Abstract
Alcohol use can cause significant harm. We examined the relationships between neighborhood disadvantage, consumption of high-alcohol-content beverages (HACB), drinking norms, and self-reported drinking consequences using data from the 2000 and 2005 National Alcohol Surveys (N = 9,971 current drinkers) and the 2000 Decennial Census. We hypothesized that (1) individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods would report more negative drinking consequences than individuals living in more affluent neighborhoods, and (2) this relationship would be mediated by HACB consumption and pro-drunkenness drinking norms. Neighborhood disadvantage was based on a composite measure of socioeconomic indicators from the 2000 Decennial Census (five-item composite, alpha = 0.89). We measured high alcohol content beverage consumption in terms of whether respondents engaged in frequent or heavy consumption of malt liquor, fortified wine, or distilled spirits/liquor. The outcome was a dichotomous indicator of two or more of 15 past-year social, legal, work, and health consequences. Simultaneous, multivariate path modeling tested direct and indirect effects of neighborhood disadvantage, HACB consumption, and pro-drunkenness norms on consequences. Individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods reported significantly more negative drinking consequences than individuals living in more affluent neighborhoods. Consumption of high-alcohol-content beverages and pro-drunkenness norms did not mediate this relationship. However, heavy distilled spirits/liquor use was a significant mediator of other neighborhood characteristics (i.e., percent African American). Living in an African American neighborhood was related to increased spirits/liquor consumption and, in turn, reporting more negative drinking consequences. Greater scrutiny of advertising and tax policies related to distilled spirits/liquor is needed to prevent future drinking problems, especially in minority neighborhoods.
Keywords: Neighborhood disadvantage, Malt liquor, Fortified wine, Spirits/liquor, Drinking norms, Drinking consequences
Introduction
Alcohol use can cause significant harm. Heavy alcohol use is associated with a variety of health and social problems including cardiovascular disease, several cancers, and liver cirrhosis, as well as drinking and driving, assaults, and domestic violence.1–5
The neighborhoods where people live can affect their drinking behavior. This is especially true in disadvantaged neighborhoods where liquor stores are over-concentrated,6–8 and alcohol is heavily promoted by commercial interests.9–11
Various theories have been used to explain how living in a disadvantaged neighborhood affects drinking behavior. For example, social ecological theory12 suggests that the high concentration of liquor stores increases the physical availability of alcohol, and the heavy promotion of alcohol makes heavy drinking more socially acceptable and encourages consumption in disadvantaged neighborhoods. Studies have found that alcohol outlet density is associated with increased alcohol consumption and drinking problems or consequences,13–15 and other studies have shown that alcohol advertising is associated with more positive attitudes regarding drinking, drinking initiation at younger ages, brand preference, and alcohol consumption.16–20
Several studies have investigated the effects of neighborhood disadvantage on substance use, but findings have been mixed.21 Some studies have found that neighborhood disadvantage is associated with increased alcohol consumption, heavy drinking,22,23 and drinking consequences.24. For example, Cerda and colleagues22 found that a one-unit increase in the proportion of adult residents living in poverty was associated with an 86 % increase in odds of heavy drinking. In contrast, other studies have found no support for the disadvantage hypothesis and substance use,25 and still other studies have shown mixed results depending on the measure of neighborhood socioeconomic status26 or the outcome examined.27,28 Karriker and colleagues29 found neighborhood disadvantage was associated with elevated risk of negative drinking consequences, particularly for White women and African American men but not to alcohol dependence symptoms. Although prior studies have examined neighborhood influences on drinking behavior, few studies have used large, nationally representative samples or examined different dimensions of neighborhood disadvantage or multiple outcomes. Even fewer have considered the pathways through which neighborhood disadvantage influences drinking problems or consequences. One important exception is the work by Chuang and colleagues30 which used a national sample to model mediators of neighborhood effects on adolescent substance use. They found that peer substance use was an important mediator of the effects of neighborhood disadvantage on adolescent alcohol use. Understanding the mechanisms by which neighborhood disadvantage affects negative drinking consequences can help identify where to target prevention programs, policies, and resources.
Two potential mediators that may be important in understanding how living in a disadvantaged neighborhood affects drinking behavior are high-alcohol-content beverage consumption and pro-drunkenness norms. High-alcohol-content beverages include malt liquor beer (7 % alcohol by volume or ABV), fortified wines (17 % ABV), and distilled spirits/liquor (40 % ABV).31 Malt liquor (e.g., Colt 45) and low-end fortified wines (e.g., Thunderbird) are inexpensive, highly available, and widely promoted in disadvantaged and African American neighborhoods.10,11,32 In some disadvantaged African American neighborhoods, it is not uncommon to see more advertisements for malt liquor than food on the storefronts of food stores.32 Distilled spirits are promoted in print media33; however, cognac and brandy have increasingly been marketed in outdoor advertising in African American and low-income neighborhoods.34 Sex, glamour, prestige, and sophistication are common themes in such outdoor advertisements in disadvantaged neighborhoods.34 The promotion of high-alcohol-content beverages is of particular concern. Malt liquor consumption is associated with frequent daily drinking, heavy drinking,35,36 and problem behaviors such as drug use, theft, disorderly conduct, assaults, and panhandling,37–40 while consumption of spirits is associated with heavy and dependent drinking and cancer mortality.41 Drinking norms reflect common rules regarding the appropriateness of drinking, and they help regulate social behavior.42 The high availability and promotion of malt liquor and fortified wines in disadvantaged and African American neighborhoods may contribute to drinking norms favoring higher consumption of alcohol in general and to a greater use of high-alcohol-content beverages in disadvantaged neighborhoods. Some studies have found that norms or attitudes supportive of drunkenness are strongly related to heavy drinking and to drinking problems.35,43 These pro-drunkenness norms also may be associated with consumption of high-alcohol-content beverages by drinkers who want to get drunk quickly.
The objectives of our study were to investigate whether (1) neighborhood disadvantage was associated with negative drinking consequences and (2) determine whether this relationship was mediated by high-alcohol-content beverage consumption (e.g., malt liquor, fortified wine, and spirits/liquor) and pro-drunkenness drinking norms. We hypothesized that (1) individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods would report more negative drinking consequences than individuals living in more advantaged neighborhoods, and (2) this relationship would be mediated by high-alcohol-content beverage consumption or pro-drunkenness drinking norms. Given the high availability and targeted marketing of high-alcohol-content beverages to African American consumers, we also examined whether effects varied by race/ethnicity and took into account neighborhood racial/ethnic composition.10,11,32
Methods
Data
The data from our study come from the 2000 and 2005 National Alcohol Surveys (NAS). Both surveys involved computer-assisted telephone interviews with a randomly selected sample of US adults, including oversamples of African Americans, Hispanics, and residents from sparsely populated US states. The similarity in methods and their virtually identical interview protocols allows the two NAS datasets to be merged for more power for subgroup analyses and investigation of rare outcomes. For more details on the NAS methodology, see Midanik and Greenfield.44
The 2000 NAS included 7,613 respondents ages 18 years and older (58 % response rate), and the 2005 NAS included 6,919 respondents ages 18 years and older (56 % response rate). The sample for the current study includes 9,971 current drinkers who reported consuming at least one drink in the past 12 months. Although the response rates for the telephone surveys are lower than those often seen in face-to-face surveys, they are typical for recent random-digit dial telephone surveys in the US,45 and do not necessarily produce biased population estimates.46,47
For this study, NAS data were geocoded and matched with indicators of neighborhood SES from the 2000 Census.48 Respondent addresses were geocoded by a commercial firm (97 % accuracy rate). Next, the geocoded survey data were linked to indicators of neighborhood disadvantage at the census tract level, which contains, on average, approximately 4,000 individuals. Census tract-level measures of neighborhood disadvantage can provide important information on contextual determinants of substance use.49–51 Most cases (60 %) had geocodes assigned based on street address; the remainder had a geocode assigned based on the ZIP Code centroid. Preliminary analyses were conducted to test whether the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and reporting negative drinking consequences differed depending on the geographic unit used to geocode the data. Results were similar using geocoded data based on a street address or a zip code centroid (p > 0.05, data available upon request).
Neighborhood-Level Measures
The neighborhood indicators included neighborhood disadvantage, racial/ethnic composition, and urbanicity. Neighborhood disadvantage was a composite measure including the proportions of adults without a high school diploma, males who were unemployed or not in the labor force, people with incomes below poverty, families with incomes below 50 % of the US median, and households without access to a car. The measure of disadvantage was validated in a preliminary study51 and had high reliability in the study sample (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.89, M = 17.6, SD = 9.5).
Models also adjusted for racial/ethnic composition and urbanicity because previous studies have shown that high-alcohol-content beverages are highly available and heavily promoted in African American urban neighborhoods.10,32,38 Neighborhood racial/ethnic composition was represented by two variables: proportion Black/African American (M = 9.9, SD = 19.4) and proportion Hispanic/Latino (M = 9.9, SD = 17.1). Urbanicity was the proportion of residents in an urbanized area or urban cluster (M = 74.2, SD = 37.7). All neighborhood variables were square-root-transformed due to skewness.
Individual-Level Measures
Consumption of High-Alcohol-Content Beverages
Three dichotomous variables indicated frequent or heavy consumption of high-alcohol-content beverages (malt liquor, fortified wine, and spirits/liquor). A beverage-specific series of items assessed how often respondents consumed each type of alcoholic beverage (“wine or a punch containing wine or wine coolers or fortified wine,” “beer or malt liquor,” and “drinks containing whiskey or any other liquor, including scotch, bourbon, gin, vodka, rum, and so on”). Eleven response options ranged from “never had [type of beverage]” to “three or more times a day.” Frequent consumption of malt liquor was assessed with a follow-up question: “When you drink beer, about how much of the time do you drink malt liquor?” Response options included “nearly every time,” “more than half of the time,” “less than half of the time,” “once in a while,” and “never.” Approximately 11 % of current drinkers reported any use of malt liquor. Responses were dichotomized such that respondents reporting use more than “once in a while” (about 4 % of current drinkers and 40 % of those reporting any use of malt liquor) were categorized as frequent malt liquor drinkers. Frequent consumption of fortified wine was assessed with a similar follow-up item: “When you drink wine, about how much of the time do you drink sherry, port, sake, or other fortified wine?” (same response options as for malt liquor). Approximately 21 % of current drinkers reported any use of fortified wine. As above, responses were dichotomized such that respondents reporting use more than “once in a while” (about 7 % of current drinkers and 30 % of those reporting any use of fortified wine) were categorized as frequent fortified wine drinkers. Heavy consumption of spirits/liquor was based on a follow-up series of beverage-specific items that uses a graduated quantity–frequency format to assess the proportion of times respondents reported drinking “five or six drinks [containing whiskey, liquor, or spirits] or more,” “only three or four drinks,” and “only one or two drinks,” with five response categories ranging from “nearly every time” to “never.” The total volume of spirits/liquor consumed in the past year was calculated by a summative algorithm,52 and respondents with total spirits/liquor volumes greater than one standard deviation above the sample mean were categorized as heavy spirits/liquor drinkers. We did not create a similar total volume measure for malt liquor and fortified wine consumption, because the NAS asked respondents about frequency of consuming malt liquor and fortified wines only.
Pro-drunkenness Norms
Pro-drunkenness norms were based on situational drinking and drunkenness norms.43 Scores were based on five items assessing how much drinking is acceptable (no drinking, one or two drinks, “enough to feel the effects but not get drunk,” or “getting drunk is sometimes all right”) in different social situations, including at a party at someone else’s home, for a man out at a bar with friends, for a woman out at a bar with friends, for a couple of co-workers out for lunch, and when going to drive a car. Each item was dichotomized with “getting drunk is sometimes all right” coded as 1 and all other levels of drinking coded as 0. A total score was calculated (range = 0–5), indicating the number of situations in which drunkenness was acceptable.
Negative Drinking Consequences
Negative drinking consequences experienced by past-year drinkers were captured by a dichotomous variable indicating whether the respondent had experienced two or more of 15 negative alcohol-related consequences, including social (such as getting into a fight while drinking), legal (such as being questioned or warned by a police officer because of drinking), workplace (such as drinking hurting chances for promotion, raises, or better jobs), and health consequences (such as drinking becoming a serious threat to physical health). The items have been validated and used in previous studies based on NAS data over many years,53 and in this sample, the reliability was good (internal consistency measured by the Kuder–Richardson formula 20 = 0.73).
Demographic Control Variables
The path models adjusted for age (continuous), race/ethnicity (mutually exclusive dummy variables for African American, Hispanic, Asian/Pacific Islander, Native American/Alaska Native, and other race/ethnicity, with Caucasian as reference), marital status (currently living with spouse/partner versus not), number of children in the household (ranging from 0 to 5 or more), education (dummy variables for less than high school, high school graduate, and some college, with college degree as reference), employment status (dummy variables for unemployed and not in workforce, with employed as reference), and household income (dummy variables for $20,000 or less in the past year; $20,001–40,000; $40,001–60,000; $60,001–80,000; and missing income, with $80,001 or more as the reference). Models also included indicators of geocoding precision (ZIP code match versus street address match).
Analysis Strategy
To test study hypotheses, we used simultaneous, multivariate path modeling using Mplus.54 In the context of multiple mediators that are expected to be correlated, this simultaneous estimation technique provides greater power for testing mediation than would separate tests of each hypothesized mediator,55 and it tests the influence of each mediator while adjusting for the relationships among all variables in the model. Specifically, in the full path model, neighborhood disadvantage was specified as being correlated with each of the other neighborhood characteristics, and all of the high-alcohol-content beverage variables were specified as correlated with one another. The mediation analysis followed procedures discussed in MacKinnon,56 with mediated effects estimated using the MODEL INDIRECT sub-command, which provides estimates of both the indirect effects and their standard errors.
We used the robust weighted least-squares estimator (WLSMV) and theta parameterization, because the model contains both continuous and categorical variables.56 The final path model was chosen based on comparisons of nested models using the DIFFTEST procedure,57 because standard chi-square difference testing is not valid for models using WLSMV estimation. For each path in the overall model, statistically non-significant control variables were trimmed to preserve degrees of freedom; changes were confirmed as maintaining model fit using difference testing and fit indices, including the comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis fit index, and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA).
After the full path model was specified, we examined subgroup differences by race/ethnicity and gender. As previously mentioned, high-alcohol-content beverages such as malt liquor are highly available and heavily promoted in African American neighborhoods.10,32 Additionally, men are more likely to be consumers of high-alcohol-content beverages such as malt liquor than women.35,58 We used multiple group analysis and difference tests to evaluate whether allowing paths to vary significantly improved the fit over models where paths were constrained to be equal across groups. Due to the limited sub-sample sizes for some of the racial/ethnic categories, we tested for differences in the path models for Whites, Blacks/African Americans, Hispanics/Latinos, and all others combined.
All analyses used weighted data to adjust for sampling and non-response by survey. Because the national samples were selected by random-digit dialing, only 23 % of the neighborhoods contained more than two respondents, and just 3 % contained five or more respondents (maximum was nine). Thus, multilevel analytic strategies were not required.59 Because there were only a small number of respondents in each census tract, testing for spatial autocorrelation or spatial clustering of data was deemed unnecessary.
Results
Descriptive Analyses
The weighted sample was half male (51 %), with an average age of 43 years (Table 1). The majority of respondents (76 %) were White/Caucasian, with 10 % Hispanic/Latino, 9 % Black/African American, 2 % Asian/Pacific Islander, 2 % Native American/Alaska Native, and 1 % another race/ethnicity. Two thirds (65 %) were married or living with a partner. Most (73 %) were employed; slightly more than half (56 %) had incomes of $60,000/year or less, and 64 % had attended at least some college. For a more detailed description of the sample, see Karriker-Jaffe et al.60
Table 1.
Total sample (Weighted N = 9,307) | <2 Negative Consequences (N = 8,838) | 2+ Negative Consequences (N = 469) | P value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Sex | <0.001 | |||
Male | 51.2 % | 50.0 % | 26.5 % | |
Female | 48.8 % | 50.0 % | 73.5 % | |
Race/ethnicity | <0.001 | |||
Asian/Pacific Islander | 2.3 % | 2.3 % | 2.1 % | |
Black/African American | 8.8 % | 8.7 % | 11.8 % | |
Hispanic/Latino | 9.9 % | 9.7 % | 13.5 % | |
Native American/AK Native | 1.7 % | 1.6 % | 3.8 % | |
White | 76.4 % | 76.8 % | 68.7 % | |
Age (mean) | 43.0 years | 43.7 years | 30.0 years | <0.001 |
Marital status | <0.001 | |||
Single | 35.2 % | 34.1 % | 56.8 % | |
Married/cohabit | 64.8 % | 65.9 % | 43.2 % | |
Number of children in home (mean) | 0.77 | 0.77 | 0.81 | >0.10 |
Education | <0.001 | |||
Less than high school | 8.2 % | 7.5 % | 20.8 % | |
High school graduate | 27.4 % | 27.1 % | 34.5 % | |
Some college | 28.0 % | 28.0 % | 28.2 % | |
College graduate | 36.4 % | 37.4 % | 16.5 % | |
Employment | <0.001 | |||
Employed | 73.1 % | 72.9 % | 77.2 % | |
Unemployed | 3.9 % | 3.6 % | 8.7 % | |
Not in workforce | 23.0 % | 23.5 % | 14.1 % | |
Income | <0.001 | |||
$20,000 or less | 16.6 % | 15.9 % | 30.3 % | |
$20,001–40,000 | 22.2 % | 22.3 % | 20.2 % | |
$40,001–60,000 | 17.2 % | 17.4 % | 13.3 % | |
$60,001–80,000 | 12.9 % | 13.0 % | 11.4 % | |
$80,001 or more | 20.0 % | 20.3 % | 14.9 % | |
High-alcohol-content beverage consumption | ||||
Frequent malt liquor use | 4.0 % | 3.3 % | 17.1 % | <0.001 |
Frequent fortified wine use | 6.5 % | 6.3 % | 10.3 % | <0.001 |
High spirits/liquor volume | 7.4 % | 6.2 % | 29.6 % | <0.001 |
Pro-drunkenness norms (mean) | 0.28 | 0.25 | 0.94 | <0.001 |
Neighborhood characteristics | ||||
Socioeconomic disadvantage (mean) | 17.60 | 17.44 | 20.45 | <0.001 |
% African American (mean) | 9.90 | 9.74 | 12.88 | <0.05 |
% Hispanic/Latino (mean) | 9.95 | 9.99 | 10.78 | >0.10 |
% Urban (mean) | 74.27 | 74.24 | 74.74 | >0.10 |
Overall, 5.0 % of respondents reported experiencing two or more negative drinking consequences in the past year. Drinking consequences were common among respondents living in disadvantaged and African American neighborhoods and among respondents who reported more pro-drunkenness norms and higher consumption of high-alcohol-content beverages, especially spirits/liquor (Table 1).
Correlations between neighborhood disadvantage and the proposed mediators are presented in Table 2. Tetrachoric correlations are presented for the association between two dichotomous variables; all others are Pearson correlation coefficients. Neighborhood disadvantage was significantly correlated with frequent malt liquor and fortified wine use but uncorrelated with pro-drunkenness drinking norms.
Table 2.
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | |
Neighborhood disadvantage | 1.00 | |||||||
% African American | 0.42*** | 1.00 | ||||||
% Hispanic | 0.34*** | 0.09*** | 1.00 | |||||
% Urban | −0.05*** | 0.26*** | 0.28*** | 1.00 | ||||
Frequent malt liquor use | 0.08*** | 0.07*** | 0.04*** | 0.03** | 1.00 | |||
Frequent fortified wine use | 0.05*** | 0.04*** | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.30*** | 1.00 | ||
High liquor/spirits volume | 0.01 | 0.03** | 0.00 | 0.02** | 0.26*** | 0.10** | 1.00 | |
Pro-drunkenness norms | −0.02 | −0.01 | −0.01 | 0.01 | 0.07*** | 0.03* | 0.14*** | 1.00 |
2+ Negative consequences | 0.07*** | 0.03* | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.43*** | 0.15** | 0.46*** | 0.20*** |
Tetrachoric correlations between two dichotomous variables indicated in italics
*P < 0.10, **P < 0.05, ***P < 0.01
Path Analyses
Figure 1 shows all significant paths in the overall final model, with significant mediation pathways highlighted by bold arrows. All paths, including additional coefficients for the control variables, are presented in Table 3. The overall path model achieved excellent fit (see fit statistics included with Figure 1) and explained 47 % of the total variance in negative drinking consequences.
Table 3.
2+ Negative consequences | Malt liquor | Fortified wine | Liquor/spirits | Pro-drunkenness norms | Neighborhood disadvantage | % African American | % Hispanic | % Urban | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neighborhood disadvantage | 0.07** | 0.06* | 0.06** | −0.02 | |||||
% African American | −0.02 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.06** | |||||
% Hispanic | −0.07** | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |||||
% Urban | −0.02 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.04 | |||||
Pro-drunkenness norms | 0.06** | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.18*** | |||||
Frequent malt liquor use | 0.15** | ||||||||
Frequent fortified wine use | −0.01 | ||||||||
High liquor/spirits volume | 0.39*** | ||||||||
Male | 0.09*** | 0.10*** | 0.04* | 0.19*** | 0.12*** | 0.02* | −0.02** | 0.01 | −0.01 |
Age | −0.36*** | −0.28*** | 0.08*** | −0.34*** | −0.02 | −0.02** | −0.02 | −0.04** | |
Married/cohabit | −0.05 | −0.06** | −0.08*** | −0.06*** | −0.01 | −0.04*** | −0.02 | −0.08*** | |
Children in home | −0.05* | −0.11*** | −0.08*** | 0.01 | −0.07*** | −0.02 | −0.20*** | ||
Race/ethnicity (vs. White) | |||||||||
Asian/Pacific Islander | −0.03 | 0.04 | 0.07** | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.04*** | 0.07*** | 0.06** | |
Black/African American | −0.02 | 0.07** | 0.06** | −0.08*** | 0.24*** | 0.58*** | 0.03*** | 0.13*** | |
Hispanic/Latino | −0.02 | −0.01 | 0.01 | −0.09*** | 0.17*** | 0.12*** | 0.44*** | 0.14*** | |
Native Amer./AK Native | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.00 | −0.02 | 0.04*** | 0.04*** | 0.02* | −0.01 | |
Other | −0.08** | 0.07** | 0.00 | −0.03 | 0.00 | 0.02* | 0.07*** | 0.03** | |
Education (vs. college graduate) | |||||||||
Less than high school | 0.11*** | 0.11*** | 0.04 | 0.11*** | −0.01 | 0.03** | −0.09*** | ||
High school graduate | 0.10*** | 0.06 | 0.02 | 0.08*** | −0.02* | −0.04** | −0.14*** | ||
Some college | 0.05 | 0.06 | 0.05* | 0.05*** | −0.02* | 0.01 | −0.08*** | ||
Employment (vs. employed) | |||||||||
Unemployed | 0.05*** | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.02* | 0.01 | ||||
Not in workforce | 0.06*** | 0.03** | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.02 | ||||
Income (vs. >$80,001/year) | |||||||||
$20,000 or less | 0.09** | 0.25*** | 0.01 | 0.02 | −0.07*** | ||||
$20,001−40,000 | 0.01 | 0.20*** | 0.00 | 0.01 | −0.10*** | ||||
$40,001–60,000 | −0.01 | 0.12*** | −0.01 | 0.01 | −0.08*** | ||||
$60,001–80,000 | 0.03 | 0.07*** | 0.00 | 0.00 | −0.05*** | ||||
Missing income | 0.01 | 0.09*** | 0.01 | 0.01 | −0.01 | ||||
Geocoding precision | 0.00 | −0.02 | 0.00 | −0.03** | |||||
R-squared | 0.47 | 0.17 | 0.02 | 0.11 | 0.13 | 0.20 | 0.35 | 0.21 | 0.11 |
*P < 0.10, **P < 0.05, ***P < 0.01
Direct Effects
Neighborhood disadvantage had significant direct paths to frequent malt liquor use, frequent fortified wine use, and to increased negative drinking consequences. Frequent malt liquor use, heavy spirits/liquor use, and pro-drunkenness norms were significantly related to negative drinking consequences.
Mediation Effects
Consumption of high-alcohol-content beverages did not mediate the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and negative drinking consequences, with all specific indirect effects yielding p > 0.10. However, heavy spirits/liquor use was a significant mediator of other neighborhood characteristics (i.e., percent African American, with standardized B = 0.02, p < 0.05). Percent African American was related to increased spirits/liquor consumption and, in turn, reporting more negative drinking consequences. This effect appeared to be strongly mediated, as the residual direct effect of percent African American on negative consequences was not significant (see Table 3).
Pro-drunkenness norms also did not mediate the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and negative drinking consequences. However, pro-drunkenness norms were significantly related to heavy spirits/liquor use, which resulted in a significant indirect path to negative drinking consequences, with standardized B = 0.07, p < 0.01. This effect was not fully mediated, as a significant direct path remained between pro-drunkenness norms and consequences (see Table 3).
Subgroup Differences
Subgroup analyses revealed few differences by gender or race/ethnicity with respect to our hypotheses (full models not shown). For example, consumption of high-alcohol-content beverages did not mediate the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and negative drinking consequences for any racial/ethnic group (all p’s > 0.10 for the specific indirect effect). We did find that the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and frequent fortified wine use was significant and positive for Whites (standardized B = 0.11, p < 0.01) and significant and negative for “others” (standardized B = −0.25, p < 0.05) but not statistically significant for either African Americans (standardized B = −0.06, p > 0.10) or Hispanics (standardized B = 0.13, p’s > 0.10). Additionally, percent Hispanic was significantly associated with reduced negative consequences for women (standardized B = −0.15, p < 0.01) but not for men (standardized B = −0.04, p > 0.10).
Discussion
Neighborhood Disadvantage and Negative Drinking Consequences
As hypothesized, individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods reported more negative work, legal, and health consequences due to their drinking than individuals living in more affluent neighborhoods. Social ecological theory suggests that negative drinking consequences are higher in disadvantaged neighborhoods because of the social and economic conditions under which individuals live.12 Individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods often work in low wage jobs with less autonomy and control over their work, and problem drinking might be less tolerated in such work environments.61 Because crime rates are higher in disadvantaged neighborhoods than in more affluent neighborhoods,62–65 individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods are also more likely to come in contact with local police. This is particularly true for African Americans who are about one and half times (1.4) times more likely than their White peers to be detained by the police.66 Additionally, lower-income individuals report poorer health, in general, than higher-income individuals.67–69
It was not surprising that malt liquor and fortified wine consumption was higher among individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods. Lower income individuals are some of the biggest consumers of malt liquor and low-end fortified wines.70–72 It is likely the low cost of malt liquor and low-end fortified wines increases their availability and consumption among individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods. In 2012, the price of a 40-ounce bottle of Colt 45 malt liquor was less than a gallon of milk in Washington, DC, a city with one of the highest costs of living.73,74
Higher consumption of malt liquor and distilled spirits/liquor was associated with reporting more negative drinking consequences. Previous studies have also shown that malt liquor use is associated with heavy drinking and more aggressive behavior and to a variety of nuisance crimes35,37–39; other studies have shown that distilled spirits/liquor consumption is linked to heavy and dependent drinking, liver cirrhosis, and other health problems such as head and neck cancer mortality.41 It is possible that the relationships between consumption of high-alcohol-content beverages and negative drinking consequences are due to heavy drinking in general. However, using logistic regression models, we verified in this sample that the findings were robust after adjustment for total annual volume of alcohol consumed.
Neighborhood Disadvantage, High Alcohol Content Beverage Consumption, Drinking Norms, and Drinking Consequences
Contrary to our hypothesis, and despite the significant associations of neighborhood disadvantage with malt liquor and fortified wine use, the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and negative drinking consequences was not significantly mediated by our high-alcohol-content beverage consumption or drinking norm variables. Other psychosocial variables not explored in this study (e.g., psychosocial distress) might be intermediate variables between neighborhood disadvantage and negative drinking consequences. Studies have shown that low-income individuals report higher levels of psychological distress,75–77 and some studies have reported that higher levels of psychological distress are associated with increased alcohol consumption and drinking problems.78–80
Although our mediation hypotheses were not supported, we found that living in a neighborhood with a higher proportion of African Americans was associated with heavy distilled spirits/liquor use and, in turn, reporting more negative drinking consequences. One plausible explanation for this finding might be related to the marketing practices of the alcohol industry. To diversify its consumer base, the alcohol industry has used a variety of marketing strategies to reach African Americans, including ethnic-oriented advertisements on billboards in predominately African American neighborhoods (see http://www.epi.umn.edu/alcohol/maltphotos/index.shtm) and product placement of products such as vodka in African American-oriented reality television shows.81 It is also plausible that the heavy consumption of distilled spirits/liquor among African Americans reflects consumer preference and demand. Graves and Kaskutas found that African Americans preferred distilled spirits such as vodka and whiskey to wine.82
Individuals with pro-drunkenness norms consumed more distilled spirits/liquor and, in turn, experienced greater drinking consequences. It is likely individuals with pro-drunkenness norms seek higher-alcohol-content beverages because of their higher alcohol content and the ability to get drunk more quickly.
Strengths and Limitations
Our study is the first to report that living in a neighborhood with a higher proportion of African Americans is associated with heavy distilled spirits/liquor use and, in turn, reporting more negative drinking consequences. This finding is noteworthy as consumption of distilled spirits/liquor in the US has steadily increased since the mid-1990s.41 Other strengths of our study include the use of a large national, multi-ethnic sample, validated measure of neighborhood disadvantaged, and the large variance (47 %) explained by our overall path model.
Several limitations of our study should also be noted. First, our study is cross-sectional, making it difficult to infer causality about whether living in a disadvantaged neighborhood leads to increased malt liquor and fortified wine consumption or whether the reverse is true. A recent literature review revealed that the relationship between indicators of neighborhood socioeconomic status and substance use did not differ markedly for cross-sectional and longitudinal study designs.50
Second, the telephone response rates for our alcohol survey data were somewhat low (56–58 %). Evidence suggests that non-response bias, if present, should have little impact on results related to consumption measures. A series of methodological studies comparing identical questions in telephone and in-person surveys has found comparable estimates across modality for mean alcohol consumption45,83 and only modest and inconsistent mode effects for alcohol harms.84
Third, our malt liquor and fortified wine consumption measures assessed frequency of consuming malt liquor and fortified wine only. Frequency measures have limitations as they do not take into account how much alcohol an individual consumes. Despite these limitations, we found significant effects between neighborhood disadvantage and malt liquor and fortified wine consumption and between malt liquor consumption and drinking consequences. Our results highlight some of the challenges of including measures of high-alcohol-content beverages such as malt liquor in large, national surveys.
Implications
Future studies are needed to identify what aspects of disadvantaged neighborhoods contribute to negative drinking consequences. Once identified, public policies can be developed to alter aspects of disadvantaged neighborhoods that contribute to drinking problems (e.g., higher concentrations of liquor stores). Especially needed are large, longitudinal studies that examine how changes in the socioeconomic characteristics of neighborhoods (e.g., income, educational levels) influence alcohol outcomes. A number of neighborhoods in major US cities have undergone gentrification over the past few decades, making it possible to compare rates of drinking problems prior and following gentrification.32
Future studies also should monitor advertising policies related to distilled spirits as well as current tax policy on distilled spirits/liquor. In the US, per capita distilled spirits/liquor sales have increased slowly since 1994, while taxes on distilled spirits/liquor have declined in real value during this period.41 Studies have shown that increases in taxes on alcohol are associated with reductions in alcohol consumption and drinking problems such as traffic crashes.85–87
Conclusion
Understanding the context in which negative drinking consequences occur is important to reducing future drinking problems. Our results suggest that where an individual lives can affect the types of alcohol outcomes they experience. We found that living in disadvantaged neighborhood was associated with reporting more drinking consequences even after adjusting for individual socioeconomic and race/ethnicity variables. We also found neighborhood racial/ethnic composition was related to negative drinking consequences and that heavy distilled spirits use was an important intermediate variable in this relationship. Prior research has emphasized malt liquor as a possible contributor to negative consequences in disadvantaged minority neighborhoods.37–40 We argue that greater scrutiny of current advertising and tax policy in the US related to distilled spirits/liquor sales is needed to prevent future drinking problems.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Dr. Jason Bond for his helpful feedback throughout this study. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) provided funding for the National Alcohol Surveys (P30 AA05595, T. Greenfield, PI) and research grant (R21 AA019175, K.J. Karriker-Jaffe, PI) that supported this study.
References
- 1.Corrao G, Bagnardi V, Zambon A, La Vecchia C. A meta-analysis of alcohol consumption and the risk of 15 diseases. Prev Med. 2004;38(5):613–619. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2003.11.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Rehm J, Room R, Monteiro M, et al. Alcohol as a risk factor for global burden of disease. Eur Addict Res. 2003;9(4):157–164. doi: 10.1159/000072222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rehm JT, Bondy SJ, Sempos CT, Vuong CV. Alcohol consumption and coronary heart disease morbidity and mortality. Am J Epidemiol. 1997;146(6):495–501. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Alaniz ML. Alcohol availability and targeted advertising in racial/ethnic minority communities. Alcohol Health Res World. 1998;22(4):286–289. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Scribner R, Cohen D, Kaplan S, Allen SH. Alcohol availability and homicide in New Orleans: conceptual considerations for small area analysis of the effect of alcohol outlet density. J Stud Alcohol. 1999;60(3):310–316. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bluthenthal RN, Cohen DA, Farley TA, et al. Alcohol availability and neighborhood characteristics in Los Angeles, California and southern Louisiana. J Urban Health. 2008;85(2):191–205. doi: 10.1007/s11524-008-9255-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gorman DM, Speer PW. The concentration of liquor outlets in an economically disadvantaged city in the northeastern United States. Subst Use Misuse. 1997;32(14):2033–2046. doi: 10.3109/10826089709035618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.LaVeist TA, Wallace JM. Health risk and inequitable distribution of liquor stores in African American neighborhoods. Soc Sci Med. 2000;51(4):613–617. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00004-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Alaniz ML, Wilkes C. Pro-drinking messages and message environments for young adults: the case of alcohol industry advertising in African American, Latino, and Native American communities. J Public Health Policy. 1998;19(4):447–472. doi: 10.2307/3343076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jones-Webb RJ, McKee P, Hannan P, et al. Alcohol and malt liquor availability and promotion and homicide in inner cities. Subst Use Misuse. 2008;43:159–177. doi: 10.1080/10826080701690557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Moore H, Jones-Webb R, Toomey T, Lenk K. Alcohol advertising on billboards, transit shelters, and bus benches in inner-city neighborhoods. Cont Drug Probl. 2008;35(2–3):509–532. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sallis JF, Owen N, Fisher EB. Ecological models of health behavior. In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K, editors. Health behavior and health education: theory, research, and practice. 4. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2008. pp. 464–485. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gruenewald PJ, Johnson FW, Treno AJ. Outlets, drinking and driving: a multilevel analysis of availability. J Stud Alcohol. 2002;63(4):460–468. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2002.63.460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Schonlau M, Scribner R, Farley TA, et al. Alcohol outlet density and alcohol consumption in Los Angeles county and southern Louisiana. Geospat Health. 2008;3(1):91–101. doi: 10.4081/gh.2008.235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Trong KD, Strum R. Alcohol outlets and problem drinking among adults in California. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68(6):923–933. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Anderson P, de Bruijn A, Angus K, Gordon R, Hastings G. Impact of alcohol advertising and media exposure on adolescent alcohol use: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Alcohol Alcohol. 2008;44(3):229–243. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agn115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ellickson PL, Collins RL, Hambarsoomians K, McCafrey DF. Does alcohol advertising promote adolescent drinking? Results from a longitudinal assessment. Addiction. 2005;100(2):235–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.00974.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kwate NOA, Meyer I. Association between residential exposure to outdoor alcohol advertising and problem drinking among African American women in New York City. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(2):228–230. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.132217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Smith LA, Foxcroft DR. The effect of alcohol advertising, marketing and portrayal on drinking behaviour in young people: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. BMC Publ Health. 2009;9:51. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-9-51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Synder LB, Milici FF, Slater M, Sun H, Strizhakova Y. Effects of alcohol advertising and exposure on drinking among youth. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160(1):18–24. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.160.1.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Karriker-Jaffe KJ. Areas of disadvantage: a systematic review of effects of area-level socioeconomic status on alcohol and other drug use. Paper presented at: Annual Alcohol Epidemiology Symposium of the Kettil Bruun Society, 2008; Victoria, BC.
- 22.Cerda M, Diez-Roux AV, Tchetgen ET, Gordon-Larsen P, Kiefe C. The relationship between neighborhood poverty and alcohol use: estimation by marginal structural models. Epidemiology. 2010;21(4):482–489. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181e13539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Stimpson JP, Nash AC, Ju H, Eschbach K. Neighborhood deprivation is associated with lower levels of serum carotenoids among adults participating in the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. J Am Diet Assoc. 2007;107(11):1895–1902. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2007.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jones-Webb RJ, Snowden L, Herd D, Short B, Hannan P. Alcohol-related problems among Black, Hispanic, and White Men: the contribution of neighborhood poverty. J Stud Alcohol. 1997;58:539–545. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1997.58.539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ecob R, Macintyre S. Small area variations in health related behaviors; do these depend on the behavior itself, its measurement, or on personal characteristics? Health Place. 2000;6(4):261–274. doi: 10.1016/S1353-8292(00)00008-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hoffmann JP. The community context of family structure and adolescent drug use. J Marriage Fam. 2002;64(2):314–330. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00314.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Galea S, Ahern J, Tracy M, Rudenstine S, Vlahov D. Education inequality and use of cigarettes, alcohol, and marijuana. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;90(Supp 1):S4–S15. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kulis S, Marsiglia FF, Sicotte D, Nieri T. Neighborhood effects on youth substance use in a southwestern city. Sociol Perspect. 2007;50(2):273–301. doi: 10.1525/sop.2007.50.2.273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Karriker-Jaffe KJ, Zemore SE, Mulia N, Jones-Webb RJ, Bond J, Greenfield TK. Neighborhood disadvantage and adult alcohol outcomes: differential risk by race and gender. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73:865-873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 30.Chuang Y-C, Ennett ST, Bauman KE, Foshee VA. Neighborhood influences on adolescent cigarette and alcohol use: mediating effects through parent and peer behaviors. J Health Soc Behav. 2005;46(2):187–204. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.National Institute on Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse. Rethinking drinking: alcohol and your health. What’s a “standard” drink? http://rethinkingdrinking.niaaa.nih.gov.WhatcountsDrink/WhatsAstandarddrink. Accessed June 13, 2012.
- 32.McKee P, Jones-Webb R, Hannan P, Pham L. Malt liquor marketing in inner cities: the role of neighborhood racial composition. J Ethn Subst Abuse. 2011;10:24–38. doi: 10.1080/15332640.2011.547793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Garfield CF, Chung PJ, Rathouz PJ. Alcohol advertising in magazines and youth readership. JAMA. 2003;289(18):2424–2429. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.18.2424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kwate NOA. Take one down, pass it around, 98 alcohol ads on the wall: outdoor advertising in New York city’s Black neighborhoods. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36:988–990. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bluthenthal RN, Brown-Taylor D, Guzan-Becerra N, Robinson PL. Characteristics of malt liquor beer drinkers in a low-income, minority community sample. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29(3):402–409. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000156118.74728.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Chen MJ, Paschall MJ. Malt liquor use, heavy problem drinking and other problem behaviors in a sample of community college students. J Stud Alcohol. 2003;64(6):835–842. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Collins RL, Bradizza CM, Vincent PC. Young-adult malt liquor drinkers: prediction of alcohol problems and marijuana use. Addict Behav. 2007;2(21):138–146. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.2.138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jones-Webb R, McKee P, Toomey T, et al. Regulating malt liquor in urban areas in the U.S. Contemp Drug Probl. 2011;38:41–59. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Tarni J. Post-assessment Seattle, Washington Alcohol Impact, Social and Economic Science Research Center (SESRC), Washington State University No. Data Report 09-032, 2009.
- 40.Vilamovska AM, Brown-Taylor D, Bluthenthal RN. Adverse drinking-related consequences among lower income, racial, and ethnic minority drinkers: cross-sectional results. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009;33(4):645–653. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kerr WC, Ye Y. Beverage-specific mortality relationships in US population data. Contemp Drug Probl. 2011;38:561–578. doi: 10.1177/009145091103800406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Room R. Normative perspectives on alcohol use and problems. J Drug Issue. 1975;5:358–368. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Greenfield TK, Room R. Situational norms for drinking and drunkenness: trends in the U.S. adult population, 1979–1990. Addiction. 1997;92(1):33–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1997.tb03636.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Midanik LT, Greenfield TK. Defining “current drinkers” in national surveys: results of the 2000 National Alcohol Survey. Addiction. 2003;98(4):517–522. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00344.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Midanik LT, Greenfield TK. Telephone versus in-person interviews for alcohol use: results of the 2000 National Alcohol Survey. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2003;72(3):209–214. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(03)00204-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Groves RM. Nonresponse rates and nonresponse bias in household surveys. Public Opin Q. 2006;70(5):646–675. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfl033. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Keeter S, Kennedy C, Dimock M, Best J, Craighill P. Gauging the impact of growing nonresponse on estimates from a national RDD telephone survey. Public Opin Q. 2006;70(5):759–779. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfl035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.U.S. Census Bureau. Census 2000 Summary File 3—United States http://www.census.gov/Press-Release/www/2002/sumfile3.html. Washington, DC: Public Information Office, U.S. Census Bureau; 2002. Accessed January 21, 2009.
- 49.Krieger N, Chen JT, Waterman PD, Soobader M-J, Subramanian SV, Carson R. Geocoding and monitoring of US socioeconomic inequalities in mortality and cancer incidence: does the choice of area-based measure and geographic level matter? Am J Epidemiol. 2002;156(5):471–482. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Karriker-Jaffe KJ. Areas of disadvantage: a systematic review of effects of area-level socioeconomic status on substance use. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2011;30(1):84–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-3362.2010.00191.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Karriker-Jaffe KJ, Kaskutas LA. Neighborhood socioeconomic context of alcohol use: a measurement validation study. [Abstract] Research Society on Alcoholism. San Diego, CA. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009;33(6):190A. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Greenfield TK, Rogers JD. Alcoholic beverage choice, risk perception, and self-reported drunk driving: effects of measurement on risk analysis. Addiction. 1999;94(11):1735–1743. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9411173510.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Caetano R, Tam TW. Prevalence and correlates of DSM-IV and ICD-10 alcohol dependence: 1990 U.S. National Alcohol Survey. Alcohol Alcohol. 1995;30(2):177–186. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Mplus version 5.1 [computer program]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2008.
- 55.Hays RD, Stacy AW, Widaman KF, DiMatteo MR, Downey R. Multistage path models of adolescent alcohol and drug use: a reanalysis. J Drug Issues. 1986;16(3):357–369. [Google Scholar]
- 56.MacKinnon DP. Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. New York: Laurence Erlbaum Associates; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 6. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2010. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Edessa K, Werch C, Moore M. High potency and other alcohol beverage consumption among adolescents. J Alcohol Drug Educ. 2005;49:45–67. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Snijders T, Bosker R. Multilevel analysis. An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage Publications; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Karriker-Jaffe KJ, Zemore SE, Mulia N, et al. Neighborhood disadvantage and adult alcohol outcomes: differential risk by race and gender. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73:865-873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 61.Ehrenreich B. Nickel and dimed: on not getting by in America. New York: Henry Hold and Company; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Gjelsvik A, Zierler S, Blume J. Homicide risk across race and class: a small-area analysis in Massachusetts and Rhode Island. J Urban Health. 2004;81(4):702–718. doi: 10.1093/jurban/jth152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Krivo LJ, Peterson RD. Extremely disadvantaged neighborhoods and urban crime. Soc Forces. 1996;75(2):619–650. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Krueger PM, Bond Huie SA, Rogers RG, Hummer RA. Neighborhoods and homicide mortality: an analysis of race/ethnic differences. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2004;58:223–230. doi: 10.1136/jech.2003.011874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Jones-Webb R, Wall M. Neighborhood racial/ethnic concentration, social disadvantage, and homicide risk: an ecological analysis of 10 U.S. cities. J Urban Health. 2008;85(5):662–676. doi: 10.1007/s11524-008-9302-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, OJJDP Statistical Briefing Book, Washington, DC, May 10, 2006.
- 67.Lynch J, Kaplan G. Socioeconomic position. In: Bekman LF, Kawachi I, editors. Social epidemiology. 1. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 13–35. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Davey Smith G. Health inequalities: life course approaches. Bristol: The Policy Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 69.Davy Smith G, Wentworth D, Neaton JD, Stamler R, Stamler J. Socioeconomic differentials in mortality risk among men screened for the multiple risk factor intervention trial: II. Black men. Am J Public Health. 1996;86:497–504. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.86.4.497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Behavioral tracking study. Milwaukee: Miller Brewing Company; 2000. p. 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Bradizza CA, Collins RL, Vincent PC, Falco DL. It does the job: young adults discuss their malt liquor consumption. Addict Behav. 2006;31(9):1559–1577. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Chen MJ, Paschall MJ, Grube JW. Motives for malt liquor consumption in a sample of community college students. Addict Behav. 2006;21(2):1295–1307. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.BeerMenus. Colt 45 (6.0) ABV. 2012. http:www.beermenus.com/beers/colt-45. Accessed on June 13, 2012.
- 74.Cost of Living in Washington DC. 2012. http://living-in-washingtondc.com/costofliving-inwashingtondec.php. Accessed on June 13, 2012.
- 75.Adler N, Boyce T, Chesney M, et al. Socioeconomic status and health: the challenge of the gradient. Am Psychol. 1994;49:15–24. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.49.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Orpana HM, Lemyre L, Gravel R. Income and psychological distress: the role of the social environment. Stat Can Health Rep. 2009;20:21–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Turner RJ, Lloyd D. The stress process and the social distribution of depression. J Health Soc Behav. 1999;40:374–404. doi: 10.2307/2676332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Geisner IM, Larimer ME, Neighbors C. The relationship among alcohol use, related problems, and symptoms of psychological distress: gender as a moderator in a college sample. Addict Behav. 2004;29:843–848. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Mathiesen EF, Nome S, Eisemann M, Richter J. Drinking patterns, psychological distress and quality of life in a Norwegian general population-based sample. Qual Life Res. 2012;21:1527-1536. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 80.Jones-Webb RJ, Jacobs J, Flack J, Liu K. Relationships between depressive symptoms, anxiety, alcohol intake and blood pressure: results from the CARDIA study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1996;20:420–427. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01069.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Bleyer S. Smirnoff looks to woo minorities with reality show. 2010. http://thehighlow.com/2010/10/smirnoff-looks-to-woo-minorities-with-reality-show. Accessed July 31, 2012.
- 82.Graves K, Kaskutas LA. Beverage choice among native American and African American.2002;26:218-222 [PubMed]
- 83.Greenfield TK, Midanik LT, Rogers JD. Effects of telephone versus face-to-face interview modes on reports of alcohol consumption. Addiction. 2000;95(2):227–284. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.95227714.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Midanik LT, Greenfield TK, Rogers JD. Reports of alcohol-related harm: telephone versus face-to-face interviews. J Stud Alcohol. 2001;62(1):74–78. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2001.62.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Chaloupka FJ, Grossman M, Saffer H. The effects of price on alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems. Alcohol Health Res World. 2002;26(1):22–34. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Cook PJ, Moore MJ. The economics of alcohol abuse and alcohol-control policies. Health Aff. 2002;21(2):120–133. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.21.2.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Cook PJ, Moore MJ. Violence reduction through restrictions on alcohol availability. Alcohol Health Res World. 1993;17(2):151–156. [Google Scholar]