Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2008 Dec 3;28(49):13248–13257. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3027-08.2008

Central Amygdala Extracellular Signal-Regulated Kinase Signaling Pathway Is Critical to Incubation of Opiate Craving

Yan-Qin Li 1,*, Fang-Qiong Li 1,*, Xiao-Yi Wang 1, Ping Wu 1, Mei Zhao 1, Chun-Mei Xu 1, Yavin Shaham 2, Lin Lu 1,
PMCID: PMC3733549  NIHMSID: NIHMS82327  PMID: 19052216

Abstract

Cue-induced drug-seeking in rodents progressively increases after withdrawal from operant self-administration of cocaine, heroin, methamphetamine, and alcohol, a phenomenon termed “incubation of drug craving.” Here, we used the opiate drug morphine and explored whether incubation of drug craving also occurs in a pavlovian conditioned place preference (CPP) procedure in which rats learn to associate drug effects with a distinct environmental context. We also explored the role of amygdala extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) and cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) in this incubation. We found that the expression of morphine CPP progressively increases over the first 14 d after the last drug exposure in rats receiving four pairings of low-dose (1 or 3 mg/kg) but not high-dose (10 mg/kg) morphine with a distinct environment. The progressive increase in low-dose (3 mg/kg) morphine CPP was associated with increased ERK phosphorylation (a measure of ERK activity) and CREB (a downstream target of ERK) phosphorylation in central but not basolateral amygdala. Furthermore, inhibition of central but not basolateral amygdala ERK and CREB phosphorylation by U0126 [1,4-diamino-2,3-dicyano-1,4-bis(o-aminophenylmercapto)butadiene] decreased the enhanced (incubated) drug CPP after 14 d of withdrawal from morphine. Finally, stimulation of central amygdala ERK and CREB phosphorylation by NMDA enhanced drug CPP after 1 d of withdrawal from morphine, an effect reversed by U0126. These findings indicate that the rat's response to environmental cues previously paired with morphine progressively increases or incubates over the first 14 d of withdrawal from low but not high morphine doses. Additionally, this “incubation of morphine craving” is mediated by acute activation of central amygdala ERK pathway.

Keywords: amygdala, morphine, conditioned place preference, incubation, ERK, CREB, relapse

Introduction

Relapse to drug use in humans can occur after prolonged abstinence and is often precipitated by exposure to craving-provoking, drug-associated cues (O'Brien et al., 1992). Results from operant self-administration studies with cocaine (Neisewander et al., 2000; Grimm et al., 2001), heroin (Shalev et al., 2001), and methamphetamine (Shepard et al., 2004) demonstrate that the rat's response to drug cues progressively increases over the first weeks after withdrawal from drugs. This “incubation” phenomenon was demonstrated in extinction (Lu et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2006), cue-induced reinstatement (Grimm et al., 2003), and acquisition of new response (Di Ciano and Everitt, 2004) procedures. These findings suggest that craving, a motivational state elicited by exposure to drug cues that often precedes and accompanies drug-seeking, incubates over time (Lu et al., 2004).

Here, we assessed whether “incubation of reward craving” is also manifested in a pavlovian conditioned place preference (CPP) procedure in which during the training phase one context is paired with drug injections, whereas another context is paired with vehicle injections. During subsequent drug-free CPP tests, rats choose between the drug- and vehicle-paired contexts. Increased preference for the drug context serves as measures of the rewarding effects of the drug (Van der Kooy, 1987) and incentive motivational effects of drug cues (Mueller and Stewart, 2000).

To date, there is no evidence that the rat's response to drug cues in the CPP procedure increases over time. Several investigators reported that CPP is maintained at similar magnitude for 2–12 weeks after withdrawal from morphine (Mucha and Iversen, 1984; Vezina et al., 1987; Mueller et al., 2002) or for 6 weeks after withdrawal from cocaine (Mueller and Stewart, 2000). In other studies, CPP became weaker over the first month after withdrawal from morphine or cocaine (Tzschentke and Schmidt, 1995; Lu et al., 2000b, 2001a; Wang et al., 2000). In these studies, the experimental procedures typically resulted in high drug CPP immediately after training. Thus, in studies in which CPP was maintained over time, a ceiling effect may have prevented the detection of additional increases in drug CPP after prolonged withdrawal.

Here, we describe experimental parameters that led to progressive increases in drug CPP after withdrawal from morphine. This was achieved by assessing drug CPP 7 or 14 d after withdrawal from low (1–3 mg/kg) or high (10 mg/kg) morphine doses, which induce weak or strong CPP, respectively, after 1 withdrawal day. We found progressive increases in low- but not high-dose morphine CPP. Subsequently, we studied the role of amygdala extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) in this incubation. The ERK pathway has been implicated in several behavioral effects of abused drugs (Lu et al., 2006; Girault et al., 2007). Previously, we found that central amygdala ERK activity mediates the incubation of cocaine craving in the operant self-administration procedure (Lu et al., 2005). We also assessed whether time-dependent increases in morphine are associated with activation of the transcription factor cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB). We studied whether CREB activity correlates with incubation of morphine craving because CREB is a downstream cellular target of ERK (Thomas and Huganir, 2004) that was implicated in morphine reward and dependence (Blendy and Maldonado, 1998; Carlezon et al., 2005).

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Male Sprague Dawley rats (weighing 220–240 g on arrival) were housed in groups of five in a temperature-controlled (23 ± 2°C) and humidity-controlled (50 ± 5%) animal facility. The rats were maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle with access to food and water ad libitum. The experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and the procedures were approved by the Local Animal Care and Use Committee. A total of 435 rats were used in the experiments described below. This number includes 13 (eight in experiment 2 and five in experiment 5) naive rats that did not participate in the behavioral experiments; the brains of these naive rats were taken for assessing basal levels of ERK and CREB, which were used as a reference point for the graphical presentation of the molecular data (see figures).

Surgery

Rats (weighing 300–320 g when surgery began) were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital anesthesia (60 mg/kg, i.p.). Guide cannulas (23 gauge; Plastics One) were bilaterally implanted 1 mm above the basolateral and central amygdala. The respective coordinates (Paxinos and Watson, 2005) were as follows: anteroposterior (AP), −2.9 mm; mediolateral (ML), ±5.0 mm; dorsoventral (DV), −8.5 mm; and AP, −2.9 mm; ML, ±4.2 mm; DV, −7.8 mm (Lu et al., 2005, 2007; Wang et al., 2008). The cannulas were anchored to the skull with stainless steel screws and dental cement. A stainless steel blocker was inserted into each cannula to keep them patent and prevent infection. The rats were allowed to recover from surgery for 5–7 d.

Intracranial injections

U0126 [1,4-diamino-2,3-dicyano-1,4-bis(o-aminophenylmercapto)butadiene] (100 ng/0.5 μl) or NMDA (250 ng/0.5 μl) was infused bilaterally into central or basolateral amygdala with Hamilton syringes connected to 30 gauge injectors (Plastics One). NMDA (Research Biochemicals) was dissolved in sterile saline, and the pH of the drug solution was adjusted to 7.4 by adding NaOH. U0126 [a MAP kinase kinase (MEK) inhibitor; Calbiochem) was dissolved in 20% DMSO. A total volume of 0.5 μl was infused bilaterally over 1 min, and the injector was kept in place for an additional 1 min to allow for diffusion. The doses of U0126 and NMDA were based on our previous study (Lu et al., 2005). We could not verify cannula placements by histological techniques because the brains of the rats were used for the Western blot assays. However, our success rates in previous studies were ∼85–90% (Lu et al., 2007), and, perhaps more importantly, the anatomical selectivity of our drug injections is verified by the demonstration of selective changes in phosphorylated ERK (pERK) and pCREB in the central but not basolateral injections and vice versa (see figures).

Tissue sample preparation

The procedure is based on that used in our previous study (Lu et al., 2005). Most rats were decapitated without anesthesia 30 min after the end of the 15 min CPP test. Other rats were decapitated at different time points after the end of 7.5 or 15 min CPP tests (see Experiment 2 and Fig. 2). Rats in the different control conditions were decapitated just before the CPP tests. After decapitation, the brains of all rats were rapidly extracted and frozen in −60°C N-hexane. The brains were then transferred to a −80°C freezer. We used a freezing cryostat (−20°C) to make 1-mm-thick coronal sections located approximately −2.5 to 3.0 mm from bregma. Bilateral tissue punches (16 gauge) of the central and basolateral amygdalae were then taken. Tissue punches were homogenized (10–15 s for three times, 5 s interval) with electrical disperser (Wiggenhauser) after 30 min in radioimmunoprecipitation assay (RIPA) lysis buffer [20 mm Tris, pH 7.5, 150 mm NaCl, 1% Triton X-100, 2.5 mm sodium pyrophosphate, 1 mm EDTA, 1% Na3VO4, 0.5 μg/ml leupeptin, and 1 mm phenylmethanesulfonyl fluoride (Beyotime Biotechnology)]. Then, the tissue homogenates were subjected to 10,000 × g centrifugation at 4°C for 20 min. The above procedures were performed at 0–4°C. The protein concentrations of all samples were determined using the bichinconinic acid assay (Beyotime Biotechnology). Samples were further diluted in RIPA lysis buffer to equalize the protein concentrations.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

The progressive increases in morphine CPP is associated with increases in ERK phosphorylation in central amygdala. A, Progressive increases in morphine (3 mg/kg) CPP after withdrawal from the drug. Data are the mean ± SEM of preference score in seconds during the CPP preference tests (4 groups). *p < 0.05, different from withdrawal day 1. B, C, Phosphorylated and total ERK in the central and basolateral amygdala; data are presented as a percentage (mean ± SEM) of phosphorylated and total ERK of naive control rats (n = 8). In the central amygdala, the morphine CPP test increased phosphorylated ERK after 14 d of withdrawal. In the basolateral amygdala, previous exposure to morphine increased phosphorylated ERK after 1 d of withdrawal; this effect was independent of the CPP test. *p < 0.05, different from the other experimental groups (n = 7–8 per group). No group differences were observed for total ERK.

Western blot assays

The procedures of the assays were based on those used in our previous studies (Lu et al., 2003, 2005). Four times loading buffer (16% glycerol, 20% mercaptoethanol, 2% SDS, and 0.05% bromophenol blue) was added to each sample (3:1; sample: loading buffer) before boiling for 3 min. Samples were cooled and subjected to SDS-PAGE (10% acrylamide/0.27% N,N′-methylenebisacryalamide resolving gel) for ∼30 min at 80 V in stacking gel and ∼1 h at 120 V in resolving gel. For each electrophoresis, increasing amounts of protein pooled from all samples were electrophoresed to produce a standard curve. Proteins were transferred electrophoretically to Immobilon-P transfer membranes (Millipore) at 0.25 A for 3 h. Membranes were washed with TBST (Tris-buffered saline plus 0.05% Tween 20, pH 7.4) and then dipped in blocking buffer (5% skimmed dry milk in TBST) overnight at 4°C. The next day, the membranes were incubated for 1 h at room temperature on orbital shaker with anti-pERK antibody (1:500; R & D Systems), anti-ERK antibody (1:500; R & D Systems), anti-pCREB antibody (1:500; Upstate Biotechnology), or anti-CREB antibody (1:500; Upstate Biotechnology) in TBST plus 5% BSA and 0.05% sodium azide. After four 6-min washes in TBST buffer, the blots were incubated for 45 min at room temperature on shaker with horseradish peroxidase (HRP)-conjugated secondary antibody (goat anti-rabbit IgG; Santa Cruz PI-1000; Vector Laboratories) diluted 1:5000 in blocking buffer. The blots were then washed four times for 6 min in TBST and then incubated with a layer of mix of Super Signal Enhanced chemiluminescence (ECL) substrate (Detection Reagents 1 and 2 at a 1:1 ratio; Pierce) for 1 min at room temperature. Excess mix was dripped off before blots were wrapped with a clean piece of Saran Wrap (no bubbles between blot and wrap) and then exposed against x-ray film (Eastman Kodak) for 5–60 s. Band intensities for ERK1 and ERK2, and pERK1 and pERK2 were quantified by two observers (blind to experimental groups) using Quantity One software (version 4.4.0) from Bio-Rad. Band intensities from each test sample were compared with the band intensities from the standard curves. The amount of protein of interest in the samples was interpolated from standard curves.

Conditioned place preference

The CPP apparatus consisted of five identical three-chamber polyvinyl chloride (PVC) boxes. Two large side chambers (27.9 × 21.0 × 20.9 high) were separated by a smaller chamber (12.1 × 21.0 × 20.9 cm with smooth PVC floor). The two larger chambers differ in their floor texture (bar or grid) and provided the distinct contexts that were paired with morphine or saline injections. The three chambers were separated by manual guillotine doors and were equipped with infrared photobeams connected to a computer that recorded the rat's location in the chambers.

To determine baseline place preference, the rats were initially placed in the middle chamber with the doors removed for 15 min (preconditioning test). Conditioning was performed using an unbiased, counterbalanced protocol, as described in our previous work (Wang et al., 2008). Twenty-one of the 435 rats that were tested for initial CPP preference showed strong unconditioned side preference (>540 s in any chamber) and therefore were excluded.

On the subsequent conditioning days, each rat was trained for 8 consecutive days with alternate injections of morphine (0, 1, 3, and 10 mg/kg, s.c.) and saline (1 ml/kg, s.c.). After each injection, the rats were confined to the morphine or saline conditioned chamber for 45 min before returning to their home cages. Subsequently, the expression of morphine CPP was assessed 1, 7, or 14 d after the last training session. The experimental conditions during the postconditioning tests were the same as those described for the preconditioning test. The CPP score was defined as the time (in seconds) spent in the morphine-paired chamber minus the time spent in the saline-paired chamber.

Experiment 1: effect of morphine training dose and withdrawal day on morphine CPP

Between-subjects assessment.

We first assessed morphine CPP in 12 different groups of rats (n = 9–10 per group) that were injected with saline or morphine (1, 3, or 10 mg/kg, s.c.) and tested for CPP 1, 7, or 14 d after the last training session. The experimental design included the between-subjects factors of morphine dose (0, 1, 3, and 10 mg/kg) and withdrawal day (1, 7, and 14); the dependent measure was CPP score in seconds.

Within-subjects assessment.

We next assessed morphine CPP in four different groups of rats (n = 9–10 per group) that were injected with saline or morphine (1, 3, or 10 mg/kg, s.c.) and were repeatedly tested for CPP 1, 7, and 14 d after the last training session. The experimental design included the between subjects factor of morphine dose and the within-subjects factor of withdrawal day; the dependent measure was CPP score.

Experiment 2: effect of morphine CPP testing after 1 or 14 withdrawal days on amygdala ERK and CREB

We initially assessed the effect of CPP testing on pERK and pCREB in central and basolateral amygdala in rats that underwent CPP training with 3 mg/kg morphine or saline. For this purpose, we used eight groups of rats (n = 7–8 per group) in an experimental design that included the between-subjects factors of morphine dose (0 and 3 mg/kg) and withdrawal day (1 and 14), and CPP test (no and yes). The dependent measures were the CPP score (assessed in the four groups that underwent CPP testing), and pERK and pCREB levels in central and basolateral amygdala (assessed in all eight groups). Thirty minutes after the end of the 15 min CPP test, all rats were decapitated and their brains were extracted for subsequent determination of pERK and pCREB in central and basolateral amygdala by Western blotting.

We subsequently assessed in eight groups of rats (n = 8 per group) the time course of pERK and pCREB activation during and after the morphine CPP tests; these tests were performed on withdrawal day 14. The brains of rats from two groups were taken within 5 or 30 min after a shorter 7.5 min CPP test, whereas the brains of rats from five other groups were taken within 5 min, or 15, 30, 60, or 180 min after a regular 15 min CPP test. The brains of rats from the eighth (control) group were taken immediately before the CPP test. The rats from all groups were trained for morphine (3 mg/kg) CPP as described above.

Experiment 3: effect of inhibition of amygdala ERK and CREB on morphine CPP after 14 withdrawal days

To determine the functional role of amygdala ERK (and potentially CREB; see Discussion) in the progressive increases in morphine CPP, we inhibited central or basolateral amygdala ERK and CREB activity with the selective MEK antagonist U0126 (Davies et al., 2000) after 14 d of withdrawal from morphine. For this purpose, we used four groups of rats (n = 7–8 per group) in an experimental design that included the between-subjects factors of U0126 dose (0 and 100 ng/side) and amygdala nucleus (central and basolateral); the dependent measures were the CPP score and pERK and pCREB levels in central and basolateral amygdala. The morphine training dose was 3 mg/kg, and U0126 or its vehicle was injected 30 min before CPP testing. Thirty minutes after the end of the 15 min postconditioning test, all rats were decapitated and their brains were extracted for subsequent determination of pERK and pCREB.

Experiment 4: effect of activation of central amygdala ERK and CREB on morphine CPP after 1 withdrawal day

To further determine the role of central amygdala ERK and CREB in the progressive increase in morphine CPP after withdrawal, we first injected NMDA (which activates ERK and CREB) into the central amygdala on withdrawal day 1. We tested whether the induction of pERK and pCREB in the central amygdala would enhance morphine CPP during early withdrawal, when the conditioned response is weak. Because there are no selective agonists of the ERK pathway, we used NMDA to induce ERK and CREB phosphorylation (Fiore et al., 1993). For this purpose, we used two groups of rats (n = 8 per group) in an experimental design that included the between-subjects factor of NMDA dose (0 and 250 ng/side); the dependent measures were the CPP score and pERK and pCREB levels in central and basolateral amygdala. The morphine training dose was 3 mg/kg. NMDA or its vehicle was injected 5 min before CPP testing.

NMDA activates several intracellular signaling pathways in addition to the ERK pathway (Thomas and Huganir, 2004). Therefore, we subsequently determined whether the increase in morphine CPP on withdrawal day 1 by central amygdala injections of NMDA is dependent on ERK activation (i.e., reversed by U0126). For this purpose, we used four groups of rats (n = 7–8 per group) in an experimental design that included the between-subjects factors of NMDA dose (0 and 250 ng/side) and U0126 dose (0 and 100 ng/side); the dependent measures were the CPP score and pERK and pCREB values in the central amygdala. U0126 or its vehicle was injected 30 min before CPP testing; NMDA or its vehicle was injected 5 min before testing.

Experiment 5: effect of morphine CPP training on ERK and CREB activity in basolateral amygdala

A surprising finding in experiment 2 was that four injections of a low morphine dose (3 mg/kg) caused increased pERK and pCREB levels in the basolateral amygdala 1 d after the last drug injection; this effect occurred independent of CPP testing (see Figs. 2, 3). Therefore, in experiment 5, we further explored this unexpected finding by determining whether this effect of morphine exposure is dependent on CPP training. For this purpose, we used four groups of rats (n = 5 per group) in an experimental design that included the between-subjects factor of CPP training (no and yes) and morphine dose (0 and 3 mg/kg); the dependent measures were pERK and pCREB levels in the basolateral amygdala. The rats that did not undergo CPP training were injected with morphine or vehicle in their home cage at the same time of the day as the rats that underwent the 8 d CPP training. One day after the last morphine injection, all rats were decapitated and their brains were extracted for subsequent determination of pERK and pCREB in the basolateral amygdala.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

The progressive increase in morphine CPP is associated with increases in CREB phosphorylation in central amygdala. A, B, Phosphorylated and total CREB in the central and basolateral amygdala; data are presented as a percentage (mean ± SEM) of phosphorylated and total CREB of naive control rats (n = 8). In the central amygdala, the morphine CPP test increased phosphorylated CREB after 14 d of withdrawal. In the basolateral amygdala, previous exposure to morphine increased phosphorylated CREB after 1 d of withdrawal; this effect was independent of the CPP test. *p < 0.05, different from the other experimental groups (n = 7–8 per group). No group differences were observed for total CREB. The morphine/saline CPP test data that correspond to the molecular data in this figure are presented in Figure 2A.

Data analysis

Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. Data were analyzed with ANOVAs using the appropriate between- and within-subjects factors for the different experiments (see Results). Significant main effects and interactions (p < 0.05) from the factorial ANOVAs were followed by simple ANOVAs and Tukey's post hoc tests. The experimental manipulations had similar effects on ERK1 and ERK2 phosphorylation. Thus, these values were added and the analyses were performed on the combined values of phosphorylated ERK (Kelleher et al., 2004; Lu et al., 2005). Because our multifactorial ANOVAs yield multiple main effects and interaction effects, we only report significant effects that are critical for the interpretation of the data in Results. Additionally, for clarity purposes, post hoc analyses are indicated by asterisks in the figures but are not described in Results.

Results

Effect of morphine training dose and withdrawal day on morphine CPP

Morphine CPP progressively increased after withdrawal from the drug in rats previously exposed to low (1 or 3 mg/kg) but not high (10 mg/kg) morphine doses (Fig. 1). This effect was observed when different groups were tested at the different withdrawal days or when rats were repeatedly tested 1, 7, or 14 d after withdrawal from morphine.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Time-dependent increases in drug CPP after withdrawal from low but not high doses of morphine. Data are the mean ± SEM of preference score in seconds (time in the morphine-paired chamber minus time in the saline-paired chamber) during the CPP preference tests performed 1, 7, or 14 d after withdrawal from morphine. A, Between-subjects assessment using 12 groups of rats (n = 9–10 per group); each group was tested at a single withdrawal day. B, Within-subjects assessment using four groups of rats (n = 9–10 per group) that were repeatedly tested after 1, 7, and 14 d of withdrawal from morphine. p < 0.05, different from withdrawal days 1 (*) and 7 (#) within each morphine training dose.

Between-subjects assessment

We assessed morphine CPP in 12 different groups of rats that were injected with saline or morphine (1, 3, or 10 mg/kg) and tested for CPP 1, 7, or 14 d after the last training session. We analyzed the CPP score (time in the morphine-paired chamber minus time in the saline-paired chamber during testing) using ANOVA with the between-subjects factors of morphine dose and withdrawal day. This analysis revealed significant effects of morphine dose (F(3,108) = 61.62, p < 0.01), withdrawal day (F(2,108) = 22.97, p < 0.01), and an interaction between these two factors (F(6,108) = 5.17, p < 0.05). One-way ANOVA within each morphine dose revealed significant effects of withdrawal day for the training CPP doses of 1 and 3 mg/kg (F (2,27) = 51.2, p < 0.01; and F (2,27) = 36.8, p < 0.01), respectively, but not for the 10 mg/kg dose (p > 0.05). Post hoc group differences within each time point are indicated in Figure 1A.

Within-subjects assessment

We assessed morphine CPP in four different groups of rats that were injected with saline or morphine (1, 3, or 10 mg/kg) and were repeatedly tested for CPP 1, 7, and 14 d after the last training session. We analyzed the CPP score using repeated-measures ANOVA with the between-subjects factors of morphine dose (0, 1, 3, and 10 mg/kg) and the within-subjects factor of withdrawal day (1, 7, and 14). This analysis revealed significant effects of morphine dose (F (3,36) = 24.3, p < 0.01), withdrawal day (F (2,36) = 32.5, p < 0.01), and an interaction between these two factors (F (6,36) = 3.8, p < 0.05). One-way ANOVA within each morphine dose revealed significant effects of withdrawal day for the training CPP doses of 1 and 3 mg/kg (F(2,27) = 12.5, p < 0.01; and F(2,27) = 8.9, p < 0.01), respectively, but not for the 10 mg/kg dose (p > 0.05). Post hoc group differences within each time point are indicated in Figure 1B.

Effect of morphine CPP testing after 1 or 14 withdrawal days on amygdala ERK and CREB

As in experiment 1, in rats trained for morphine CPP with a low morphine dose (3 mg/kg), the CPP score during testing was higher after 14 withdrawal days than after 1 d (Fig. 2A). This progressive increase in morphine CPP after withdrawal from the drug was associated with increased pERK and pCREB in central but not basolateral amygdala (Figs. 2, 3). The experimental manipulations had no effects on total ERK and CREB in the central or basolateral amygdala (Figs. 2, 3) (statistical analyses not presented). Additionally, independent of the CPP testing, morphine exposure was associated with increased pERK and pCREB in the basolateral amygdala after 1 but not 14 withdrawal days.

Morphine CPP

We analyzed the morphine CPP score with ANOVA using the between-subjects factors of morphine dose (0 and 3 mg/kg) and withdrawal day (1 and 14). This analysis revealed significant effects of morphine dose (F(1,36) = 8.1, p < 0.01), withdrawal day (F(1,36) = 38.7, p < 0.01), and an interaction between these two factors (F(1,36) = 8.36, p < 0.01).

pERK and pCREB

Analyses were performed separately for central and basolateral amygdala and included the between-subjects factors of morphine dose (0 and 3 mg/kg), withdrawal day (1 and 14), and CPP test (no and yes).

Central amygdala

The statistical analysis for both pERK and pCREB revealed significant interactions of morphine dose × withdrawal day by CPP test (F(1,56) = 6.2, p < 0.01 for pERK; and F(1,56) = 4.3, p < 0.05 for pCREB, respectively). These triple interactions are attributable to the fact that pERK and pCREB levels in morphine-exposed rats that underwent the CPP test after 14 withdrawal days were higher than those of the other seven experimental groups (p < 0.01) (Figs. 2, 3).

Basolateral amygdala

The statistical analysis for both pERK and pCREB revealed significant interactions of morphine dose × withdrawal day (F(1,56) = 6.8, p < 0.01 for pERK; and F(1,56) = 10.7, p < 0.01 for pCREB, respectively). The interactions between these two factors and CPP test were not significant. The second-order interaction between morphine dose and withdrawal day is attributable to the fact that, independent of CPP testing, pERK and pCREB levels were higher in day 1 morphine withdrawal rats compared with all other experimental groups (p < 0.01) (Figs. 2, 3).

Finally, because the data described in Figures 2 and 3 are of pERK and pCREB values of rats with their brains taken 30 min after the end of the CPP tests, we also determined the time course pERK and pCREB during and after CPP tests that were conducted on withdrawal day 14. We found that pERK and pCREB values are rapidly increased during and immediately after the CPP test session; these values remained elevated for at least 60 min and returned to baseline (no test) levels after 180 min (Fig. 4). As mentioned above, the brains of rats from two groups were taken within 5 or 30 min after a shorter 7 min CPP test, whereas the brains of rats from five other groups were taken within 5 min, or 15, 30, 60, or 180 min after a regular 15 min CPP test. The brains of rats from the eighth (control) group were taken immediately before the CPP test. The statistical analyses included the between-subjects factor of group and revealed a significant effect of this factor for both pERK and pCREB (F(7,56) = 18.2 and F(7,56) = 16.7; p values <0.01, respectively). Post hoc group differences are depicted in Figure 4. The mean CPP score of the five groups of rats exposed to the 15 min test was 227.6 ± 12.3 s.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Time course of ERK and CREB activity during and after exposure to a CPP test on withdrawal day 14. A, B, Phosphorylated ERK and CREB in the central amygdala before (no test), during, and after morphine CPP testing. Data are presented as a percentage (mean ± SEM) of phosphorylated ERK and CREB of naive control rats (n = 8). *p < 0.05, different from the no-test group.

Effect of inhibition of amygdala ERK and CREB on morphine CPP after 14 withdrawal days

Inhibition of central amygdala but not basolateral amygdala pERK and pCREB decreased morphine (3 mg/kg) CPP after 14 withdrawal days (Fig. 5). The ANOVA for the CPP score included the between-subjects factors of U0126 dose (0 and 100 ng/side) and amygdala nucleus (central and basolateral). This analysis revealed a significant interaction between U0126 dose and amygdala nucleus (F(1,28) = 8.9, p < 0.01). The analysis of the data from the Western blot assays revealed that central amygdala infusions of U0126 decreased pERK and pCREB in the central (F(1,14) = 12.6, p < 0.01; and F(1,14) = 25.2, p < 0.01, respectively) but not the basolateral (p > 0.05) amygdala, whereas basolateral amygdala infusions of U0126 decreased pERK and pCREB in the basolateral (F(1,14) = 6.1, p < 0.01; and F(1,14) = 16.2, p < 0.01, respectively) but not the central (p > 0.05) amygdala (Fig. 5).

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Inhibition of ERK and CREB phosphorylation in the central amygdala decreased morphine CPP after 14 withdrawal days. A, B, Central (CeA) but not basolateral (BLA) amygdala injections of U0126 decreased morphine (3 mg/kg) CPP. Data are the mean ± SEM of preference score in seconds during the CPP preference tests conducted 14 d after withdrawal from morphine. *p < 0.05, different from the vehicle condition. C, E, Central amygdala U026 injections decreased ERK and CREB phosphorylation in the central but not basolateral amygdala. D, F, Basolateral amygdala U026 injections decreased ERK and CREB phosphorylation in the basolateral but not central amygdala. Data are presented as a percentage of the respective vehicle groups. *p < 0.05, different from the vehicle group within each amygdala sub-nucleus (n = 7–8 per group).

Effect of activation of central amygdala ERK and CREB on morphine CPP after 1 withdrawal day

Activation of central amygdala pERK and pCREB increased morphine (3 mg/kg) CPP after 1 withdrawal day (Fig. 6). The simple ANOVA for the CPP score included the between-subjects factor of NMDA dose (0 and 250 ng/side). This analysis revealed a significant effect of NMDA dose (F(1,12) = 30.3, p < 0.01). The analysis of the data from the Western blot assays revealed that central amygdala infusions of NMDA increased pERK and pCREB in the central (F(1,12) = 14.4, p < 0.01; and F(1,12) = 33.3, p < 0.01, respectively) but not the basolateral (p > 0.05) amygdala (Fig. 6).

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

NMDA-induced increases in ERK and CREB phosphorylation in the central amygdala enhanced morphine CPP after 1 withdrawal day. A, Central amygdala injections of NMDA increased morphine (3 mg/kg) CPP. Data are the mean ± SEM of preference score in seconds during the CPP preference tests conducted 1 d after withdrawal from morphine. *p < 0.05, different from the vehicle condition. B, C, Central amygdala NMDA injections increased ERK and CREB phosphorylation in the central (CeA) but not basolateral (BLA) amygdala. The NMDA injections had no effect on total ERK and CREB. Data are presented as a percentage of the naive group. *p < 0.05, different from the vehicle group within each amygdala subnucleus (n = 8 per group).

In a subsequent experiment, we found that central amygdala U0126 injections reversed the effect of local NMDA injections on morphine CPP (Fig. 7A). Data were analyzed using the factors of NMDA dose (0 and 250 ng/side) and U0126 dose (0 and 100 ng/side). This analysis revealed a significant interaction between these two factors for CPP score (F(1,26) = 15.7, p < 0.01). Additionally, central amygdala U0126 injections decreased NMDA-induced increases in phosphorylated ERK and CREB. The analysis of the data from the Western blot assays revealed a significant interaction between NMDA dose and U0126 dose for both phosphorylated ERK and CREB (F(1,26) = 19.6, p < 0.01; and F(1,26) = 16.7, p < 0.01, respectively) (Fig. 7B,C). Post hoc group differences are indicated in Figure 7.

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

U0126 reversed the enhancement of morphine CPP and ERK and CREB phosphorylation in central amygdala by NMDA 1 d after withdrawal from the drug. A, Central amygdala injections of U0126 attenuated local NMDA-induced increases in morphine (3 mg/kg) CPP. Data are the mean ± SEM of preference score in seconds during the CPP preference tests conducted 1 d after withdrawal from morphine. B, C, Central amygdala injections of U0126 attenuated local NMDA-induced increases in ERK and CREB phosphorylation. Data are presented as a percentage of the naive group. *p < 0.05, different from the vehicle–NMDA group; #p < 0.05, different from the vehicle–vehicle group (n = 7–8 per group). Veh, Vehicle.

Effect of morphine CPP training on ERK and CREB activity in basolateral amygdala

The aim of this experiment was to verify the unexpected finding that repeated exposure to low morphine dose (3 mg/kg) resulted in increased pERK and pCREB in basolateral amygdala after 1 withdrawal day, an effect that was independent of CPP testing. Here, we assessed whether this effect is dependent on training for morphine CPP. As can be seen in Figure 8, increased pERK and pCREB in basolateral amygdala after 1 withdrawal day was observed in both rats conditioned to morphine in the CPP apparatus and rats injected with morphine in their home cage. The statistical analysis included the between-subjects factor of CPP training (yes and no) and morphine dose (0 and 3 mg/kg). This analysis revealed a significant effect of morphine dose for pERK and pCREB (F(1,16) = 60.7, p < 0.01; and F(1,16) = 39.2, p < 0.01, respectively) but not of CPP training or an interaction between CPP training and morphine dose.

Figure 8.

Figure 8.

Repeated exposure to morphine increased ERK and CREB phosphorylation in the basolateral amygdala on withdrawal day 1, an effect independent of CPP training. The “conditioned” groups underwent morphine (3 mg/kg)–saline or saline–saline CPP training (4 pairings in each chamber over 8 d). The “unconditioned” rats were injected with morphine or saline in their home cage. ERK and CREB phosphorylation in the basolateral amygdala were assessed 1 d after the last morphine (or saline) injection (withdrawal day 1). Data are presented as a percentage (mean ± SEM) of phosphorylated and total ERK or CREB of naive control rats (n = 5). A, Phosphorylated and total ERK in basolateral amygdala. B, Phosphorylated and total CREB in basolateral amygdala. *p < 0.05 different from the saline-exposed rats (n = 5 per group).

Discussion

We found that the expression of low-dose but not high-dose drug CPP progressively increased over the first 14 d after withdrawal from morphine. Our data indicate that this incubation of morphine craving is mediated by central amygdala ERK activity. Our data also indicate that incubation of morphine craving correlates with central amygdala CREB activity. Thus, enhanced morphine CPP after 14 withdrawal days was accompanied by enhanced ERK and CREB phosphorylation in central but not basolateral amygdala. Inhibition of central but not basolateral amygdala ERK and CREB phosphorylation by U0126 prevented the enhanced morphine CPP after 14 withdrawal days, whereas activation of central amygdala ERK and CREB phosphorylation by NMDA restored morphine CPP after 1 withdrawal day; NMDA-induced increases in morphine CPP was reversed by central amygdala U0126 injections. We also found that repeated exposure to morphine (either in home cage or CPP apparatus) increased ERK and CREB phosphorylation in the basolateral amygdala after 1 withdrawal day.

Role of central amygdala ERK in incubation of morphine craving

Our results indicating a role of central amygdala ERK in incubation of morphine craving, as assessed in the CPP procedure, are in agreement with our previous work on the role of central amygdala ERK in incubation of cocaine craving, as assessed in the self-administration procedure (Lu et al., 2005). It is likely that central amygdala glutamate transmission mediates the ERK-dependent incubation of drug craving. We found that central amygdala NMDA injections increased ERK phosphorylation and enhanced morphine CPP on withdrawal day 1. Additionally, we found previously that these injections increased ERK phosphorylation and cue-induced cocaine-seeking on withdrawal day 1; these effects were reversed by local U0126 injections (Lu et al., 2005). We also previously found that inhibition of local glutamate transmission by central amygdala injections of the NMDA receptor antagonist AP-5 or the metabotropic glutamate receptor subunit 2/3 agonist LY379268 (1R,4R,5S,6R-2-oxa-4-aminobicyclo[3.1.0]hexane-4,6-dicarboxylate) (which decreases glutamate transmission) attenuated the enhanced cue-induced cocaine seeking after 3–4 weeks of withdrawal (Lu et al., 2005, 2007).

The downstream cellular mechanisms of central amygdala ERK activation that contribute to incubation of drug craving are unknown. Because of the timeframe of our behavioral assessments (minutes), we proposed that the expression of incubation of drug craving involves cue-triggered, ERK-mediated, rapidly induced (within minutes) (Fig. 4) increases in neuronal excitability and synaptic transmission (Lu et al., 2006). These acute effects may be attributable to ERK-mediated inactivation of voltage-gated potassium channel subunit Kv4.2, leading to decreasing outward K+ current, resulting in increased membrane depolarization (Yuan et al., 2002). This depolarization would make neurons more responsive to external stimuli, such as drug cues. The role of ERK in incubation of reward craving may also involve cue-induced ERK-mediated potentiation of excitatory neurotransmission by AMPA receptor insertion into cell membranes (Qin et al., 2005).

Other investigators reported that systemic or accumbens, acute injections of U0126, or other inhibitors of ERK activity, decreased the expression of psychostimulant CPP (Mizoguchi et al., 2004; Miller and Marshall, 2005; Valjent et al., 2006a) and pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer of food cues (the potentiating effect of pavlovian food cues on operant responding for the same food) (Shiflett et al., 2008). Thus, we propose that time-dependent increases in cue-induced drug-seeking, as well as responses to reward cues in some behavioral procedures, involves ERK-mediated, acute, central effects on synaptic transmission (Lu et al., 2006). This mechanism is distinct from the more established ERK-related mechanism (long-term stable alterations in synaptic plasticity triggered by ERK-induced genomic activation via downstream cellular target) that contributes to diverse learning and memory processes (Sweatt, 2001; Adams and Sweatt, 2002), cocaine psychomotor sensitization (Valjent et al., 2006b; Boudreau et al., 2007), and consolidation and reconsolidation of memories for cocaine cues (Valjent et al., 2000, 2006a; Miller and Marshall, 2005).

Role of central amygdala CREB in incubation of morphine craving

We studied whether CREB activity correlates with incubation of morphine craving because CREB is a downstream cellular target of ERK (Thomas and Huganir, 2004) and because CREB knock-out mice do not acquire morphine CPP (Walters and Blendy, 2001). Additionally, cocaine CPP expression is associated with increased CREB phosphorylation in the accumbens and dorsal hippocampus (Miller and Marshall, 2005; Tropea et al., 2008), and nicotine CPP expression correlates with increased CREB activity in ventral tegmental area (Walters et al., 2005).

We extended these findings to demonstrate that morphine CPP expression correlates with increased central amygdala CREB phosphorylation. This effect is reversed by U0126 and is thus ERK dependent. These data extend previous findings indicating that drug- or cue-induced activation of accumbens CREB is ERK dependent (Brami-Cherrier et al., 2005; Mattson et al., 2005; Miller and Marshall, 2005). Increased CREB phosphorylation occurred selectively in the central amygdala and was correlated with the time-dependent increases in drug CPP after withdrawal from morphine. Therefore, one interpretation of these data are that local CREB activity mediates the incubation of drug craving. However, activation of the transcription factor CREB is not likely to control conditioned responses within the timeframe of our behavioral assessment (15 min), because CREB-induced gene transcription typically requires longer time periods (Montminy et al., 1990; Nestler, 1993; Frank and Greenberg, 1994). Thus, the time-dependent increase in central amygdala CREB phosphorylation after 14 withdrawal days is probably a consequence, rather than a cause, of incubation of morphine craving. This issue can be addressed by using a manipulation that, unlike U0126, selectively prevents CREB phosphorylation without affecting ERK phosphorylation.

Generality of incubation of reward craving

Incubation of drug craving was inferred from the findings of time-dependent increases in cue-induced drug-seeking after withdrawal from cocaine (Grimm et al., 2001). This incubation was demonstrated under different experimental conditions (Sorge and Stewart, 2005; Hollander and Carelli, 2007; Conrad et al., 2008; Freeman et al., 2008) and was also observed in rats trained to self-administer heroin (Shalev et al., 2001), methamphetamine (Shepard et al., 2004), and alcohol (Bienkowski et al., 2004). Incubation of reward craving is also evident with nondrug rewards. Seventy years ago, Youtz (1938) demonstrated time-dependent increases in extinction responding in hungry rats previously trained to lever press for food. More recently, Grimm et al. (2002, 2005) demonstrated time-dependent increases in both extinction responding and cue-induced reinstatement in rats previously trained to lever press for sucrose.

Here we showed that incubation of reward craving can be detected in a pavlovian CPP procedure that is widely used to assess the rewarding effects of abused drugs (Bardo and Bevins, 2000; Tzschentke, 2007). Under our experimental conditions, the critical factor is the drug training dose: increases in CPP over 14 d after last drug exposure were only observed in rats trained with low (1 or 3 mg/kg) but not high (10 mg/kg) doses. These data are different from those obtained in previous studies by Lu et al. (2000a, 2001b) and Wang et al. (2000, 2002) who reported that CPP for morphine (10 or 4 mg/kg, respectively) decreased over time. Potential reasons for these different results are the use of a three-chamber CPP apparatus here versus a two-chamber CPP apparatus in previous studies and differences in the contextual cues used here versus those used in the previous studies of Lu and colleagues.

Our data on time-dependent increases in morphine CPP are also different from those of Mucha and Iversen (1984), Vezina and Stewart (1987), and Mueller and Stewart (2002) who reported that drug CPP is maintained at similar magnitude over weeks after withdrawal from morphine. The reasons for these different results are unknown, but we note that, unlike these previous studies, in our study we systematically characterized the effect of morphine CPP training dose on CPP expression over time. Interestingly, data from Mueller and Stewart (2002) may be in agreement with our data. They trained rats for morphine CPP with three drug doses (1, 5, or 10 mg/kg) but assessed CPP expression data over time for all rats regardless of their training dose (Mueller and Stewart, 2002, their Fig. 2, p 392). They also excluded rats that did not demonstrate day 1 morphine CPP. As can be seen in this figure, morphine CPP, which was strong after 1 d, was even stronger after 6 weeks; this effect, however, was not statistically significant. Perhaps, had the authors assessed morphine CPP expression within each training dose and not excluding rats demonstrating weak day 1 drug CPP, they might have obtained significant time-dependent increases in morphine CPP.

Concluding remarks

Our results indicate that central amygdala ERK activity mediates the incubation of morphine craving, as assessed in the CPP procedure. These results extend previous data on the role of central amygdala ERK in incubation of cocaine craving, as assessed in the self-administration procedure. This generality across drugs and procedures suggest that central amygdala ERK plays a general role in incubation of reward craving. We also found that central amygdala CREB activity is associated with incubation of morphine craving. Whether this activity is a cause or a consequence of incubation of drug craving is a subject for future research.

Footnotes

This work was supported in part by National Basic Research Program of China Grants 2007CB512302 and 2009CB522004, National High Technology Research and Development Program of China 863 Program/Grant 2006AA02Z4D1, Natural Science Foundation of China Grants 30670713 and 30725016, and the National Institute on Drug Abuse, Intramural Research Program. We thank Drs. Bruce Hope and Eisuke Koya for their helpful comments on previous versions of this manuscript and Drs. Julie Blendy and Bill Carlezon for helpful comments on issues related to CREB role in the behavioral effects of drugs.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  1. Adams JP, Sweatt JD. Molecular psychology: roles for the ERK MAP kinase cascade in memory. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2002;42:135–163. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.42.082701.145401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bardo MT, Bevins RA. Conditioned place preference: what does it add to our preclinical understanding of drug reward? Psychopharmacology. 2000;153:31–43. doi: 10.1007/s002130000569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bienkowski P, Rogowski A, Korkosz A, Mierzejewski P, Radwanska K, Kaczmarek L, Bogucka-Bonikowska A, Kostowski W. Time-dependent changes in alcohol-seeking behaviour during abstinence. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2004;14:355–360. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2003.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blendy JA, Maldonado R. Genetic analysis of drug addiction: the role of cAMP response element binding protein. J Mol Med. 1998;76:104–110. doi: 10.1007/s001090050197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boudreau AC, Reimers JM, Milovanovic M, Wolf ME. Cell surface AMPA receptors in the rat nucleus accumbens increase during cocaine withdrawal but internalize after cocaine challenge in association with altered activation of mitogen-activated protein kinases. J Neurosci. 2007;27:10621–10635. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2163-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brami-Cherrier K, Valjent E, Hervé D, Darragh J, Corvol JC, Pages C, Arthur SJ, Simon AJ, Girault JA, Caboche J. Parsing molecular and behavioral effects of cocaine in mitogen- and stress-activated protein kinase-1-deficient mice. J Neurosci. 2005;25:11444–11454. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1711-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Carlezon WA, Jr, Duman RS, Nestler EJ. The many faces of CREB. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28:436–445. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Conrad KL, Tseng KY, Uejima JL, Reimers JM, Heng LJ, Shaham Y, Marinelli M, Wolf ME. Formation of accumbens GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors mediates incubation of cocaine craving. Nature. 2008;454:118–121. doi: 10.1038/nature06995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Davies SP, Reddy H, Caivano M, Cohen P. Specificity and mechanism of action of some commonly used protein kinase inhibitors. Biochem J. 2000;351:95–105. doi: 10.1042/0264-6021:3510095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Di Ciano P, Everitt BJ. Conditioned reinforcing properties of stimuli paired with self-administered cocaine, heroin or sucrose: implications for the persistence of addictive behaviour. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47(Suppl 1):202–213. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fiore RS, Murphy TH, Sanghera JS, Pelech SL, Baraban JM. Activation of p42 mitogen-activated protein kinase by glutamate receptor stimulation in rat primary cortical cultures. J Neurochem. 1993;61:1626–1633. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1993.tb09796.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Frank DA, Greenberg ME. CREB: a mediator of long-term memory from mollusks to mammals. Cell. 1994;79:5–8. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90394-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Freeman WM, Patel KM, Brucklacher RM, Lull ME, Erwin M, Morgan D, Roberts DC, Vrana KE. Persistent alterations in mesolimbic gene expression with abstinence from cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1807–1817. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Girault JA, Valjent E, Caboche J, Hervé D. ERK2: a logical AND gate critical for drug-induced plasticity? Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2007;7:77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2006.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature. 2001;412:141–142. doi: 10.1038/35084134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Grimm JW, Shaham Y, Hope BT. Effect of cocaine and sucrose withdrawal period on extinction behavior, cue-induced reinstatement, and protein levels of the dopamine transporter and tyrosine hydroxylase in limbic and cortical areas in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2002;13:379–388. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200209000-00011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Grimm JW, Lu L, Hayashi T, Hope BT, Su TP, Shaham Y. Time-dependent increases in brain-derived neurotrophic factor protein levels within the mesolimbic dopamine system after withdrawal from cocaine: implications for incubation of cocaine craving. J Neurosci. 2003;23:742–747. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-03-00742.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Grimm JW, Fyall AM, Osincup DP. Incubation of sucrose craving: effects of reduced training and sucrose pre-loading. Physiol Behav. 2005;84:73–79. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hollander JA, Carelli RM. Cocaine-associated stimuli increase cocaine seeking and activate accumbens core neurons after abstinence. J Neurosci. 2007;27:3535–3539. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3667-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kelleher RJ, 3rd, Govindarajan A, Jung HY, Kang H, Tonegawa S. Translational control by MAPK signaling in long-term synaptic plasticity and memory. Cell. 2004;116:467–479. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(04)00115-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lee JL, Milton AL, Everitt BJ. Cue-induced cocaine seeking and relapse are reduced by disruption of drug memory reconsolidation. J Neurosci. 2006;26:5881–5887. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0323-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lu L, Ceng X, Huang M. Corticotropin-releasing factor receptor type I mediates stress-induced relapse to opiate dependence in rats. Neuroreport. 2000a;11:2373–2378. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200008030-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lu L, Liu D, Ceng X, Ma L. Differential roles of corticotropin-releasing factor receptor subtypes 1 and 2 in opiate withdrawal and in relapse to opiate dependence. Eur J Neurosci. 2000b;12:4398–4404. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lu L, Liu D, Ceng X. Corticotropin-releasing factor receptor type 1 mediates stress-induced relapse to cocaine-conditioned place preference in rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 2001a;415:203–208. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(01)00840-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lu L, Huang M, Ma L, Li J. Different role of cholecystokinin (CCK)-A and CCK-B receptors in relapse to morphine dependence in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2001b;120:105–110. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00361-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lu L, Grimm JW, Shaham Y, Hope BT. Molecular neuroadaptations in the accumbens and ventral tegmental area during the first 90 days of forced abstinence from cocaine self-administration in rats. J Neurochem. 2003;85:1604–1613. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.01824.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lu L, Grimm JW, Hope BT, Shaham Y. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal: a review of preclinical data. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47(Suppl 1):214–226. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lu L, Hope BT, Dempsey J, Liu SY, Bossert JM, Shaham Y. Central amygdala ERK signaling pathway is critical to incubation of cocaine craving. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:212–219. doi: 10.1038/nn1383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lu L, Koya E, Zhai H, Hope BT, Shaham Y. Role of ERK in cocaine addiction. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29:695–703. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lu L, Uejima JL, Gray SM, Bossert JM, Shaham Y. Systemic and central amygdala injections of the mGluR(2/3) agonist LY379268 attenuate the expression of incubation of cocaine craving. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:591–598. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mattson BJ, Bossert JM, Simmons DE, Nozaki N, Nagarkar D, Kreuter JD, Hope BT. Cocaine-induced CREB phosphorylation in nucleus accumbens of cocaine-sensitized rats is enabled by enhanced activation of extracellular signal-related kinase, but not protein kinase A. J Neurochem. 2005;95:1481–1494. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2005.03500.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Miller CA, Marshall JF. Molecular substrates for retrieval and reconsolidation of cocaine-associated contextual memory. Neuron. 2005;47:873–884. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mizoguchi H, Yamada K, Mizuno M, Mizuno T, Nitta A, Noda Y, Nabeshima T. Regulations of methamphetamine reward by extracellular signal-regulated kinase 1/2/ets-like gene-1 signaling pathway via the activation of dopamine receptors. Mol Pharmacol. 2004;65:1293–1301. doi: 10.1124/mol.65.5.1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Montminy MR, Gonzalez GA, Yamamoto KK. Regulation of cAMP-inducible genes by CREB. Trends Neurosci. 1990;13:184–188. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90045-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mucha RF, Iversen SD. Reinforcing properties of morphine and naloxone revealed by conditioned place preferences: a procedural examination. Psychopharmacology. 1984;82:241–247. doi: 10.1007/BF00427782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mueller D, Stewart J. Cocaine-induced conditioned place preference: reinstatement by priming injections of cocaine after extinction. Behav Brain Res. 2000;115:39–47. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00239-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mueller D, Perdikaris D, Stewart J. Persistence and drug-induced reinstatement of a morphine-induced conditioned place preference. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136:389–397. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00297-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Neisewander JL, Baker DA, Fuchs RA, Tran-Nguyen LT, Palmer A, Marshall JF. Fos protein expression and cocaine-seeking behavior in rats after exposure to a cocaine self-administration environment. J Neurosci. 2000;20:798–805. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-02-00798.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Nestler EJ. Cellular responses to chronic treatment with drugs of abuse. Crit Rev Neurobiol. 1993;7:23–39. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. O'Brien CP, Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R. Classical conditioning in drug dependent humans. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1992;654:400–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25984.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Ed 5. Amsterdam: Elsevier Academic; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  42. Qin Y, Zhu Y, Baumgart JP, Stornetta RL, Seidenman K, Mack V, van Aelst L, Zhu JJ. State-dependent Ras signaling and AMPA receptor trafficking. Genes Dev. 2005;19:2000–2015. doi: 10.1101/gad.342205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Shalev U, Morales M, Hope B, Yap J, Shaham Y. Time-dependent changes in extinction behavior and stress-induced reinstatement of drug seeking following withdrawal from heroin in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:98–107. doi: 10.1007/s002130100748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Shepard JD, Bossert JM, Liu SY, Shaham Y. The anxiogenic drug yohimbine reinstates methamphetamine seeking in a rat model of drug relapse. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:1082–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.02.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Shiflett MW, Martini RP, Mauna JC, Foster RL, Peet E, Thiels E. Cue-elicited reward-seeking requires extracellular signal-regulated kinase activation in the nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci. 2008;28:1434–1443. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2383-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sorge RE, Stewart J. The contribution of drug history and time since termination of drug taking to footshock stress-induced cocaine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:210–217. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0160-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sweatt JD. The neuronal MAP kinase cascade: a biochemical signal integration system subserving synaptic plasticity and memory. J Neurochem. 2001;76:1–10. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.00054.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Thomas GM, Huganir RL. MAPK cascade signalling and synaptic plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:173–183. doi: 10.1038/nrn1346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Tropea TF, Kosofsky BE, Rajadhyaksha AM. Enhanced CREB and DARPP-32 phosphorylation in the nucleus accumbens and CREB, ERK and GluR1 phosphorylation in the dorsal hippocampus is associated with cocaine conditioned place preference behavior. J Neurochem. 2008;106:1780–1790. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05518.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Tzschentke TM. Measuring reward with the conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm: update of the last decade. Addict Biol. 2007;12:227–462. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2007.00070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tzschentke TM, Schmidt WJ. N-methyl-D-aspartic acid-receptor antagonists block morphine-induced conditioned place preference in rats. Neurosci Lett. 1995;193:37–40. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)11662-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Valjent E, Corvol JC, Pages C, Besson MJ, Maldonado R, Caboche J. Involvement of the extracellular signal-regulated kinase cascade for cocaine-rewarding properties. J Neurosci. 2000;20:8701–8709. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-08701.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Valjent E, Corbillé AG, Bertran-Gonzalez J, Hervé D, Girault JA. Inhibition of ERK pathway or protein synthesis during reexposure to drugs of abuse erases previously learned place preference. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006a;103:2932–2937. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0511030103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Valjent E, Corvol JC, Trzaskos JM, Girault JA, Herve D. Role of the ERK pathway in psychostimulant-induced locomotor sensitization. BMC Neurosci. 2006b;7:20. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-7-20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Van der Kooy D. Place conditioning: A simple and effective method for assessing the motivational properties of drugs. In: Bozarth MA, editor. Methods of assessing the reinforcing properties of abused drugs. New York: Springer; 1987. pp. 229–240. [Google Scholar]
  56. Vezina P, Kalivas PW, Stewart J. Sensitization occurs to the locomotor effects of morphine and the specific mu opioid receptor agonist, DAGO, administered repeatedly to the ventral tegmental area but not to the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res. 1987;417:51–58. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90178-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Walters CL, Blendy JA. Different requirements for cAMP response element binding protein in positive and negative reinforcing properties of drugs of abuse. J Neurosci. 2001;21:9438–9444. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-23-09438.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Walters CL, Cleck JN, Kuo YC, Blendy JA. Mu-opioid receptor and CREB activation are required for nicotine reward. Neuron. 2005;46:933–943. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wang B, Luo F, Zhang WT, Han JS. Stress or drug priming induces reinstatement of extinguished conditioned place preference. Neuroreport. 2000;11:2781–2784. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200008210-00034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wang B, Luo F, Ge XC, Fu AH, Han JS. Effects of lesions of various brain areas on drug priming or footshock-induced reactivation of extinguished conditioned place preference. Brain Res. 2002;950:1–9. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)02980-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wang XY, Zhao M, Ghitza UE, Li YQ, Lu L. Stress impairs reconsolidation of drug memory via glucocorticoid receptors in the basolateral amygdala. J Neurosci. 2008;28:5602–5610. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0750-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Youtz REP. The change with time of a Thorndikian response in the rat. J Exp Psychol. 1938;23:128–140. [Google Scholar]
  63. Yuan LL, Adams JP, Swank M, Sweatt JD, Johnston D. Protein kinase modulation of dendritic K+ channels in hippocampus involves a mitogen-activated protein kinase pathway. J Neurosci. 2002;22:4860–4868. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-12-04860.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES