Abstract
We investigated fitness, military rank and survival of facial phenotypes in large-scale warfare using 795 Finnish soldiers who fought in the Winter War (1939–1940). We measured facial width-to-height ratio—a trait known to predict aggressive behaviour in males—and assessed whether facial morphology could predict survival, lifetime reproductive success (LRS) and social status. We found no difference in survival along the phenotypic gradient, however, wider-faced individuals had greater LRS, but achieved a lower military rank.
Keywords: lifetime reproductive success, social dominance, survival, war, facial morphology
1. Introduction
A constant feature throughout human evolution has been intraspecific conflict [1–3], and simulations have identified warfare as a possible major component of human social evolution [2]. Aggressive individuals who have participated in small-scale warfare and revenge killings may receive fitness costs [4] or benefits [5]. While these two previous studies allow some inferences to be made about small-scale tribal warfare, little is known of the survival and fitness benefits for individuals in large-scale conflicts.
Facial morphology provides a particularly useful proxy measure for male behaviour, and the wealth of historical photographs and data available allow hypotheses essential to the study of human evolution to be tested. Research has demonstrated that male facial morphology can predict social dominance [6], sexual attractiveness [7,8], reproductive success [9,10], testosterone levels [11] as well as strength and fighting ability [12]. In particular, the facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR, [13]; figure 1) predicts a suite of characters in males: aggressiveness [14,15], mortality in violent conflicts [16], cooperative ability [17] and trustworthiness [18,19]. Wider-faced men have higher testosterone levels [20], are more aggressive ([14,15]; but see [21,22]), and are perceived by others to be more aggressive [23]. Wider-faced males exploit trust more often, and others tend to trust thinner-faced males more readily [18,19], however, in the presence of competition, wider-faced males have also been shown to demonstrate greater cooperation with peers [17].
On 30 November 1939, the Soviet Red Army invaded Finland, starting the 3.5 month long Winter War. We used Finnish archives to collate data on survival and number of offspring for soldiers who fought in this war to explore the correlation of fWHR with rank, fitness and survival. Mueller & Mazur [6] found that dominance scores given to faces of recruits in an elite US military academy predicted future rank and lifetime reproductive success (LRS), but not survival during conflicts. Previous research has shown that fWHR predicts mortality with wider-faced males less likely to die violent deaths, but only when close physical contact is involved, and not when technology is used [16], and a positive relationship between fWHR and LRS has been found in a historic Austrian population [24].
2. Material and methods
To enable comparison of soldiers with a similar probability of survival, data (available via doi:10.5061/dryad.d5vh1) were primarily gathered from two infantry regiments (JR 16, n = 333 and JR 21, n = 312), and one artillery regiment (KTR 3, n = 60). These regiments were selected based on the availability of photographs from the sources listed below. Photographs of soldiers who died during the war (n = 510) were scanned from their source [25]. Birth and death dates, birth place, rank, regiment, marital status and number of children were found for these soldiers from the online database of the National Archives of Finland (http://kronos.narc.fi/menehtyneet/). Data for surviving soldiers (n = 285) were scanned from their source [26]. Scanning focused on the three regiments, however, scanned pages that contained soldiers from other regiments (n = 90) were included in analysis. Records include photographs, military rank, birth date and place, regiment(s) served with and birth date of children. For analysis, rank at the start of the Winter War was divided into three categories: enlisted ranks (ranks below officers), junior officers and senior officers. See the electronic supplementary material, S1 for more information on these data.
We measured fWHR [13] for subjects whose head was turned up to approximately 15° to the side (our analyses are robust to including this amount of turn). Photographs were standardized to a width of 600 pixels and measured using TPSDig v. 2.10 [27]. Additional information on fWHR measurements can be found from the electronic supplementary material, S2.
Individual fitness was calculated as the expected number of children, averaging over whether a father survived the Winter War, i.e. Pr(died in war) × (number of children|died) + Pr(Survived war) × (number of children|survived). Dying in the war was modelled as a logistic regression, with face, regiment, birth place, age and rank as effects (face and age as continuous, others as factors). Similarly, the number of children was modelled as following a Poisson distribution with the same effects, but also with survival as an additional factor, along with an interaction between age and survival. Measurements of face shape were assumed to be normally distributed with the mean set at the true face value, which was used in the analysis. The model was fitted with a Bayesian approach using vague priors (see the electronic supplementary material, S3 for more information).
3. Results
Initial analysis showed a weak effect of fWHR on survival, (log odds ratio of survival for soldier with face 1 s.d. wider: −0.19, 95% HPDI: −0.37 to −0.04; figure 2b; Pr(OR > 0) = 0.007), however, this effect disappeared when only soldiers from the three main regiments were analysed (posterior mode of log odds ratio: −0.014, HPDI: −0.20 to 0.17; Pr(OR > 0) = 0.44). Soldiers with wider faces had more children after controlling for wartime survival, (analysis with full data; a soldier with a face 1 s.d. wider has 1.88 times as many children: 95% HPDI: 1.17–2.88, Pr(Ratio < 1) = 0.0017, figure 2c), and males with thinner faces achieved higher rank within the Finnish military prior to the start of hostilities (analysis with full data; log odds ratio of soldier with face 1 s.d. thinner being a higher rank: 0.40, 95% HPDI: 0.19–0.61, figure 2d; and see electronic supplementary material, S4).
4. Discussion
As fWHR appears to reflect aggressiveness [14–16,20], our finding that LRS increases with facial width is in agreement with evidence that aggression is a sexually selected trait [28]. Human male mating success is correlated with testosterone levels, a trend possibly attributable to increased mating effort [29,30]. It is unlikely that facial width per se is under selection: selection operates on a suite of traits, rather than on one single trait [31], and since facial morphology is correlated with several behavioural and physical attributes [6,11,12,15,17,19,20], its evolution is likely to be the effect of pleiotropy. While fWHR did predict LRS, as in previous research the relationship was not a strong one ([24]; figure 2), as might be expected given the amount of other factors that determine LRS. From the perspective of evolutionary biology, fWHR by itself is probably of minor significance; however, it is a useful trait to advance the study of human evolution because of its ease of measurement and its role as an indicator of other behavioural and morphological traits of evolutionary significance.
The result that thinner-faced soldiers achieved higher rank before the start of conflict is somewhat surprising because male social dominance can be predicted by testosterone levels [32], which are presumably higher in wider-faced males. However, dominance in the military may be better predicted by leadership qualities than aggressiveness. The greater trustworthiness [18,19] associated with thinner faces may explain our result that thinner-faced males were able to attain higher rank (and therefore positions of trust) in the military. The military relies on a strict hierarchy, which requires trust and/or fear of punishment to be maintained. In this way, the social structure supports the functioning of the military because individuals who are perceived to be more trustworthy attain higher social dominance, a necessity when leading subordinates into situations of high mortality risk.
In the full data, we found a minor correlation between facial width and survival, however, this effect disappeared when only the three main regiments were analysed, which is a better test of the survival hypothesis. Previous research has found that wider-faced males are less likely to die violent deaths, but only when close physical contact is involved (e.g. death by knife wounds or strangling), and not when technology is used (death by gunshot or poisoning; [16]). Given that technology was involved in wartime mortality, our result is in agreement with this previous work. It is also unclear how alternative tactics (such as avoidance of conflict) might affect overall survival in the entire population. In the case of Finland, mandatory military service for males guaranteed high participation in the Second World War.
An overall picture emerges where positions of trust and power are held by thinner-faced, probably less aggressive, yet possibly more trustworthy individuals. Wider-faced, more aggressive individuals have higher LRS, and the almost equal probability of mortality during the war meant that war did not change this relationship between LRS and fWHR. It is perhaps surprising that aggressive individuals do not achieve any significant fitness or survival benefits from war, however, this may be due to the advent of technology [16]. The fitness outcomes of other conflicts may differ from the case studied here, for example, if an attacking force successfully invades territory and conquering soldiers sire offspring (due to forced copulation or mutual consent) with resident females.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank P. Kullberg, T. Leinonen, J. Merilä, the staff of the Finnish National Defence Library and Finnish National Archives for their assistance and support, as well as two anonymous referees for their constructive comments. J.L. received financial support from Emil Aaltonen Foundation, and B.O'H was financially supported by the programme ‘LOEWE-Landes-Offensive zur Entwicklung Wissenschaftlich-ökonomischer Exzellenz’ from the Ministry of Higher Education, Research, and the Arts, Hesse, Germany.
References
- 1.Darwin CR. 1871. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex, vol. 1, 1st edn London, UK: John Murray [Google Scholar]
- 2.Choi J-K, Bowles S. 2007. The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science 318, 636–640 10.1126/science.1144237 (doi:10.1126/science.1144237) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lehmann L, Feldman M. 2008. War and the evolution of belligerence and bravery. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 2877–2885 10.1098/rspb.2008.0842 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0842) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Beckerman S, Erickson PI, Yost J, Regalado J, Jaramillo L, Sparks C, Iromenga M, Long K. 2009. Life histories, blood revenge, and reproductive success among the Waorani of Ecuador. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 8134–8139 10.1073/pnas.0901431106 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0901431106) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Chagnon NA. 1988. Life histories, blood revenge and warfare in a tribal population. Science 239, 985–992 10.1126/science.239.4843.985 (doi:10.1126/science.239.4843.985) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mueller U, Mazur A. 1997. Facial dominance in Homo sapiens as honest signaling of male quality. Behav. Ecol. 8, 569–579 10.1093/beheco/8.5.569 (doi:10.1093/beheco/8.5.569) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI, Castles DL, Kobayashi T, Burt DM, Murray LK, Minamisawa R. 1999. Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature 399, 741–742 10.1038/21557 (doi:10.1038/21557) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fink B, Penton-Voak I. 2002. Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 154–158 10.1111/1467-8721.00190 (doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00190) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Jokela M. 2009. Physical attractiveness and reproductive success in humans: evidence from the late 20th century United States. Evol. Hum. Behav. 30, 342–350 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.03.006 (doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.03.006) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Prokop P, Fedor P. 2011. Physical attractiveness influences reproductive success of modern men. J. Ethol. 29, 453–458 10.1007/s10164-011-0274-0 (doi:10.1007/s10164-011-0274-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Penton-Voak IS, Chen JY. 2004. High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 229–241 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.04.003 (doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.04.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sell A, Cosmides L, Tooby J, Sznycer D, von Rueden C, Gurven M. 2008. Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 575–584 10.1098/rspb.2008.1177 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1177) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Weston EM, Friday AE, Lio P. 2007. Biometric evidence that sexual selection has shaped the Hominin face. PLoS ONE 2, e710. 10.1371/journal.pone.0000710 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000710) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Carré JM, McCormick CM. 2008. In your face: facial metrics predict aggressive behaviour in the laboratory and in varsity and professional hockey players. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 2651–2656 10.1098/rspb.2008.0873 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0873) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Carré JM, McCormick CM, Mondloch CJ. 2009. Facial structure is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1194–1198 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02423.x (doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02423.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Stirrat M, Stulp G, Pollet TV. 2012. Male facial width is associated with death by contact violence: narrow-faced males are more likely to die from contact violence. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 551–556 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.02.002 (doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.02.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Stirrat M, Perrett DI. 2012. Face structure predicts cooperation: men with wider faces are more generous to their in-group when out-group competition is salient. Psychol. Sci. 23, 718–722 10.1177/0956797611435133 (doi:10.1177/0956797611435133) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Stirrat MR, Perrett DI. 2010. Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychol. Sci. 21, 349–354 10.1177/0956797610362647 (doi:10.1177/0956797610362647) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Haselhuhn MP, Wong EM. 2012. Bad to the bone: facial structure predicts unethical behaviour. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 571–576 10.1098/rspb.2011.1193 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.1193) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lefevre CE, Lewis GJ, Perrett DI, Penke L. In press. Telling facial metrics: facial width-to-height ratio is associated with testosterone levels in men. Evol. Hum. Behav. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Özener B. 2011. Facial width-to-height ratio in a Turkish population is not sexually dimorphic and is unrelated to aggressive behavior. Evol. Hum. Behav. 3, 169–173 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.08.001 (doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.08.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Deaner RO, Goetz SMM, Shattuck K, Schnotala T. 2012. Body weight, not facial width-to-height ratio, predicts aggression in pro hockey players. J. Res. Pers. 46, 235–238 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.005 (doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Carré JM, Morrissey MD, Mondloch CJ, McCormick CM. 2010. Estimating aggression from emotionally neutral faces: which facial cues are diagnostic? Perception 39, 356–377 10.1068/p6543 (doi:10.1068/p6543) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Gómez-Valdés J, et al. 2013. Lack of support for the association between facial shape and aggression: a reappraisal based on a worldwide population genetics perspective. PLoS ONE 8, e52317. 10.1371/journal.pone.0052317 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052317) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Anon 1941. Vapautemme Hinta. Helsinki, Finland: Suomen Kuvalehti [Google Scholar]
- 26.Antila O. 1978. Suomen Rintamamiehet 18. div. Lieto, Finland: Etelä-Suomen kustannus [Google Scholar]
- 27.Rohlf FJ. 2009. tpsDig. Version 2.14. New York, NY: State University of New York [Google Scholar]
- 28.Archer J. 2009. Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? Behav. Brain Sci. 32, 249–266 10.1017/S0140525X09990951 (doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990951) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Bogaert AF, Fisher WA. 1995. Predictors of university men's number of sexual partners. J. Sex Res. 32, 119–130 10.1080/00224499509551782 (doi:10.1080/00224499509551782) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Peters M, Simmons LW, Rhodes G. 2008. Testosterone is associated with mating success but not attractiveness or masculinity in human males. Anim. Behav. 76, 297–303 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.008 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kruuk LEB, Garant D. 2007. A wake-up call for studies of natural selection? J. Evol. Biol. 20, 30–33 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01223.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01223.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mazur A, Booth A. 1998. Testosterone and dominance in men. Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 353–363 10.1017/S0140525X98001228 (doi:10.1017/S0140525X98001228) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]