Skip to main content
Translational Psychiatry logoLink to Translational Psychiatry
. 2013 Jul 9;3(7):e280. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.54

CD4+ but not CD8+ T cells revert the impaired emotional behavior of immunocompromised RAG-1-deficient mice

L Rattazzi 1, G Piras 1, M Ono 2, R Deacon 3, C M Pariante 4, F D'Acquisto 1,*
PMCID: PMC3731786  PMID: 23838891

Abstract

An imbalanced immune system has long been known to influence a variety of mood disorders including anxiety, obsessive-compulsive disorders and depression. In this study, we sought to model the impact of an immunocompromised state on these emotional behaviors using RAG-1−/− mice, which lack T and B cells. We also investigated the relative contribution of CD4+ or CD8+ T cells to these manifestations using RAG-1−/−/OT-II and RAG-1−/−/OT-I transgenic mice, respectively. Our results show that RAG-1−/− mice present a significant increase in digging and marble-burying activities compared with wild-type mice. Surprisingly, these anxiety-like behaviors were significantly reverted in RAG-1−/−/OT-II but not RAG-1−/−/OT-I transgenic mice. Immunodepletion experiments with anti-CD4 or anti-CD8 in C57/BL6 mice or repopulation studies in RAG-1−/− mice did not reproduce these findings. Microarray analysis of the brain of RAG-1−/− and RAG-1−/−/OT-II mice revealed a significantly different gene fingerprint, with the latter being more similar to wild-type mice than the former. Further analysis revealed nine main signaling pathways as being significantly modulated in RAG-1−/− compared with wild-type mice. Taken together, these results suggest that life-long rather than transient immunodeficient conditions influence the emotional behaviors in mice. Most interestingly, these effects seem to correlate with a specific absence of CD4+ rather than CD8+ T cells. Validation of these findings in man might provide new clues on the mechanism by which early life immune modulation might impact mood response in adults and provide a further link between immune and emotional well-being.

Keywords: CD4 T cells, CD8 T cells, immunodeficient mice, OT-I and OT-II transgenic mice, RAG knockout mice

Introduction

A correlation between mental diseases and immune dysfunction has been reported and debated in the literature since the late 1980s.1 Indeed, direct and indirect evidences in both human and animal experimental systems indicate that changes in the immune repertoire significantly influence cognitive functions2, 3 and neurodegeneration.4, 5, 6 More recent studies also suggest that a healthy immune system is a prerequisite for a balanced and functional emotional response.7, 8, 9, 10

The link between emotion and immunity has been documented in a variety of studies addressing psychosocial changes in patients treated with immunosuppressive drugs. Cyclosporine, a drug widely used in organ transplantation, has been shown to induce a range of neuropsychological problems ranging from depression to anxiety.11, 12, 13, 14, 15 Similarly, other studies described psychological side effects like anxiety, depression and obsessive-compulsive disorders in patients treated with a variety of structurally unrelated immunosuppressant including methotrexate,16 azathioprine,17 and chemotherapy.18

The recombination-activating gene RAG-1 encodes proteins necessary for immunoglobulin and T-cell receptor gene recombination. RAG-1-deficient mice have small lymphoid organs that do not contain mature B and T lymphocytes.19 Seminal work on RAG-1−/− mice by Cushman and co-workers20 reported an increased locomotor activity, reduced levels of fearfulness and no differences in spatial learning and memory. However, given the shared expression of the RAG-1 protein by lymphocytes and central nervous system tissues, the authors concluded their seminal paper stating: ‘Whether these changes are due to the loss of RAG-1 gene expression in the brain, the result of the absence of the RAG-1 gene in the immune system or some combination of both effects remains to be determined in future research'.

To address this question, McGowan et al.21 used a very elegant approach to assess the function of RAG-1 in the central nervous system and dissect it from the lack of T and B cells in the periphery.21 The authors compared the behavior of RAG-1 toRAG-2-deficient mice and found an impaired social recognition memory in the first but not the latter. Because both lines are immunodeficient and RAG-2 is not expressed in the brain, the authors claimed a specific function of RAG-1 in controlling memory formation.

In this study, we expanded on these notions and explored first if the immunodeficient state of RAG-1−/− mice had any influence on their emotional behavior and second the specific contribution of CD4+ or CD8+ T cells to these changes using RAG-1−/−/OT-II and the RAG-1−/−/OT-I transgenic lines, respectively. Second, we explored possible changes in mood-modifying circulating factors or gross brain structural differences in these mice. The results obtained suggest that CD4+ but not CD8+ T cells are capable of partially reverting anxiety-like behavior and lack of self-care characteristic of immunodeficient mice. Interestingly, these behavioral changes were mirrored in specific brain gene fingerprint. These unexpected new findings might provide a mechanistic explanation for the increased emotional distress observed in patients suffering from a wide variety of immune disorders.

Materials and methods

Mice

We used 5-week-old male mice for all the experiments. Mice were housed in groups of six per cage under specific pathogen-free conditions and with free access to food and water. Mice were housed for at least 7 days before testing. Wild-type (WT) C57BL/6 mice purchased from Charles River (Manston, UK) or RAG-1+/+ littermate were used as control. B6.SJL-Ptprca Pepcb/BoyJ-Tg(TcraTcrb)1100Mjb/J-B6.129S7-Rag1tm1Mom (RAG−/−/OT-I) and B6.SJL-Ptprca Pepcb/BoyJ-Tg(TcraTcrb)425Cbn/J-B6.129S7-Rag1tm1Mom (RAG−/−/OT-II) mice were kindly provided by Professor Hans Stauss (University College London, London, UK) and bred in our animal facility. Apart from the nest construction test, all experiments were performed during the light phase of the light–dark cycle, and no more than two tests per day were performed. All tests were conducted in a blinded manner and according to the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986.

Flow cytometric analysis

Thymocytes and lymphocytes were stained in 100 μl of fluorescence-activated cell sorting buffer (phosphate-buffered saline containing 5% fetal calf serum and 0.02% of NaN2). The antibodies used were anti-CD3 phycoerythrin (clone 145-2C11), anti-CD4 fluorescein isothiocyanate (clone GK 1.5), anti-CD8 Cy5 (clone 53-6.7) (all from eBioscience, San Diego, CA, USA). Cells were labeled with the appropriate concentration of conjugated antibodies for 1 h at 4 °C as described previously.22 After labeling, cells were washed and analyzed. In all experiments, stained cells were acquired with FACScalibur flow cytometer and analyzed using the FlowJoTM software (Tree Star, Ashland, OR, USA, Oregon Corporation).

In vivo T-cell depletion

Male C57/BL6 mice (6 weeks old) received an intraperitoneal injection of anti-CD4 (250 μg; clone GK1.5; BioLegend) or anti-CD8 (250 μg; clone 53-6.7; BioLegend, San Diego, CA, USA) or immunoglobulin G control. T-cell immunodepletion was verified by staining peripheral blood mononuclear cells at different time points (Day 2, Day 5 and Day 7) after the treatment. Briefly, blood samples were collected by intracardiac puncture in syringes containing sodium citrate 3.2% (w v−1). The cells were centrifuged to pellet at 300 g and resuspended in fluorescence-activated cell sorting buffer containing 1:500 Fc blocking (anti-mouse CD16/32) and stained with anti-CD4 or anti-CD8. Red blood cells were lysed with RBC Lysis Buffer according to the manufacturer's instruction (eBioscience).

RAG1−/− repopulation studies

Purified CD4 or CD8 T cells were obtained from male C57/BL6 mice (6 weeks old) by negative selection following the manufacturer's instructions (Dynabeads® Untouched™ Mouse CD8 Cells and Dynabeads® Untouched™ Mouse CD4 Cells; Invitrogen, Invitrogen Life Technologies Ltd, Paisley, UK). Purity was tested by fluorescence-activated cell sorter and was >98%. Cells were resuspended in phosphate-buffered saline (2 × 106/300 l) and transferred into male RAG1−/− mice (6 weeks old) by intraperitoneal injection.

Digging and marble-burying tests

Marble-burying and digging tests were carried out as described previously23 with some modifications. Briefly, mice were individually placed in a clear plastic box (14 cm × 10 cm × 11 cm) filled with approximately 5-cm-deep wood chip bedding lightly pressed to give a flat surface. The same bedding substrate was used for all the mice and flattened after each test. Fifteen glass marbles were placed on the surface in five rows of three marbles each. The latency to start digging, the number of digging bouts and the number of buried marbles (to 2/3 their depth) were recorded during the 15-min test. Two trials were performed, the second trial taking place 24 h after the first trial.

Open field activity test

The open filed test was performed as described previously with some modifications.24 The open field consisted of a white PVC arena (50 cm × 30 cm) divided into 10 cm × 10 cm squares (n=15). Mice were brought into the experimental room 15 min before testing. Each mouse was placed in one of the corner squares facing the wall, observed and recorded for 5 min. The total number of squares crossed, latency to the first rear and the total number of rears were recorded. After each test, the arena was cleaned with water to attenuate and homogenize olfactory traces. Two trials were performed, the second trial taking place 24 h after completion of the first trial.

Nest construction test

The nest construction test was performed as described previously.25 Mice were transferred into individual cages 1 h before the dark phase (1700 hours) and individually housed overnight. The results were assessed the next morning. Food, water and wood chip bedding were provided. No other environmental enrichment was added. One 2–3 g, 5 cm × 5 cm pressed cotton square (nestlet; Ancare, Ancare Bellmore, NY, USA) was placed in each cage. The weight of nesting material shredded was calculated by weighing the nestlet before and after the overnight test. The quality of the nest was evaluated on a five-point scale as detailed in Deacon.25

Plasma corticosterone and cytokine measurement

Blood was collected by intracardiac puncture performed under anesthesia. Serum was obtained from the clotted blood by centrifugation (8000 r.p.m., 5 min) and stored at −80 °C before the assay. Corticosterone concentrations were measured in diluted (1:32) plasma by EIA assay following the manufacturer's instructions (Enzo Life Sciences, Exeter, UK). Cytokine levels in the same samples were measured (dil. 1:100) using mouse T-helper type 1 (Th1)/Th2/Th17/Th22 16plex Kit Flowcytomix and according to the manufacturer's instructions (eBioscience).

Histology

Brains were collected either before or at the end of experiments and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 72 h. Thereafter, tissues were sectioned on a sagittal or coronal plane and embedded in paraffin by our in-house histology facility. Sections (5 μm) were deparaffinized and stained with hematoxylin and eosin. In all cases, a minimum of three sections per animal was evaluated. Phase-contrast digital images were taken using the Image Image-Pro (Media Cybernetics, Rockville, MD, USA) analysis software package.

Microarray analysis

Total RNA was extracted from brains of WT (n=3), RAG−/− (n=2) and OT-II/RAG−/− (n=2) mice using RNeasy® Microarray Tissue Mini Kit (Qiagen®, West Sussex, UK). Total RNA was hybridized to Affymetrix Mouse Gene 1.0 ST array chips at UCL Genomics (London, UK) with standard Affymetrix protocols, using GeneChip Fluidics Station 450, and scanned using the Affymetrix GeneChip Scanner (Affymetrix, Santa Clara, CA, USA). Data were normalized by rma of the Bioconductor package, affy. Differentially expressed genes were identified by the Bioconductor package, limma, considering the false discovery rate (adjusted P-value <0.05). The gene and sample scoring system was made by canonical correspondence analysis. Canonical correspondence analysis is a variant of correspondence analysis, where the main data are linearly regressed onto explanatory variables (environmental variables), and subsequently the regressed data are analyzed by correspondence analysis. In this study, we regressed the whole data set onto an explanatory variable, which was defined as the difference between ‘average' WT and ‘average' RAG-1−/−. Detailed methodology is described elsewhere.26 Signaling pathway impact analysis was performed using the Bioconductor package, SPIA, by comparing WT and RAG-1−/−.

Real-time polymerase chain reaction

Total RNA was extracted from whole brains (n=6 for each mouse line) with RNeasy® Microarray Tissue Mini Kit (Qiagen®) according to the manufacturer's protocol and reverse transcribed using 2 μg oligo(dT)15 primer, 10 U AMV reverse transcriptase, 40 U RNase inhibitor (all from Promega Corporation, Madison, WI, USA) and 1.25 mℳ each dNTP (Bioline, London, UK) for 45 min at 42 °C. Real-time polymerase chain reaction was carried out by using ABsoluteTM QPCR ROX Mix (Thermo Scientific, Epsom, UK) and fluorescent QuantiTect primers. Cycling conditions were set according to the manufacturer's instructions. Sequence-specific fluorescent signal was detected by 7900HT Fast Real-Time PCR System (Applied Biosystems, Warrington, Cheshire, UK). mRNA data were normalized relative to glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase and then used to calculate expression levels. We used the comparative Ct method to measure the gene transcription in samples. The results are expressed as relative units based on calculation of 2−ΔΔCt, which gives the relative amount of gene normalized to endogenous control (glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase) and to the sample with the lowest expression set as 1.

Data analysis

Initially, we determined if the data distribution was parametric. Pairwise comparisons were made by t-test and the results expressed as mean±s.e.m. For non-parametric data, the Mann–Whitney U-test was applied, and results were expressed as medians (interquartile range).

Results

Immune repertoire of RAG-1−/− , RAG-1−/− /OT-I and RAG-1−/− /OT-II

Crossing OT-I and OT-II TCR transgenic mice onto RAG-1−/− background generates mice with a single population of mature CD8+ or CD4+ T cells. Figure 1 shows a typical immature and mature T-cell profiles of RAG-1−/−, RAG-1−/−/OT-I and RAG-1−/−/OT-II compared with WT C57BL/6 control mice. Control mice show a typical profile with a 1:2 ratio of CD8+ and CD4+ single-positive T cells in the thymus as well as in the periphery (Figure 1, first top and bottom panels, respectively). As expected, the majority of RAG-1−/− thymocytes are CD4 and CD8 double-negative cells and have no mature CD4 or CD8 single-positive T cells in the periphery (Figure 1, second top and bottom panels, respectively).19 The presence of OT-I and OT-II TCR transgene overcomes the block at the stage of double-negative 3 (DN3) of RAG-1−/− thymocytes and allows the generation of a peripheral T-cell immune repertoire constituted by 72% of CD8+ in RAG-1–/–/OT-I and 65% of CD4+ T cells RAG-1–/–/OT-II mice (Figure 1, third top and bottom panels).27

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Immune repertoire of RAG-1−/−, RAG-1−/−/OT-I and RAG-1−/−/OT-II mice. Thymocytes and lymphocytes from male wild-type (WT), RAG-1−/−, RAG-1−/−/OT-I and RAG-1−/−/OT-II mice were analyzed for CD4 and CD8 expression. The dot plots show the T-cell profiles of RAG-1−/−, RAG-1−/−/OT-I and RAG-1−/−/OT-II mice in the thymus (upper panel) and in the periphery (lower panel) compared with WT C57BL/6 control mice. The percentage of the cells in each quadrant is given.

Increased digging and marble-burying behavior of RAG-1−/− mice

We first investigated differences in anxiety- and obsessive-compulsive-like behavior in WT and RAG-1−/− mice using the digging and marble-burying tests. As shown in Figure 2a, RAG-1−/− mice presented a significant increase (two- to threefold) in the number of digging bouts and buried marble compared with WT mice. The latency to dig was higher in control mice compared with RAG-1−/− mice, although the difference was not significant.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Increased digging and marble-burying behavior of RAG-1−/− mice (upper panel). The bar graphs in a shows the total number of digging bouts, buried marbles and the latency to dig (expressed in seconds) measured during the 15min marble-burying test. The bar graphs in b show the total number of rears, the latency to rear (expressed in seconds) and the total number of squares crossed assessed during the 5-min open field test. Values are expressed as mean±s.e.m. of six mice and are representative of n=3–4 separate experiments. **P<0.01 and ***P<0.001 indicate significant values compared with wild-type (WT) C57BL/6 control mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

To further corroborate the anxiety-like behavior and simultaneously rule out possible intrinsic impairment in locomotor activity, we used the open field activity test. Here, we considered a number of parameters including exploration (number of rears and latency) and locomotor activity (number of squares crossed). Although RAG-1−/− mice showed a slight reduction compared with the mice in all the parameters observed, these differences were not statistically significant (Figure 2b).

CD4+ but not CD8+ T cells revert the increased digging and marble-burying behavior of RAG-1−/− mice

We next tested the hypothesis that the presence of CD4+ or CD8+ T cells might influence the heightened digging and marble-burying behavior of the RAG-1−/− mice. RAG-1–/–/OT-I showed no difference in either number of bouts, buried marbles or latency compared with RAG-1−/− mice (Figure 3a). In contrast, RAG-1–/–/OT-II behaved differently from RAG-1−/− mice, showing a significant reduction in the number of bouts (about 25%) and buried marbles (about 20%), and almost doubled latency time (P<0.05; Figure 3b).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

CD4+ but not CD8+ T cells revert the increased digging and marble-burying behavior of RAG-1−/− mice. The bar graphs show the total number of digging bouts, buried marbles and the latency to dig (expressed in seconds) in RAG-1−/−/OT-I (a) or RAG-1−/−/OT-II (b) compared with RAG-1−/− during the 15-min marble-burying test. Values are expressed as mean±s.e.m. of six mice and are representative of n=3–4 separate experiments. **P<0.01 indicates significant values compared with RAG-1−/− mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

With the open field test, we observed no difference in the behavior of RAG-1–/–/OT-I and RAG-1–/– (Figure 4a). Similarly, RAG-1–/–/OT-II showed no significant difference in either the number of rears or of squares crossed as compared with RAG-1−/−, except for an increase in the latency, which this time reached a statistical significance (Figure 4b). When we compared the number of center entries (considered an anxiety-like behavior parameter) in the three lines, we observed a significant reduction in RAG-1−/− mice compared with WT mice. However, this difference was not significantly reverted in either RAG-1−/−/OT-I or RAG-1−/−/OT-II (Figure 5).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

No differences in the open field activity between RAG-1−/−/OT-I, RAG-1−/−/OT-II and RAG-1−/−. The bar graphs show the total number of rears, the latency to rear (expressed in seconds) and squares crossed in RAG-1−/−/OT-I (a) or RAG-1−/−/OT-II (b) compared with RAG-1−/− during the 5-min open field test. Values are expressed as mean±s.e.m. of six mice and are representative of n=3–4 separate experiments. *P<0.05 indicates significant values compared with RAG-1−/− mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

CD4+ but not CD8+ T cells might revert the decreased number of center entries showed by RAG-1−/− mice. The bar graphs show the comparison of the entries into the center between RAG-1−/− and wild-type mice (left panel), RAG-1−/− and RAG−/−/OT-I (middle panel) and RAG−/− and RAG−/−/OT-II (right panel) during the 5-min open field test. Values are expressed as mean±s.e.m. of six mice and are representative of n=3–4 separate experiments. **P<0.01 indicates significant values compared with wild-type C57BL/6 control mice (Mann–Whitney U-test).

Transient depletion of CD4+ or CD8+ T cells does not affect the emotional behavior of C57/BL6 mice

We next wondered if we could reproduce the results obtained with RAG-1–/–/OT-I and RAG-1–/–/OT-II using anti-CD4- or anti-CD8-depleting antibodies in C57/BL6 mice. As shown in Figure 6, neither anti-CD4 nor anti-CD8 antibodies significantly modified the digging, or marble-burying activities or the latency to digging (top, middle and bottom panels, respectively) compared with control IgG-treated mice at the indicated times.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Depletion of CD4 or CD8 T cells does not induce anxiety-like behavior in C57/BL6 mice. C57/BL6 mice received an intraperitoneal injection of anti-CD4 (250 μg), anti-CD8- (250 μg) depleting antibodies or IgG control, and then tested in the digging and marble-burying test. The bar graphs show the total number of digging bouts, buried marbles and the latency to dig (expressed in seconds) in mice treated as indicated and assessed before the treatment (day 0) or after 2, 5 and 7 days (day 2, day 5 and day 7, respectively). Values are expressed as mean±s.e.m. of 6–8 mice.

To further confirm these results, we sought to investigate if the repopulation of RAG-1−/− with T cells would rescue the increased anxiety observed in these mice. Consistent with previous results, reconstitution of RAG-1−/− mice with purified CD4 or CD8 T cells did not affect the increase in the number of digging bouts compared with vehicle phosphate-buffered saline-injected mice (Figure 7).

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Repopulation of RAG1−/− mice with CD4 or CD8 T cells does not affect their anxiety-like behavior. RAG1−/− mice received an intraperitoneal injection of purified CD4 (2 × 106) or CD8 (2 × 106) T cells and then tested in the digging and marble-burying test. The bar graphs show the total number of digging bouts, buried marbles and the latency to dig (expressed in seconds) in mice assessed before the cell transfer (day 0) or after 4 and 7 days (day 4 and day 7, respectively). Values are expressed as mean±s.e.m. of 6–8 mice.

CD4+ T cells revert the impaired nest construction of RAG-1−/− mice

To explore the impact of T cells on other emotional behavior, we tested the three transgenic lines for their nesting activity, a standard test for measuring activities of daily living. Figure 8a shows representative pictures of the results obtained, whereas in Figure 8b, we report the quantitative results. Measurement of nestlet shredding showed a decreased ability of RAG-1−/− mice to perform this task compared with WT mice (Figure 8b). Similar to previous analysis, this impaired behavior was significantly reverted in RAG-1−/−/OT-II but not RAG-1−/−/OT-I mice (Figure 8b). Comparable results were obtained scoring nest quality (Supplementary Figure 1).

Figure 8.

Figure 8

CD4+ but not CD8+ T cells revert the impaired nest construction of RAG-1−/− mice. (a) Representative pictures of the nestlet shredding activity of wild-type (WT), RAG-1−/−, RAG-1−/−/OT-I and RAG-1−/−/OT-II during an overnight test. (b) Quantitative analysis of nestlet shredding activity expressed as grams of nestlet shredded after an overnight test. Values are expressed as mean±s.e.m. of six mice and are representative of n=3–4 separate experiments. §P<0.05 and **P<0.01 indicate significant values compared with WT C57BL/6 control and RAG−/− mice, respectively (Mann–Whitney U-test).

No differences in systemic or gross brain structure between RAG-1−/− , RAG-1–/– /OT-I and RAG-1–/– /OT-II mice

We next investigated whether the behavioral changes observed were due to changes in known behavioral modulating factors such as corticosterone. As shown in Figure 9a, there were no significant differences in the levels of circulating corticosterone between WT and RAG-1−/− mice or between RAG-1−/− and RAG-1−/−/OT-I and RAG-1−/−/OT-II mice.

Figure 9.

Figure 9

No differences in corticosterone or inflammatory cytokine serum levels between RAG-1−/−, RAG-1–/–/OT-I and RAG-1–/–/OT-II mice. Levels of corticosterone (a) or interleukin (IL)-17, IL-18 and interferon (IFN)-γ (b) in the serum of wild-type (WT), RAG-1−/−, RAG-1−/−/OT-I and RAG-1−/−/OT-II. Values are expressed as ng ml−1 or as pg ml−1 and are cumulative of n=2–3 experiments.

Cytokines can induce behavioral changes (also known as sickness behavior28), a consequence of their modulatory effects on brain function. When we scanned the same samples for classical Th1, Th2, Th17 and ThGM-CSF cytokines, only interleukin (IL)-17, IL-18 and interferon-γ could be detected. However, none of these mediators was differentially modulated in RAG-1−/− and RAG-1−/−/OT-I and RAG-1−/−/OT-II mice compared with WT mice (Figure 9b), excluding the possibility that cytokines released by T cells or a latent state of infection as being responsible for the behavioral changes observed.

Similarly, analysis of general brain morphology and architecture of WT, RAG-1−/−, RAG-1−/−/OT-I and RAG-1−/−/OT-II mice showed comparable hematoxylin–eosin (Figures 10a–d, respectively) or luxol fast blue staining (data not shown) ruling out any contribution of infiltrated immune cells or general neuronal defect in the altered emotional behavior of tested mice.

Figure 10.

Figure 10

No differences in gross brain structure between RAG-1−/−, RAG-1–/–/OT-I and RAG-1–/–/OT-II mice. The pictures show the coronal hematoxylin and eosin -stained sections of brain from wild-type (a), RAG-1−/− (b), RAG-1−/−/OT-I (c) and RAG-1−/−/OT-II (d). The higher magnification represents the hippocampal area. The figures are representative of n=3–4 mice.

Brain gene fingerprint of RAG-1−/− and RAG-1−/− /OT-II

To unveil cellular and molecular mechanisms potentially responsible for the observed changes in emotional behavior, we took an unbiased approach and compared whole brain gene fingerprint of WT, RAG-1−/−, RAG-1−/−/OT-II mice. The flowchart in Figure 11a summarizes the results of this analysis. From the 34 760 probes present in the chip, 6635 were significantly modulated (P<0.05). This corresponded to 782 differentially expressed genes with a fold change (FC) value <−1 or >1, and 111 of them were annotated genes (genes with Entrez ID.)

Figure 11.

Figure 11

Heatmap and Canonical Correspondence Analysis on Microarray data of brain from wild-type (WT), RAG-1−/− and RAG-1–/–/OT-II mice. (a) Genes were filtered by a moderated t-statistics and fold change (FC). The heatmap analysis used annotated genes only (genes with EntrezID). (b) Hierarchical clustering and heatmap analysis of the filtered genes. RAG-1−/− samples showed a distinct cluster. (c) Similarity analysis for the features of RAG-1−/− in comparison with WT. Note that RAG-1−/−/OT-II mice had low scores and were equivalent to WT mice.

Hierarchical clustering and heatmap analysis of these selected 111 genes and brain samples (Figure 11b and Table 1) showed that RAG-1−/− mice showed a distinct pattern of gene expression compared with RAG-1−/−/OT-II and WT mice. This result also suggested that RAG-1−/−/OT-II was similar to WT at the transcriptomic level. To further determine whether and how much RAG-1−/−/OT-II was more similar to WT than RAG-1−/−, we analyzed the similarities between the samples based on the gene expression pattern, focusing on the difference between WT and RAG-1−/−, which is our major interest in this study (see Materials and methods). The similarity analysis using these 111 genes showed that RAG-1−/−/OT-II was far more similar to WT than RAG-1−/− (Figure 11c). Importantly, the result of this similarity analysis was very similar using all the differentially expressed genes (data not shown), indicating that the result is robust and not dependent on the selection of genes by FC and annotation. These results were compatible with those of the behavioral analysis.

Table 1. Top-ranked genes (n=111) for the features of RAG-1−/− brain in comparison with wild-type.

Gene symbol Probe ID EntrezID RAG score (gene score) FC (RAG-WT) FC (RAG-OTII) P-value adj.RAG vs WT P-value adj.RAG vs OTII
Mageb16 1 054 5096 71 967 −2.040851531 −1.382 −2.412 0.080489 0.022479
Vmn2r42 1 055 9883 22 310 −1.93209053 −2.152 −1.793 0.021505 0.020203
Mir300 10 398 412 723 833 −1.88308172 −1.677 −2.085 0.017506 0.016937
9530091C08Rik 10 586 718 320 440 −1.794710154 −1.667 −2.619 0.104888 0.032618
Phxr4 10 583 203 18 689 −1.697861707 −1.052 −2.198 0.236372 0.040404
Mir539 10 398 418 723 917 −1.68592755 −1.867 −1.625 0.032518 0.037203
A130014H13Rik 10 402 560 319 630 −1.666278107 −1.767 −2.305 0.029595 0.018413
Mir380 10 398 388 723 859 −1.579352318 −1.198 −1.896 0.039846 0.018076
Mir487b 10 398 416 723 940 −1.515646393 −1.591 −1.606 0.029403 0.025115
Gm9911 10 578 017 10 010 1427 −1.495726048 −1.097 −1.857 0.171881 0.045496
Tbrg3 10 430 929 21 378 −1.480314578 −1.246 −1.955 0.059237 0.02181
Mir323 10 398 390 723 839 −1.459997834 −1.189 −1.849 0.084182 0.027581
Mir680-2 10 602 221 751 551 −1.436650249 −1.111 −2.109 0.136616 0.028476
Mir665 10 398 338 751 555 −1.426506241 −1.668 −1.866 0.0336 0.023984
Mir376b 10 398 408 723 934 −1.390698725 −1.236 −1.411 0.034845 0.023984
Krtap2-4 10 390 913 71 453 −1.383924657 −1.713 −2.176 0.049788 0.026248
F830001A07Rik 10 586 722 320 055 −1.360180695 −1.16 −1.984 0.170697 0.043923
Mir382 10 398 420 723 912 −1.358674308 −1.517 −1.887 0.029403 0.01938
Mir154 10 398 428 387 172 −1.302334846 −1.148 −1.479 0.022707 0.016937
Gm10048 10 529 953 625 026 −1.295861539 −1.173 −1.861 0.06009 0.02181
Mir329 10 398 392 723 842 −1.295645605 −1.373 −1.719 0.043559 0.024869
Ephb1 10 596 115 270 190 −1.28329288 −1.144 −1.534 0.044139 0.022479
D230041D01Rik 10 548 727 1 000 38615 −1.249725567 −1.103 −1.502 0.050443 0.023984
Mir679 10 398 396 751 539 −1.239666734 −1.275 −1.743 0.092767 0.038914
Mir344 10 564 235 723 931 −1.227043517 −1.01 −1.991 0.109837 0.023058
4931406H21Rik 10 413 216 77 592 −1.20459512 −1.236 −1.209 0.030727 0.027375
Mir9-1 10 493 191 387 133 −1.188744017 −1.132 −1.618 0.04865 0.02181
Mir341 10398350 723 846 −1.185006997 −1.323 −2.14 0.109543 0.032187
Snord95 10 375 501 1 002 16540 −1.140100065 −1.165 −1.229 0.049459 0.037703
Tra2a 10 544 638 101 214 −1.085592515 −1.154 −1.518 0.05765 0.027615
Gm5887 10 542 834 545 893 −1.068780678 −1.213 −1.068 0.023897 0.024869
Snord37 10 365 003 100 217 454 −1.052485206 −1.061 −0.627 0.017506 0.02181
Mir543 10 398 400 723 881 −1.046459099 −1.036 −1.118 0.021685 0.017462
Myh9 10 430 245 17 886 −0.951892542 −1.048 −0.884 0.021822 0.021872
Il11ra1 10 504 106 16 157 −0.935378718 −1.033 −1.167 0.02919 0.021613
Vwf 10 541 910 22 371 −0.913653403 −1.116 −1.213 0.024473 0.019653
Malat1 10 465 244 72 289 −0.881007189 −1.116 −1.756 0.136615 0.04157
Celsr3 10 589 130 107 934 −0.876228519 −1.171 −1.127 0.02257 0.019787
Dock6 10 591 614 319 899 −0.837490045 −1.295 −1.147 0.014287 0.016937
Dock6 10 591 630 319 899 −0.808120768 −1.142 −0.959 0.021097 0.018349
Dock6 10 591 612 319 899 −0.805918085 −1.083 −1.032 0.021505 0.017355
Trank1 10 589 761 320 429 −0.803294001 −1.008 −1.337 0.062753 0.029244
Pkd1 10 442 495 18 763 −0.798266404 −1.173 −1.073 0.020247 0.016937
Snhg11 10 478 073 319 317 −0.792852186 −1.038 −0.89 0.03061 0.03547
Fn1 10 355 403 14 268 −0.75577708 −1.188 −1.149 0.018698 0.016937
Leng8 10 549 615 232 798 −0.755496969 −1.053 −1.088 0.029574 0.024595
Mll2 10 432 298 381 022 −0.679798747 −1.088 −0.886 0.017506 0.016937
Erdr1 10 608 711 170 942 −0.516986696 −1.069 −0.546 0.019742 0.035677
Mkks 10 488 048 59 030 0.457287879 1.043 0.341 0.022707 0.18131
Dynlt1c 10 548 785 100 040 563 0.459997269 1.004 0.654 0.017506 0.01714
Rpl10a 10 355 173 19 896 0.485867583 1.117 0.793 0.021854 0.029315
Nsa2 10 411 363 59 050 0.552510543 1.192 0.814 0.018273 0.020521
Atp6v1f 10 536 895 66 144 0.573664963 1.027 1.039 0.021685 0.018076
Rpsa 10 385 034 16 785 0.573730493 1.054 0.675 0.017506 0.017355
Plrg1 10 492 757 53 317 0.578165545 1.006 0.899 0.017506 0.016937
Hsd17b10 10 602 592 15 108 0.594214684 1.012 0.98 0.021685 0.018382
Snrpd2 10 367 073 107 686 0.601279671 1.014 0.881 0.021505 0.019125
Ly6c2 10 429 573 100 041 546 0.616556062 1.066 0.885 0.024294 0.027767
Bloc1s1 10 373 594 14 533 0.639650101 1.016 1.1 0.021993 0.017462
Rpp38 10 479 749 227 522 0.640345029 1.128 0.834 0.017506 0.018347
Wdr61 10 593 740 66 317 0.668426554 1.012 1.07 0.022823 0.01851
Cd53 10 501 063 12 508 0.668970684 1.139 0.627 0.021505 0.040581
Tm4sf1 10 498 273 17 112 0.675247588 1.016 0.827 0.017506 0.016937
Rpl29 10 505 090 19 944 0.686092788 1.1 1.086 0.030792 0.026878
Adh5 10 496 475 11 532 0.693727155 1.039 1.025 0.017802 0.016937
Nkain4 10 490 551 58 237 0.698189047 1.015 1.213 0.021505 0.016937
Rpl11 10 454 097 67 025 0.70163422 1.163 1.003 0.039409 0.046635
Rpl11 10 517 457 67 025 0.706880397 1.009 0.903 0.021685 0.020078
Ndufa2 10 458 386 17 991 0.706987075 1.075 1.157 0.017506 0.016937
Trim13 10 415 784 66 597 0.708945695 1.285 0.768 0.021505 0.034966
Rpl10a 10 443 360 19 896 0.711172011 1.402 1.194 0.021505 0.018349
Tspan6 10 606 609 56 496 0.71734053 1.097 1.001 0.017506 0.016937
Slc39a12 10 469 389 277 468 0.724078447 1.08 1.054 0.018698 0.016937
Txndc9 10 396 064 98 258 0.737153463 1.094 1.077 0.021505 0.017462
Kcnj13 10 356 403 100 040 591 0.746292444 1.237 1.062 0.017506 0.016937
S100a10 10 493 995 20 194 0.749982194 1.295 1.167 0.017506 0.016937
Trappc1 10 377 508 245 828 0.753853858 1.109 1.257 0.022707 0.017462
Ly6c1 10 429 568 17 067 0.755009369 1.168 1.228 0.032378 0.025501
Slc19a3 10 356 145 80 721 0.76123195 1.042 0.765 0.02554 0.037851
Bloc1s1 10 457 924 14 533 0.763087509 1.124 1.198 0.021505 0.016937
Tmem100 10 380 285 67 888 0.763927693 1.001 0.715 0.021589 0.026199
P2ry13 10 498 367 74 191 0.766062492 1.278 0.969 0.017506 0.017355
Hddc2 10 362 394 69 692 0.799560244 1.01 1.071 0.021505 0.016937
Sncg 10 418 921 20 618 0.812109509 1.236 1.33 0.011381 0.012815
Churc1 10 396 694 211 151 0.830334656 1.017 1.336 0.037868 0.021562
Scrg1 10 571 865 20 284 0.838580022 1.22 1.228 0.025882 0.022109
Rpl29 10 490 824 19 944 0.8434953 1.052 1.155 0.039564 0.028208
Hsd11b1 10 361 234 15 483 0.850759562 1.097 1.145 0.017506 0.016937
Snrpd2 10 498 595 107 686 0.851183191 1.075 1.138 0.026256 0.021562
Pgcp 10 423 556 54 381 0.868297012 1.256 1.28 0.017506 0.016937
Tpmt 10 409 021 22 017 0.88208296 1.027 1.412 0.021822 0.016937
Cd63 10 367 436 12 512 0.888313271 1.17 1.461 0.022707 0.016937
5730469M10Rik 10 419 082 70 564 0.895272141 1.268 1.91 0.03237 0.017355
Ly86 10 404 606 17 084 0.897001413 1.274 1.271 0.021685 0.018349
Akr1b10 10 537 157 67 861 0.902194098 1.175 1.305 0.021505 0.016937
Lgals1 10 425 161 16 852 0.912182029 1.268 1.398 0.021505 0.016937
N6amt2 10 420 385 68 043 0.91317463 1.241 1.256 0.021685 0.018347
Arhgdib 10 548 892 11 857 0.929045264 1.003 1.328 0.058924 0.027787
Ly6a 10 429 564 110 454 0.94058187 1.438 1.499 0.025555 0.021402
Fcer1g 10 360 070 14 127 0.963847748 1.2 1.306 0.055767 0.039132
Akr1c18 10 407 435 105 349 0.964325333 1.047 0.762 0.024294 0.036537
Serpinb9 10 404 429 20 723 0.966649984 1.023 0.968 0.017506 0.016937
Rfc4 10 438 690 106 344 0.99077814 1.162 1.23 0.020247 0.016937
Serpinb1a 10 408 557 66 222 1.057850202 1.247 1.558 0.017506 0.013705
Gstk1 10 537 712 76 263 1.150030127 1.592 1.697 0.021505 0.016937
Rpl11 1 050 2745 67 025 1.167088017 1.307 1.558 0.037379 0.023823
Stxbp4 1 038 9795 20 913 1.173500167 1.086 1.948 0.091593 0.023603
Rpl11 1 045 1301 67 025 1.243624445 1.26 1.699 0.053335 0.025053
Ppia 1 054 5337 268 373 1.52055618 1.462 1.698 0.034726 0.023458
Mela 1 058 2545 17 276 3.221879948 4.675 2.776 0.002256 0.016937
Rpl36 10 378 783 54 217 3.792674002 2.358 3.245 0.109543 0.045167

Abbreviations: FC, fold change; WT, wild type.

The table shows the identity and statistical values of the top-ranked genes that were used for heatmap analysis (Figure 9b) and calculating the similarity score (RAG score) for the RAG-1−/− phenotype compared with the wild-type one (Figure 9c). Genes are ordered according to their relative contributions to the RAG score (i.e. the association with RAG-1−/− or WT; positive values indicate association with RAG-1−/−, whereas negative ones indicate association with WT). See Figure 9a for how genes were selected.

Probe ID is affymetrix ID.

FC is logged value.

Pathway analysis provided further clues on the main differences between WT and RAG-1−/−. Using a third-generation pathway analysis approach,29 nine pathways were identified as been significantly modulated in RAG-1−/− compared with WT: two being activated (Parkinson's disease and RNA transport) and seven inhibited (Huntington's diseases, Alzheimer's disease, extracellular matrix–receptor interaction, olfactory transduction, focal adhesion, calcium signaling and small-cell lung cancer; summarized in Table 2 and reported singularly in Supplementary Figures 2–10).

Table 2. Signaling pathway impact analysis of RAG-1−/− brain.

Name ID pSize NDE pNDE tA pPERT pG pGFdr pGFWER Status KEGGLINK
Parkinson's disease 5012 117 62 4.21E−19 4.097 0.2 3.78E−18 4.73E−16 4.73E−-16 Activated http://www.genome.jp/dbget-bin/show_pathway? mmu05012+66725+22202+22195+57320+68943+13063+104130+17991+17992+17993+225887+226646+227197+230075+407785+54405+595136+66046+66108+66218+66495+67130+67184+67264+67273+68198+68349+69875+72900+66925+66945+66152+66576+66594+66694+67003+67530+110323+12858+12859+12865+12866+12868+12869+20463+66142+75483+11949+11950+11957+228033+28080+67126+67942+71679+11739+11740+22334+22335+140499+67128+214084
Huntington's disease 5016 172 77 9.54E−18 −0.372 0.797 3.07E−16 1.92E−14 3.84E−14 Inhibited http://www.genome.jp/dbget-bin/show_pathway?mmu05016+104130+17991+17992+17993+225887+226646+227197+230075+407785+54405+595136+66046+66108+66218+66495+67130+67184+67264+67273+68198+68349+69875+72900+66925+66945+66152+66576+66594+66694+67003+67530+110323+12858+12859+12865+12866+12868+12869+20463+66142+75483+11949+11950+11957+228033+28080+67126+67942+71679+15194+15182+433759+12914+328572+20020+20022+231329+66420+67710+69241+69920+14810+14812+108071+18798+16438+11739+11740+22334+22335+13063+72504+21780+12757+381917+68922+12913
Alzheimer's disease 5010 164 70 5.83E−15 −1.6765 0.558 1.12E−13 4.66E−12 1.40E−11 Inhibited http://www.genome.jp/dbget-bin/show_pathway?mmu05010+66340+11487+13063+104130+17991+17992+17993+225887+226646+227197+230075+407785+54405+595136+66046+66108+66218+66495+67130+67184+67264+67273+68198+68349+69875+72900+66925+66945+66152+66576+66594+66694+67003+67530+11949+11950+11957+228033+28080+67126+67942+71679+110323+12858+12859+12865+12866+12868+12869+20463+66142+75483+20192+16438+16439+18798+14810+14811+14812+12288+12289+15108+14102+18125+78943+11816+19059+16971+14433+234664
ECM–receptor interaction 4512 84 28 0.000175059 −13.7865 5.00E−06 1.91E−08 5.98E−07 2.39E−06 Inhibited http://www.genome.jp/dbget-bin/show_pathway?mmu04512+16772+16774+16775+16776+16779+226519+23928+12814+12825+12826+12827+12830+12833+12835+12842+12843+245026+81877+22371+20750+14268+21825+15529+20971+15530+16400+19699+11603
RNA transport 3013 161 56 2.67E−08 0.7715 0.271 1.43E−07 3.57E−06 1.79E−05 Activated http://www.genome.jp/dbget-bin/show_pathway?mmu03013+433702+68092+56698+53975+69731+237221+20901+66069+60365+192170+56215+56009+237082+71805+114671+408191+72124+69912+54563+77595+107939+227720+20610+22218+13627+97112+66235+68969+53356+54709+56347+68135+78655+70047+102614+117109+208366+227522+54364+66161+67053+69961+386612+66231+26905+67204+218693+13681+13682+218268+230861+217869+108067+13667+209354+69482
Small-cell lung cancer 5222 86 22 0.026137813 −20.07421825 5.00E−06 2.20E−06 4.59E−05 0.000275271 Inhibited http://www.genome.jp/dbget-bin/show_pathway?mmu05222+16400+56469+218772+17187+19211+30955+13063+12826+12827+12830+14268+16772+16774+16775+16776+16779+226519+23928+11797+19225+12567+12571
Olfactory transduction 4740 989 40 1 −23.1386 5.00E−06 6.60E−05 0.001179114 0.008253795 Inhibited http://www.genome.jp/dbget-bin/show_pathway?mmu04740+100038860+18345+18369+235256+257884+257939+258027+258228+258247+258266+258278+258286+258407+258421+258446+258482+258483+258502+258533+258541+258570+258620+258648+258656+258677+258683+258712+258743+258922+258972+259006+259103+259105+404335+404336+56015+56860+57272+333329+19092
Focal adhesion 4510 199 41 0.095748092 −20.21166302 0.001 0.000981781 0.01486948 0.122722602 Inhibited http://www.genome.jp/dbget-bin/show_pathway?mmu04510+16400+192176+286940+67268+67938+19211+30955+21894+70549+12814+12825+12826+12827+12830+12833+12835+12842+12843+14268+16772+16774+16775+16776+16779+19699+20750+21825+22371+226519+23928+245026+81877+12389+12390+11797+12445+16001+18596+107746+109905+57257
Calcium signaling pathway 4020 179 47 0.000966948 −5.8395 0.109 0.001070603 0.01486948 0.133825321 Inhibited http://www.genome.jp/dbget-bin/show_pathway?mmu04020+320404+432530+18125+22334+22335+110891+20541+19059+12494+13869+18596+14810+14811+18438+228139+18803+18802+16438+16439+102093+18679+18682+68961+20190+20191+20192+18798+12288+12289+12286+12287+12290+108071+11550+12669+21338+21390+243764+26361+21924+11739+11740+11515+12291+239556+58226+11941

Abbreviations: ECM, extracellular matrix; pG, global P-values, obtained by combining the pPERT and pNDE using Fisher's method; pGFdr, global P-values after fdf correction; pGFWER, global P-value adjusted by the Bonferroni's method; pNDE, P-value by the number of differentially expressed genes (classical test for the enrichment of genes in a certain pathway); pPERT, P-value by perturbation (calculated based on the amount of perturbation measured in each pathway).

Microarray data of RAG-1−/− and wild-type brains were analyzed by a moderate t-statistics and fold change, and subsequently analyzed for the pathway enrichment with a bootstrap technique using the Bioconductor package, SPIA. Significantly modulated pathways were selected by a global pathway significance P-value with considering false discovery rate (P<0.05), which combines the enrichment and perturbation P-values.

The analysis was perfomed by SPIA (Bioconductor package).

Discussion

The idea that a balanced mental state is a directly associated with general well-being can be traced back to the time of Decimus Iunius Iuvenalis. He was the first to state that a healthy mind is found in a healthy body (‘mens sana in corpore sano'). However, evidences gathered in our modern time suggests also the reverse, that is, that a corpore sano, and in particular a healthy immune system, might contribute to our mental well-being. In this study, we sought to provide direct experimental evidence of this hypothesis investigating first the emotional behavior of mice genetically void of T and B cells (the RAG-1−/− mice) and secondly assessing the specific contribution of CD4+ or CD8+ T cells.

Our results show a significant increase in anxiety-like behavior in RAG-1−/− mice as evaluated by the number of center entries in the open field test as well as the increased digging and marble-burying activities. Most interestingly, we also observed that CD4+ but not CD8+ T cells are able to revert significantly the exaggerated emotional response of RAG-1−/− mice. These results were not due to T-cell activation upon exposure of the mice to the behavioral paradigms (Supplementary Figure 11). The notion that CD4+ T cells has a preferential role as ‘mood stabilizer' compared with CD8+ T cells has long been suggested. Early studies on adult patients suffering from anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorders have demonstrated immunological alterations including a significant increase of CD8+ and decrease of CD4+ lymphocytes compared with the healthy control group.30 The same abnormality has been observed in patients with autism, a disorder characterized also by obsessive-compulsive symptoms and anxiety disorders.31 A recent study has also demonstrated in a quantitative manner an inverse relationship between CD4 count and hospital-associated anxiety and depression.32 Finally, in one of his recent review AH Miller33 provided a comprehensive account of the multiple links between CD4+ T cells and depression, highlighting the importance of trafficking of T cells to the brain following stress as a way to reduce stress-induced anxiety-like behavior.33 However, a direct functional association between T cells and altered emotional behavior is still missing.

One of the major drawbacks in the life of patients suffering from anxiety-like behavior and/or obsessive-compulsive disorders is their inability to perform normal daily activities. Insightful epidemiological studies suggest that this ‘inability to cope' might be due to an emotional rather than a cognitive impairment.34 Our results are in line with this hypothesis and show a significant reduction in self-caring of RAG-1−/− mice compared with WT mice, as evaluated by the nesting test. Although these results suggest a link between self-neglect, anxiety-like behavior and immune suppression, more studies are needed to corroborate this hypothesis. These could include burrowing and hoarding, two other methods to test and quantify experimentally daily living activities.35

Nestlet shredding together with marble burying is also a reliable experimental model for obsessive-compulsive disorders and anxiety.36 We could not observe a significant difference in nestlet shredding after 60 min (data not shown), whereas we found a dramatic reduction in nest construction after an overnight test. We are tempted to explain these findings with the fact that, contrary to innate cells, adaptive immune cells are more involved in chronic disease and hence they usually exert their functions over a long period of time. In a similar way, one might speculate that immunosuppressed patients might present more difficulties in coping with long-term illness while being able to face problems as they came in.

Textbook immunology reports CD4+ T cells as ‘helper' cells because of their ability to modulate other cell functions, and these ‘helping' properties may go beyond antigen-presenting cell-mediated immunity. Groundbreaking studies by Kipnis and Schwartz37, 38, 39 has put CD4+ T cells at the center stage of neuroimmunology.2 Repopulation of scid mice with T cells from WT donors has been shown to improve significantly the impaired cognitive functions of these mice.40 Most interestingly, circulating and patrolling CD4+ T cells have been reported to convey constantly protective signaling to the brain, thus contributing to what is now known as ‘protective autoimmunity'.41, 42, 43, 44 In light of this concept, antigen-specific T cells, like myelin basic protein T-cell clones,45 circulate through the brain and sustain key neuronal processes and function such as neurogenesis, cognition and memory.39 Considering that the OT-II TCR transgene recognizes a non-endogenous antigenic peptide like OVA323–339, it is tempting to speculate that, at difference from cognition and memory, emotional behavior might require a less-stringent condition, that is, the simple presence of circulating CD4+ T cells. Indeed, their circulation through the brain or meningeal spaces, as suggested by Derecki et al.,46 might be enough to restore the emotional impairment that flares in immunocompromised conditions.46

Previous studies assessing the role of T cells on brain functions have used immunodepleting antibodies or cellular replacement in immunodeficient host like the scid mice.40 When we adopted similar approaches in our settings, we could not find any significant differences. Neither the depletion of T cells in C57/BL6 mice using anti-CD4 or anti-CD8 antibodies nor the repopulation of RAG-1−/− with purified CD4 or CD8 T cells had any effect on emotional behavior. The reason behind this apparent discrepancy might lay in the nature of the immunodeficient state that has been studied. In the RAG-1−/− mice, we have investigated how the absence of immune cells from prenatal development to adulthood influences neuronal networks and emotional behavior, whereas the immunodepletion or repopulation experiments refer to transient conditions (up to 2 weeks in our tests). This is quite an important aspect when one considers that neuropsychiatric disorders have often been linked to problems occurring at the developmental stage.47, 48, 49 Inflammation and infection at pre- and perinatal stages have been proved to be as powerful as maternal stress and trauma in causing long-term consequences on neuronal development and mental health.50, 51, 52 These clinical evidences hold true in experimental settings. Both peri- and prenatal administration of immunomodulatory agents such as TLR3 ligands and viral mimic polyinosine-polycytidylic, or TLR4 ligand and bacterial surrogate lipopolysaccharide, induced the development of schizophrenia- and autism-related behavioral changes including decreased exploratory activity and social interaction as adults.53, 54, 55, 56, 57, 58

Previous studies have shown that, like in pregnancy,59 CD4+ T cells with a skewed Th2 phenotype contribute to a controlled and trophic microenvironment in the brain upregulating neurotrophic factors such as glial-derived neurotrophic factor, brain-derived neurotrophic factor and insulin growth factor 1, or suppressing inflammatory mediators like tumor necrosis factor-α and IL-6.46, 60 Conversely, direct and indirect evidence have linked Th1 and Th17 cells to emotional disorders. T cells from individuals with generalized anxiety disorders show an enhanced capability to differentiate in Th17 cells.61, 62 Experimental evidences also suggest that Th17 cells preferentially accumulate in the brain of mice subjected to chronic restrain stress, whereas mice deficient in the RORγT (transcription factor necessary for Th17 differentiation) exhibited resistance to learned helplessness.63 T-bet (transcription factor necessary for Th1 differentiation) knockout mice show significantly reduced depressive-like behaviors provoked by repeated restraint stress.64 This is in line with clinical studies showing, for instance, the contribution of Th1 cytokines to the pathogenesis of neuropsychiatric manifestations of systemic lupus erythematosus.65 In light of these findings, we decided to investigate whether the absence or presence of circulating CD4+ T cells would impact neuronal gene development. Our microarray analysis provided us with a number of interesting findings.

A large number of micro-RNA(s) was found among the genes that were mostly downregulated. These gene expression modulators have been recently highlighted for their role in mental heath and are becoming increasingly popular in this field.66 Literature search for known targets of the micro-RNA we have identified provided us with few indications (Table 3). Further studies are needed to investigate the functions of these micro-RNA and their role in emotional behavior. Several quantitative polymerase chain reaction validated genes (data not shown) in our screening are known to control a variety of neuropsychological conditions. Synuclein-γ has been associated with Parkinson's disease67 and reported to be implicated in both cognitive and emotional functions.68 Von Willebrand factor has been shown to be significantly increased in schizophrenia,69, 70, 71 whereas changes in polycystic kidney disease 1 and tetratricopeptide repeat and ankyrin repeat containing 1) expression have been associated with bipolar disorders.72, 73, 74 S100a10, a recently suggested potential biomarker for suicide risk in mental disorders,75 was upregulated in RAG-1−/− and downregulated in RAG-1−/−/OT-II to WT level. Other interesting differentially expressed genes included ephrin type-B receptor 1 (Ephb1), whose genetic deletion in mice causes neuronal loss in the substantia nigra and spontaneous locomotor hyperactivity,76 myeloid/lymphoid or mixed-lineage leukemia 2 (Mll2), whose activity is required for memory formation,77 and Churchill domain containing protein 1 (Churc1), a neuronal development gene implicated with the occurrence of autism.78

Table 3. miRNA modulated in RAG-1−/− brain and their relative targets.  .

miRNA Cited in Known targets
Mir539 Bao B et al. J Nutr 2010; Holocarboxylase synthetase
  Haga CL and Phinney DG. J Biol Chem 2012 Twist-related protein 1, polycomb complex protein BMI-1
Mir380 Hu K et al. BMC Neurosci 2012; Matsumoto S et al. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 2012 Unknown
Mir487b Xi S et al. J Clin Invest 2013 Polycomb protein SUZ12, polycomb complex protein BMI-1, protein Wnt-5a, Myc proto-oncogene protein, GTPase KRas
Mir323 Qiu S et al. J Transl Med 2013; Fenoglio C et al. Int J Mol Sci 2012 Unknown
Mir680-2 None Unknown
Mir665 Si H et al. J Cancer Res Clin Oncol 2013 Unknown
Mir376b Korkmaz G et al. Autophagy 2012 Cysteine protease ATG4C and beclin-1
Mir382 Kriegel AJ et al. Physiol Genom 2012 Kallikrein 5
  Haga CL and Phinney DG. J Biol Chem 2012 Twist-related protein 1, polycomb complex protein BMI-1
Mir154 Milosevic J et al. Am J Respir Cell Mol Biol 2012 WNT/β-catenin pathway
Mir329 Khudayberdiev S et al. Commun Integr Biol 2009; Qiu S et al. J Transl Med 2013 Unknown
Mir679 None Unknown
Mir344 Qin L et al. BMC Genom 2010 WNT/β-catenin pathway
Mir9-2 Rodriguez-Otero P et al. Br J Haematol 2011 Fibroblast growth factor receptor 1 and cyclin-dependent kinase 6
Mir341 None Unknown
Mir543 Haga CL and Phinney DG. J Biol Chem 2012 Twist-related protein 1, polycomb complex protein BMI-1

Abbreviations: miR, micro-RNA. The table shows a list of miRNAs modulated in RAG-1−/− compared to RAG-1−/−/OT-II and wild type brains and the relative studies describing their molecular target(s).

Some of the implications of these changes in gene expression have been investigated with SPIA pathway analysis software. This showed inhibition of signaling pathways that control neurodegenerative disorders, such as Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease, in WT compared with RAG-1−/− mice further supporting the emerging view of these diseases as endowed with an autoimmune-related diseases component.79, 80, 81 Further differences were observed in a wide range of neuronal, sensory and basic cellular pathways that will be explored in future studies to detail the complex crosstalk between the neuronal and immune systems.

In conclusion, the results of this study shed new light on the complex crosstalk between the immune system and our emotional well-being, although future investigations are needed to corroborate our hypothesis. In fact, it would be interesting to explore the possible contribution of B cells to the emotional behavior of the RAG-1−/− mice as well as to confirm these results in mice expressing TCR transgene with different strength of signaling. Equally important, one might speculate the existence of CD4+ T-cell-specific factors that control emotional behaviors and their exploitation for the treatment of wide variety of mental disorders.

Beyond these experimental questions, the most important challenge for the future is to understand how T cells influence behavior and vice versa. The answer might lay in shared signaling pathway like RAG or the immune synapse: a signaling complex that has been named after the neuromuscular synapse and that allows the exchanges of information between antigen-presenting cells and T cells.82, 83, 84 Along these lines, recent studies have shown the existence of a subset of memory T cell in mice that produces acetylcholine in response to noradrenaline providing another way by which the immune system communicate with the nervous system.85, 86 Taken together, these findings might help the design of new therapies for mental health by restoring an impaired or absent immune system as observed in several autoimmune diseases.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr Mangesh Thorat (Cancer Research UK Center for Epidemiology, Mathematics and Statistics) for helping us with brain section picture acquisition and Dr Dianne Cooper for carefully reading the manuscript. MO is a Human Frontier Science Program Long-term Fellow.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Translational Psychiatry website (http://www.nature.com/tp)

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Figure 1
Supplementary Figure 2
Supplementary Figure 3
Supplementary Figure 4
Supplementary Figure 5
Supplementary Figure 6
Supplementary Figure 7
Supplementary Figure 8
Supplementary Figure 9
Supplementary Figure 10
Supplementary Figure 11
Supplementary Figure Legends

References

  1. Irwin M, Patterson T, Smith TL, Caldwell C, Brown SA, Gillin JC, et al. Reduction of immune function in life stress and depression. Biol Psychiatry. 1990;27:22–30. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(90)90016-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ziv Y, Schwartz M. Immune-based regulation of adult neurogenesis: implications for learning and memory. Brain Behav Immun. 2008;22:167–176. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Yirmiya R, Goshen I. Immune modulation of learning, memory, neural plasticity and neurogenesis. Brain Behav Immun. 2011;25:181–213. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aktas O, Kieseier B, Hartung HP. Neuroprotection, regeneration and immunomodulation: broadening the therapeutic repertoire in multiple sclerosis. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33:140–152. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. German DC, Eagar T, Sonsalla PK.Parkinson's disease: a role for the immune system Curr Mol Pharmacol 2011(e-pub ahead of print). [PubMed]
  6. Lucin KM, Wyss-Coray T. Immune activation in brain aging and neurodegeneration: too much or too little. Neuron. 2009;64:110–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Littrell J. The mind–body connection: not just a theory anymore. Soc Work Health Care. 2008;46:17–37. doi: 10.1300/j010v46n04_02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Costa-Pinto FA, Palermo-Neto J. Neuroimmune interactions in stress. Neuroimmunomodulation. 2010;17:196–199. doi: 10.1159/000258722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Rook GA, Lowry CA, Raison CL. Lymphocytes in neuroprotection, cognition and emotion: is intolerance really the answer. Brain Behav Immun. 2011;25:591–601. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barak Y. The immune system and happiness. Autoimmun Rev. 2006;5:523–527. doi: 10.1016/j.autrev.2006.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Sato Y, Takayanagi Y, Onaka T, Kobayashi E. Impact of cyclosporine upon emotional and social behavior in mice. Transplantation. 2007;83:1365–1370. doi: 10.1097/01.tp.0000263332.65519.1f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. von Horsten S, Exton MS, Schult M, Nagel E, Stalp M, Schweitzer G, et al. Behaviorally conditioned effects of cyclosporine A on the immune system of rats: specific alterations of blood leukocyte numbers and decrease of granulocyte function. J Neuroimmunol. 1998;85:193–201. doi: 10.1016/s0165-5728(98)00011-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. von Horsten S, Exton MS, Voge J, Schult M, Nagel E, Schmidt RE, et al. Cyclosporine A affects open field behavior in DA rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;60:71–76. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(97)00467-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bechstein WO. Neurotoxicity of calcineurin inhibitors: impact and clinical management. Transpl Int. 2000;13:313–326. doi: 10.1007/s001470050708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Wijdicks EF. Neurotoxicity of immunosuppressive drugs. Liver Transpl. 2001;7:937–942. doi: 10.1053/jlts.2001.27475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. van der Meer A, Wulffraat NM, Prakken BJ, Gijsbers B, Rademaker CM, Sinnema G. Psychological side effects of MTX treatment in juvenile idiopathic arthritis: a pilot study. Clin Exp Rheumatol. 2007;25:480–485. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. van der Hoeven J, Duyx J, de Langen JJ, van Royen A. Probable psychiatric side effects of azathioprine. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:508. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000160473.31162.4d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Pandey M, Sarita GP, Devi N, Thomas BC, Hussain BM, Krishnan R. Distress, anxiety, and depression in cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy. World J Surg Oncol. 2006;4:68. doi: 10.1186/1477-7819-4-68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Mombaerts P, Iacomini J, Johnson RS, Herrup K, Tonegawa S, Papaioannou VE. RAG-1-deficient mice have no mature B and T lymphocytes. Cell. 1992;68:869–877. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(92)90030-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cushman J, Lo J, Huang Z, Wasserfall C, Petitto JM. Neurobehavioral changes resulting from recombinase activation gene 1 deletion. Clin Diagn Lab Immunol. 2003;10:13–18. doi: 10.1128/CDLI.10.1.13-18.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. McGowan PO, Hope TA, Meck WH, Kelsoe G, Williams CL. Impaired social recognition memory in recombination activating gene 1-deficient mice. Brain Res. 2011;1383:187–195. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.02.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Paschalidis N, Huggins A, Rowbotham NJ, Furmanski AL, Crompton T, Flower RJ, et al. Role of endogenous annexin-A1 in the regulation of thymocyte positive and negative selection. Cell Cycle. 2010;9:784–793. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Deacon RM. Digging and marble burying in mice: simple methods for in vivo identification of biological impacts. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:122–124. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Deacon RM. Housing, husbandry and handling of rodents for behavioral experiments. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:936–946. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Deacon RM. Assessing nest building in mice. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:1117–1119. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ono M, Tanaka RJ, Kano M, Sugiman T. Visualising the cross-level relationships between pathological and physiological processes and gene expression: analyses of haematological diseases. PLoS One. 2013;8:e53544. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Barnden MJ, Allison J, Heath WR, Carbone FR. Defective TCR expression in transgenic mice constructed using cDNA-based alpha- and beta-chain genes under the control of heterologous regulatory elements. Immunol Cell Biol. 1998;76:34–40. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1711.1998.00709.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Sharpley CF, Agnew LL. Cytokines and depression: findings, issues, and treatment implications. Rev Neurosci. 2011;22:295–302. doi: 10.1515/RNS.2011.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Tarca AL, Draghici S, Khatri P, Hassan SS, Mittal P, Kim JS, et al. A novel signaling pathway impact analysis. Bioinformatics. 2009;25:75–82. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btn577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Marazziti D, Presta S, Pfanner C, Gemignani A, Rossi A, Sbrana S, et al. Immunological alterations in adult obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:810–814. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00371-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Yonk LJ, Warren RP, Burger RA, Cole P, Odell JD, Warren WL, et al. CD4+ helper T cell depression in autism. Immunol Lett. 1990;25:341–345. doi: 10.1016/0165-2478(90)90205-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Zena-Castillo D, Mezones-Holguin E, Valdiviezo-Garcia G, La-Chira-Alban A, Rodriguez-Morales AJ, Dickson-Gonzalez S. Impact of hospital-associated anxiety and depression on the CD4 counts of naive HIV/AIDS patients from locations in Northern Peru. Int J Infect Dis. 2009;13:e75–e76. doi: 10.1016/j.ijid.2008.06.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Miller AH. Depression and immunity: a role for T cells. Brain Behav Immun. 2010;24:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Huppert JD, Simpson HB, Nissenson KJ, Liebowitz MR, Foa EB. Quality of life and functional impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison of patients with and without comorbidity, patients in remission, and healthy controls. Depress Anxiety. 2009;26:39–45. doi: 10.1002/da.20506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Deacon R. Assessing burrowing, nest construction, and hoarding in mice. J Vis Exp. 2012;59:e2607. doi: 10.3791/2607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Li X, Morrow D, Witkin JM. Decreases in nestlet shredding of mice by serotonin uptake inhibitors: comparison with marble burying. Life Sci. 2006;78:1933–1939. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2005.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Schwartz M. The emergence of a new science of the mind: immunology benefits the mind. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:337–338. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kipnis J, Cohen H, Cardon M, Ziv Y, Schwartz M. T cell deficiency leads to cognitive dysfunction: implications for therapeutic vaccination for schizophrenia and other psychiatric conditions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:8180–8185. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402268101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kipnis J, Gadani S, Derecki NC. Pro-cognitive properties of T cells. Nat Rev Immunol. 2012;12:663–669. doi: 10.1038/nri3280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Brynskikh A, Warren T, Zhu J, Kipnis J. Adaptive immunity affects learning behavior in mice. Brain Behav Immun. 2008;22:861–869. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kipnis J, Mizrahi T, Hauben E, Shaked I, Shevach E, Schwartz M. Neuroprotective autoimmunity: naturally occurring CD4+CD25+ regulatory T cells suppress the ability to withstand injury to the central nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:15620–15625. doi: 10.1073/pnas.232565399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Schwartz M, Kipnis J. Protective autoimmunity and neuroprotection in inflammatory and noninflammatory neurodegenerative diseases. J Neurol Sci. 2005;233:163–166. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2005.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schwartz M, Ziv Y. Immunity to self and self-maintenance: a unified theory of brain pathologies. Trends Immunol. 2008;29:211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.it.2008.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Schwartz M, Shechter R. Protective autoimmunity functions by intracranial immunosurveillance to support the mind: The missing link between health and disease. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:342–354. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Barouch R, Schwartz M. Autoreactive T cells induce neurotrophin production by immune and neural cells in injured rat optic nerve: implications for protective autoimmunity. FASEB J. 2002;16:1304–1306. doi: 10.1096/fj.01-0467fje. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Derecki NC, Cardani AN, Yang CH, Quinnies KM, Crihfield A, Lynch KR, et al. Regulation of learning and memory by meningeal immunity: a key role for IL-4. J Exp Med. 2010;207:1067–1080. doi: 10.1084/jem.20091419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Aas M, Navari S, Gibbs A, Mondelli V, Fisher HL, Morgan C, et al. Is there a link between childhood trauma, cognition, and amygdala and hippocampus volume in first-episode psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2012;137:73–79. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Aas M, Dazzan P, Fisher HL, Morgan C, Morgan K, Reichenberg A, et al. Childhood trauma and cognitive function in first-episode affective and non-affective psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2011;129:12–19. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Pawlby S, Hay D, Sharp D, Waters CS, Pariante CM. Antenatal depression and offspring psychopathology: the influence of childhood maltreatment. Br J Psychiatry. 2011;199:106–112. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.087734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Monk C, Fitelson EM, Werner E. Mood disorders and their pharmacological treatment during pregnancy: is the future child affected. Pediatr Res. 2011;69 (Part 2:3R–10R. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e3182131a2e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Harvey L, Boksa P. Prenatal and postnatal animal models of immune activation: relevance to a range of neurodevelopmental disorders. Dev Neurobiol. 2012;72:1335–1348. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schlotz W, Phillips DI. Fetal origins of mental health: evidence and mechanisms. Brain Behav Immun. 2009;23:905–916. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ellis S, Mouihate A, Pittman QJ. Neonatal programming of the rat neuroimmune response: stimulus specific changes elicited by bacterial and viral mimetics. J Physiol. 2006;571 (Pt 3:695–701. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.102939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hsiao EY, McBride SW, Chow J, Mazmanian SK, Patterson PH. Modeling an autism risk factor in mice leads to permanent immune dysregulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:12776–12781. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202556109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hsiao EY, Patterson PH. Placental regulation of maternal–fetal interactions and brain development. Dev Neurobiol. 2012;72:1317–1326. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hodyl NA, Walker FR, Krivanek KM, Clifton VL, Hodgson DM. Prenatal endotoxin exposure alters behavioural pain responses to lipopolysaccharide in adult offspring. Physiol Behav. 2010;100:143–147. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hodyl NA, Krivanek KM, Lawrence E, Clifton VL, Hodgson DM. Prenatal exposure to a pro-inflammatory stimulus causes delays in the development of the innate immune response to LPS in the offspring. J Neuroimmunol. 2007;190:61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2007.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mouihate A, Galic MA, Ellis SL, Spencer SJ, Tsutsui S, Pittman QJ. Early life activation of toll-like receptor 4 reprograms neural anti-inflammatory pathways. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7975–7983. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6078-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Saito S, Nakashima A, Shima T, Ito M. Th1/Th2/Th17 and regulatory T-cell paradigm in pregnancy. Am J Reprod Immunol. 2010;63:601–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0897.2010.00852.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Gadani SP, Cronk JC, Norris GT, Kipnis J. IL-4 in the brain: a cytokine to remember. J Immunol. 2012;189:4213–4219. doi: 10.4049/jimmunol.1202246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ferreira TB, Kasahara TM, Barros PO, Vieira MM, Bittencourt VC, Hygino J, et al. Dopamine up-regulates Th17 phenotype from individuals with generalized anxiety disorder. J Neuroimmunol. 2011;238:58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2011.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Vieira MM, Ferreira TB, Pacheco PA, Barros PO, Almeida CR, Araujo-Lima CF, et al. Enhanced Th17 phenotype in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder. J Neuroimmunol. 2010;229:212–218. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2010.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Beurel E, Harrington LE, Jope RS. Inflammatory T helper 17 cells promote depression-like behavior in mice. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;73:622–630. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.09.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kim SJ, Lee H, Joung HY, Lee G, Lee HJ, Shin MK, et al. T-bet deficient mice exhibit resistance to stress-induced development of depression-like behaviors. J Neuroimmunol. 2011;240–241:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2011.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Postal M, Pelicari KO, Sinicato NA, Marini R, Costallat LT, Appenzeller S. Th1/Th2 cytokine profile in childhood-onset systemic lupus erythematosus. Cytokine. 2013;61:785–791. doi: 10.1016/j.cyto.2012.11.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. O'Connor RM, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. Little things on which happiness depends: microRNAs as novel therapeutic targets for the treatment of anxiety and depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17:359–376. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Nishioka K, Wider C, Vilarino-Guell C, Soto-Ortolaza AI, Lincoln SJ, Kachergus JM, et al. Association of alpha-, beta-, and gamma-Synuclein with diffuse lewy body disease. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:970–975. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kokhan VS, Bolkunov AV, Ustiugov AA, Van'kin GI, Shelkovnikova TA, Redkozubova OM, et al. Targeted inactivation of gamma-synuclein gene affects anxiety and exploratory behaviour of mice. Zh Vyssh Nerv Deiat Im I P Pavlova. 2011;61:85–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Brietzke E, Teixeira AL. Similar immune profile in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: selective increase in soluble tumor necrosis factor receptor I and von Willebrand factor. Bipolar Disord. 2010;12:453–454. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00822.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Ho-Yen DO, Kay DS, Henderson R, Todd AS, Naylor GJ. Factor VIII-related antigen and procoagulant activity in manic-depressive patients. J Affect Disord. 1983;5:209–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(83)90043-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Hope S, Melle I, Aukrust P, Steen NE, Birkenaes AB, Lorentzen S, et al. Similar immune profile in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: selective increase in soluble tumor necrosis factor receptor I and von Willebrand factor. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11:726–734. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00757.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Kimmel RJ, Kovacs I, Vrabel C, Wood B, Schalling M, Kelsoe JR. Cosegregation of bipolar disorder and autosomal-dominant medullary cystic kidney disease in a large family. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1972–1974. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wylie K, de Silva D, Jerram T, Mindham RH. Simultaneous kidney disease and manic-depressive psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162:275–276. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.2.275a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Chen DT, Jiang X, Akula N, Shugart YY, Wendland JR, Steele CJ, et al. Genome-wide association study meta-analysis of European and Asian-ancestry samples identifies three novel loci associated with bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:195–205. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Zhang L, Su TP, Choi K, Maree W, Li CT, Chung MY, et al. P11 (S100A10) as a potential biomarker of psychiatric patients at risk of suicide. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45:435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Richards AB, Scheel TA, Wang K, Henkemeyer M, Kromer LF. EphB1 null mice exhibit neuronal loss in substantia nigra pars reticulata and spontaneous locomotor hyperactivity. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:2619–2628. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05523.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Kerimoglu C, Agis-Balboa RC, Kranz A, Stilling R, Bahari-Javan S, Benito-Garagorri E, et al. Histone-methyltransferase MLL2 (KMT2B) is required for memory formation in mice. J Neurosci. 2013;33:3452–3464. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3356-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Griswold AJ, Ma D, Sacharow SJ, Robinson JL, Jaworski JM, Wright HH, et al. A de novo 1.5 Mb microdeletion on chromosome 14q23.2–23.3 in a patient with autism and spherocytosis. Autism Res. 2011;4:221–227. doi: 10.1002/aur.186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Villoslada P, Moreno B, Melero I, Pablos JL, Martino G, Uccelli A, et al. Immunotherapy for neurological diseases. Clin Immunol. 2008;128:294–305. doi: 10.1016/j.clim.2008.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wisniewski T, Konietzko U. Amyloid-beta immunisation for Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7:805–811. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70170-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Cardoso F.Huntington disease and other choreas Neurol Clin 200927719–736.vi. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Monks CR, Freiberg BA, Kupfer H, Sciaky N, Kupfer A. Three-dimensional segregation of supramolecular activation clusters in T cells. Nature. 1998;395:82–86. doi: 10.1038/25764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Reichardt P, Dornbach B, Gunzer M. APC, T cells, and the immune synapse. Curr Top Microbiol Immunol. 2010;340:229–249. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-03858-7_12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Valitutti S, Dupre L. Plasticity of immunological synapses. Curr Top Microbiol Immunol. 2010;340:209–228. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-03858-7_11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Rosas-Ballina M, Olofsson PS, Ochani M, Valdes-Ferrer SI, Levine YA, Reardon C, et al. Acetylcholine-synthesizing T cells relay neural signals in a vagus nerve circuit. Science. 2011;334:98–101. doi: 10.1126/science.1209985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Woodman I. Neuroimmunology: nervous ChAT. Nat Rev Immunol. 2011;11:720. doi: 10.1038/nri3091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Figure 1
Supplementary Figure 2
Supplementary Figure 3
Supplementary Figure 4
Supplementary Figure 5
Supplementary Figure 6
Supplementary Figure 7
Supplementary Figure 8
Supplementary Figure 9
Supplementary Figure 10
Supplementary Figure 11
Supplementary Figure Legends

Articles from Translational Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES