Abstract
Functional MRI (fMRI) studies have linked the posteromedial cortex to episodic learning (encoding) and remembering (retrieval) processes. The posteromedial cortex is considered part of the default network and tends to deactivate during encoding, but activate during retrieval, a pattern known as the encoding/retrieval flip. Yet, the exact relationship between the neural correlates of memory performance (hit/miss) and memory stage (encoding/retrieval) and the extent of overlap with intrinsic cortical networks remains to be elucidated. Using task-based fMRI, we isolated the pattern of activity associated with memory performance, memory stage and the interaction between both. Using resting-state fMRI, we identified which intrinsic large-scale functional networks overlapped with regions showing task-induced effects. Our results demonstrated an effect of successful memory performance in regions associated with the control network and an effect of unsuccessful memory performance in the ventral attention network. We found an effect of memory retrieval in brain regions that span the default and control networks. Finally, we found an interaction between memory performance and memory stage in brain regions associated with the default network, including the posteromedial cortex, posterior parietal cortex and parahippocampal cortex. We discuss these findings in relation to the encoding/retrieval flip. In general, the findings demonstrate that task-induced effects cut across intrinsic cortical networks. Furthermore, regions within the default network display functional dissociations and this may have implications for the neural underpinnings of age-related memory disorders.
Introduction
Neuroimaging has revealed that learning (encoding) and remembering (retrieval) episodic memories are associated with distinct patterns of activity across the brain. A set of regions – overlapping the default network – tends to decrease when individuals encode information into episodic memory (Daselaar, Prince, & Cabeza, 2004; Kim, 2011; Otten & Rugg, 2001; Shrager, Kirwan, & Squire, 2008). In contrast, retrieval of episodic memories tends to increase activity in a subset of the same regions, including posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and posteromedial cortex (PMC) (Hutchinson, Uncapher, & Wagner, 2009; Konishi, Wheeler, Donaldson, & Buckner, 2000; Schacter, Buckner, Koutstaal, Dale, & Rosen, 1997). Several studies have recently demonstrated overlap between these encoding-related deactivations and retrieval-related activations, a pattern known as the encoding/retrieval flip (E/R-flip) (Daselaar et al., 2009; Gilbert, Armbruster, & Panagiotidi, 2011; Huijbers, Pennartz, Cabeza, & Daselaar, 2009, 2011; Kim, Daselaar, & Cabeza, 2010; Vannini et al., 2012; Vannini et al., 2011). However, there has been considerable variation between studies in the exact location of the E/R-flip and whether the flip occurs primarily in the default network.
A related question is whether successful (hits) versus unsuccessful (miss) memory performance has an impact on the E/R-flip. The E/R-flip was originally defined as overlap between the negative encoding success effect (encoding hit < miss) and the positive retrieval success effect (retrieval hit > miss) (Daselaar et al., 2009; Huijbers et al., 2009), and was observed in both PPC and PMC. This method of defining the E/R-flip has the advantage of being independent from an fMRI baseline, but can be influenced by differential performance across encoding and retrieval stages. Recent studies have also observed an E/R-flip in the PMC using a method contrasting successful encoding and successful retrieval to baseline (Vannini et al., 2012; Vannini et al., 2011). This baseline-dependent approach could also be influenced by differential performance across memory stages, but may also be affected by baseline differences between the different stages, especially when encoding and retrieval are tested in different sessions. Hence, the extent to which the E/R-flip is driven by memory performance (hit/miss) versus memory stage (encoding/retrieval) remains unclear.
In the current study, we examine the question: does the E/R-flip represent an interaction between memory performance (hit/miss) and memory stage (encoding/retrieval) or alternatively, is the E/R-flip driven primarily by either memory stage or performance? To identify the influence of memory performance and stage on fMRI activity -and the interaction between these processes- we designed an experiment in which healthy young subjects simultaneously encoded and retrieved information. The design allowed separate modeling of the main effects and statistical interactions between memory performance (hit/miss) and memory stage (encoding/retrieval). This methodological approach alleviates some of the difficulties associated with interpreting statistical interactions in fMRI, which stem from the fact that the fMRI signal is a relative measure (Gusnard & Raichle, 2001; Stark & Squire, 2001). Because activity levels can be modulated by the cognitive state of the subject (Andrews-Hanna, Reidler, Huang, & Buckner, 2010; Hayden, Smith, & Platt, 2009; Weissman, Roberts, Visscher, & Woldorff, 2006), having encoding and retrieval occur during the same run, rather than in different runs, allows a more direct comparison between the two memory stages in the context of a common baseline. Guided by earlier E/R-flip studies (Daselaar et al., 2009; Huijbers et al., 2009; Vannini et al., 2011), we predicted an interaction between memory performance and stage within the parietal cortices, including the PPC and PMC. In addition, we explored main effects of memory performance and memory stage and the possibility that an interaction could also be observed in regions that have not been associated with the default network.
Our second aim was to clarify the location of task-induced effects – including the E/R-flip – in relation to intrinsic cortical networks. Therefore, we acquired resting-state fMRI within the same set of subjects. Recently, performance-related deactivations have been observed within regions overlapping both the ventral attention network and the default network using a working-memory task (Anticevic, Repovs, Shulman, & Barch, 2010). Furthermore, retrieval-related activations have been observed in regions overlapping both the control and default networks using a autobiographical memory task (Spreng, Stevens, Chamberlain, Gilmore, & Schacter, 2010). These findings suggest that memory performance and memory stage-related activity may span multiple networks. Other neuroimaging studies indicate that the default network can be fractionated into multiple sub-networks (Andrews-Hanna, Reidler, Sepulcre, Poulin, & Buckner, 2010; Damoiseaux, Prater, Miller, & Greicius, 2012; Leech, Kamourieh, Beckmann, & Sharp, 2011), suggesting that memory-related effects might involve select regions within a larger network (i.e. the default network). The combination of task-induced activity and resting-state fMRI allowed us to clarify if memory-related patterns occur within individual cortical networks or across regions spanning multiple networks. The current study attempts to dissociate the neurocorrelates of memory performance, memory stage and the interaction between both, and to clarify the location of memory-related activity in relation to intrinsic large-scale cortical networks.
Methods
Participants
Forty-five young, cognitively normal adults (28 female), age 24.5±0.5 years were recruited from the Boston area. All subjects were native English speakers, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were right-handed. No subject had a history of psychiatric or neurological disorders or reported taking medications that affect the central nervous system. Informed written consent was obtained from every subject prior to experimental procedures and the study was approved by and conducted in accordance with the Partners Human Research Committee at the Brigham and Women Hospital and Massachusetts General Hospital (Boston, MA). Data from two additional subjects were excluded due to data collection errors during scanning.
Experimental paradigm
The experimental paradigm consisted of a memory task designed to acquire fMRI images both during learning (encoding) and remembering (retrieval) (Figure 1). The paradigm consisted of three sessions in one visit: (1) Pre-Scan, (2) Scan and (3) Post-Scan sessions. Across all sessions, subjects observed in total, a set of 450 faces with an emotionally neutral expression paired with fictional first names, similar as those used in Minear & Park (2004) and Sperling et. al. (2001). Subjects initially learned a set of face-name pairs (“novel” pairs) and subsequently identified previously seen (“old”) face-name pairs, “re-paired” faces and names and completely novel face-name pairs.
In the Pre-Scan session (outside the scanner) 150 novel face-name pairs were encoded. During this session, color photographs of faces were presented against a black background with a fictional first name printed in white below the face (face-name pairs). Subjects were explicitly told to remember the name associated with the face. Subjects were asked to press a button indicating whether the name was a good “fit” for the face and another button if the name was not a good “fit” for the face. This type of judgment is known to facilitate learning and ensured that the subjects attended both the face and the name (Sperling et al., 2003). The Pre-Scan session consisted of 3 runs. In each run, 50 face-name pairs were shown for 3750 ms apiece with a constant 250 ms inter-trial interval, during which a white crosshair was presented on the screen.
-
In the Scan session, subjects again viewed face-name pairs. 50% of the face-name pairs were completely novel. 40% of the face-name pairs were “old”, thus previously presented during the Pre-Scan session. 10% of the face-name pairs were “re-paired”. Re-paired face-name pairs were created by re-pairing a previously seen face, presented during the Pre-scan session, with a previously seen name. However, the face and name presented together for “re-paired” stimuli were not previously seen together. In total, the Scan session consisted of retrieval of 120 old and 30 re-paired face-name pairs and encoding of 150 new face-name pairs.
Re-paired faces-name pairs were included to ensure that subjects attended to both the face and the name. Subjects were asked to press one button to indicate that they had seen the exact face-name pair before and a different button if they had not seen the exact face-name pair before (hence, re-paired pairs were to be assigned the same response as novel pairs). Subjects were also told that they should try to learn the novel face-name pairs, as they would be tested in the Post-Scan session. To ensure that subjects were attentive and understood the instructions, detailed oral instructions were recited prior to each run. The Scan session consisted of 6 runs. In each run, 50 face-name pairs were shown for 3750 ms with an inter-trial interval varying between 250 and 10250 ms (average 3300 ms), during which a white crosshair was presented on the screen. The range of the inter-trial intervals and trial order was optimized using optseq2 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq Dale, Greve, & Burock, 1999).
In the Post-Scan session (outside the scanner) subjects conducted a task almost identical to the Scan session. Again 50% of the face-name pairs were completely novel, 40% of the face-name pairs were “old”, presented during the Scan session and 10% of the face-name pairs were “re-paired”.” Re-paired pairs were created by re-pairing the “novel” face-name pairs from the Scan session. Subjects were again asked to press a button indicating whether they had seen the exact face-name pair before. The Post-Scan session consisted of 3 runs. In each run 100 face-name pairs were shown for a maximum of 9000 ms. When a response was made, the next item appeared on the screen after a 1000 ms delay. On average, subjects responded within the same time as during the Scan session (see Results). This nearly self-paced timing was used to minimize the number of omissions. Note that the Post-Scan was only used to determine the subsequent memory of novel pairs encoded inside the scanner. In total, the subjects retrieved 120 old and 30 re-paired face-name pairs (from the 150 face-name pairs encoding during scanning), interspersed with an additional novel 150 face-name pairs.
All subjects completed practice sessions both outside and inside of the MR-scanner before the fMRI data were collected. The paradigm was designed and generated on an MacBook Pro laptop (Apple Inc. Cupertino, CA, USA) using the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997) and MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). Stimuli were projected by means of an MR compatible goggle-system (VisuaStim XGA, Resonance Technology Inc., Los Angeles, CA, USA). Inside the scanner, subjects responded with an MRI compatible fiber-optical key press device with two buttons held in their right hand and behavioral responses (accuracy and reaction time) were recorded by a computer using the Psychophysics Toolbox software in the scanner control room. In the Pre- and Post-scan session, subjects directly viewed the stimuli on the Apple computer (19″ inch screen) and responded via two buttons on a USB pocket keypad (Kensington Technology Group, Redwood Shores, CA, USA).
Imaging acquisition
The MR images were acquired using a GE Signa 3.0 Tesla MR system equipped with an eight-channel head coil (General Electric Company, Fairfield, CT, USA). High-resolution T1-weighted structural images were acquired using an accelerated 3D inversion recovery spoiled gradient echo (IR-SPGR) sequence: 196 sagittal slices, repetition time (TR) = 6.4 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.8 ms, inversion time (TI) = 400 ms, flip angle (FA) = 11°, field of view (FOV) = 256 mm, matrix 256 × 256, resolution of 1 × 1 × 1.2 mm. Task-related blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) fMRI data was acquired using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo planar (EPI) sequence. We acquired six functional runs of 180 volumes with repetition time (TR) 2000 ms; echo time (TE) 30 ms; flip angle 90º, 64×64 matrix size. We acquired 32 oblique 3mm thick slices in ascending order, with a skip of 0.8, aligned parallel to the AC-PC plane. This resulted in an effective voxel size of 3×3×3.8mm. After the experimental task, we also acquired 1 resting-state run of 120 volumes with a repetition time (TR) 3000 ms; echo time (TE) 30 ms; flip angle 85º, 64×64 matrix size. We acquired 47 oblique 3 mm thick slices in interleaved order aligned parallel to the AC-PC. This resulted in an effective voxel size of 3×3×3mm.
fMRI preprocessing
The functional data was preprocessed and analyzed using SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, UCL, London: http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Functional MRI data were slice time-corrected, realigned, normalized to the MNI152 EPI template, resampled to 3×3×3mm voxels and smoothed with 8mm full width half maximum Gaussian kernel. Resting-state fMRI data was band-pass filtered between 0.01 and 0.08 Hz and nuisance regressors for the white-matter, lateral ventricles, and global brain signals, and the motion parameters plus the first derivatives of these nuisance regressors, were modeled.
Task-based fMRI
For the subject-level fMRI task analysis, we used the general linear model (GLM) as implemented in SPM8. Face-name pairs were coded into one of three types: previously seen (old), not-seen (new) or re-paired. Depending on the responses, these items were classified as correct: retrieval hit, correct rejection, re-paired hit. Or when incorrect as: retrieval miss, false alarm or re-paired miss. The new face-name pairs were also coded depending on the Post-Scan subsequent memory: either as encoding hit, encoding miss or re-paired. Omissions were also coded as separate events, resulting into a total of 11 different trial types. The onsets plus the response time for these events were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function to create task regressors. Additionally, the models included the motion parameters and a high-pass filter (1/128Hz). Contrasts were created for the trial-types: retrieval hit (mean number of trials = 81±2.6), retrieval miss (34±2.5), encoding hit (55±2.8), encoding miss (51±2.9). Note that encoding hit and miss were only included in the subject-level contrasts when they were initially correctly rejected inside the scanner.
For the group-level analysis, we used SPM8 in combination with in-house MATLAB scripts (GLM-Flex, Harvard Aging Brain Study, Martinos Center, MGH, Charlestown, USA, (http://nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/harvardagingbrain/People/AaronSchultz/Aarons_Scripts). These scripts implement a standard partitioned error GLM (consistent with the method used in SPSS, R, and SAS), as opposed to the corrected pooled error approach implemented in SPM8. The partitioned error approach makes fewer assumptions regarding independence, equality of variance, and the stability of covariance patterns across voxels, and as such is more appropriate for a multi-factor repeated measures interaction test. Similar as SPM8, GLM-Flex also corrects for violations of sphericity within each factor by pooling covariance estimates across voxels. The four beta-maps (encoding hit/miss, retrieval hit/miss) from each subject were entered into a second level model with factors for task-memory performance (hit = success / miss = failure), memory stage (new = encoding / old = retrieval), and subject. The model also included the interaction between memory performance and memory stage. Whole-brain statistical analysis were thresholded at P < 0.05, FDR corrected, with an extent threshold >5 voxels (Table 1). For the figures, the resulting T-maps were projected to the cortical surface using the standard MNI to FS-average transformation as implemented in FreeSurfer 5.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/).
Table 1.
Effect of succesfull memory performance : (hit > miss): | ||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Label | Side | BA | T | Size | X | Y | Z | Task-Based Effects | Resting-State Effects | |||||||
P | S | P x S | CON | DMN | DAN | MOT | VAN | VIS | ||||||||
Dorsolateral PFC | L | 9/46/47 | 6.02 | 693 | −48 | 11 | 35 | 41.43 | 6.97 | 0.36 | 10.33 | −1.71 | 3.59 | −0.67 | 3.76 | −5.65 |
R | 9 | 4.81 | 64 | 42 | 8 | 29 | 20.40 | 2.91 | 0.58 | 10.21 | −5.40 | 8.37 | −2.49 | 8.60 | −3.50 | |
R | 47 | 4.67 | 11 | 33 | 29 | −19 | 21.49 | 2.91 | 0.22 | −0.87 | 3.04 | −5.96 | −3.89 | −2.99 | −3.92 | |
R | 47 | 3.60 | 18 | 39 | 20 | −7 | 13.18 | 7.77 | 0.19 | 4.74 | −8.48 | 0.65 | −7.04 | 5.73 | −0.84 | |
R | 46 | 4.30 | 34 | 48 | 29 | 17 | 17.11 | 0.02 | 1.01 | 4.73 | 2.19 | −0.14 | −0.38 | 1.88 | −4.44 | |
PPC | R | 7/40 | 4.63 | 67 | 33 | −64 | 47 | 18.89 | 0.17 | 1.18 | 14.95 | 0.60 | 7.60 | −5.64 | −3.24 | −5.33 |
ACC / SMA | L | 8/32 | 4.28 | 61 | −6 | 20 | 47 | 18.20 | 11.14 | 1.11 | 8.56 | −1.75 | 1.95 | −2.00 | 1.90 | −5.12 |
Brainstem | L | 3.97 | 14 | −6 | −28 | −10 | 15.38 | 0.62 | 0.43 | −2.49 | 2.41 | −2.03 | −5.60 | −2.70 | 1.78 | |
R | 4.11 | 19 | 6 | −25 | −13 | 16.98 | 0.00 | 0.19 | −1.68 | 2.28 | −1.88 | −6.30 | −1.86 | 1.51 | ||
Visual Crtx. | L | 18/19 | 6.78 | 322 | −27 | −82 | −7 | 30.51 | 0.91 | 0.56 | −4.27 | −8.91 | 0.89 | −0.11 | −1.18 | 17.58 |
L | 19 | 3.58 | 17 | −27 | −73 | 32 | 15.36 | 1.43 | 1.61 | −0.45 | −7.12 | 10.38 | 1.01 | 0.40 | 6.15 | |
R | 18/19 | 6.09 | 413 | 36 | −88 | 8 | 34.10 | 4.55 | 10.48 | −6.43 | −7.79 | 1.63 | 0.79 | −2.20 | 15.17 | |
R | 18 | 3.98 | 11 | 6 | −94 | 14 | 14.73 | 22.17 | 0.89 | −2.98 | −5.98 | −1.10 | −3.18 | 0.52 | 13.47 |
Effect of unsuccesfull memory performance (miss > hit): | ||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Label | Side | BA | T | Size | X | Y | Z | P | S | P x S | CON | DMN | DAN | MOT | VAN | VIS |
Dorsal Precuneus | L | 7 | 4.52 | 122 | −6 | −52 | 50 | 23.70 | 2.28 | 0.03 | 1.60 | 6.22 | 6.10 | −1.47 | 2.15 | −6.72 |
R | 7 | 3.64 | 7 | 9 | −58 | 59 | 12.81 | 0.12 | 0.12 | 1.78 | −2.38 | 12.84 | 0.66 | 6.62 | −2.50 | |
TPJ | L | 22/40/41 | 4.30 | 131 | −63 | −31 | 23 | 23.27 | 7.67 | 0.1 | −5.13 | −7.74 | −0.42 | 5.24 | 9.59 | 1.81 |
R | 22/40/41 | 6.58 | 174 | 57 | −40 | 29 | 31.66 | 5.15 | 0.84 | 1.14 | −9.31 | 6.91 | −0.20 | 19.52 | −1.07 | |
Lateral Temp. Crtx | R | 41 | 3.82 | 11 | 57 | −25 | 11 | 16.99 | 1.64 | 0.45 | −4.89 | −8.59 | −0.22 | 7.15 | 9.69 | 1.63 |
R | 22 | 3.66 | 13 | 57 | −55 | 17 | 14.73 | 0.93 | 0.11 | −5.16 | 3.40 | −3.40 | −0.87 | 4.04 | −3.94 | |
Superior PFC | L | 9/10 | 4.64 | 144 | −33 | 37 | 34 | 30.84 | 10.00 | 0.15 | 2.10 | 0.69 | 2.34 | −3.56 | 10.65 | −4.28 |
R | 9/10 | 5.59 | 294 | 27 | 38 | 38 | 42.74 | 0.97 | 0.01 | 7.49 | −1.88 | 3.74 | −6.78 | 10.78 | −3.24 | |
Post. Cing. Crtx. | R | 31 | 3.84 | 14 | 15 | −37 | 44 | 15.83 | 0.62 | 2.47 | 3.06 | −3.03 | 10.18 | −3.50 | 12.58 | −2.01 |
Somatosensory | R | 5 | 3.37 | 6 | 24 | −46 | 71 | 12.42 | 0.42 | 0.35 | 6.99 | 0.45 | −1.03 | −3.38 | 1.11 | −3.19 |
Visual Crtx. | R | 18 | 4.12 | 32 | 15 | −76 | 2 | 20.34 | 13.38 | 3.49 | −3.70 | −9.09 | 1.59 | −0.45 | −0.42 | 15.07 |
Top panel demonstrates effects of successful memory performance (hit > miss), lower panel an effect of unsuccessful memory performance (miss > hit). Left column shows activated regions: BA = Brodmann Area, T-value of task-based effect; Size = cluster size in voxels, Coordinates X, Y, Z are in MNI space. Middle column shows F-values for the 2×2 post-hoc ANOVA for each cluster: P = memory performance (selected for by task-based analysis), S = memory stage, P x S = interaction. Significant F-values are indicated in bold. Right column shows resting-state effects: CON = control network, DMN = default network, DAN = dorsal attention network, MOT = motor network, VAN = ventral attention network. VIS = visual network. Significant T-values are indicated in bold, negative values are listed in grey.
Resting-state fMRI
To identify the location of task-induced fMRI effects in relation to intrinsic large-scale networks, we used resting-state fMRI data gathered within the same subjects immediately after the task. Using seed-based resting state connectivity analyses, we identified six intrinsic cortical networks using the following set of seed locations as published by Yeo et. al. (2011). Note that that visual seeds were based on previous studies (e.g. Fischl et al., 2008; see table 1 from Yeo et. al. 2011) and the motor seeds were based on a localizer task using motion of the tongue, hand and foot (see figure 20 from Yeo et. al. 2011). The seed-locations for the other four network; control, default, dorsal- and ventral-attention network are based on independent cluster analysis using resting data from two datasets of 500 subjects and confirmed using estimated from the second dataset (see table 5 from Yeo et. al. 2011). Below we list the MNI location for these seeds in the left hemisphere (for right hemisphere flip the sign of the x-coordinate):
control network: MNI(x,y,z) = (−40,50,7),( −43, −50,64),( −57, −50, −9),( −5,22,47),( −6,4,29),( −4, −76,45)
default network: MNI(x,y,z) = (−27,23,48),( −41, −60,29),( −64, −20, −9),( −7,49,18),( −25, −32, −18),( −7, −52,26)
dorsal attention network: MNI(x,y,z) = (−22, −8,54),( −34, −38,44),( −18, −69,51),( −51, −64, −2),( −8, −63,57),( −49,3,34)
motor network: MNI(x,y,z) = (−41, −20,62),( −6 −26 76),( −55, −4,26)
ventral attention network: MNI(x,y,z) = (−31,39,30),( −54, −36,27),( −60, −59,11),( −5,15,32),( −8, −24,39),( −31,11,8)
visual network: MNI(x,y,z) = (−12, −67, −3),( −3, −74,23),( −16, −74,7),( −23, −91, −15),( −32, −89, −1),( −13, −100, −8)
To define each network in each subject, we computed the correlation maps using the time course from spherical seeds with a 6mm radius based on coordinates in both left and right hemisphere, by multiplying the x-MNI coordinate with −1 (Biswal, Yetkin, Haughton, & Hyde, 1995). Each correlation map was transformed to a Z-score using Fisher’s r-to-Z transform. Next, we masked out spherical regions with 12mm radius to remove some of the autocorrelations immediately surrounding each seed. The resulting 12 correlation maps for each network (6 for the motor network) were averaged to obtain a single map for the control network (CON), default-mode network (DMN), dorsal attention network (DAN), motor network (MOT), ventral attention network (VAN) and visual network (VIS) for each subject (Yeo et al., 2011). Note, we have labeled these networks consistent with Yeo et. al. (2011), but the literature varies in the exact terminology (Seeley et al., 2007; Smith et al., 2009). For example the ventral attention networks is sometimes labeled “salience” network or the control network as “executive” network. Next, we conducted one sample t-tests across the subject-maps to obtain group-maps. The resulting T-maps were projected to the cortical surface using FreeSurfer (see Figure 2D), where the group maps are displayed using a “winner takes all” approach, eliminating overlap between the six intrinsic cortical networks and assigning each voxel to the network possessing the strongest correlation to the voxel’s time course. Note that these maps are shown with no threshold in order to demonstrate the strongest connectivity of each voxel.
In order to clarify the location of task-induced activity in relation to the intrinsic cortical networks we conducted the following analysis. First, we isolated the voxels as identified by group-level SPM analysis. Next, for a given cluster of voxels we computed the average connectivity using the individual’s mean connectivity z-maps. Note, that for each individual, the average connectivity of a cluster is based on the average of 12 seed-based maps (6 maps for the motor network) and is computed for each of the six cortical resting-state networks (CON, DMN, DAN, MOT, VAN and VIS). This results in six-cluster averaged connectivity values for each individual subject. Next, we conducted one-sample t-tests using the cluster-averaged z-scores to clarify if a given cluster was significantly overlapping one of six cortical networks (Tables 1–3). For illustration, we show the T-values from these one-sample t-tests using spider-plots (Figures 3–5). Note, each spider plot is scaled such that the center reflects the lowest (negative) value for each region. The statistical significance for each cluster of activity with each intrinsic cortical network is shown in Tables 1–3 (− = negative, # = trending, * = P < 0.05, ** = P < 0.005,*** = P < 0.001).
Table 3.
Performance (hit/miss) x Stage (encoding/retrieval): | ||||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Label | Side | BA | T | Size | X | Y | Z | Task-Based Effects | Resting-State Effects | Post-Hoc tests | ||||||||||
ENC | RET | ENC | RET | |||||||||||||||||
P | S | P x S | CON | DMN | DAN | MOT | VAN | VIS | FIX-HIT | HIT-FIX | MISS-HIT | HIT-MISS | ||||||||
Ventral Precuneus / PCC | L/R | 7/31 | 6.36 | 288 | −12 | −70 | 26 | 0.01 | 14.94 | 36.27 | 2.13 | 7.23 | 1.43 | −1.26 | −2.03 | 4.58 | 4.26 | 2.18 | 4.73 | 4.49 |
Ventral PPC | L | 7/39 | 4.31 | 9 | −36 | −67 | 44 | 2.51 | 32.77 | 15.25 | 9.72 | 7.42 | 1.08 | −3.31 | −7.02 | −7.32 | 1.76 | 3.18 | 1.04 | 3.91 |
Dorsal PPC | L | 40 | 3.88 | 31 | −39 | −49 | 47 | 1.70 | 13.25 | 19.72 | 15.98 | −4.69 | 13.29 | −4.01 | 2.88 | −6.14 | 1.44 | 7.21 | 1.05 | 3.93 |
Parahip. Crtx. | L | 35 | 4.43 | 6 | −21 | −34 | −13 | 5.24 | 1.94 | 17.94 | −2.50 | 6.78 | −2.83 | −4.23 | −4.72 | 2.12 | 1.02 | 2.72 | 1.19 | 4.49 |
Caudate | L | - | 4.48 | 6 | −9 | 8 | 10 | 2.08 | 10.09 | 21.68 | −4.48 | 1.43 | −3.88 | −3.55 | −3.56 | −1.43 | 1.74 | 3.40 | 1.05 | 4.16 |
Visual Crtx. | L | 18 | 4.47 | 25 | −12 | −73 | −4 | 7.67 | 14.26 | 19.07 | −3.36 | −8.04 | 0.30 | −0.97 | 0.09 | 15.16 | 4.84 | 7.01 | 0.94 | 4.24 |
Table shows regions that demonstrated an interaction between memory performance (hit/miss) and memory stage (encoding/retrieval). Outer-left column shows task-based analysis: BA = Brodmann Area, T-value of task-based effects; Size = cluster size in voxels, Coordinates X, Y, Z are in MNI space. Middle-left column shows F-values for the 2×2 post-hoc ANOVA for each cluster: P = memory performance (selected for by task-based analysis), S = memory stage, P x S = interaction. Significant F-values are indicated in bold. Middle-right column shows resting-state effects: CON = control network, DMN = default network, DAN = dorsal attention network, MOT = motor network, VAN = ventral attention network. VIS = visual network. Significant T-values are indicated in bold, negative values are listed in grey. Outer-right column shows post-hoc tests in the same regions for individual contrasts. Significant T-values are indicated in bold. From left to right: (1) baseline (BASE) – encoding (ENC) hits (HIT), (2) retrieval (RET) hits – baseline, (3) encoding hits – encoding misses (MISS) and (4) retrieval hits – retrieval misses.
Results
Behavioral results
During scanning, retrieval of the Pre-scan face-name pairs resulted in average of 81±2.6 hits 34±2.5 misses, while the novel pairs resulted in 17±1.8 false alarms and 126±2.6 correct rejections. During the Post-Scan session, retrieval of the Scan face/name pairs resulted in 63±3.1 its, 56±3.1 misses, while the novel pairs resulted 17±1.7 false alarms and 132±1.9 correct rejections. Memory performance (old vs. new), as defined by d-prime (d′) was 1.84±0.08 during the Scan session and 1.31±0.09 during the Post-Scan session A t-test between the d′ indicated that memory performance was better inside than outside the scanner (p < 0.001). During the Scan session, on average 13 out of 30 repaired items were correctly rejected, resulting in d′ of 0.50±0.04. In the Post-Scan session, 19 out of 30 repaired items were correctly rejected, resulting in a d′ of 0.40±0.10. A t-test between these “repaired” d′ scores indicated that performance for the repaired items was not significantly different inside and outside the scanner (p = 0.12). Note, that the repaired items were not included in the fMRI analysis and only constituted 10% of the trials. During the Scan session, the average response time for hits was 1669±36 ms, miss 1833±44 ms, false alarms 1952±60 ms, correct rejections 1589±33 ms, 1884±51 ms for correctly rejected repaired items and 1731±41 ms for missed repaired items. For During the Post-Scan session, the response time for hits was 1702±73 ms, miss 1678±91 ms, false alarms 1822±163 ms, correct rejections 1689±85 ms, 1987±124 ms for correctly rejected repaired items and 2091±139 ms for missed repaired items. Paired t-tests between the Scan and Post-Scan response times for hits, miss, false alarms or correct rejections indicated that the average response times were not significantly different between the Scan and Post-Scan sessions.
fMRI results
We used a 2×2 repeated-measures ANOVA of memory performance (hit/miss) and memory stage (encoding/retrieval) to identify brain regions associated with memory performance, memory stage and their interaction (the encoding/retrieval-flip) (Figure 2A–C). Within the 2×2 ANOVA we directly contrast the factors performance and stage. Therefore, activations isolated for unsuccessful performance (misses) are identical to deactivation isolated for successful performance (hits), and likewise for encoding/retrieval-activations and deactivations. For simplicity, we present our data in terms of effects of performance: successful memory performance (hit > miss), unsuccessful memory performance (miss > hits) and memory stage: memory retrieval (retrieval > encoding) and memory encoding (encoding > retrieval). To visualize the intrinsic resting-state networks, we calculated the membership of each voxel with the control, default, dorsal attention, motor, ventral attention and visual networks using a winner takes all approach (Figure 2D), based on the seeds published by Yeo et. al. (2011).
Effects of Memory Performance
First, the task-based analysis identified brain regions that showed an effect of memory performance (hit/miss), that is, regions that showed a significant effect as a function of successful versus unsuccessful memory performance (Figure 3). Several regions, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and anterior cingulate cortex / supplemental motor area (ACC/SMA) showed an effect of successful memory performance (hit > miss). See Table 1 for the full list of regions (P < 0.05, FDR corrected, cluster size > 5). These regions exhibited more activity when memory performance was successful versus unsuccessful. For each significant cluster of activity, we also conducted a second 2×2 post-hoc ANOVA using the average beta-values for encoding hit, encoding miss, retrieval hit and retrieval miss for each subject, in order to check if these regions demonstrated an effect of memory stage (encoding/retrieval) or an interaction between both, in addition to demonstrating effects of memory performance. We found that several regions showing an effect of performance, including the DLPFC and ACC/SMA simultaneously showed an effect of memory stage, particularly memory retrieval (retrieval > encoding; Table 1). The connectivity analysis indicated that the DLPFC and ACC/SMA were strongly associated with to the control network (Table 1). Pair-wise t-tests confirmed that the left DLPFC and ACC/SMA were more connected to control network than to other significantly connected networks, including ventral- and dorsal-attention networks (DLPFC: CON > DAN, P < 0.001; CON > VAN, P < 0.001), (ACC/SMA: CON > DAN, P < 0.001; DON > VAN, P < 0.001). However, many of the activated brain regions, including the DLPFC and ACC/SMA showed strong connectivity to multiple networks (Table 1).
The task-based analysis also identified brain regions that showed an effect of successful memory performance (miss > hit). The dorsal precuneus (another sub-region of the PMC), bilateral temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and superior prefrontal cortex (sPFC) displayed more activity during unsuccessful versus successful memory performance (Figure 3). See Table 1 for the full list of regions (P < 0.05, FDR corrected, cluster size > 5). For each significant cluster of activity, we conducted a 2×2 post-hoc ANOVA to evaluate whether or not these regions also showed an effect of memory stage or an interaction. We found that TPJ showed an effect of memory stage, in particular encoding (encoding > retrieval: Table 1). The connectivity analysis indicated that the dorsal precuneus, TPJ and superior PFC were all associated with the ventral attention network (Table 1). Pair-wise t-tests demonstrated that dorsal precuneus was more strongly connected to both the default-network and dorsal attention network (dorsal precuneus: VAN < DMN, P = 0.0047; VAN < DAN, P < 0.001), while connectivity between the default-network and dorsal attention network was not significantly different (DAN < DMN, P = 0.87). Both left and right TPJ showed the strongest connected to ventral attention network (right TPJ: VAN > DAN, P < 0.001, VAN > CON, P < 0.001), although for the left TPJ the connectivity difference between motor and ventral attention network was only trending (left TPJ: VAN > VIS, P < 0.001; VAN > MOT, P = 0.078). These results again demonstrate that many of the activated brain regions showed strong connectivity to multiple networks. In general, these findings demonstrate that effect of successful memory performance occurs in regions connected to the control network, while the effect of unsuccessful memory performance occurs in regions connected to the ventral attention network.
Effects of Memory Stage
Secondly, the task-based analysis identified brain regions that showed an effect of memory stage (encoding/retrieval); that is, regions that display a significant difference as a function of retrieval versus encoding (Figure 4). The ventral precuneus (a sub-region of the PMC), posterior parietal cortex (PPC), anterior prefrontal cortex (PFC), and anterior cingulate cortex / supplemental motor area (ACC/SMA) showed an effect of memory retrieval stage (retrieval > encoding). These regions exhibited more activity when retrieving previously seen information (old) than when encoding novel information (new). See Table 2 for the full list of regions (P < 0.05, FDR corrected, cluster size > 5). For each significant cluster of activity, we conducted a 2×2 post-hoc ANOVA to evaluate whether these regions also showed an effect of memory performance or an interaction. Several regions, including ventral precuneus and PPC, also showed an interaction with memory performance (Table 2). Several other regions, including the ACC/SMA, showed an effect of successful memory, but no interaction, in addition to the effect of memory retrieval. Connectivity analyses indicated that many of these regions, including the ventral precuneus, PPC, anterior PFC and ACC/SMA were strongly connected to the control network and default-network. Pairwise t-tests demonstrated that the PPC and ACC/SMA showed the greatest connectivity to the control network (left PPC: CON > DMN, P = 0.013; CON > DAN, P < 0.001; right PPC: CON > DMN, P < 0.001; CON > DAN, P < 0.001; ACC/SMA: CON > DAN, P < 0.001; CON > VAN, P < 0.001). In contrast, the left ventral precuneus was predominantly connected to the default-network (DMN > CON, P < 0.001).
Table 2.
Effect of memory retrieval (retrieval > encoding): | ||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Label | Side | BA | T | Size | X | Y | Z | Task-Based Effects | Resting-State Effects | |||||||
P | S | P x S | CON | DMN | DAN | MOT | VAN | VIS | ||||||||
Ventral Precuneus | L | 7 | 5.77 | 142 | −12 | −67 | 29 | 0.01 | 34.42 | 22.68 | 5.55 | 10.01 | −1.21 | −3.87 | −3.57 | −0.18 |
R | 7 | 3.57 | 6 | 12 | −70 | 26 | 0.09 | 13.47 | 23.31 | 0.92 | 3.91 | 4.65 | 0.14 | 0.95 | 3.51 | |
PPC | L | 7/40 | 5.73 | 201 | −42 | −61 | 44 | 2.37 | 38.12 | 13.33 | 13.51 | 7.52 | 3.78 | −4.23 | −7.87 | −7.68 |
R | 3.78 | 15 | 42 | −55 | 50 | 1.57 | 16.32 | 3.76 | 15.52 | 1.44 | 3.46 | −6.03 | −3.29 | −6.24 | ||
Anterior PFC | L | 9/10 | 5.16 | 91 | −39 | 44 | 8 | 5.20 | 34.66 | 5.40 | 11.60 | −2.88 | 2.54 | −1.86 | 3.51 | −4.69 |
ACC / SMA | L | 32 | 4.97 | 45 | −6 | 17 | 50 | 11.02 | 19.16 | 0.80 | 11.05 | −0.67 | 2.40 | −3.35 | 1.20 | −6.07 |
L | 24 | 3.97 | 11 | −6 | 32 | 35 | 1.61 | 16.32 | 1.36 | 4.44 | 2.80 | −1.81 | −5.08 | 6.96 | −4.93 | |
Premotor Crtx. | L | 6 | 4.99 | 114 | −33 | 2 | 62 | 10.06 | 31.00 | 1.56 | 8.43 | 6.63 | 1.53 | −2.10 | −6.37 | −8.77 |
Insula | L | 13/47 | 5.87 | 64 | −30 | 26 | −10 | 19.90 | 29.56 | 2.26 | 0.46 | −0.33 | −3.51 | −6.33 | 0.66 | −2.70 |
R | 13/47 | 4.49 | 28 | 33 | 29 | −4 | 6.90 | 20.84 | 0.36 | 5.60 | −6.39 | 0.20 | −7.09 | 3.89 | −2.04 | |
Caudate | L | - | 7.32 | 135 | −9 | 14 | −4 | 4.17 | 52.91 | 1.58 | −1.05 | −1.55 | −5.01 | −2.53 | 0.20 | −3.80 |
R | 5.85 | 98 | 12 | 11 | 2 | 3.18 | 38.37 | 1.86 | 1.07 | −2.49 | −3.71 | −3.45 | 0.07 | −3.71 | ||
Visual Crtx. | L | 18 | 4.32 | 60 | −12 | −79 | 2 | 4.07 | 17.25 | 11.92 | −3.20 | −8.18 | 0.50 | −1.28 | −0.40 | 16.13 |
Effect of memory encoding (encoding > retrieval): | ||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Label | Side | BA | T | Size | X | Y | Z | Task-Based Effects | Resting-State Effects | |||||||
P | S | P x S | CON | DMN | DAN | MOT | VAN | VIS | ||||||||
Superior PFC | L | 9 | 4.25 | 15 | −33 | 38 | 35 | 16.95 | 22.27 | 0.28 | 1.91 | −4.93 | 5.61 | −2.04 | 11.44 | −1.94 |
Superior Temp. Crtx. | R | 22 | 4.19 | 19 | 57 | −58 | 8 | 1.08 | 20.69 | 1.25 | −2.01 | −2.82 | 1.98 | −1.29 | 5.05 | −1.32 |
Premotor Crtx. | R | 6 | 4.85 | 9 | 60 | 5 | 35 | 0.66 | 18.11 | 2.06 | −0.52 | −5.33 | 8.14 | 7.14 | 5.44 | −1.82 |
Caudate | L | - | 4.09 | 18 | −23 | 11 | 22 | 0.17 | 14.39 | 0.93 | −1.87 | 1.97 | −2.39 | 2.56 | −1.93 | 0.47 |
Visual Crtx. | L/R | 18 | 5.52 | 308 | 18 | −91 | 14 | 2.42 | 31.03 | 1.17 | −4.46 | −7.48 | −1.01 | −3.20 | −2.09 | 16.68 |
Top panel demonstrates effects of memory retrieval (retrieval > encoding), lower panel effect of memory encoding (encoding > retrieval). Left column shows task-based analysis: BA = Brodmann Area, T-value of task-based effects; Size = cluster size in voxels, Coordinates X, Y, Z are in MNI space. Middle column shows F-values for the 2×2 post-hoc ANOVA for each cluster: P = memory performance (selected for by task-based analysis), S = memory stage, P x S = interaction. Significant F-values are indicated in bold. Right column shows resting-state effects: CON = control network, DMN = default network, DAN = dorsal attention network, MOT = motor network, VAN = ventral attention network. VIS = visual network. Significant T-values are indicated in bold, negative values are listed in grey.
The task-based analysis also identified brain regions that showed an effect of memory encoding stage (encoding > retrieval) (Figure 4). The visual cortex and left superior PFC displayed more activity during memory encoding versus retrieval (Figure 2). See Table 2 for the full list of regions (P < 0.05, FDR corrected, cluster size > 5). For each significant cluster of activity, we conducted a 2×2 post-hoc ANOVA to evaluate whether regions also showed an effect of memory performance or an interaction (Table 2). The superior PFC also showed an effect of unsuccessful memory performance. Next, the connectivity analysis of the activated clusters indicated that the superior PFC showed strong connectivity to both dorsal and ventral attention networks. Pair-wise t-tests demonstrated that the superior PFC was predominantly connected to the ventral attention network (superior PFC: VAN > DAN, P < 0.001; VAN > CON, P < 0.001), while the region in the visual cortex showed was predominantly connectivity to the visual network (Table 2). In general, these findings indicate that a number of brain-regions activated by memory retrieval are connected to both the control network and default network.
Interactions between memory performance and memory stage
Finally, the task-based analysis also identified brain regions that showed an interaction between memory performance and memory stage (Figure 5, Table 3) including the ventral precuneus / posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) (another sub-region of the PMC), left dorsal and ventral posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and parahippocampal cortex. Note, that for each cluster of activity, we tested the direction of interaction. All clusters demonstrated more activity during encoding, and less activity during retrieval. No cluster of activity that survived the threshold (P < 0.05, FDR corrected) demonstrated the reverse interaction. For each significant cluster of activity, we conducted a 2×2 post-hoc ANOVA to evaluate whether or not the regions also showed a main effect of memory performance or memory stage. In addition to the interaction, the ventral precuneus/PCC, and the dorsal and ventral PPC showed an effect of memory retrieval, while the parahippocampal cortex at the same time showed an effect of successful memory performance. Note, that these effects are included for completeness and should be interpreted cautiously because of the interaction. To identify if the location of the interaction overlapped with the original definition of the E/R-flip (Daselaar et al., 2009), we conducted a control analysis defining the E/R-flip as the conjunction between encoding miss – encoding hit (P < 0.05, FDR corrected) and retrieval hit – retrieval miss (P < 0.05, FDR corrected). Two cluster in the ventral precuneus survived this strict conjunctive threshold: (left precuneus: cluster size = 37, T-max (encoding) = 5.16, MNI(x,y,z) (encoding) = −16, −70,26, T-max (retrieval) = 4.63, MNI(x,y,z) (retrieval) = −13, −70,29; right precuneus: cluster size = 3, T-max (encoding) = 3.91, MNI(x,y,z) (encoding) = 14, −73,29, T-max (retrieval) = 3.99, MNI(x,y,z) (retrieval) = 14, −73,29). Furthermore, the maxima as identified by interaction (MNI(x,y,z) =12, −70,26) overlapped with the conjunction analysis, indicating the interaction within the ventral precuneus / posterior cingulate cortex was driven by the encoding/retrieval-flip (see also post-hoc tests Table 3).
Next, the connectivity analysis indicated that many of these brain regions, including the ventral precuneus/PCC, ventral PPC and parahippocampal cortex, showed strong connectivity to the default network (Table 3). Pair-wise t-tests demonstrated that connectivity of the ventral precuneus was stronger to the default-network than control network (DMN > CON, P = 0.009), but not significantly different between visual and default-network (DMN > VIS, P = 0.13). The ventral PPC demonstrated connectivity to both control and default-network, and also, this connectivity was not significantly different (DMN > CON, P = 0.70). The parahippocampal cortex was predominantly connected to the default-network (DMN > VIS, P = 0.0014). The dorsal PPC was not significantly connected to the default network, but instead was similarly connected to the control network and dorsal attention network (CON > DAN, P = 0.88). Similar to the effects of performance and stage, almost all of the activated clusters demonstrating an interaction were connected to multiple cortical networks. Below, we discuss these neuroimaging results in relation to the encoding/retrieval flip
Discussion
In this event-related fMRI study, we identified activity patterns related to memory performance (hit/miss), memory stage (encoding/retrieval) and their interaction. Using resting-state functional connectivity analysis, we examined the intrinsic networks associated with regions showing each activity pattern. First, we found an effect of successful memory performance in brain regions connected to the control network and an effect of unsuccessful memory performance in regions connected to the ventral attention network. Second, we found an effect of memory retrieval in regions spanning both control and default networks. Third, we found an interaction between memory performance and memory stage in regions associated with the default network, a pattern related to the encoding/retrieval flip (E/R-flip).
Effects of memory performance
A set of regions, most notably the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), demonstrated an effect of successful memory performance (Figure 3). These findings are coherent with neuroimaging studies that link these regions to cognitive control (Niendam et al., 2012). The left DLPFC also showed a significant effect of memory retrieval, which corresponds to a recent meta-analysis comparing encoding and retrieval (Spaniol et al., 2009). The overlap in the DLPFC between both effects of successful memory performance and memory retrieval is consistent with a critical role in episodic memory. Clinical studies indicate that cortical damage or transcranial magnetic stimulation to the DLPFC can result in memory impairments (Alexander, Stuss, & Fansabedian, 2003; Rossi et al., 2001). The connectivity analysis indicates that the left DLPFC was predominantly connected to the control network. However, the DLPFC also showed strong connectivity to both the ventral and dorsal attention networks. Thus, the DLPFC appears to be functionally connected to multiple cortical networks, and as such, may integrate information across the dorsal, ventral and control networks.
A second set of regions, most notably the dorsal precuneus and temporoparietal junction (TPJ), demonstrated an effect of unsuccessful memory performance (Figure 3). These findings are consistent with neuroimaging studies linking task-induced deactivations to successful cognitive performance (Huijbers, Pennartz, Rubin, & Daselaar, 2011; Mazoyer et al., 2001; McKiernan, Kaufman, Kucera-Thompson, & Binder, 2003). The TJP also showed a significant effect of memory encoding, again in line with a recent meta-analysis (Spaniol et al., 2009). The overlap between both effects of unsuccessful memory performance and memory encoding within the TPJ also suggest that this region plays an important role in episodic memory. However, it remains unclear if the effect of performance should be interpreted in terms of beneficial deactivations or detrimental activations (Daselaar et al., 2004; Otten & Rugg, 2001). In line with the latter “detrimental” interpretation, TPJ activity is sometimes interpreted in terms of bottom-up distractions (Corbetta, Patel, & Shulman, 2008; Weissman et al., 2006). The connectivity analysis indicated that the TPJ was strongly connected to the ventral attention network, suggestive of a bottom-up function. In contrast, the dorsal precuneus also showed an effect unsuccessful memory performance, but, unlike the TPJ, did not show an effect of memory stage. The connectivity analysis also indicated that the dorsal precuneus was more connected to the default and dorsal attention networks. Together, these findings are consistent with recent studies that found encoding-related deactivations in both the TPJ and the default network (Anticevic et al., 2010) and supports the idea that effects of performance are not restricted to regions within the default network (Spreng, 2012).
Effects of memory stage
A third set of brain regions, most notably the ventral precuneus and posterior parietal cortex (PPC) demonstrated an effect of memory retrieval (Figure 4), displaying more activity during retrieval compared to encoding. These findings converge with fMRI studies comparing previously seen (old) to novel (new) information (Hayama, Vilberg, & Rugg, 2012; Spaniol et al., 2009; Wagner, Shannon, Kahn, & Buckner, 2005). We also found some regions that displayed more activity during encoding compared to retrieval, an effect of memory encoding, (Figure 4, Table 2). The connectivity analysis indicated that many of the regions showing an effect of retrieval were connected to the control and default networks. Note that although we used a face-name paradigm, the task included few repaired items, making it essentially a recognition paradigm. Memory performance – as defined by d-prime (d′) – was very high for distinguishing old versus new pairs, but relatively poor for discriminating repaired items (see results). Accordingly, in the fMRI analysis, we excluded repaired items. Thus, the effects of memory stage are likely to reflect neurocorrelates related to recognition memory (familiarity), rather then associative memory retrieval (recollection). This interpretation is supported by the fMRI results, since the effect of retrieval seems to occur in brain regions commonly linked to familiarity (Vilberg & Rugg, 2007). Similar co-activations in regions spanning the control and default networks have recently been reported in studies of autobiographical memory retrieval (Gerlach, Spreng, Gilmore, & Schacter, 2011; Spreng et al., 2010). Of importance, we demonstrate that several regions in the default network, including the ventral precuneus and PPC, demonstrate not only an effect of memory retrieval, but also an interaction between memory performance and memory stage. This finding indicates that activity in parietal regions is likely to reflect distinct cognitive processes operating during the encoding and retrieval stages. The interaction demonstrates that the effects of memory stage in the ventral precuneus and PPC cannot be interpreted in terms of encoding/retrieval alone.
Interactions between performance and stage
Several regions demonstrated an interaction between memory performance and memory stage, including the ventral precuneus/PCC, the dorsal and ventral PPC and parahippocampal cortex (Figure 5). Most previous memory studies tested encoding and retrieval stages in separate experimental sessions (see however Gilbert et al., 2011) and modeled the effects of both memory stages separately. The current design measured encoding and retrieval simultaneously and this allows simultaneous modeling of effects of performance (hit/miss) and stage (encoding/retrieval). As a consequence, the E/R-flip - as defined by the interaction - cannot be explained by shifts in the fMRI baseline (Gusnard & Raichle, 2001; Stark & Squire, 2001). Note, that previous studies E/R-flip studies vary in terms of the contrasts used to define the E/R-flip, either using hits versus misses (Daselaar et al., 2009) or contrasting hits versus fixation (Vannini et al., 2011). Results between these studies also vary in terms of locating an E/R-flip in the PPC or medial PFC. Yet, across studies, the E/R-flip was consistently reported in the ventral precuneus/PCC (Huijbers et al., 2012). The control analysis (see results), also demonstrated, that only ventral precuneus/PCC shows an E/R-flip, when defined by a whole-brain conjunction (Daselaar et al., 2009). Post-hoc t-tests further confirmed that the ventral precuneus/PCC -as identified by the interaction- demonstrated an E/R-flip regardless of the exact contrast-definition (Table 3). The connectivity analysis indicated that the ventral precuneus/PCC and ventral PPC were connected to the default network, confirming previous studies that combined task and resting-state fMRI data (Huijbers, Pennartz, Cabeza et al., 2011). Together these findings provide strong support for the hypothesis that the E/R-flip represents an interaction between performance related deactivations during encoding and activations during retrieval that both occur within the posteromedial and possibly other default-mode regions.
In addition to the interaction, the ventral precuneus/PCC and ventral PPC also showed an effect of memory retrieval. These findings may have consequences for understanding the contribution of the parietal cortices to episodic memory. Typical activation patterns observed in the parietal cortices – when contrasting previously seen (old) and novel (new) information – should be interpreted in terms of successful and unsuccessful encoding and retrieval processes. One recent hypothesis suggests that the E/R-flip in the ventral PPC can be explained in terms of reorienting by bottom-up attention to memory (AtoM) (Cabeza, Ciaramelli, Olson, & Moscovitch, 2008; Ciaramelli, Grady, & Moscovitch, 2008). The bottom-up attention, as described by AtoM, hypothesizes that during encoding, external to-be encoded information is typically in the focus of attention and reorienting is likely to coincide with distraction by unrelated stimuli or internal thoughts (Cabeza, Ciaramelli, & Moscovitch, 2012). Yet, during retrieval, successful performance requires that attention is reoriented from external cues guiding the task toward internal information retrieved from episodic memory. The E/R-flip may therefore reflect the interaction between activation driven by external sources that are beneficial during encoding, but detrimental during retrieval. This would account for our observed interaction between memory performance and memory stage. However, our findings also indicate that the TPJ, the region most commonly linked to bottom-up attention (Corbetta et al., 2008), showed an effect of unsuccessful memory performance during both memory stages and did not exhibit an interaction. Thus, our findings dissociate the TPJ from the dorsal and ventral PPC consistent with a review of encoding-processes in the parietal cortices (Uncapher & Wagner, 2009). However, the current results also demonstrate how subtle this dissociation may be, since the TPJ and ventral PCC show a distinct pattern during retrieval, but a similar pattern during encoding. Whether the AtoM account can be extended to the medial parietal cortices (ventral precuneus/PCC), or if another cognitive process such as self-referential processes can explain the E/R-flip in the ventral precuneus/PCC remains a topic of current debate (Huijbers et al., 2012).
We also found an interaction in the parahippocampal cortex, a finding not previously reported (e.g. Daselaar et al., 2009). The hippocampus - in contrast to the parahippocampus - is known to show patterns of activity distinct from the E/R-flip, as it is commonly not deactivated during memory encoding (Huijbers, Pennartz, Cabeza et al., 2011; Vannini et al., 2011). Although functional connectivity studies have demonstrated connectivity between the medial temporal lobe and the default network (Greicius, Krasnow, Reiss, & Menon, 2003; Kahn, Andrews-Hanna, Vincent, Snyder, & Buckner, 2008), recent results indicate that this connectivity is mediated via the parahippocampal cortex (Libby, Ekstrom, Ragland, & Ranganath, 2012; Ward et al., In Press). In line with this possibility, the connectivity analysis confirmed that the location of the interaction effect within the parahippocampus was predominantly connected to the default network (Figure 5). The interaction with the parahippocampus is also consistent with the hypothesis that the hippocampus is coupled with the default network during memory retrieval but not during memory encoding (Huijbers, Pennartz, Cabeza et al., 2011). The parahippocampus also showed connectivity with the visual network. These data suggest that the parahippocampus could mediate the effective connectivity between the hippocampus and the visual or default network during encoding and retrieval. For example, the parahippocampus could gate hippocampal activity so that it is associated with the visual cortex during memory encoding, but with the default network during retrieval. Future studies should explore the dynamics of parahippocampal connectivity, since a loss of medial temporal lobe connectivity is believed to underlie memory problems associated with Alzheimer’s’ Disease (Allen et al., 2007; Greicius, Srivastava, Reiss, & Menon, 2004; Hedden et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2006). In sum, the interaction demonstrates that within several brain regions, including the PMC and PPC, a simple contrast between old versus new-items should be interpreted with caution, as activity is dependent on both memory stage and memory performance.
Task-induced activity versus resting-state functional connectivity
In general our findings indicate that task-induced activity is not restricted within the boundaries of intrinsic cortical networks as identified by resting-state functional connectivity. Rather, task-induced activity often occurs in brain regions spanning multiple cortical networks (Tables 1–3). Other recent task-based fMRI studies have also reported task-based activity outside of the boundaries of a single cortical network (Anticevic et al., 2010; Spreng et al., 2010). This mismatch between task-based activity and functional connectivity might be fundamental to the underlying neuronal processes. Resting-state functional connectivity is primarily driven by slow periodic fluctuations in the BOLD signal (Biswal et al., 1995). Moreover, these fluctuations likely reflect an indirect measure for anatomical connectivity, as they are restrained by the anatomy of the brain (Fox & Raichle, 2007; Van Dijk et al., 2010). Task-evoked changes in BOLD signal are believed to reflect the brain’s metabolic response to the activity of populations of neurons and correlate with local field potentials (Logothetis, Pauls, Augath, Trinath, & Oeltermann, 2001; Ogawa, Lee, Kay, & Tank, 1990). Thus, the mismatch between task-based activity and resting-state functional connectivity might simply reflect task-induced BOLD activity versus coherent slow-periodic changes in BOLD signal that occur throughout large networks. However, this does not imply that cognitive processes do not alter the connectivity of large-scale intrinsic cortical networks. The connectivity of cortical networks can be detected within task-based fMRI data and may be modulated by cognitive processes (Albert, Robertson, Mehta, & Miall, 2009; Andrews-Hanna, Reidler, Huang et al., 2010; Niazy, Xie, Miller, Beckmann, & Smith, 2011). Yet, if we assume that functional connectivity primarily reflects the underlying anatomy of the brain, our findings may simply imply that task-induced effects are likely to occur in regions situated in-between or associated with multiple large-scale networks.
Conclusion
In this study we identified the neural correlates of memory performance (hit/miss) and memory stage (encoding/retrieval), the interaction between these processes, and the relationship to large-scale intrinsic networks identified during the resting state. First, we found an effect of successful memory performance in brain regions associated with the control network, and an effect of unsuccessful memory performance in brain regions associated with the ventral attention network. Second, we found an effect of memory retrieval in brain regions that tend to span the default network and control network. Third, we found an interaction between memory performance and memory stage in regions associated with the default network. Together, these findings demonstrate that the task-based effects of memory performance and memory stage dissociate regions connected to the default network and occur in regions that span multiple networks.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the European Molecular Biology Organization: ALTF 318-2011 [W.H], the Marie Curie Fellowship: FP7-PEOPLE-2007-4-1-IOF from the European Union [P.V], the Swedish Brain Foundation and Swedish Society for Medicine [P.V], the National Institutes of Health: K01 AG040197 [T.H.], K24 AG035007 [R.S.], R01 AG027435-S1 [R.S], P01AG036694 [R.S], P50AG00513421 [R.S], and the Alzheimer’s Association: IIRG-06-27374 [R.S]. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official view of the NIH.
References
- Albert NB, Robertson EM, Mehta P, Miall RC. Resting state networks and memory consolidation. Commun Integr Biol. 2009;2(6):530–532. doi: 10.4161/cib.2.6.9612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander MP, Stuss DT, Fansabedian N. California Verbal Learning Test: performance by patients with focal frontal and non-frontal lesions. Brain. 2003;126(Pt 6):1493–1503. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen G, Barnard H, McColl R, Hester AL, Fields JA, Weiner MF, et al. Reduced hippocampal functional connectivity in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2007;64(10):1482–1487. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.10.1482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Huang C, Buckner RL. Evidence for the default network’s role in spontaneous cognition. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(1):322–335. doi: 10.1152/jn.00830.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron. 2010;65(4):550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anticevic A, Repovs G, Shulman GL, Barch DM. When less is more: TPJ and default network deactivation during encoding predicts working memory performance. Neuroimage. 2010;49(3):2638–2648. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1995;34(4):537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10(4):433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Moscovitch M. Cognitive contributions of the ventral parietal cortex: an integrative theoretical account. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Olson IR, Moscovitch M. The parietal cortex and episodic memory: an attentional account. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(8):613–625. doi: 10.1038/nrn2459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciaramelli E, Grady CL, Moscovitch M. Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: a hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(7):1828–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58(3):306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale AM, Greve DN, Burock MA. Optimal Stimulus sequences for Event-Related fMRI. June 11–16, 1999; Paper presented at the 5th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain; Duesseldorf, Germany. 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Damoiseaux JS, Prater KE, Miller BL, Greicius MD. Functional connectivity tracks clinical deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(4):828, e819–830. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daselaar SM, Prince SE, Cabeza R. When less means more: deactivations during encoding that predict subsequent memory. Neuroimage. 2004;23(3):921–927. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daselaar SM, Prince SE, Dennis NA, Hayes SM, Kim H, Cabeza R. Posterior midline and ventral parietal activity is associated with retrieval success and encoding failure. Front Hum Neurosci. 2009;3:13. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.013.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Rajendran N, Busa E, Augustinack J, Hinds O, Yeo BT, et al. Cortical folding patterns and predicting cytoarchitecture. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(8):1973–1980. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(9):700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerlach KD, Spreng RN, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Solving future problems: Default network and executive activity associated with goal-directed mental simulations. Neuroimage. 2011;55(4):1816–1824. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert SJ, Armbruster DJN, Panagiotidi M. Similarity between Brain Activity at Encoding and Retrieval Predicts Successful Realization of Delayed Intentions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;0(0):1–13. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(1):253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V. Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(13):4637–4642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2(10):685–694. doi: 10.1038/35094500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayama HR, Vilberg KL, Rugg MD. Overlap between the Neural Correlates of Cued Recall and Source Memory: Evidence for a Generic Recollection Network? J Cogn Neurosci. 2012 doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayden BY, Smith DV, Platt ML. Electrophysiological correlates of default-mode processing in macaque posterior cingulate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(14):5948–5953. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812035106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedden T, Van Dijk KR, Becker JA, Mehta A, Sperling RA, Johnson KA, et al. Disruption of functional connectivity in clinically normal older adults harboring amyloid burden. J Neurosci. 2009;29(40):12686–12694. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3189-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbers W, Pennartz CM, Cabeza R, Daselaar SM. When learning and remembering compete: a functional MRI study. PLoS Biol. 2009;7(1):e11. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbers W, Pennartz CM, Cabeza R, Daselaar SM. The Hippocampus Is Coupled with the Default Network during Memory Retrieval but Not during Memory Encoding. PLoS One. 2011;6(4):e17463. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbers W, Pennartz CM, Rubin DC, Daselaar SM. Imagery and retrieval of auditory and visual information: neural correlates of successful and unsuccessful performance. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(7):1730–1740. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbers W, Vannini P, Sperling RA, CMP, Cabeza R, Daselaar SM. Explaining the encoding/retrieval flip: Memory-related deactivations and activations in the posteromedial cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(14):3764–3774. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutchinson JB, Uncapher MR, Wagner AD. Posterior parietal cortex and episodic retrieval: convergent and divergent effects of attention and memory. Learn Mem. 2009;16(6):343–356. doi: 10.1101/lm.919109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahn I, Andrews-Hanna JR, Vincent JL, Snyder AZ, Buckner RL. Distinct cortical anatomy linked to subregions of the medial temporal lobe revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100(1):129–139. doi: 10.1152/jn.00077.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim H. Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: a meta-analysis of 74 fMRI studies. Neuroimage. 2011;54(3):2446–2461. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim H, Daselaar SM, Cabeza R. Overlapping brain activity between episodic memory encoding and retrieval: roles of the task-positive and task-negative networks. Neuroimage. 2010;49(1):1045–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konishi S, Wheeler ME, Donaldson DI, Buckner RL. Neural correlates of episodic retrieval success. Neuroimage. 2000;12(3):276–286. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leech R, Kamourieh S, Beckmann CF, Sharp DJ. Fractionating the default mode network: distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. J Neurosci. 2011;31(9):3217–3224. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5626-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Libby LA, Ekstrom AD, Ragland JD, Ranganath C. Differential connectivity of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices within human hippocampal subregions revealed by high-resolution functional imaging. J Neurosci. 2012;32(19):6550–6560. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3711-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logothetis NK, Pauls J, Augath M, Trinath T, Oeltermann A. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature. 2001;412(6843):150–157. doi: 10.1038/35084005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazoyer B, Zago L, Mellet E, Bricogne S, Etard O, Houde O, et al. Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Res Bull. 2001;54(3):287–298. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(00)00437-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKiernan KA, Kaufman JN, Kucera-Thompson J, Binder JR. A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15(3):394–408. doi: 10.1162/089892903321593117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minear M, Park DC. A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2004;36(4):630–633. doi: 10.3758/bf03206543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niazy RK, Xie J, Miller K, Beckmann CF, Smith SM. Spectral characteristics of resting state networks. Prog Brain Res. 2011;193:259–276. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00017-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niendam T, Laird A, Ray K, Dean Y, Glahn D, Carter C. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;12(2):241–268. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0083-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogawa S, Lee TM, Kay AR, Tank DW. Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1990;87(24):9868–9872. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.24.9868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otten LJ, Rugg MD. When more means less: neural activity related to unsuccessful memory encoding. Curr Biol. 2001;11(19):1528–1530. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(01)00454-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi S, Cappa SF, Babiloni C, Pasqualetti P, Miniussi C, Carducci F, et al. Prefrontal [correction of Prefontal] cortex in long-term memory: an “interference” approach using magnetic stimulation. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4(9):948–952. doi: 10.1038/nn0901-948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Buckner RL, Koutstaal W, Dale AM, Rosen BR. Late onset of anterior prefrontal activity during true and false recognition: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 1997;6(4):259–269. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci. 2007;27(9):2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shrager Y, Kirwan CB, Squire LR. Activity in both hippocampus and perirhinal cortex predicts the memory strength of subsequently remembered information. Neuron. 2008;59(4):547–553. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, et al. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(31):13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spaniol J, Davidson PS, Kim AS, Han H, Moscovitch M, Grady CL. Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(8–9):1765–1779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sperling RA, Bates JF, Chua EF, Cocchiarella AJ, Rentz DM, Rosen BR, et al. fMRI studies of associative encoding in young and elderly controls and mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003;74(1):44–50. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.1.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sperling RA, Bates JF, Cocchiarella AJ, Schacter DL, Rosen BR, Albert MS. Encoding novel face-name associations: A functional MRI study. Human Brain Mapping. 2001;14(3):129–139. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN. The fallacy of a “task-negative” network. Front Psychol. 2012;3:145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, Stevens WD, Chamberlain JP, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. Neuroimage. 2010;53(1):303–317. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark CE, Squire LR. When zero is not zero: the problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(22):12760–12766. doi: 10.1073/pnas.221462998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uncapher MR, Wagner AD. Posterior parietal cortex and episodic encoding: insights from fMRI subsequent memory effects and dual-attention theory. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;91(2):139–154. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Dijk KR, Hedden T, Venkataraman A, Evans KC, Lazar SW, Buckner RL. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(1):297–321. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vannini P, Hedden T, Huijbers W, Ward AM, Johnson KA, Sperling RA. The Ups and Downs of the Posteromedial Cortex: Age- and Amyloid-Related Functional Alterations of the Encoding/Retrieval Flip in Cognitively Normal Older Adults. Cerebral Cortex. 2012 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vannini P, O’Brien J, O’Keefe K, Pihlajamaki M, Laviolette P, Sperling RA. What goes down must come up: role of the posteromedial cortices in encoding and retrieval. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(1):22–34. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilberg KL, Rugg MD. Dissociation of the neural correlates of recognition memory according to familiarity, recollection, and amount of recollected information. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(10):2216–2225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner AD, Shannon BJ, Kahn I, Buckner RL. Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(9):445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang L, Zang Y, He Y, Liang M, Zhang X, Tian L, et al. Changes in hippocampal connectivity in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2006;31(2):496–504. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward AM, Schultz AP, Huijbers W, van Dijk RA, Hedden T, Sperling RA. Resting-State Default Mode Network Connectivity in the Medial Temporal Lobe is Distinct from Hippocampal Regions Supporting Successful Memory Formation. Human Brain Mapping (In Press) [Google Scholar]
- Weissman DH, Roberts KC, Visscher KM, Woldorff MG. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(7):971–978. doi: 10.1038/nn1727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]