. This procedure has been used in several previous studies including non-clinical, community-based samples (Gamaldo et al., 2010; Manly, Tang, Schupf, Stern, Vonsattel, & Mayeux, 2008; Larrieu, Letenneur, Orgogozo, Fabrigoule, Amieva, Carret et al., 2002; Wadley, Crowe, Marsiske, Cook, Unverzagt, Rosenberg, & Rexroth, 2007). Our computer algorithm defined MCI status based upon a modified version of the age-associated cognitive decline criteria (Ritchie, Artero, & Touchon, 2001), which classifies individuals if they indicate impairment on at least one cognitive ability (i.e. language, memory, reasoning, executive functioning, constructional praxis, and perceptual speed). Unlike the original criteria, the modified version does not include a subjective memory complaint. As mentioned previously, all of the cognitive tests were standardized to a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10. The influence of age and education was residualized from each standardized cognitive score which were standardized (M = 50, SD = 10) again. Individuals were divided into two groups: “normal” and “MCI”. For the “normal” group, individuals had to score at or above 1.5 SD below the sample mean on all the cognitive measures. For the “MCI” group, individuals had to score 1.5 SD (i.e. the bottom 6.68% of the sample distribution) below the sample mean on any of the cognitive measures (Ritchie et al., 2001).

Brief Description of Logistic Multilevel Modeling

Logistic multi-level models (MLM) were run to examine the association between blood pressure/sleep duration and MCI. These models predicted the dichotomous dependent variable of MCI status. Two models were conducted. In the first model, the “coupling parameters” (level 1 predictor), between-person parameters (or level 2), linear time, quadratic time, and those covariates (i.e. days between first and final testing, gender, education, CES-D total, Non-CVRFS CVRFS, and stress) were included in the model to assess potential main effects. All covariates that did not have meaningful zero were grand-mean centered by the sample’s average score, so the intercept in the model could be interpreted. The coupling parameters (Occasion SBPij – Mean SBPi or Occasion Sleep Durationij – Mean Sleep Durationi; i range from 1 to 50; j range from 1 to 8) were group-mean centered by each individual’s average score across all 8 testing occasions. The quadratic effect of occasion SBP and sleep duration were included in the model by squaring the within-person variables.

The between-person parameters (i.e. covariates and level 2 predictors of interest) were grand-mean centered by the sample’s average score. For example, the models reflected the relationship between cognition and an individual’s mean SBP (see Appendix A) or mean sleep duration (see Appendix B) across the 8 occasions (γ01). The quadratic effect of mean SBP and sleep duration were also calculated and included in the model by squaring these between-person sleep variables. Additionally, the models included a level 2 predictor that reflected the relationship between cognition and individual’s average stress levels (γ02). Finally, the model with sleep duration included a level 2 predictor that reflected the relationship between cognition and average PSQI scores (γ03). The model with sleep duration also included a covariate of sleep medication use. Several interactions (i.e. a between-person interaction, two cross-level interactions, and a three-way cross-level interaction) were added to the initial model in a second model to assess potential moderating relationships.

Results

The current study enrolled 50 (39 women and 11 men) older African American adults ranging in age from 50 to 80 years (M = 65.40, SD = 8.53). Participants’ average monthly income was $950 (SD = $500; range = < $100 – >$2300) and the average years of education was 11.62 years (range = 6–18, SD = 2.38 years). Although 50 participants were tested at the baseline assessment, 3 participants withdrew (1 male; 2 females) before completing all 8 daily assessments. Participants’ average PSQI score was approximately 7 (SD = 4; Table 1) and on average, they reported approximately 6 hours of sleep (SD = 1.99). Based on a PSQI global score of 5 or more (Buysee, Reynolds, Monk, Berman, Kupfer, 1989), 52% of the sample would be considered poor sleepers. Participants, on average, typically report taking none or few sleep medications in the past month. On average, approximately two CVRFS (SD = 1.33) as well as two Non-CVRFS (SD = 1.03) were reported. Average systolic blood pressure was approximately 135.21 (SD = 26.95), which is considered pre-hypertensive (Chobanian et al., 2003). Mean CES-D scores were 11 (SD = 8.87), suggesting that the sample on average did not have high depressive symptoms. Mean stress scores were approximately 14 (SD = 12.25), which suggest that the sample on average did not report extremely high stress, and the scale distribution was positively skewed. A median split was performed on this scale to create two levels of stress: low stress (8 or less) and stressed (9 or more).

Table 1.

PSQI Global and Component Score Mean, Standard Deviation and Range

Variable Mean SD range
PSQI global score 6.50 4.14 0–18
PSQI components
 1. Subjective sleep quality 0.79 0.87 0–3
 2. Sleep latency 1.08 1.01 0–3
 3. Sleep duration 1.24 1.05 0–3
 4. Sleep efficiency 0.54 1.10 0–3
 5. Sleep disturbance 1.34 0.63 0–3
 6. Use of sleep medication 0.86 1.23 0–3
 7. Daytime dysfunction 0.76 0.82 0–2

Note.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index

Variability or Consistency in MCI Classification Across Testing Occasions

As illustrated in Table 2, 14 individuals (28%) met MCI criteria at baseline testing. The frequency of these cases remaining MCI tended to fluctuate over the occasions. At occasion 1, 9 out of the 14 baseline MCI cases (64%) were still considered MCI. As indicated in the parentheses, 2 out of the 14 baseline MCI cases (14%) were considered between MCI and normal. The remaining 3 out of the 14 (22%) baseline MCI cases were considered normal at the subsequent testing occasion. Six new cases of MCI were observed at occasion 1 (new cases of MCI at each occasion are illustrated in Table 2 in the rows following the baseline assessment), and, subsequently, 5 (83%) remain MCI at occasion 2. After the initial classification of MCI, 3 individuals consistently met MCI criteria at subsequent occasions. Two of these individuals were first identified as MCI at baseline, and one individual was first identified as MCI at occasion 1. One of these participants had eventually withdrawn from the study after occasion 5. In addition, 3 MCI cases at baseline and 1 new MCI case at occasion 1 often met the MCI criteria at subsequent occasions, but on 1–2 occasions performed between MCI and normal.

Table 2.

Frequency of MCI Participants (and Participants Between MCI and Normal cutoffs) at Each Occasion

MCI Status Occasions
Baseline 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Baseline 14 9 (2) 9 (4) 5 (5) 8 (4) 8 (5) 8 (4) 5 (5)
1 - 6 5 (0) 3 (1) 2 (2) 4 (0) 3 (2) 4 (0)
2 - - 5 1 (1) 1 (4) 1 (2) 3 (1) 3 (1)
3 - - - 2 0 (2) 0 (2) 0 (1) 0 (2)
4 - - - - 3 0 (1) 0 (1) 1 (0)
5 - - - - - 2 0 (1) 1 (0)
6 - - - - - - 2 1 (0)
7 - - - - - - - 1

Note: The number in the first cell of each row represents the frequency of new MCI cases at each session. Numbers in the subsequent cells within the same row represent the frequency of those MCI cases who continue to meet MCI criteria. The numbers in the parentheses represent those MCI cases that went above the −1.5 SD cutoff, but below the normal cutoff of −1 SD at a particular occasion.

Dynamic Relationship Between Blood Pressure/Sleep Duration and MCI Status

Occasion SBP and mean SBP were not significant predictors of MCI status, but occasion sleep duration was a significant predictor of MCI status across the testing occasions. Specifically, the quadratic effect of occasion sleep was a significant predictor of MCI status (see Table 3). On those occasions where an individual reported fewer hours of sleep than on average, he/she was more likely to meet criteria for MCI. Although the risk for being classified as MCI tended to decline on those occasions where an individual reported sleeping their average hours of sleep across the occasions, the risk for meeting MCI criteria appeared to slightly increase on those occasions where an individual reported sleeping considerably more than on average. Linear occasion sleep duration, linear mean sleep duration, and the quadratic effects of mean sleep duration were not significant predictors of MCI status. Males were significantly at greater risk for being classified as MCI across the occasions. Finally, a high number of cardiovascular risk factors were a significant risk for MCI classification. No significant interactions were observed in the second model.

Table 3.

Dynamic Relationship Between Sleep Duration and MCI Status

Step 1
Step 2
Coefficients Odds Ratio Coefficients Odds Ratio
Within-person
 Occ Sleep Duration 0.05 (0.10) 1.05 0.21 (0.21) 1.23
 Quadratic Occ Sleep Duration 0.10* (0.04) 1.10 0.09 (0.05) 1.10
 Time of Day 0.00 (0.01) 1.00 −0.00 (0.01) 1.00
Between-person
 Mean Sleep Duration −0.22 (0.24) 0.80 0.27 (0.51) 1.31
 Quadratic Mean Sleep Duration −0.14 (0.11) 0.87 −0.11 (0.15) 0.90
 PSQI 0.34 (0.84) 1.40 0.81 (1.29) 2.24
 Sleep Medication Use 0.22 (0.34) 0.51 0.24 (0.37) 1.27
 Days Between First & Last Test −0.14 (0.10) 0.87 −0.11 (0.12) 0.90
 Gender −2.14* (1.02) 0.12 −2.61* (1.24) 0.07
 Education −0.23 (0.15) 0.80 −0.18 (0.17) 0.83
 CVRFS 0.76** (0.28) 2.13 0.76* (0.32) 2.13
 Non-CVRFS −0.20 (0.34) 0.82 −0.38 (0.41) 0.68
 CES-D 0.01 (0.04) 1.01 −0.01 (0.05) 0.99
 Stress 0.12 (0.78) 1.13 0.95 (1.30) 2.59
Interactions
 Mean Sleep Duration x PSQI −0.16 (0.58) 0.85
 Mean Sleep Duration x Stress −0.79 (0.62) 0.46
 PSQI x Stress −0.98 (1.75) 0.37
 Occ Sleep x Mean Duration Sleep −0.05 (0.11) 0.95
 Occ Sleep Duration x PSQI −0.39 (0.34) 0.68
 Occ Sleep Duration x Stress −0.03 (0.36) 0.97
 Occ Sleep Duration x Mean Sleep Duration x PSQI −0.22 (0.20) 0.81
 Occ Sleep Duration x Mean Sleep Duration x Stress 0.18 (0.19) 1.20
 Occ Sleep Duration x PSQI x Stress 0.08 (0.52) 1.08

Note:

p = .05;

*

p < .05;

**

p<.01.

Occ = Occasion. CVRFS = Cardiovascular risk factors.

Discussion

Participants who were classified as MCI after one assessment often did not meet MCI criteria at subsequent occasions, which has been previously reported (Larrieu et al., 2002; Ritchie et al., 2001). At subsequent testing occasions, many of these MCI cases were considered to be performing between the MCI and normal cognition cutoffs or performing normally. Daily blood pressure, one of the predictors of interest, was not significantly associated with MCI status across the occasions. The current study, however, observed that on those occasions where individuals slept less or substantially more hours the previous night than on average, they were at greater risk for meeting classification criteria for MCI. Cases of MCI associated with sleep difficulties may represent a miscellaneous MCI sub-type, which can be successfully treated (Gauthier & Touchon, 2005). Studies, however, have suggested that sleep disturbances may not only be observed in this miscellaneous MCI sub-type (Lyketsos et al., 2002; Muangpaisan, Intalapaporn, & Assantachai, 2008). In fact, sleep difficulties have shown to be more common in amnestic MCI participants than normal participants (Muangpaisan et al., 2008). More research is needed to explore whether poor sleep quantity can differentiate MCI from normal cognitive function as well as differentiate the MCI sub-types.

The relationship between sleep duration and MCI status may also reflect diagnostic issues with the MCI classification criteria. Since poor performance on one cognitive measure could result in MCI classification, a lack of sleep the prior night could result in poor cognitive performance the following day and, consequently, a MCI classification. Indeed, several studies have consistently reported that tasks of memory (Schmutte, Harris, Levin, Zweig, Katz, & Lipton, 2007; Ferrie, Shipley, Akbaraly, Marmot, Kivimäki, & Singh-Manoux, 2011; Xu et al., 2011), attention-concentration (Blackwell et al., 2011; Ohayon & Vecchierini, 2002), and executive function (Ferrie et al., 2011; Blackwell et al., 2011) are vulnerable to poor sleep duration. Thus, impaired performance on these abilities may be incorrectly classified as MCI, when performance is possibly related to disturbed sleep.

It is also possible that the instability in cognitive performance, which may have influence the instability in MCI classification, may indeed represent an early indication of cognitive impairment. In fact, previous literature has suggested that fluctuations in cognitive performance are a predictor of future cognitive decline and dementia (Bielak, Hultsch, Strauss, MacDonald, & Hunter, 2010). Given our study is observing a potential within-person coupling relationship between MCI status and sleep duration, it might be beneficial to further explore wither fluctuation in sleep duration may also be a predictor of future cognitive decline and dementia.

Study Limitations

There were several limitations to this study that should be noted. First, the study was the small sample size, which reduces the statistical power to detect significance, particularly for the between-person relationships. Second, the sample is homogeneous as evidenced by the sample’s high average blood pressure. With there being less variance of blood pressure, the current study was less likely to detect a relationship between blood pressure and cognition. The current sample is also homogeneous as evidenced by the high percentage of participants that would be considered poor sleepers. Thus, the lack of variance in sleep quality may potentially explain the non-significant relationship between sleep quality and cognition. Previous studies have suggested that African Americans, particularly in urban settings, tend to report poor sleep quality and quantity (Bidulescu et al., 2010; Pigeon, Heffner, Duberstein, Fiscella, Moynihan, & Chapman, 2011). For example, in a large community sample of African Americans living in Atlanta, 50% of the sample had global PSQI scores suggestive of poor sleep (Bidulescu et al., 2010). Several factors such as social disadvantages, stress, weight, diabetes, and/or vascular disease may explain the high percentage of sleep disturbances in the sample (Bidulescu et al., 2010; Pigeon et al., 2011; Williams, 1998), but further research is needed to disentangle the mechanisms behind sleep disturbances in African Americans. Third, the impairment cutoff scores for MCI were established using the current study’s sample distribution, which may not have been representative of other African American samples. MCI was not evaluated using a clinical criterion standard, which may have led to some misclassification of participants. A limited number of measures were also used to assess impairment on each of the cognitive domains used for MCI classification, which may not be an accurate representation of performance in a particular domain. Several studies, however, have used a post-hoc approach for MCI classification, particularly in community samples (Allaire, Gamaldo, Ayotte et al., 2009; Manly et al., 2008). Fourth, our measurements of stress and depressive symptomatology did not capture the participants’ daily stress and/or mood levels, which limited our analyses from examining whether changes in stress and/or mood might explain the dynamic within-person coupling relationship between sleep duration and cognitive performance.

Conclusions

The current study suggests that intraindividual variability in MCI status, particularly for African Americans, may be due to common health issues (i.e. sleep difficulties) observed within this population. It reveals a promising direction for future research to explore how variability on objective cognitive assessments might be a useful in identifying an individual’s cognitive competency as well as detecting the earliest sign of cognitive impairment.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH, National Institute on Aging. The Baltimore Study of Black Aging is supported by grants from the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG24108 and AG024108-02S1) to K.E.W.

Appendix A. Blood Pressure MLM Model Equation

Level1:Cognitionij=β0ij+β1(OccasionSBPij-MeanSBPi)+rijLevel2:β0i=γ00+γ01(MeanSBPi-SampleMeanSBP)+γ02(MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress)+γ03(MeanSBPi-SampleMeanSBP×MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress)+u0iβ1i=γ10+γ11(MeanSBPi-SampleMeanSBP)+γ12(MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress)+γ13(MeanSBPi-SampleMeanSBP×MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress)+u1i
  • β0ij = Intercept; Cognition for person i on day j

  • γ00 = Grand mean/point estimate of cognition

  • γ01 = Between-person relationship between mean SBP and cognition

  • γ02 = Between-person relationship between mean stress and cognition

  • γ03 = Interaction between mean SBP and mean stress

  • γ10 = Within-person relationship between occasion SBP and cognition

  • γ11 = Cross-level interaction between occasion SBP and mean SBP

  • γ12 = Cross-level interaction between occasion SBP and mean stress

  • γ13 = Cross-level interaction among occasion SBP, mean SBP, and mean stress

  • rij = Within-person variability/random error

  • u0j = Between-person variability/random error

  • u1i = Variability around the slope between SBP and cognition

Appendix B. Sleep Duration MLM Model Equation

Level1:Cognitionij=β0ij+β1(OccasionSleepDurationij-MeanSleepDurationi)+rijLevel2:β0i=γ00+γ01(MeanSleepDurationi-SampleMeanSleepDuration)+γ02(MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress)+γ03(MeanPSQIi-SampleMeanPSQI)+γ04(MeanSleepDurationi-SampleMeanSleepDuration×MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress)+γ05(MeanSleepDurationi-SampleMeanSleepDuration×MeanPSQIi-SampleMeanPSQI)γ06(MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress×MeanPSQIi-SampleMeanPSQI)+u0iβ1i=γ10+γ11(MeanSleepDurationi-SampleMeanSleepDuration)+γ12(MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress)+γ13(MeanPSQIi-SampleMeanPSQI)γ14(MeanSleepDurationi-SampleMeanSleepDuration×MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress)+γ15(MeanSleepDurationi-SampleMeanSleepDuration×MeanPSQIi-SampleMeanPSQI)γ16(MeanStressi-SampleMeanStress×MeanPSQIi-SampleMeanPSQI)+u1i
  • β0ij = Intercept; Cognition for person i on day j

  • γ00 = Grand mean/point estimate of cognition

  • γ01 = Between-person relationship between mean sleep duration and cognition

  • γ02 = Between-person relationship between mean stress and cognition

  • γ03 = Between-person relationship between PSQI and cognition

  • γ04 = Interaction between mean sleep duration and mean stress

  • γ05 = Interaction between mean sleep duration and PSQI

  • γ06 = Interaction between mean stress and PSQI

  • γ10 = Within-person relationship between occasion sleep duration and cognition

  • γ11 – γ16 = Cross-level interactions between occasion sleep duration and Level 2 predictors

  • rij = Within-person variability/random error

  • u0j = Between-person variability/random error

  • u1i = Variability around the slope between sleep duration and cognition

Contributor Information

Alyssa A. Gamaldo, National Institute on Aging.

Jason C. Allaire, North Carolina State University.

Keith E. Whitfield, Duke University.

References

  1. Aldwin CM. The Elders Life Stress Inventory: Egocentric and nonegocentric stress. In: Stephens MAP, Crowther JH, Hobfall SE, Tennenbaum DL, editors. Stress and coping in later life families. New York, NY: Hemisphere Publishing Corporation; 1990. pp. 49–69. [Google Scholar]
  2. Allaire JC, Gamaldo A, Ayotte BJ, Sims R, Whitfield KE. Mild Cognitive Impairment and Objective Instrumental Everyday Functioning: The Everyday Cognition Battery Memory Test. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2009;57:120–125. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02054.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beery KE, Buktenica NA, Beery NA. The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visuo-motor integration: VMI, with supplemental developmental tests of visual perception and motor coordination: administration, scoring and teaching manual. Parsippany, NJ: Modern Curriculum Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bidulescu A, Din-Dzietham R, Coverson DL, Chen Z, Meng Y-X, Buxbaum SG, et al. Interaction of sleep quality and psychosocial stress on obesity in African Americans: the cardiovascular health epidemiology study (CHES) BMC Public Health. 2010;10:581. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bielak AM, Hultsch DF, Strauss E, MacDonald SWS, Hunter MA. Intraindividual variability in reaction time predicts cognitive outcomes 5 years later. Neuropsychology. 2010;24:731–741. doi: 10.1037/a0019802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Redline S, Ensrud KE, Stefanick ML, Laffan A, Stone KL. Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: the MrOS sleep study. Sleep. 2011;34:1347–1356. doi: 10.5665/SLEEP.1276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research. 1988;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chobanian AV, Bakris GL, Black HR, Cushman WC, Green LA, Izzo JL, et al. The seventh report of the joint national committee on prevention, detection, evaluation, and treatment of high blood pressure. JAMA. 2003;289:2560–2572. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.19.2560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Durrence H Heith, Kenneth L Lichstein. The sleep of African Americans: A comparative review. Behavioral Sleep Medicine. 2006;4:29–44. doi: 10.1207/s15402010bsm0401_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH, Derman D. Kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Educational Testing Service; Princeton, NJ: 1976. [Google Scholar]
  11. Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, Akbaraly TN, Marmot MG, Kivimäki M, Singh-Manoux A. Change in sleep duration and cognitive function: Findings from the Whitehall II study. Sleep. 2011;34:565–573. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Foley KL, Reed PS, Mutran EJ, DeVellis RF. Measurement adequacy of the CES-D among a sample of older African-Americans. Psychiatry Research. 2002;109:61–69. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(01)00360-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ganguli M, Chang CCH, Snitz BE, Saxton JA, Vanderbilt J, Lee CW. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment by multiple classifications : The MYHAT project. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18:674–683. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181cdee4f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gamaldo AA, Allaire JC, Sims RC, Whitfield KE. Assessing mild cognitive impairment among older African Americans. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2010;25:748–755. doi: 10.1002/gps.2417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gamaldo AA, Allaire JC, Whitfield KE. The relationship between reported problems falling asleep and cognition among African American elderly. Research on Aging. 2008;30:752–767. [Google Scholar]
  16. Gamaldo AA, Weatherbee SR, Allaire JC. Exploring the within-person coupling of blood pressure and cognition in the elderly. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2008;63:386–389. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.6.p386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gauthier S, Touchon J. Mild cognitive impairment is not a clinical entity and should not be treated. Archives of Neurology. 2005;62:1164–1166. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.7.1164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE) Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: Lea and Febiger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  19. Larrieu S, Letenneur L, Orgogozo JM, Fabrigoule C, Amieva H, Carret N Le, et al. Incidence and outcome of mild cognitive impairment in a population-based prospective cohort. Neurology. 2002;59:1594–1599. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000034176.07159.f8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lyketsos CG, Lopez O, Jones B, Fitzpatrick AL, Breitner J, DeKosky S. Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: Results from the cardiovascular health study. JAMA. 2002;288:1475–1483. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.12.1475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. MacDonald SWS, Li SC, Bäckman L. Neural underpinnings of withinperson variability in cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging. 2009;24:792–808. doi: 10.1037/a0017798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Manly JJ, Tang M-X, Schupf N, Stern Y, Vonsattel J-PG, Mayeux R. Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Annuals of Neurology. 2008;63:494–506. doi: 10.1002/ana.21326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Manos PJ, Wu R. The ten point clock test: A quick screen and grading method for cognitive impairment in medical and surgical patients. International Journal of Psychiatry Medicine. 1994;24:229–244. doi: 10.2190/5A0F-936P-VG8N-0F5R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Muangpaisan W, Intalapaporn S, Assantachai P. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in the community-based patients with mild cognitive impairment and the influence of demographic factors. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008;23:699–703. doi: 10.1002/gps.1963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Nebes RD, Buysse DJ, Halligan EM, Houck PR, Monk TM. Self-reported sleep quality predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2009;10:1–8. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbn037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Neupert SD, Almeida DM, Mroczek DK, Sprio A. Daily stressors and memory failures in a naturalistic setting: Findings from the VA Normative Aging Study. Psychology and Aging. 2006;21:424–429. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ohayon MM, Vecchierini MF. Daytime sleepiness and cognitive impairment in the elderly population. Arch Intern Med. 2002;162:201–208. doi: 10.1001/archinte.162.2.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2004;256:183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Pigeon WR, Heffner K, Duberstein P, Fiscella K, Moynihan J, Chapman BP. Elevated sleep disturbance among blacks in an urban family medicine practice. J Am Board Fam Med. 2011;24:161–168. doi: 10.3122/jabfm.2011.02.100028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Radloff L. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;3:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  31. Rey A. Psychological examination of traumatic encephalopathy. Archives de Psychologie. 1941;28:286–340. [Google Scholar]
  32. Ritchie K, Artero S, Touchon J. Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2001;56:37–42. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Schmutte T, Harris S, Levin R, Zweig R, Katz M, Lipton R. The relation between cognitive functioning and self-reported sleep complaints in nondemented older adults: Results from the Bronx Aging Study. Behavioral Sleep Medicine. 2007;5:39–56. doi: 10.1207/s15402010bsm0501_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Shulman KI, Shedletsky R, Silver IL. The challenge of time: clock-drawing and cognitive function in the elderly. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 1986;1:135–140. [Google Scholar]
  35. Strauss E, MacDonald SWS, Hunter M, Moll A, Hultsch DF. Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in three groups of older adults: Cross-domain links to physical status and selfperceived affect and beliefs. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8:893–906. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702870035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Sundquist J, Winkleby MA, Pudaric S. Cardiovascular disease risk factors among older black, Mexican-American, and white women and men: An analysis of NHANES III, 1988–1994. American Geriatrics Society. 2001;42:109–116. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49030.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Thurstone TG. Primary mental ability for Grades 9–12. Chicago, IL: Science Research Associates; 1962. (Rev. ed.) [Google Scholar]
  38. Trenerry MR, Crosson D, DeBoe J, Leber WR. Stroop neurological screening test. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  39. Wadley VG, Crowe M, Marsiske M, Cook SE, Unverzagt FW, Rosenberg AL, Rexroth D. Changes in everyday function in individuals with psychometrically defined mild cognitive impairement in the advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly study. Journal of American Geriatric Society. 2007;55:1192–1198. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01245.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Weatherbee SR, Gamaldo AA, Allaire JC. Exploring the within-person coupling of reading vision and cognition in the elderly. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 2009;16:671–682. doi: 10.1080/13825580902871034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Whitfield KE, Allaire J, Aiken-Morgan AT, Gamaldo A, Sims R, Edwards CL. Blood pressure and memory in older African Americans. Journal of Ethnicity and Disease. 2008;18:181–186. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Whitfield KE, Baker-Thomas TA, Heyward K, Gatto M, Williams Y. Evaluating a measure of everyday problem solving for use in African-Americans. Experimental Aging Research. 1999;25:209–222. doi: 10.1080/036107399243995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Williams DR. African-American health: the role of social environment. J Urban Health. 1998;75:300–321. doi: 10.1007/BF02345099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Xu L, Jiang CQ, Lam TH, Liu B, Jin YL, Zhu T, et al. Short or long sleep duration is associated with memory impairment in older chinese : the Guangzhou Biobank cohort study. Sleep. 2011;34:575–580. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES