Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neural Circuits logoLink to Frontiers in Neural Circuits
. 2013 Aug 8;7:131. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00131

Mind the fish: zebrafish as a model in cognitive social neuroscience

Rui F Oliveira 1,2,*
PMCID: PMC3737460  PMID: 23964204

Abstract

Understanding how the brain implements social behavior on one hand, and how social processes feedback on the brain to promote fine-tuning of behavioral output according to changes in the social environment is a major challenge in contemporary neuroscience. A critical step to take this challenge successfully is finding the appropriate level of analysis when relating social to biological phenomena. Given the enormous complexity of both the neural networks of the brain and social systems, the use of a cognitive level of analysis (in an information processing perspective) is proposed here as an explanatory interface between brain and behavior. A conceptual framework for a cognitive approach to comparative social neuroscience is proposed, consisting of the following steps to be taken across different species with varying social systems: (1) identification of the functional building blocks of social skills; (2) identification of the cognitive mechanisms underlying the previously identified social skills; and (3) mapping these information processing mechanisms onto the brain. Teleost fish are presented here as a group of choice to develop this approach, given the diversity of social systems present in closely related species that allows for planned phylogenetic comparisons, and the availability of neurogenetic tools that allows the visualization and manipulation of selected neural circuits in model species such as the zebrafish. Finally, the state-of-the art of zebrafish social cognition and of the tools available to map social cognitive abilities to neural circuits in zebrafish are reviewed.

Keywords: social neuroscience, zebrafish, social cognition, cognitive modules, social behavior, social brain

INTRODUCTION: A COGNITIVE APPROACH TO COMPARATIVE SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

In social species animals interact frequently with their conspecifics and have to adjust the expression of their social behavior according to previous social experience and to social context. This behavioral flexibility in the social domain (aka social competence; Taborsky and Oliveira, 2012) allows the animal to navigate daily changes in the social environment and should be viewed as an adaptive performance trait that impacts the Darwinian fitness of the animal, for example by allowing it to avoid getting involved in costly social interactions or being ejected from its social group (Oliveira, 2009, 2012). Understanding social competence at the proximate level is a major challenge in contemporary neuroscience. The new field of social neuroscience has emerged in the past two decades in an attempt to understand how biological systems in general, and the brain in particular, implement social behavior on one hand, and how social processes feedback on biological mechanisms and the brain, on the other (Cacioppo and Decety, 2011). One major challenge in this new field is finding the appropriate level of analysis when relating social to biological phenomena. Given the enormous complexity of both the neural networks of the brain and social systems, mapping adaptive social behaviors in the real world onto putative underlying neural circuits in the brain is a daunting task (Adolphs, 2010). One promising approach to this challenge is to use the cognitive level of analysis as an interface level of explanation between brain and behavior, which enables the development of manageable theories of social behavior that can generate testable predictions of observable behavior. Cognition is used here in an information processing perspective, that is as a set of neuronal processes concerned with the acquisition, retention, and use of information, that enables the animal to integrate input information with stored information, when making ecologically relevant decisions (Shettleworth, 2001; Dukas, 2004). These encompass a wide array of cognitive processes such as: perception, learning, memory, attention, and decision making. It should be stressed that the use of the term cognition as proposed here is neither in opposition to association learning explanations of animal behavior in the associative vs. cognitive debate (e.g., Byrne and Bates, 2006; Heyes, 2012a), nor does it equate with intelligence, intentionality or consciousness, as sometimes suggested in anthropomorphic accounts of animal behavior (e.g., Ristau, 1991).

Arguably, it has been proposed that the mechanisms controlling the organism interactions with other behavioral agents (i.e., social interactions) differ from those involved in the interactions of the organism with its physical environment, and therefore the term social cognition has been created to refer specifically to cognitive processes involved in social interactions (e.g., Zuberbuhler and Byrne, 2006). Social phenomena that have been examined under the label of social cognition include recognition of individuals or social categories, social partner preferences, development and management of social relationships (attachment, reconciliation, alliances), triadic relationships (requesting transitive inference), learning new skills from conspecifics (social learning), social coordination, manipulation, and deception, and theory of mind among many others (Jensen et al., 2011). Most research on comparative social cognition has focused mainly on declarative human-like cognitive abilities apparently needed to navigate highly complex social systems such as those of primates (e.g., “theory of mind,” Premack and Woodruff, 1978; Penn and Povinelli, 2007; “Machiavellian intelligence,” Whiten and Byrne, 1988, 1997), and not so much on the basic information processing mechanisms that make up the building blocks of the behavioral control systems involved in social behavior irrespective of its complexity (Barrett et al., 2007). This approach is limiting since highly complex social systems do not necessarily request highly complex individual cognitive abilities, as can be illustrated by insect societies or by elaborated mutualistic relationships in cleaner fish (Chittka and Niven, 2009; Bshary, 2011), and as a consequence most “simple-minded” species have not been considered in comparative studies of social cognition. Moreover, functionally similar social phenomena may rely on different underlying mechanisms in different species (e.g., different cognitive mechanisms underlying transitive inference, see below for details). Therefore, a more productive approach to comparative social cognition would be the adoption of a rationale that can be applied universally across species with varying degrees of complexity of their social structures and that takes into account the underlying mechanisms.

In this paper, I propose a conceptual framework for comparative social neuroscience based on: (1) the identification of the functional building blocks of social behavior and the underlying cognitive mechanisms across different species with varying social systems, and (2) how these information processing mechanisms are inbuilt in the brain, which is viewed as an information processing organ. Following Krogh’s principle, that “for many problems there is an animal on which it can be most conveniently studied” (Krebs, 1975), teleost fish are presented here as a golden model to develop this approach given the diversity of social systems present in closely related species that allows for planned phylogenetic comparisons of cognitive abilities (e.g., MacLean et al., 2012), and the availability of genetic tools that allows the visualization and manipulation of selected neural circuits in model species such as the zebrafish (e.g., Muto and Kawakami, 2011).

COGNITIVE MODULES OF SOCIAL COMPETENCE

The first step of the conceptual framework proposed here is to identify the information processing problems posed by the social domain of the environment in order to identify the cognitive abilities underlying social skills. For instance, what are the mechanisms required for an individual to tolerate the presence of conspecifics, to recognized different classes of conspecifics and assess their behavior, to use public information available in social environments and to choose the appropriate responses from the available behavioral repertoire? Once we identify these building blocks of social competence we can investigate their phylogenetic distribution and how they map onto neural networks underlying behavior. The putative building blocks of social competence are identified and discussed below (see Table 1 and Figure 1 for summary and selected examples in teleost fish, respectively).

Table 1.

Social skills, their putative underlying cognitive mechanisms and selected examples of their occurrence in teleost fish.

Social skill Cognitive mechanisms* Species Reference
Prosociality
social reward Innate response; Selective attention Zebrafish, Danio rerio Al-Imari and Gerlai (2008)
Social preferences
Shoal mate preference discrimination learning Zebrafish, Danio rerio Engeszer et al. (2007)
Mate choice discrimination learning Peacock blenny, Salaria pavo Fagundes et al. (2007)
Pair bond recognition learning and social memory African cichlid, Tropheus moorii Egger et al. (2006)
Cognitive appraisal
Predictability S-S learning Mozambique tilapia, Oreochromis mossambicus Galhardo et al. (2011)
Social recognition and memory
Individual recognition Single stimulus learning + long-term Cleaner wrasse, Labroides dimidiatus Tebbich et al. (2002)
Kin recognition memory Zebrafish, Danio rerio Gerlach and Lysiak (2006)
Social status Mozambique tilapia, Oreochromis Barata et al. (2007)
Familiarity mossambicus
Social inference
Social eavesdropping Selective attention, S–R learning Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens Oliveira et al. (1998)
Transitive inference Associative strength, ordinal representation African cichlid, Astatotilapia burtoni Grosenick et al. (2007)
Audience effects Selective attention, S–R learning Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens Doutrelant et al. (2001)
Deception Selective attention, S–R learning Cleaner wrasse, Labroides dimidiatus Pinto et al. (2011)
Social learning
Stimulus enhancement Single stimulus learning Zebrafish, Danio rerio Lindeyer and Reader (2010)
Observational conditioning S–S learning Zebrafish, Danio rerio Suboski et al. (1990)
Copying S–R learning Sailfin molly, Poecilia latipinna Witte and Ryan (2002)
Intertemporal choice
Spatial discounting Reversal learning Guppies, Poecilia reticulata Muhlhoff et al. (2011)
*

Following the terminology used by Shettleworth (2010)

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Examples of two complex social abilities in teleost fish. (A) Transitive inference in a cichlid fish (Astatotilapia burtoni; left panel) experimental set-up for transitive inference training; the bystander male is placed in the middle compartment and allowed to observe a sequence of paired fights among neighboring conspecifics; in each fight the scheduled loser male is placed in the territory of the scheduled winner (e.g., A wins over B), so that the following sequence of fights is produced: A > B, B > C, C > D, D > E; in order to test for transitive inference of social dominance focal males were given a preference test between the pairs AE and BD (where A > E and B > D; right panel) focal males prefer to associate with the lower ranking male from each dyad both in familiar (filled bars) and in novel contexts (open bars), suggesting tha males are able to infer the relative rankings (i.e., A > E and B > D) from the observed dominance hierarchy A > B > C > D > E (reproduced with permission from Grosenick et al., 2007). (B) Spatial discounting in guppies (Poecilia reticulata; upper panel) experimental set-up for the study of spatial discounting; subjects were given a choice between two vs. six items (food items in the food- reward condition and conspecifics in the social-reward condition); the six items (food or conspecifics) were placed at increasing distances (20, 40, 60, 80, 100, 120cm), whereas the two items were always located at 20 cm from the starting place; (lower panel) in choice tests subjects show a preference for the larger reward that decreases with the distance to the larger reward (reproduced with permission from Muhlhoff et al., 2011).

SOCIAL VALUE AND SOCIAL PREFERENCES

Approach/ avoidance is a basic behavioral mechanism present in all animals. Given that an a priori condition for social groups to form is that individuals show a predisposition to approach conspecifics and tolerate their presence, this prosocial tendency has to overcome the one for social withdrawal. At the cognitive level prosocial behavior relies on a value system that attributes valence (on a negative–positive continuum) and salience (on a low to high continuum) to social agents (Paul et al., 2005), such that conspecifics tend to have reward value (i.e., high salience, positive valence; Thiel et al., 2008) hence eliciting approach responses. However, different conspecifics may pose different challenges/opportunities, hence not all group members are expected to have the same social value. Some might be competitors, others potential partners or mates. These differences in reward value of different conspecifics lead to social preferences (e.g., mate choice preferences; Ryan et al., 2007), which in turn may lead to social bonding, when individuals establish long-lasting relationships.

COGNITIVE APPRAISAL

Given the wide array of social signals conveyed in multiple sensory modalities it is postulated that a general appraisal mechanism that assesses the valence and salience of social stimuli across different sensory modalities and functional domains must operate. According to this view the evaluation of valence and salience of social information is not just a result of direct effects of perceptual information (e.g., image of conspecific elicits approach), but rather a function of what that perceptual information means to the organism at that moment in time (e.g., image of conspecific is appraised and its valence elicits appropriate response, such as: if dominant avoid; Paul et al., 2005; Mendl et al., 2010). This subjective value of social stimuli is assessed through a set of stimulus evaluation checks which include intrinsic valence of the stimuli, novelty (as defined by suddenness, familiarity, and predictability), prediction error and controllability (Paul et al., 2005; Mendl et al., 2010). Despite the fact that some of these checks have been described in animals (e.g., predictability in fish; Galhardo et al., 2011), a systematic study of stimulus evaluation checks in animals is still lacking. Cognitive appraisal classifies social stimuli in terms of their valence, salience and the organism capacity for control, therefore decoupling stimulus and response and allowing the animal to give a flexible response.

SOCIAL RECOGNITION AND MEMORY

For the expression of both prosocial behavior and social preferences individuals need to discriminate between classes of social agents, namely conspecifics from heterospecifics and between conspecifics with different social valences. Different forms of social recognition might occur, from individual recognition, where individuals are recognized by unique cues (Tibbetts and Dale, 2007), to the recognition of social classes of individuals, such as kin (Hepper, 1986) or social rank conveyed by status badges (Johnstone and Norris, 1993). Individual recognition is expected to evolve in semi-permanent groups where individuals engage in repeated interactions, since it reduces the costs associated with agonistic interactions and it stabilizes dominance hierarchies (Barnard and Burk, 1979; Pagel and Dawkins, 1997). Any kind of social recognition requires memory for conspecifics so that the acquired discrimination of different individuals or classes of individuals is carried forward in time in a computational way that is accessible for retrieval by the animal at a future time. There is some evidence that social memory is independent of asocial memory. For example, AVP V1b receptor knockout mice have impaired memory for social odors, despite having normal olfactory ability and other memory functions (e.g., spatial memory; Wersinger et al., 2004).

SOCIAL INFERENCE

In a social group, individuals may gain information by observing social interactions between third parties, thus avoiding the potential costs involved in direct agonistic interactions (McGregor and Peake, 2000). Bystanders may use the information about interacting partners in order to adjust their future behavior in subsequent interactions with the observed individuals (social eavesdropping; Peake, 2005). Similarly, the presence of bystanders may influence current behavior of interacting individuals that will try to manipulate the information available to bystanders (audience effects; Matos and Schlupp, 2005). In social groups information obtained from observing relationships between third parties (e.g., A > B and B > C) can be used to infer unknown relationships among group members (e.g., A > C; Paz-Y-Miño et al., 2004; Grosenick et al., 2007). Therefore, together with individual recognition, transitive inference can stabilize hierarchies in groups with repeated interactions among individuals, since the relative dominance of unfamiliar individuals can be estimated from observing these interacting with familiar ones. Thus, transitive inference (i.e., if A > B and B > C then A > C) is a skill that is expected to develop with increasing social complexity. Indeed, in two independent comparative studies of transitive inference abilities in closely related species differing in sociality, it was found in both cases that the more highly social of the two species performed better in the transitive inference task (corvids: Bond et al., 2003; prosimian primates: MacLean et al., 2008). It should be pointed out that transitive inference can be achieved using different cognitive mechanisms (transfer of associative strength, i.e., value transfer vs. representation of ordinal list), hence it does not necessarily require high-order reasoning abilities (Von Fersen et al., 1991; Allen, 2006); see also (De Lillo et al., 2001) for a description of neural network that solves a transitive inference task using a simple error-correcting rule.

SOCIAL LEARNING

Public information is readily available in social networks, which allows individuals to acquire adaptive information produced by others without paying the costs typically associated with exploring the environment to learn about its contingencies (e.g., Burns et al., 2011; Mery, 2013). Extracting potentially useful information from observing or interacting with other behavioral agents or their products (aka social learning; Heyes, 1994; Galef and Laland, 2005) has been considered to rely on social-domain specific cognitive modules and in learning mechanisms that are distinct from those used in individual learning by trial and error. However, recent research challenges these assumptions and it has been proposed that asocial and social learning share the same basic learning mechanisms, namely single stimulus learning (e.g., habituation and sensitization vs. local/stimulus enhancement), stimulus–stimulus learning (Pavlovian conditioning vs. observational conditioning), or stimulus–response learning (instrumental learning vs. copying/ imitation; Heyes, 2012b). For example, it has been pointed out that prediction error (i.e., the difference between predicted and obtained outcomes), that is considered to be the learning signal asocial associative learning, is not directly experienced in social learning. However, a new form of observational prediction error has been proposed recently that acts as a learning signal based on externally observed information (e.g., Burke et al., 2010). Social learning is usually use strategically by animals depending on who and when rules, so that they optimize the trade-off between the accuracy and the costs of personal vs. social information (Kendal et al., 2009).

INTERTEMPORAL CHOICE

In order to maintain social relationships individuals have sometimes to choose a smaller immediate reward to guarantee larger future benefits. This trade-off between two or more pay-offs at different points in time is called temporal discounting. For example, in the cleaning mutualism between the cleaner wrasse (Labroides dimidiatus) and its reef fish clients, cleaners feed against their preference (ectoparasites instead clients’ mucus) in order to secure the possibility for numerous future interactions (Grutter and Bshary, 2003), hence exhibiting temporal discounting.

TRAFFIC RULES

Group-living animals need to synchronize and coordinate their behavior in order to maintain the functionality of the social group (e.g., in collective motion a group member needs to move with the group in order to keep the benefits of group membership such as predator avoidance). This behavioral synchronization and coordination of individuals within social groups has lead to the emergence of collective patterns that can be impressive, such as the aerial movements of bird flocks, or the spatial behavior of insect swarms, fish schools, ungulate herds, and even human crowds. In most of these cases complex collective patterns can be explained by individual decision-making rules and by the way information flows between group members (Sumpter, 2006; Couzin, 2009). For example, fish schools can be modeled using individual-based models that follow as few as three rules: avoid individuals that are too close, align with individuals at intermediate distance, and move toward those further away (Parrish and Turchin, 1997; Parrish and Viscido, 2005). Therefore, simple heuristics at the level of the individual may explain the emergence of self-organized social patterns without the need of complex cognitive abilities. This does not mean, however, that the cognitive abilities mentioned above are not needed for optimized social behavior in varying social environments.

More complex social systems are predicted to impose a higher cognitive demand, hence recruiting quantitatively more resources of the abovementioned social skills or promoting qualitative progress of new social skills (e.g., “theory of mind” in humans and questionably in non-human primates; Byrne and Bates, 2010). Evidence supporting this view came from comparative work in primates that established an association between brain size (in particular neocortex), social complexity (as measured by social group size or by occurrence of long-term relationships), and social skills (as a proxy of cognitive complexity), which has been interpreted as evidence for positive selection on executive brain size driven by social complexity (aka “Social brain hypothesis;” Dunbar, 1998; Reader and Laland, 2002; Byrne and Corp, 2004; Dunbar and Shultz, 2007). Although originally developed in primates (Dunbar, 1992; Barton, 1996), the social brain hypothesis has been extended to other taxa, including non-primate mammals (e.g., Perez-Barberia et al., 2007), birds (e.g., Burish et al., 2004; Emery et al., 2007), fish (e.g., Pollen et al., 2007; Gonzalez-Voyer et al., 2009) and insects, on which the relationship between sociality and a brain area size was first described (see review in Lihoreau et al., 2012). However, it has been pointed out that apparently complex social skills may require little information-processing capabilities (Chittka and Niven, 2009) and that even qualitative enhancements in information-processing behind behavioral innovations may be achieved with minor changes in the connectivity of neural networks (see below). Therefore, future research on the co-evolution of brain and social cognition/behavior should move beyond comparative analyses of brain size and focus on unraveling the neural circuitry underlying specific social cognitive abilities.

FUNCTIONAL ARCHITECTURE OF THE SOCIAL BRAIN

The second step of the proposed conceptual framework is to map the cognitive processes involved in social competence onto the brain. There is ample evidence indicating that complex cognitive functions are associated with distributed brain networks, rather than with single brain regions, such that their behavioral manifestations are better reflected by the overall pattern of activation across the different loci of the network than by the activity of any of the single nodes (McIntosh, 2000; Sporns, 2010). These networks are also dynamic so that each node (i.e., brain region) may participate in multiple cognitive functions by rapid functional connectivity reconfigurations (Sporns, 2010). The combination of functional specialization in domain-specific modules with the integration at the neural network level provides coherence to mental states and to behavioral (motor) decision making, allowing for the expression of complex and flexible behavior. Each functional network may exhibit a variety of states, as defined by the configuration of activated nodes, each of which expressing the network encoded knowledge regarding a specific input. The existence of social domain-specific modules within these networks has been demonstrated both at the sensory and central levels as can be illustrated by the parallel stream of social odor processing by the mammalian vomeronasal system relative to asocial odors processed by the main olfactory system (Døving and Trotier, 1998), or by the specialized face recognition areas in the brains of humans, macaques (Macaca mulatta) and sheep (Kendrick and Baldwin, 1987; Kanwisher and Yovel, 2006; Tsao et al., 2006, 2008).

Recently, the occurrence of an evolutionary conserved social decision-making network in vertebrates has been proposed, based on conserved patterns of expression of developmental genes and neurochemical systems in the telencephalon (O’Connell and Hofmann, 2011, 2012). This vertebrate social decision-making network would be composed of two interconnected sister networks: the basal forebrain reward system and the social behavior network (sensu; Goodson, 2005). The reward system would provide value information to other domain-specific modules in the network so that the valence and salience of social stimuli can be integrated in social decision-making, allowing for the reinforcement of adaptive behaviors through natural rewards (Kelley and Berridge, 2002). The social behavior network would be involved in the regulation of multiple forms of social behavior and includes the extended medial amygdala, the lateral septum, the preoptic area, the anterior hypothalamus, the ventromedial hypothalamus, and the periaqueductal gray in mammals and their homologs in non-mammals (Newman, 1999; Goodson, 2005; Goodson and Kabelik, 2009; O’Connell and Hofmann, 2011; see Figure 2). Functional evidence for the presence of the social behavior network is difficult to obtain since it requires the simultaneous recording of neural activity in multiple brain regions. Given the difficulty of obtaining large-scale electrophysiological recordings indirect measures of neural activity, such as the expression of immediate early genes (e.g., c-fos, egr-1) or the activity of cytochrome oxidase, in relation to the expression of different social behaviors, have been used to test this hypothesis. In recent years this type of studies has been accumulating evidence in favor of the social brain network hypothesis. In the African cichlid Astatotilapia burtoni subordinate males given the opportunity to rise in social rank show higher expression of immediate early genes in all nodes of the social behavior network when compared either to stable subordinate or dominant males (Maruska et al., 2013). In the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis) repeated exposure to video-playbacks of aggressive displays of conspecific males induced changes in functional connectivity within the network (Yang and Wilczynski, 2007). And in estrildid finches, different nodes of the network are differentially activated in response to the presence of a conspecific, in a way that is related to inter-specific differences in sociality (Goodson et al., 2005). Despite this evidence for the association of this brain network with social behavior, a systematic approach to the study of the relationship between specific social phenomena (e.g., individual recognition, social inference, etc.) and network state, that would potentially allow the identification of specific social cognitive modules and their integration, is still missing. So far the study of these processes in relation to large-scale brain activity has been mainly restricted to humans and other primates for which functional brain imaging techniques (e.g., MRI, PET) are available (e.g., Sallet et al., 2011; Kumaran et al., 2012). However, given the unique role of the neocortex in human and non-human primate social cognition the relevance of this type of data for testing the wider social brain network hypothesis outlined above has been limited (Adolphs, 2009).

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

The brain social behavior network (BSBN) presents different activation states associated with the expression of different social behaviors both at the intra- (A) and interspecific level (B). (A) Pattern of activation of the BSBN in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis), as measured by the activity of cytochrome oxidase (μmol/min/g tissue), elicited by the exposure to videoplaybacks of an aggressive display, a non-social stimulus (moving balls) or no stimulation (adapted from Yang and Wilczynski, 2007). (B) Divergent patterns of activation of the BSBN, as measured by the expression of the immediate early gene c-fos (arbitrary units), elicited by the exposure to a conspecific in closely related songbird species with divergent social systems: territorial (violet-eared waxbill, Uraeginthus granatina), gregarious (Angolan blue waxbill, Uraeginthus angolensis), highly colonial species (zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata). Note the higher activation of EmA, AH, VMH, and LS in the territorial species (adapted from Goodson et al., 2005). EmA, extended medial amygdala; LS, lateral septum; PAG, periaqueductal gray; VMH, ventromedial hypothalamus; AH, anterior hypothalamus; POA, pre-optic area.

At the evolutionary level the combination of functional segregation and integration in neural networks also provides a simple explanation for qualitative enhancements in information-processing leading to behavioral innovations with gains in flexibility, that may coexist with ancestral stereotyped responses. For example, in honeybees vertical functional modules, such as those specialized in processing conspecific odors, provide rapid and stereotyped responses, while central integration across multiple interconnected domain-specific modules provides novel and adaptive solutions (Menzel and Giurfa, 2001).

Finally, it must be stressed that evidence supporting the occurrence of this putative social behavior network does not mean that that the nodes of this network are exclusively involved in social decision-making. On the contrary, it is expected each node to be shared by multiple brain networks (e.g., stress and social behavior networks).

TELEOSTS AS MODELS FOR COMPARATIVE SOCIAL COGNITION

A successful comparative research program in cognitive social neuroscience has two key requirements: (1) the possibility for cladistic research on the evolution of social behavior and cognition aiming to uncover how pre-existing cognitive modules may evolve quantitatively (e.g., increase in memory storage capacity) and how networks may be reconfigured leading to the emergence of qualitatively new solutions to adaptive problems; and (2) the possibility for reductionist research on the mapping of cognitive function into neural networks, which requires model organisms with appropriate social behavior and with an available “tool box” for the analysis of neural circuits. Therefore, the combination of comparative behavioral work on selected species in naturalistic settings covering the expected diversity in cognitive abilities with neuroethological research on a phylogenetically related model organism is a promising approach. Teleost fish fulfill both requirements and therefore they offer an excellent opportunity to fulfill such a research program on comparative cognitive social neuroscience.

First, they offer a unique possibility for planned phylogenetic comparisons on social skills and underlying cognitive modules. With over 29,000 species described so far, teleost fish are the most diverse of the vertebrate taxa, and this diversity also translates into a wide variation within closely related groups of species in modes of social organization (e.g., variation in mating systems and parental care type in African cichlids, Machado et al., 2009). Fish also excel in social plasticity, as can be illustrated by the profound behavioral and phenotypic changes induced by the social environment, which ranges from fish of different social status displaying different neurobehavioral profiles to socially driven sex-change (Godwin, 2010; Fernald, 2012). Complex social behavior is also present among teleost fish, as is the case of transitive inference shown by cichlid fish (Grosenick et al., 2007), or strategic behavior, including deception, punishment, reconciliation, partner choice, manipulation, and social prestige, displayed by the obligatory cleaning wrasse (Labroides dimidiatus) on the context of cleaning mutualism (Bshary, 2011). So complex forms of social behavior are present in fish and they offer ample opportunity for comparative work both at the inter- and intra-specific levels.

Second, a number of model organisms have been developed among teleost fish (e.g., zebrafish, medaka) for which neurobiological and genetic tools are becoming increasingly available. Among the current teleost model organisms zebrafish offers the best conditions for research in social neuroscience due to a combination of relevant social behavior with availability of relevant tools for studying brain function in relation to behavior.

SOCIAL ZEBRAFISH

Zebrafish are highly social animals that live in groups with structured social relationships including shoaling, dominance hierarchies, and territoriality (Spence et al., 2008; Spence, 2011). Furthermore, social behavior in zebrafish shows considerable flexibility as recently shown by the occurrence of acute winner and loser effects (i.e., winner/loser effects; Oliveira et al., 2011), where short-term social interactions are effective in inducing changes in social behavior that are paralleled by massive changes in the profile of gene expression in the brain (Oliveira et al., unpublished data). This richness and flexibility of social behavior predicts that at least some of the social cognitive modules discussed above must be present in zebrafish. Below the available evidence for the occurrence of some of these modules in zebrafish will be discussed.

SOCIAL VALUE IN ZEBRAFISH

The sight of conspecifics has rewarding value in zebrafish and can be used as a reinforcer in an associative learning task (Al-Imari and Gerlai, 2008). Given the rewarding value of conspecifics it is not surprising that zebrafish form aggregations (shoals) from early in development and that shoaling behavior, as measured by the cohesion of the social aggregation (i.e., distance between each pair of shoal members), increases with age (Engeszer et al., 2007; Buske and Gerlai, 2011). Shoal cohesion varies with social context, increasing in the presence of a predator and decreasing during feeding, which is coherent with the function of shoals in reducing predation risk and enhancing foraging efficiency (Miller and Gerlai, 2007). The maturation of shoaling during development is paralleled by an increase in whole brain dopaminergic and serotonergic activity (Buske and Gerlai, 2012). However, the exposure to conspecific images only induces an increase in brain dopamine, but not in serotonin levels, suggesting a specific involvement of the dopaminergic system in social reward in zebrafish (Saif et al., 2013). This result is in agreement with the well established role of dopamine in the reward circuitry of mammals (Schultz, 2010). However, in anamniotes a midbrain dopaminergic population similar to the ventral tegmental area (VTA), that plays a key role in the mammalian mesolimbic reward system, is missing (Smeets et al., 2000), and the identification of a homologous dopamine reward circuit in fish has remained elusive for many years. The identification of dopaminergic cell groups in the ventral diencephalon projecting to the subpallium (Rink and Wullimann, 2001, 2002), raised the hypothesis that this ascending dopaminergic pathway could be homologous to the mammalian mesostriatal pathway. However, recent data on the expression of developmental factors in larval zebrafish made clear that the ventral diencephalic dopaminergic neurons, that have ascending projections to the telencephalon (i.e., dopaminergic groups DC2 and DC4), are homologous to the A11 mammalian diencephalic dopaminergic cell group (Lohr et al., 2009), rather than to the midbrain mammalian dopaminergic group (i.e., A10). Morever, a detailed projectome of the dopaminergic circuitry in zebrafish showed that most subpallial dopaminergic inputs originate in a local subpallial system that also connects to the ventral telencephalon (Tay et al., 2011). In summary, a the ventral diencephalic-subpallial dopamine system has been characterize in zebrafish that is a good candidate for the dopamine reward system in fish. However, it cannot be seen as homologous to the mammalian dopaminergic mesolimbic pathway, which apparently emerged later in the evolution of dopaminergic modulatory systems (Yamamoto and Vernier, 2011).

SOCIAL PREFERENCES IN ZEBRAFISH

Although conspecifics act as social rewards zebrafish are not equally attracted to all conspecifics, exhibiting shoaling preferences that emerge during the juvenile phase (Engeszer et al., 2007). These shoaling preferences are visually mediated, so that when given a choice between shoal mates with different coloration patterns, individuals prefer to shoal with those sharing the same coloration pattern as the fish with whom they were raised (Engeszer et al., 2007). Once established shoaling preference remains stable and it is not reversed by changing their social environment (Engeszer et al., 2007). In accordance to this finding, once adults wild type zebrafish do not exhibit a shoaling preference based on visual cues for different phenotypic variants (e.g., leopard danios; Spence and Smith, 2007); transgenic red fluorescent Glofish (Snekser et al., 2006). Other characteristics of the shoal than the phenotype of shoal mates are also relevant for the expression of shoaling preferences in zebrafish, that tend to prefer larger and more active shoals (Pritchard et al., 2001; Ruhl and McRobert, 2005). In mammals social preferences and social bonding are known to be moderated by oxytocin and arginine-vasopressin (Donaldson and Young, 2008). Interestingly, it has been shown that their homologs in fish, isotocin and arginine-vasotocin, respectively, also regulate social preference in zebrafish (Braida et al., 2012), suggesting a conserved mechanism for prosocial behavior involving these two neuropeptides. Social preferences can also be expressed in the sexual context, as mate choice preferences according to which individuals do not mate randomly but prefer males with specific characteristics (Spence and Smith, 2006). Female mating preferences for larger males have been described in zebrafish (Pyron, 2003). Given that fin size can increase perceived body size the preference of female zebrafish for longer fins as also been tested but yielded negative results (Kitevski and Pyron, 2003; Gumm et al., 2009).

SOCIAL RECOGNITION IN ZEBRAFISH

As mentioned before a prerequisite for individuals to express social preferences is the ability to recognize individuals or classes of individuals. Zebrafish use both visual and olfactory cues in social recognition. Studies on zebrafish reared in social isolation and on cross-reared intra-specific phenotypes showed that visually mediated species recognition is based on a mechanism of phenotype matching against a learned template early in life (McCann and Matthews, 1974; McCann and Carlson, 1982; Engeszer et al., 2004). Olfaction also plays a role in species recognition as well as in kin recognition in zebrafish, again through a process of phenotype matching (Gerlach and Lysiak, 2006). Olfactory kin recognition is based on imprinting with a 24 h critical period on day 6 post-fertilization during which exposure to kin necessary and sufficient (Gerlach et al., 2008). Neither exposure to own chemical cues nor exposure to non-kin in the critical period results in imprinting. Although individual recognition has not been investigated yet in zebrafish, the occurrence of dominance hierarchies in both sexes (Grant and Kramer, 1992; Delaney et al., 2002; Spence and Smith, 2005; Paull et al., 2010), suggests that it may be present.

COGNITIVE APPRAISAL IN ZEBRAFISH

Cognitive appraisal and cognitive bias are recent research areas that only now are starting to be explored in zebrafish. In our lab we have collected preliminary evidence suggesting that in dyadic agonistic interactions it is the perception that the individual has of the event rather than its objective structure that triggers the physiological and genomic responses differentially observed in winners and losers (Oliveira et al., unpublished data). These results are in accordance with previous work on cichlid fish that showed that ambiguous agonistic interactions between fish and their own image on a mirror (i.e., where the expression of aggressive behavior is decoupled from the experience of gaining or losing social status) failed to elicit the physiological responses observed in winners and losers of real opponent fights (Oliveira et al., 2005).

SOCIAL LEARNING IN ZEBRAFISH

So far the use of public information in zebrafish has been documented mainly in the context of response to aversive stimuli. A first suggestion of social learning in zebrafish comes from data showing that groups of zebrafish learn an avoidance response to an electric shock faster than single individuals (Gleason et al., 1977). However, the better performance while in a social group may be explained by other mechanism, including motivational factors related to the stress of being in isolation. In another set of studies social facilitation of fear response to a predator cue was established (Suboski et al., 1990; Hall and Suboski, 1995). Like many other ostariophysian fishes zebrafish release an alarm substance to the water when injured which causes a fright response in other fish (Waldman, 1982; Speedie and Gerlai, 2008). This innate fear response can be conditioned by pairing a conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., innocuous odor or red light) with the exposure to the alarm substance unconditioned stimulus (US; Suboski et al., 1990; Hall and Suboski, 1995). Conditioned individuals can be subsequently used as demonstrators to naïve fish in trained-naïve mixed groups in the presence of the CS alone (i.e., red light in the absence of the alarm substance). In these conditions all fish of the mixed groups exhibited the alarm response indicating social transmission of the conditioned response from the demonstrators to the naïve fish. Moreover, when sorted out from the mixed groups, naïve individuals kept the response to the CS, indicating the acquisition of the response to the predator cue by observational conditioning in naïve individuals (Suboski et al., 1990; Hall and Suboski, 1995). More recently, it has been shown that zebrafish can learn escape routes from trained demonstrators, and that the presence of demonstrators in groups of naïve individuals increased the escape response (i.e., escaped faster) from an approaching trawl. Moreover, observers successfully became demonstrators for further groups of naïve fish and escape responses were experimentally propagated across three generations of social learning (Lindeyer and Reader, 2010). Interestingly, route traditions (i.e., preference for a particular escape route) were not kept along the chain of social transmission, suggesting a mechanism of social facilitation that increases escape response without learning the specific route followed by the demonstrator. Social learning in zebrafish may also be impacted by attributes of the demonstrator. Zebrafish shoals are structured social networks with different individuals having a differential involvement in social interactions (Vital and Martins, 2011), so that central individuals in the network (aka Keystone individuals; Sih et al., 2009) can be recognized. Such keystone individuals play important roles in social groups, acting as learning models and as leaders in group movement (King and Cowlishaw, 2009; Bode et al., 2011). In a recent study keystone and non-key (i.e., less central in the social network) individuals in zebrafish shoals were identified, individually trained in an aversive response task and returned to their shoal. Shoals with trained keystone individuals escaped aversive stimuli more rapidly than those with trained non-key individuals, supporting the hypothesis that social roles play a critical role in social learning also in zebrafish. Apart from the social learning abilities described above, zebrafish also exhibit a wide range of asocial learning abilities in different ecological domains (e.g., Gerlai, 2011; Karnik and Gerlai, 2012), therefore offering the possibility for contrasting the mechanisms underlying learning in the social and physical domains.

INTERTEMPORAL CHOICE IN ZEBRAFISH

Temporal discounting has not been studied in fish so far. However, spatial discounting (i.e., when animals choose between smaller and closer vs. larger and distant rewards) for social rewards has been recently demonstrated in guppies (Muhlhoff et al., 2011). Both types of discounting require impulse control which is usually tested using reversal learning paradigms. A recent study established the occurrence of reversal learning in zebrafish, hence opening the possibility for the occurrence of intertemporal choices in this species (Parker et al., 2012).

TRAFFIC RULES IN ZEBRAFISH

Fish groups can be classified either as “shoals” or as “schools”, depending on the degree of synchronization and polarization among group members. Thus, shoals are aggregations of individuals [with four body lengths (BLs) commonly used as a criterion for shoal membership in cyprinid species; Pitcher et al., 1983], whereas schools are highly synchronized and coordinated shoals with polarized orientation of individuals (Pitcher and Parrish, 1993). Both types of groups are present in zebrafish, with schools being faster and less dense than zebrafish shoals, and occurring at lower densities (Miller and Gerlai, 2012). Zebrafish groups may vary in size and are characterized by a high degree of changes in individual relative position within the group and by motion pathways with a high rate of changes in direction (Miller and Gerlai, 2007, 2008, 2011; see also Viscido et al., 2004 for data on the giant danio, Danio aequipinnatus). The average distance among zebrafish shoal mates is approximately of 20 cm, and it responds to environmental factors, increasing in the presence of food and predators to over 30 cm (Miller and Gerlai, 2007). Considering that adult zebrafish BL varies between 3 and 4 cm, the average distance between any shoal mates corresponds to 5–6.6 BLs, which is above the proposed threshold of four BLs for shoal membership. However, this distance to the nearest neighbor is stable over time suggesting temporal shoal cohesion in zebrafish (Miller and Gerlai, 2007, 2008, 2011). Although many traffic rules have been developed to explain schooling and shoaling behavior in fish (e.g., Parrish and Turchin, 1997; Parrish and Viscido, 2005), only recently one of these models have been tested in zebrafish. This model showed that zebrafish follows a simple rule in social decision-making based on Bayesian estimation that uses the behaviors of other individuals to improve the estimation (Arganda et al., 2012), therefore confirming the idea that simple heuristics may explain apparently complex collective behavior also in zebrafish.

TOOLS FOR STUDYING BRAIN FUNCTION IN ADULT ZEBRAFISH

A significant number of genetic and neuroanatomy tools and resources are becoming available for zebrafish, making it a tractable species to study brain behavior relationships. Detailed brain atlases are now available for adult zebrafish (Wullimann et al., 1996), and homologies, based on topological and functional data, between zebrafish and mammalian brain areas have been established (Wullimann and Mueller, 2004). More recently, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) techniques were developed for zebrafish and a detailed MRI three-dimensional atlas is now available for adult zebrafish (Ullmann et al., 2010c,d). The use of MRI will potentially allow non-invasive acquisition of brain morphological data and provides more precise estimates of brain area size than those obtained by classical histological methods, which are prone to tissue deformations due to dissection or histological processing (Ullmann et al., 2010b). This technique has a high potential not only for intra-specific studies with a model organism like the zebrafish, but also for inter-specific comparative studies of brain volumes in relation to social behavior (Pollen et al., 2007). As an example of the rapid development of the field recently 3D MRI atlases became available for two more fish species (i.e., Tilapia, Oreochromis mossambicus: Simões, 2012; and barramundi, Lates calcarifer: Ullmann et al., 2010a).

The functional study of neural circuits in zebrafish has benefited from the development of optogenetic and transgenic techniques that together allow the close monitoring of activity in neural networks and experimental gain and loss of function manipulations to assess causal relationships between specific neural patterns and specific behaviors (Baier and Scott, 2009; Portugues et al., 2013). Imaging of neural activity in the brain of both larvae and adults (explants in the later case) has been achieved using genetically encoded calcium indicators, of which successive versions of GCaMP have been the more widely used (Baier and Scott, 2009; Portugues et al., 2013). The use of these fluorescent reporters requires the restrain of the animal during image acquisition which limits the behavioral tasks that can be investigated. In order to overcome this limitation a virtual reality system has been recently developed for zebrafish larvae, in which the larvae is stationary but the putative motor output is recorded from the motor neuron axons in the tail and is used in real time to drive movement in the virtual environment (Ahrens et al., 2012). Gain and loss of function studies at the level of cell type or small groups of neurons have used opsin photoswitchable probes, such as channelrhodopsin (ChR2) and halorhodopsin (NpHR), that activate neurons in a reversible way in response to light pulses of specific wave-lengths (e.g., Douglass et al., 2008; Arrenberg et al., 2009). Loss of function studies have also used chemical or photo inducible probes (e.g., Tetanus toxin, nitroreductase, Killer red) to selectively silence specific neurons in neural circuits (e.g., Koide et al., 2009). Viral transfection and transgenesis have been used as two alternative ways to restrict the expression of the abovementioned reporters and manipulators of neural activity to specific components of the neural networks (Zhu et al., 2009). In particular the Gal-UAS binary transgenic system has been used successfully to specify genetically targeted cell populations and to relate them to specific behaviors, even in adults (e.g., Agetsuma et al., 2010; Okamoto et al., 2012; Muto et al., 2013).

In contrast to the optogenetics toolbox available to study brain function in larval zebrafish, the available tools for adults are far more limited. With the ossification of the skull during development the efficacy of optogenetic techniques decreases and at most they can still be used in vivo in the juvenile phase. On the other hand, the repertoire of social behavior is very limited in larvae, whose ethogram is limited to locomotor action patterns involved in swimming and in prey capture (Budick and O’Malley, 2000). Thus, a major challenge for future research on zebrafish social neuroscience is to try to match in development the efficient use of optogenetic tools with the availability of relevant behavior. For doing this the period during which optogenetic tools can be efficiently used will have to be moved forward in development on one hand, and on the other a detailed characterization of the ontogeny of the cognitive abilities underlying social skills is needed in order to identify how early different cognitive abilities can be successfully studied in zebrafish. Whenever the use of the zebrafish optogenetic toolbox becomes available to the study of social abilities, it will offer an unprecedented opportunity to characterize the neural networks underlying social cognitive modules, and to experimentally manipulate particular nodes of the network and infer their potential role on domain-specific modules on a vertebrate model. Until then mapping of the neural activity that underlies cognitive processes in adult zebrafish is still recurring to the expression of immediate early genes as markers of neural activity (e.g., Lau et al., 2011).

PROSPECTS

The field of social neuroscience has emerged in the past two decades as a vibrant and very successful branch of twenty-first century neuroscience, and understanding the relationship between social cognition and the social brain became a hot topic. However, different research traditions coexist within the field with parallel research programs. Researchers from a more Biomedical or Human Psychology background have been mainly focused on human social behavior and on translational research (e.g., Cole et al., 2007; Norman et al., 2012). Primatologists and comparative psychologists have focused on testing the occurrence of “uniquely human” cognitive abilities (e.g., theory of mind, deception, cooperation) in a small subset of “cognitively complex” animals such as primates, dolphins, and more recently corvids and elephants (Connor, 2007; Byrne and Bates, 2009, 2010; Bugnyar, 2011). Finally, behavioral ecologists and neuroethologists focus their research on understanding the functional value and the underlying neural mechanisms of social cognitive skills in a wide range of “simple-minded” animals (e.g., insects, fish), that are seen as adaptive traits that help survival and successful breeding (Bshary et al., 2002; Chittka and Niven, 2009; Bshary, 2011; Chittka and Skorupski, 2011; Taborsky and Oliveira, 2012). In recent years significant efforts to integrate these different research streams have been made, namely through the organization of thematic discussions and meetings. Although challenging due to the multitude of concepts, methodologies, and conflicting points of view (e.g., associative vs. cognitive explanations of animal behavior), an integration of these traditions would be a major breakthrough for the understanding of the basic proximate and ultimate mechanisms of social cognition and behavior. The present paper was written in that spirit and would finish by identifying some outstanding questions and future challenges in the field that would benefit from an integrated approach and that the adoption of the conceptual framework proposed here will hopefully help to address:

  • (1)

    The creation of a common cognitive lexicon and taxonomy so that clear concepts are shared – the occurrence of complex social behaviors does not necessarily request complex cognitive abilities; therefore functionally similar cognitive abilities that rely on different underlying mechanisms should be recognized (e.g., transitive inference and transitive inference-like abilities).

  • (2)

    The modularity of social cognition – experimental approaches are needed to clarify the conflicting results between intra- and inter-specific analyses of social cognitive modularity. Group living species whose social behavior can be easily recreated in captivity, with relatively short inter-generation times (when compared to the 6 weeks of the mice that is the standard model for behavioral genetics) and easy to breed in the lab, can be successfully used in the future to address this question using experimental evolution paradigms, as suggested by a recent study on artificial selection for larger brains in guppies (Poecilia reticulata) that had an impact on cognitive skills within two generations (Kotrschal et al., 2013).

  • (3)

    The distributed nature of information processing in neural networks should be taken into account when trying to map cognitive processes onto brain activity; therefore comparative analyses should move beyond the comparison of brain sizes and focus on inter-specific convergence/divergence in functional connectivity in the social decision making neural network.

    In answering all these questions I foresee a relevant role for species that offer the possibility to integrate imaging of brain activity with relevant behavioral tasks, as is the case of zebrafish.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The writing of this paper was supported by the research grant PTDC/PSI-PCO/118776/2010 from Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT, Portugal) and by the European Union Framework Program 7 project COPEWELL (# 265957). Rui F. Oliveira is a member of the FCT Eco-Ethology R&D Unit (PEst-OE/MAR/UI0331/2011). I thank Jim Goodson for giving permission to use the songbird pictures presented in Figure 2 (photo credits: violet-eared waxbill by D. Swanepoel, Angolan waxbill by C. Sekercioglu, and zebra finch by J. Ownby).

REFERENCES

  1. Adolphs R. (2009). The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60 693–716 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adolphs R. (2010). Conceptual challenges and directions for social neuroscience. Neuron 65 752–767 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Agetsuma M., Aizawa H., Aoki T., Nakayama R., Takahoko M., Goto M., et al. (2010). The habenula is crucial for experience-dependent modification of fear responses in zebrafish. Nat. Neurosci. 13 1354–1356 10.1038/nn.2654 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ahrens M. B., Li J. M., Orger M. B., Robson D. N., Schier A. F., Engert F., et al. (2012). Brain-wide neuronal dynamics during motor adaptation in zebrafish. Nature 485 471–477 10.1038/nature11057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Al-Imari L., Gerlai R. (2008). Sight of conspecifics as reward in associative learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 189 216–219 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Allen C. (2006). “Transitive inference in animals: reasoning or conditioned associations,” in Rational Animals eds Hurley S., Nudds M. (Oxford: Oxford University Press; ) 175–185 [Google Scholar]
  7. Arganda S., Perez-Escudero A, de Polavieja G. G. (2012). A common rule for decision-making in animal collectives across species. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109 20508–20513 10.1073/pnas.1210664109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Arrenberg A. B., Del Bene F., Baier H. (2009). Optical control of zebrafish behavior with halorhodopsin. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106 17968–17973 10.1073/pnas.0906252106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Baier H., Scott E. K. (2009). Genetic and optical targeting of neural circuits and behavior – zebrafish in the spotlight. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19 553–560 10.1016/j.conb.2009.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barata E. N., Hubbard P. C., Almeida O. G., Miranda A, Canário A. V. M. (2007). Male urine signals social rank in the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus). BMC Biol. 5:54 10.1186/1741-7007-5-54 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barnard C., Burk T. (1979). Dominance hierarchies and the evolution of “individual recognition”. J. Theor. Biol. 81 65–73 10.1016/0022-5193(79)90081-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Barrett L., Henzi P., Rendall D. (2007). Social brains, simple minds: does social complexity really require cognitive complexity? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 362 561–575 10.1098/rstb.2006.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Barton R. A. (1996). Neocortex size and behavioural ecology in primates. Proc. R. Soc. B 263 173–177 10.1098/rspb.1996.0028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bode N. W., Wood A. J., Franks D. W. (2011). Social networks and models for collective motion in animals. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65 117–130 10.1007/s00265-010-1111-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bond A. B., Kamil A. C., Balda R. P. (2003). Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids. Anim. Behav. 65 479–487 10.1006/anbe.2003.2101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Braida D., Donzelli A., Martucci R., Capurro V., Busnelli M., Chini B., et al. (2012). Neurohypophyseal hormones manipulation modulate social and anxiety-related behavior in zebrafish. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 220 319–330 10.1007/s00213-011-2482-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bshary R. (2011). “Machiavellian intelligence in fishes,” in Fish Cognition and Behavior eds Brown C., Laland K., Krause J. (Oxford: Blackwell; ) 277–297 [Google Scholar]
  18. Bshary R., Wickler W., Fricke H. (2002). Fish cognition: a primate’s eye view. Anim. Cogn. 5 1–13 10.1007/s10071-001-0116-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Budick S. A, O’Malley D. M. (2000). Locomotor repertoire of the larval zebrafish: swimming, turning and prey capture. J. Exp. Biol. 203 2565–2579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bugnyar T. (2011). Knower–guesser differentiation in ravens: others’ viewpoints matter. Proc. R. Soc. B 278 634–640 10.1098/rspb.2010.1514 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Burish M. J., Kueh H. Y., Wang S. S. (2004). Brain architecture and social complexity in modern and ancient birds. Brain Behav. Evol. 63 107–124 10.1159/000075674 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Burke C. J., Tobler P. N., Baddeley M., Schultz W. (2010). Neural mechanisms of observational learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107 14431–14436 10.1073/pnas.1003111107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Burns J. G., Foucaud J., Mery F. (2011). Costs of memory: lessons from ‘mini’ brains. Proc. R. Soc. B 278 923–929 10.1098/rspb.2010.2488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Buske C., Gerlai R. (2011). Shoaling develops with age in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35 1409–1415 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Buske C., Gerlai R. (2012). Maturation of shoaling behavior is accompanied by changes in the dopaminergic and serotoninergic systems in zebrafish. Dev. Psychobiol. 54 28–35 10.1002/dev.20571 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Byrne R. W., Bates L. A. (2006). Why are animals cognitive? Curr. Biol. 16 R445–R448 10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Byrne R. W., Bates L. A. (2009). Elephant cognition in primate perspective. Comp. Cog. Behav. Rev. 4 1–15 10.3819/ccbr.2009.40009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Byrne R. W., Bates L. A. (2010). Primate social cognition: uniquely primate, uniquely social, or just unique? Neuron 65 815–830 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Byrne R. W., Corp N. (2004). Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proc. R. Soc. B 271 1693–1699 10.1098/rspb.2004.2780 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cacioppo J. T., Decety J. (2011). Social neuroscience: challenges and opportunities in the study of complex behavior. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1224 162–173 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05858.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Chittka L., Niven J. (2009). Are bigger brains better? Curr. Biol. 19 R995–R1008 10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Chittka L., Skorupski P. (2011). Information processing in miniature brains. Proc R. Soc. B 278 885–888 10.1098/rspb.2010.2699 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Cole S. W., Hawkley L. C., Arevalo J. M., Sung C. Y., Rose R. M., Cacioppo J. T. (2007). Social regulation of gene expression in human leukocytes. Genome Biol. 8 R189 10.1186/gb-2007-8-9-r189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Connor R. C. (2007). Dolphin social intelligence: complex alliance relationships in bottlenose dolphins and a consideration of selective environments for extreme brain size evolution in mammals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 362 587–602 10.1098/rstb.2006.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Couzin I. D. (2009). Collective cognition in animal groups. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13 36–43 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Delaney M., Follet C., Ryan N., Hanney N., Lusk-Yablick J., Gerlach G. (2002). Social interaction and distribution of female zebrafish (Danio rerio) in a large aquarium. Biol. Bull. 203 240–241 10.2307/1543418 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. De Lillo C., Floreano D., Antinucci F. (2001). Transitive choices by a simple, fully connected, backpropagation neural network: implications for the comparative study of transitive inference. Anim. Cogn. 4 61–68 10.1007/s100710100092 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Donaldson Z. R., Young L. J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322 900–904 10.1126/science.1158668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Douglass A. D., Kraves S., Deisseroth K., Schier A. F., Engert F. (2008). Escape behavior elicited by single, channelrhodopsin-2-evoked spikes in zebrafish somatosensory neurons. Curr. Biol. 18 1133–1137 10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Doutrelant C., Mcgregor P. K., Oliveira R. F. (2001). The effect of an audience on intrasexual communication in male Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens. Behav. Ecol. 12 283–286 10.1093/beheco/12.3.283 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Døving K. B., Trotier D. (1998). Structure and function of the vomeronasal organ. J. Exp. Biol. 201 2913–2925 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dukas R. (2004). Evolutionary biology of animal cognition. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 35 347–374 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.35.112202.130152 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Dunbar R. I., Shultz S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science 317 1344–1347 10.1126/science.1145463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Dunbar R. I. M. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. J. Hum. Evol. 22 469–493 10.1016/0047-2484(92)90081-J [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Dunbar R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evol. Anthropol. 6 178–190 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Egger B., Obermuller B., Phiri H., Sturmbauer C., Sefc K. M. (2006). Monogamy in the maternally mouthbrooding Lake Tanganyika cichlid fish Tropheus moorii. Proc. R. Soc. B 273 1797–1802 10.1098/rspb.2006.3504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Emery N. J., Seed A. M., Von Bayern A. M., Clayton N. S. (2007). Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 362 489–505 10.1098/rstb.2006.1991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Engeszer R. E., Barbiano L. A., Ryan M. J., Parichy D. M. (2007). Timing and plasticity of shoaling behaviour in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Anim. Behav. 74 1269–1275 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Engeszer R. E., Ryan M. J., Parichy D. M. (2004). Learned social preference in zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 14 881–884 10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fagundes T., Goncalves D. M., Oliveira R. F. (2007). Female mate choice and mate search tactics in a sex role reversed population of the peacock blenny Salaria pavo (Risso, 1810). J. Fish Biol. 71 77–89 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2007.01466.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Fernald R. D. (2012). Social control of the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35 133–151 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Galef B. G., Laland K. N. (2005). Social learning in animals: empirical studies and theoretical models. Bioscience 55 489–499 10.1641/0006-3568(2005)055[0489:SLIAES]2.0.CO;2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Galhardo L., Vital J., Oliveira R. F. (2011). The role of predictability in the stress response of a cichlid fish. Physiol. Behav. 102 367–372 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gerlach G., Hodgins-Davis A., Avolio C., Schunter C. (2008). Kin recognition in zebrafish: a 24-hour window for olfactory imprinting. Proc. R. Soc. B 275 2165–2170 10.1098/rspb.2008.0647 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gerlach G., Lysiak N. (2006). Kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance in zebrafish, Danio rerio, is based on phenotype matching. Anim. Behav. 71 1371–1377 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Gerlai R. (2011). “Chapter 12 – Associative learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio),” in Methods Cell Biology eds William Detrich M. W. H., Leonard I. Z. (USA: Academic Press; ) 249–270 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gleason P. E., Weber P. G, Weber S. P. (1977). Effect of group size on avoidance learning in zebra fish, Brachydanio rerio (Pisces: Cyprinidae). Anim. Learn. Behav. 5 213–216 10.3758/BF03214081 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Godwin J. (2010). Neuroendocrinology of sexual plasticity in teleost fishes. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 31:203–216 10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.02.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Gonzalez-Voyer A., Winberg S., Kolm N. (2009). Social fishes and single mothers: brain evolution in African cichlids. Proc. R. Soc. B 276 161–167 10.1098/rspb.2008.0979 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Goodson J. L. (2005). The vertebrate social behavior network: evolutionary themes and variations. Horm. Behav. 48 11–22 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Goodson J. L., Evans A. K., Lindberg L., Allen C. D. (2005). Neuro-evolutionary patterning of sociality. Proc. R. Soc. B 272 227–235 10.1098/rspb.2004.2892 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Goodson J. L., Kabelik D. (2009). Dynamic limbic networks and social diversity in vertebrates: from neural context to neuromodulatory patterning. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30:429–441 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Grant J. W. A., Kramer D. L. (1992). Temporal clumping of food arrival reduces its monopolization and defence by zebrafish, Brachydanio rerio. Anim. Behav. 44 101–110 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80759-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Grosenick L., Clement T. S., Fernald R. D. (2007). Fish can infer social rank by observation alone. Nature 445 429–432 10.1038/nature05511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Grutter A. S., Bshary R. (2003). Cleaner wrasse prefer client mucus: support for partner control mechanisms in cleaning interactions. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270(Suppl. 2) S242–S244 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Gumm J. M., Snekser J. L., Iovine M. K. (2009). Fin-mutant female zebrafish (Danio rerio) exhibit differences in association preferences for male fin length. Behav. Processes 80 35–38 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Hall D., Suboski M. D. (1995). Visual and olfactory stimuli in learned release of alarm reactions by zebra danio fish (Brachydanio rerio). Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 63 229–240 10.1006/nlme.1995.1027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hepper P. G. (1986). Kin recognition: functions and mechanisms. A review. Biol. Rev. 61 63–93 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1986.tb00427.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Heyes C. (1994). Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biol. Rev. 69 207–231 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1994.tb01506.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Heyes C. (2012a). Simple minds: a qualified defence of associative learning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 367 2695–2703 10.1098/rstb.2012.0217 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Heyes C. (2012b). What’s social about social learning? J. Comp. Psychol. 126 193–202 10.1037/a0025180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Jensen K., Silk J. B., Andrews K., Bshary R., Cheney D. L., Emery N., et al. (2011). “Social knowledge,” in Animal Thinking: Contemporary Issues in Comparative Cognition eds Menzel R., Fischer J. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; ) 267–291 [Google Scholar]
  73. Johnstone R. A., Norris K. (1993). Badges of status and the cost of aggression. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 32 127–134 10.1007/BF00164045 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kanwisher N., Yovel G. (2006). The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 361 2109–2128 10.1098/rstb.2006.1934 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Karnik I., Gerlai R. (2012). Can zebrafish learn spatial tasks? An empirical analysis of place and single CS-US associative learning. Behav. Brain Res. 233 415–421 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.05.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Kelley A. E., Berridge K. C. (2002). The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance to addictive drugs. J. Neurosci. 22 3306–3311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Kendal R. L., Coolen I., Laland K. N. (2009). “Adaptive trade-offs in the use of social and personal information,” in Cognitive Ecology II eds Dukas R., Ratcliffe J.M. (Chicago: University of Chicago Press; ) 249–271 [Google Scholar]
  78. Kendrick K., Baldwin B. (1987). Cells in temporal cortex of conscious sheep can respond preferentially to the sight of faces. Science 236 448 10.1126/science.3563521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. King A. J., Cowlishaw G. (2009). Leaders, followers and group decision-making. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2 147–150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Kitevski B., Pyron M. (2003). Female zebrafish (Danio rerio) do not prefer mutant longfin males. J. Freshw. Ecol. 18 501–502 10.1080/02705060.2003.9663988 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  81. Koide T., Miyasaka N., Morimoto K., Asakawa K., Urasaki A., Kawakami K., et al. (2009). Olfactory neural circuitry for attraction to amino acids revealed by transposon-mediated gene trap approach in zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106 9884–9889 10.1073/pnas.0900470106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Kotrschal A., Rogell B., Bundsen A., Svensson B., Zajitschek S., Brännström I., et al. (2013). Artificial selection on relative brain size in the guppy reveals costs and benefits of evolving a larger brain. Curr. Biol. 23 168–171 10.1016/j.cub.2012.11.058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Krebs H. A. (1975). The august Krogh principle:“for many problems there is an animal on which it can be most conveniently studied”. J. Exp. Zool. 194 221–226 10.1002/jez.1401940115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Kumaran D., Melo H. L., Duzel E. (2012). The emergence and representation of knowledge about social and nonsocial hierarchies. Neuron 76 653–666 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Lau B. Y., Mathur P., Gould G. G., Guo S. (2011). Identification of a brain center whose activity discriminates a choice behavior in zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108 2581–2586 10.1073/pnas.1018275108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Lihoreau M., Latty T., Chittka L. (2012). An exploration of the social brain hypothesis in insects. Front. Physiol. 3:442 10.3389/fphys.2012.00442 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Lindeyer C. M., Reader S. M. (2010). Social learning of escape routes in zebrafish and the stability of behavioural traditions. Anim. Behav. 79 827–834 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.12.024 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lohr H., Ryu S., Driever W. (2009). Zebrafish diencephalic A11-related dopaminergic neurons share a conserved transcriptional network with neuroendocrine cell lineages. Development 136 1007–1017 10.1242/dev.033878 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Machado H. E., Pollen A. A., Hofmann H. A., Renn S. C. (2009). Interspecific profiling of gene expression informed by comparative genomic hybridization: a review and a novel approach in African cichlid fishes. Integr. Comp. Biol. 49 644–659 10.1093/icb/icp080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. MacLean E. L., Matthews L. J., Hare B. A., Nunn C. L., Anderson R. C., Aureli F., et al. (2012). How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology. Anim. Cogn. 15 223–238 10.1007/s10071-011-0448-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. MacLean E. L., Merritt D. J., Brannon E. M. (2008). Social complexity predicts transitive reasoning in prosimian primates. Anim. Behav. 76 479–486 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Maruska K. P., Zhang A., Neboori A., Fernald R. D. (2013). Social opportunity causes rapid transcriptional changes in the social behaviour network of the brain in an African cichlid fish. J. Neuroendocrinol. 25 145–157 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2012.02382.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Matos R. J., Schlupp I. (2005). “Performing in front of an audience: signalers and the social environment,” in Animal Communication Networks ed. McGregor P. K. (Cambridge: Cambrige University Press; ) 63–83 [Google Scholar]
  94. McCann L. I., Carlson C. C. (1982). Effect of cross-rearing on species identification in zebra fish and pearl danios. Dev. Psychobiol. 15 71–74 10.1002/dev.420150110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. McCann L. I., Matthews J. J. (1974). The effects of lifelong isolation on species identification in zebra fish (Brachydanio rerio). Dev. Psychobiol. 7 159–163 10.1002/dev.420070209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. McGregor P. K., Peake T. M. (2000). Communication networks: social environments for receiving and signalling behaviour. Acta Ethol. 2 71–81 10.1007/s102110000015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  97. McIntosh A. (2000). Towards a network theory of cognition. Neural Netw. 13 861–870 10.1016/S0893-6080(00)00059-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Mendl M., Burman O. H., Paul E. S. (2010). An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proc. R. Soc. B 277 2895–2904 10.1098/rspb.2010.0303 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Menzel R., Giurfa M. (2001). Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5 62–71 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01601-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Mery F. (2013). Natural variation in learning and memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23 52–56 10.1016/j.conb.2012.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Miller N., Gerlai R. (2007). Quantification of shoaling behaviour in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 184 157–166 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Miller N., Gerlai R. (2008). Oscillations in shoal cohesion in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 193 148–151 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.05.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Miller N., Gerlai R. (2011). Shoaling in zebrafish: what we don’t know. Rev. Neurosci. 22 17–25 10.1515/RNS.2011.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Miller N., Gerlai R. (2012). From schooling to shoaling: patterns of collective motion in zebrafish (Danio rerio). PLoS ONE 7:e48865 10.1371/journal.pone.0048865 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Muhlhoff N., Stevens J. R., Reader S. M. (2011). Spatial discounting of food and social rewards in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Front. Psychol. 2:68. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Muto A., Kawakami K. (2011). Imaging functional neural circuits in zebrafish with a new GCaMP and the Gal4FF-UAS system. Commun. Integr. Biol. 4 566–568 10.4161/cib.4.5.15848 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Muto A., Ohkura M., Abe G., Nakai J., Kawakami K. (2013). Real-time visualization of neuronal activity during perception. Curr. Biol. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.040[Epubaheadofprint]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Newman S. W. (1999). The medial extended amygdala in male reproductive behavior a node in the mammalian social behavior network. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 877 242–257 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09271.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Norman G. J., Hawkley L. C., Cole S. W., Berntson G. G., Cacioppo J. T. (2012). Social neuroscience: the social brain, oxytocin, and health. Soc. Neurosci. 7 18–29 10.1080/17470919.2011.568702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. O’Connell L. A., Hofmann H. A. (2011). The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 519 3599–3639 10.1002/cne.22735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. O’Connell L. A., Hofmann H. A. (2012). Evolution of a vertebrate social decision-making network. Science 336 1154–1157 10.1126/science.1218889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Okamoto H., Agetsuma M., Aizawa H. (2012). Genetic dissection of the zebrafish habenula, a possible switching board for selection of behavioral strategy to cope with fear and anxiety. Dev. Neurobiol. 72 386–394 10.1002/dneu.20913 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Oliveira R. F. (2009). Social behavior in context: hormonal modulation of behavioral plasticity and social competence. Integr. Comp. Biol. 49 423–440 10.1093/icb/icp055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Oliveira R. F. (2012). Social plasticity in fish: integrating mechanisms and function. J. Fish Biol. 81 2127–2150 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2012.03477.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Oliveira R. F., Carneiro L. A., Canario A. V. (2005). No hormonal response in tied fights. Nature 437 207–208 10.1038/437207a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Oliveira R. F., Mcgregor P. K., Latruffe C. (1998). Know thine enemy: fighting fish gather information from observing conspecific interactions. Proc. R. Soc. B 265 1045–1049 10.1098/rspb.1998.0397 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  117. Oliveira R. F., Silva J. F., Simoes J. M. (2011). Fighting zebrafish: characterization of aggressive behavior and winner-loser effects. Zebrafish 8 73–81 10.1089/zeb.2011.0690 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Pagel M., Dawkins M. (1997). Peck orders and group size in laying hens: futures contracts’ for non-aggression. Behav. Processes 40 13–25 10.1016/S0376-6357(96)00761-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Parker M. O., Gaviria J., Haigh A., Millington M. E., Brown V. J., Combe F. J., et al. (2012). Discrimination reversal and attentional sets in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 232 264–268 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Parrish J. K., Turchin P. (1997). “Individual decisions, traffic rules, and emergent pattern in schooling fish,” in Animal Groups in Three Dimensions eds Parrish J. K., Hamner W. M. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; ) 126–142 [Google Scholar]
  121. Parrish J. K., Viscido S. V. (2005). “Traffic rules of fish schools: a review of agent-based approaches,” in Self-Organisation and the Evolution of Social Behaviour eds Hemelrijk C. K. (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; ) 81–107 [Google Scholar]
  122. Paul E. S., Harding E. J., Mendl M. (2005). Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29 469–491 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Paull G. C., Filby A. L., Giddins H. G., Coe T. S., Hamilton P. B., Tyler C. R. (2010). Dominance hierarchies in zebrafish (Danio rerio) and their relationship with reproductive success. Zebrafish 7 109–117 10.1089/zeb.2009.0618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Paz-Y-Miño C. G., Bond A. B., Kamil A. C., Balda R. P. (2004). Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance. Nature 430 778–781 10.1038/nature02723 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Peake T. M. (2005). “Eavesdropping in communication networks. Animal communication networks,” in Animal Communication Networks ed. McGregor P. K. (Cambridge: Cambrige University Press; ) 13–37 [Google Scholar]
  126. Penn D. C., Povinelli D. J. (2007). On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 362 731–744 10.1098/rstb.2006.2023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Perez-Barberia F. J., Shultz S., Dunbar R. I. (2007). Evidence for coevolution of sociality and relative brain size in three orders of mammals. Evolution 61 2811–2821 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00229.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Pinto A., Oates J., Grutter A., Bshary R. (2011). Cleaner wrasses Labroides dimidiatus are more cooperative in the presence of an audience. Curr. Biol. 21 1140–1144 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Pitcher T. J., Magurran A. E., Allen J. R. (1983). Shifts of behaviour with shoal size in cyprinids. Proc. Br. Freshw. Fish Conf. 3 220–228 10.1371/journal.pone.0032411 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  130. Pitcher T. J., Parrish J. K. (1993). “Functions of shoaling behavior in teleosts,” in Behaviour of Teleost Fishes ed. Pitcher T. J. (New York: Chapman & Hall; ) 363–440 10.1007/978-94-011-1578-0_12 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  131. Pollen A. A., Dobberfuhl A. P., Scace J., Igulu M. M., Renn S. C., Shumway C. A., et al. (2007). Environmental complexity and social organization sculpt the brain in Lake Tanganyikan cichlid fish. Brain Behav. Evol. 70 21–39 10.1159/000101067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Portugues R., Severi K. E., Wyart C., Ahrens M. B. (2013). Optogenetics in a transparent animal: circuit function in the larval zebrafish. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23 119–126 10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Premack D., Woodruff G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1 515–526 10.1017/S0140525X00076512 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  134. Pritchard V. L., Lawrence J., Butlin R. K., Krause J. (2001). Shoal choice in zebrafish, Danio rerio: the influence of shoal size and activity. Anim. Behav. 62 1085–1088 10.1006/anbe.2001.1858 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Pyron M. (2003). Female preferences and male male interactions in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Can. J. Zool. 81 122–125 10.1139/z02-229 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  136. Reader S. M., Laland K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99 4436–4441 10.1073/pnas.062041299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Rink E., Wullimann M. F. (2001). The teleostean (zebrafish) dopaminergic system ascending to the subpallium (striatum) is located in the basal diencephalon (posterior tuberculum). Brain Res. 889 316–330 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)03174-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Rink E., Wullimann M. F. (2002). Connections of the ventral telencephalon and tyrosine hydroxylase distribution in the zebrafish brain (Danio rerio) lead to identification of an ascending dopaminergic system in a teleost. Brain Res. Bull. 57 385–387 10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00696-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Ristau C. A. (1991). Cognitive Ethology: The Minds of Other Animals: Essays in Honor of Donald R. Griffin. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates [Google Scholar]
  140. Ruhl N., McRobert S. (2005). The effect of sex and shoal size on shoaling behaviour in Danio rerio. J. Fish Biol. 67 1318–1326 10.1111/j.0022-1112.2005.00826.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  141. Ryan M. J., Akre K. L., Kirkpatrick M. (2007). Mate choice. Curr. Biol. 17 R313–316 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Saif M., Chatterjee D., Buske C., Gerlai R. (2013). Sight of conspecific images induces changes in neurochemistry in zebrafish. Behav. Brain Res. 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.01.020[Epubaheadofprint]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Sallet J., Mars R. B., Noonan M. P., Andersson J. L., O’Reilly J. X., Jbabdi S., et al. (2011). Social network size affects neural circuits in macaques. Science 334 697–700 10.1126/science.1210027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Schultz W. (2010). Review Dopamine signals for reward value and risk: basic and recent data. Behav. Brain Funct. 6 24 10.1186/1744-9081-6-24 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Shettleworth S. J. (2001). Animal cognition and animal behaviour. Anim. Behav. 61 277–286 10.1006/anbe.2000.1606 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  146. Shettleworth S. J. (2010). Cognition, Evolution and Behavior, 2nd Edn. New York: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
  147. Sih A., Hanser S. F., Mchugh K. A. (2009). Social network theory: new insights and issues for behavioral ecologists. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63 975–988 10.1007/s00265-009-0725-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  148. Simõeses J. M., Teles M. C., Oliveira R. F., Van Der Linden A., Verhoye M. (2012). A three-dimensional stereotaxic MRI brain atlas of the cichlid fish Oreochromis mossambicus. PLoS ONE 7:e44086 10.1371/journal.pone.0044086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Smeets W. J. A. J., Marin O., Gonzalez A. (2000). Evolution of the basal ganglia: new perspectives through a comparative approach. J. Anat. 196 501–517 10.1046/j.1469-7580.2000.19640501.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Snekser J. L., Mcrobert S. P., Murphy C. E., Clotfelter E. D. (2006). Aggregation behavior in wildtype and transgenic zebrafish. Ethology 112 181–187 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01139.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  151. Speedie N., Gerlai R. (2008). Alarm substance induced behavioral responses in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 188 168–177 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.10.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Spence R. (2011). “Zebrafish ecology and behaviour,” in Zebrafish Models in Neurobehavioral Research eds Kalueff A.V., Cachat J. M. (Berlin: Springer Verlag; ) 1–46 10.1007/978-1-60761-922-2_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  153. Spence R., Gerlach G., Lawrence C., Smith C. (2008). The behaviour and ecology of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Biol. Rev. 83 13–34 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00030.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Spence R., Smith C. (2005). Male territoriality mediates density and sex ratio effects on oviposition in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Anim. Behav. 69 1317–1323 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.10.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  155. Spence R., Smith C. (2006). Mating preference of female zebrafish, Danio rerio, in relation to male dominance. Behav. Ecol. 17 779–783 10.1093/beheco/arl016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  156. Spence R., Smith C. (2007). The role of early learning in determining shoaling preferences based on visual cues in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Ethology 113 62–67 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01295.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  157. Sporns O. (2010). Networks of the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  158. Suboski M. D., Bain S., Carty A. E., Mcquoid L. M., Seelen M. I., Seifert M. (1990). Alarm reaction in acquisition and social transmission of simulated-predator recognition by zebra danio fish (Brachydanio rerio). J. Comp. Psychol. 104 101–112 10.1037/0735-7036.104.1.101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  159. Sumpter D. J. T. (2006). The principles of collective animal behaviour. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 361 5–22 10.1098/rstb.2005.1733 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Taborsky B., Oliveira R. F. (2012). Social competence: an evolutionary approach. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27 679–688 10.1016/j.tree.2012.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Tay T. L., Ronneberger O., Ryu S., Nitschke R., Driever W. (2011). Comprehensive catecholaminergic projectome analysis reveals single-neuron integration of zebrafish ascending and descending dopaminergic systems. Nat. Commun. 2 171 10.1038/ncomms1171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Tebbich S., Bshary R., Grutter A. S. (2002). Cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus recognise familiar clients. Anim. Cogn. 5 139–145 10.1007/s10071-002-0141-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Thiel K. J., Okun A. C., Neisewander J. L. (2008). Social reward-conditioned place preference: a model revealing an interaction between cocaine and social context rewards in rats. Drug Alcohol Depend 96 202–212 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.02.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Tibbetts E. A., Dale J. (2007). Individual recognition: it is good to be different. Trends Ecol. Evol. 22 529–537 10.1016/j.tree.2007.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Tsao D. Y., Freiwald W. A., Tootell R. B., Livingstone M. S. (2006). A cortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science 311 670–674 10.1126/science.1119983 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Tsao D. Y., Schweers N., Moeller S., Freiwald W. A. (2008). Patches of face-selective cortex in the macaque frontal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 11 877–879 10.1038/nn.2158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Ullmann J. F., Cowin G., Collin S. P. (2010a). Magnetic resonance microscopy of the barramundi (Lates calcarifer) brain. J. Morphol. 271 1446–1456 10.1002/jmor.10887 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Ullmann J. F., Cowin G., Collin S. P. (2010b). Quantitative assessment of brain volumes in fish: comparison of methodologies. Brain Behav. Evol. 76 261–270 10.1159/000321467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Ullmann J. F., Cowin G., Kurniawan N. D., Collin S. P. (2010c). Magnetic resonance histology of the adult zebrafish brain: optimization of fixation and gadolinium contrast enhancement. NMR Biomed. 23 341–346 10.1002/nbm.1465 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Ullmann J. F., Cowin G., Kurniawan N. D., Collin S. P. (2010d). A three-dimensional digital atlas of the zebrafish brain. Neuroimage 51 76–82 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Viscido S. V., Parrish J. K, Grünbaum D. (2004). Individual behavior and emergent properties of fish schools: a comparison of observation and theory. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 273 239–249 10.3354/meps273239 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  172. Vital C., Martins E. P. (2011). Strain differences in zebrafish (Danio rerio) social roles and their impact on group task performance. J. Comp. Psychol. 125 278–285 10.1037/a0023906 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Von Fersen L., Wynne C., Delius J. D., Staddon J. (1991). Transitive inference formation in pigeons. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process 17 334–341 10.1037/0097-7403.17.3.334 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  174. Waldman B. (1982). Quantitative and developmental analysis of the alarm reaction in the zebra Danio Brachydanio rerio. Copeia 1982 1–9 [Google Scholar]
  175. Wersinger S. R., Kelliher K. R., Zufall F., Lolait S. J., O’Carroll A.-M, Young W. S. (2004). Social motivation is reduced in vasopressin 1b receptor null mice despite normal performance in an olfactory discrimination task. Horm. Behav. 46 638–645 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Whiten A., Byrne R. W. (1988). “The Machiavellian intelligence hypothesis,” in Machiavellian Intelligence eds Byrne R.W., Whiten A. (Oxford: Oxford University Press; ) 1–9 [Google Scholar]
  177. Whiten A., Byrne R. W. (1997). Machiavellian Intelligence II: Extensions and Evaluations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
  178. Witte K., Ryan M. J. (2002). Mate choice copying in the sailfin molly, Poecilia latipinna, in the wild. Anim. Behav. 63 943–949 10.1006/anbe.2001.1982 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  179. Wullimann M. F., Mueller T. (2004). Teleostean and mammalian forebrains contrasted: evidence from genes to behavior. J. Comp. Neurol. 478 427–428 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Wullimann M. F., Rupp B., Reichert H. (1996). Neuroanatomy of the zebrafish brain: a topological atlas. Basel: Birkhäuser [Google Scholar]
  181. Yamamoto K., Vernier P. (2011). The evolution of dopamine systems in chordates. Front. Neuroanat. 5:21 10.3389/fnana.2011.00021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Yang E. J., Wilczynski W. (2007). Social experience organizes parallel networks in sensory and limbic forebrain. Dev. Neurobiol. 67 285–303 10.1002/dneu.20347 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Zhu P., Narita Y., Bundschuh S. T., Fajardo O., Scharer Y. P., Chattopadhyaya B., et al. (2009). Optogenetic dissection of neuronal circuits in zebrafish using viral gene transfer and the tet system. Front. Neural Circuits 3:21 10.3389/neuro.04.021.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Zuberbuhler K., Byrne R. W. (2006). Social cognition. Curr. Biol. 16 R786–R790 10.1016/j.cub.2006.08.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Neural Circuits are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES