Skip to main content
BMJ Open logoLink to BMJ Open
. 2013 Aug 8;3(8):e002749. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002749

A meta-analysis of cognitive-based behaviour change techniques as interventions to improve medication adherence

Claire Easthall 1, Fujian Song 2, Debi Bhattacharya 1
PMCID: PMC3740257  PMID: 23935093

Abstract

Objective

To describe and evaluate the use of cognitive-based behaviour change techniques as interventions to improve medication adherence.

Design

Systematic review and meta-analysis of interventions to improve medication adherence.

Data sources

Search of the MEDLINE, EMBASE, PsycINFO, CINAHL and The Cochrane Library databases from the earliest year to April 2013 without language restriction. References of included studies were also screened to identify further relevant articles.

Review methods

We used predefined criteria to select randomised controlled trials describing a medication adherence intervention that used Motivational Interviewing (MI) or other cognitive-based techniques. Data were extracted and risk of bias was assessed by two independent reviewers. We conducted the meta-analysis using a random effects model and Hedges’ g as the measure of effect size.

Results

We included 26 studies (5216 participants) in the meta-analysis. Interventions most commonly used MI, but many used techniques such as aiming to increase the patient's confidence and sense of self-efficacy, encouraging support-seeking behaviours and challenging negative thoughts, which were not specifically categorised. Interventions were most commonly delivered from community-based settings by routine healthcare providers such as general practitioners and nurses. An effect size (95% CI) of 0.34 (0.23 to 0.46) was calculated and was statistically significant (p < 0.001). Heterogeneity was high with an I2 value of 68%. Adjustment for publication bias generated a more conservative estimate of summary effect size of 0.21 (0.08 to 0.33). The majority of subgroup analyses produced statistically non-significant results.

Conclusions

Cognitive-based behaviour change techniques are effective interventions eliciting improvements in medication adherence that are likely to be greater than the behavioural and educational interventions largely used in current practice. Subgroup analyses suggest that these interventions are amenable to use across different populations and in differing manners without loss of efficacy. These factors may facilitate incorporation of these techniques into routine care.

Keywords: Medication Adherence, Meta-Analysis, Behaviour Change, Motivational Interviewing, Adherence Intervention


Article summary.

Article focus

  • Medication non-adherence is widespread and represents a notable barrier in achieving optimal effects from therapeutic intervention.

  • Despite the magnitude and consequences of non-adherence, a gold standard intervention to improve it remains elusive.

  • Cognitive-based behaviour change techniques (CBCTs) may represent a useful tool in improving medication adherence, but their use in this domain has not been established using meta-analytic techniques.

Key messages

  • CBCTs are effective interventions for improving medication adherence and capable of eliciting improvements in adherence beyond those achieved with educational and behavioural interventions which form the mainstay of current practice.

  • According to the results of subgroup analyses, CBCTs can be effectively delivered by routine healthcare providers, and the effectiveness of interventions is not associated with intervention exposure.

  • Healthcare providers may wish to consider incorporation of these techniques into their medication adherence consultations.

Strengths and limitations of this study

  • The studies pooled in this meta-analysis are restricted to randomised controlled trials, which strengthens their robustness.

  • Techniques to account for publication bias have been utilised to provide a conservative effect size estimate offering robustness to our estimate.

  • Notable heterogeneity was reported when studies were combined, which may be a limitation.

Introduction

Estimates suggest that 30–50% of patients prescribed medications for chronic illnesses do not adhere to their prescribed medication regimen.1 This non-adherence has been demonstrated to diminish treatment effect which can result in prolonged illness, additional investigations and prescriptions that may otherwise have been unnecessary.2 A link between poor adherence and an increased risk of mortality is also well established.3 Consequently, the WHO has described non-adherence as ‘a worldwide problem of striking magnitude’ and a priority for healthcare researchers and policymakers.1

Despite the magnitude and potential gravity of suboptimal medication adherence, a gold standard intervention remains elusive; a recent Cochrane review highlighted the paucity of effective interventions in current practice.4 Evidence suggests that complex, multifaceted interventions tailored to meet individual needs are most likely to be efficacious,4 5 which is intuitive given the complex, multistage process that is taking medication.

Non-adherent behaviour is traditionally categorised into unintentional and intentional. Unintentional non-adherence includes behaviours arising from forgetfulness, misunderstanding and confusion. Intentional non-adherence describes patient choice that deviates from the prescribed medication regimen. Unintentional and intentional non-adherence are not mutually exclusive; thus, an amalgam of these behaviours often exists in any one patient. An understanding of patient behaviour and its underpinning psychology plus the wealth of factors, both internal and external that may influence medication taking, is crucial to understand how to change patient behaviour and thus improve medication adherence.6

Historically, adherence interventions have encompassed behaviour change techniques such as simplifying dosage regimens and providing adherence aids or education to address the practical issues of adherence in terms of knowing how and being able to take the medication as prescribed. Pooled data for such studies have demonstrated marginal effects,4 yet such interventions continue to form the cornerstone of routine healthcare provision.2 These interventions may have particularly poor efficacy in cases of intentional non-adherence as the provision of persuasive advice may evoke further resistance to change.7 8 Through an understanding of the challenges faced in changing behaviours and the motivation necessary to achieve change, novel, cognitive-based behaviour change techniques (CBCTs) have emerged. These interventions aim to change a patient's behaviour by altering their thoughts, feelings, confidence or motivation to adhere. CBCT interventions can vary widely in content such as incorporating techniques to enhance patient sense of self-efficacy, problem solve and increase motivation to adhere.

Motivational Interviewing (MI) is one of the most widely recognised CBCTs and is designed to facilitate behaviour change by resolving patient ambivalence about change.9 It therefore primarily targets intentional non-adherence but also enables patients to reflect on any unintentional barriers to adherence and seek out solutions. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses have reported MI efficacy in facilitating health-related behaviour change such as smoking cessation and alcohol withdrawal10–16 but have not explored its effects on medication adherence. Adaptations of MI such as Behaviour Change Counselling17 additionally allow the facilitator to educate and advise, and thus application to both intentional and unintentional non-adherence may be effective.

Best practice guidelines state that evidence of intervention efficacy should ideally be pooled from the literature in a systematic review or meta-analysis wherever possible to offer a robust and cohesive evidence base.18 This study provides a systematic review and meta-analysis of MI and other cognitive-based techniques as interventions to improve medication adherence.

Methods

We used standard systematic review methods18 19 and registered the study protocol (PROSPERO register reference CRD42011001721). Randomised controlled trials (RCTs) reporting an adherence intervention using MI and/or other cognitive-based techniques with medication adherence as an outcome measure were eligible for inclusion. All definitions of adherence, such as the percentage of doses taken over a given time period and percentage of patients achieving a specified adherence level, were considered. All adherence measures were also considered, including self-report and electronic monitoring. Where multiple measures were reported, the percentage of patients achieving a specified adherence level was selected as this was common to more studies.

Any intervention using some form of psychological technique to change a patient's adherence behaviour, as well as their thoughts, feelings, confidence or motivation towards adherence, was defined as a cognitive-based technique. Studies examining adherence to medications for the treatment of addiction and/or mental health conditions were excluded as these interventions tend to be specific to these domains.

Search strategies

We developed a search strategy to avoid restriction to predetermined terms such as ‘motivational interviewing’ as many of the techniques of interest are not classified using specific or consistent terms. MeSH terms were also used to enhance retrieval of relevant studies. Truncations (*), wild cards ($), hyphens and other relevant Boolean operators were used where permitted. Scoping searches were conducted prior to finalising the search strategy to ensure suitability of terms in generating a good coverage of relevant material.

We applied the search strategy (as shown in online supplementary appendix 1) to the MEDLINE, EMBASE, PsychINFO, CINAHL and Cochrane databases in April 2013 without date or language restrictions. The reference lists of all screened full-text articles were also used to identify further relevant articles.

Study selection and data extraction

Two researchers (CE and EP) independently screened titles and abstracts against the inclusion and exclusion criteria using a piloted abstract screening tool. Inter-reviewer agreement using Cohen's κ was assessed for the abstract and full-text screening stages. The level of agreement was characterised using a qualitative scale.20 Discrepancies were resolved by discussion between the two reviewers and, if necessary, referral to a third independent reviewer (DB) until consensus was reached.

Data extraction was also undertaken by CE and EP, independently using piloted forms. Data extracted included study details (such as year and journal of publication, country and study design); study characteristics (including setting, population, delivery methods and personnel); intervention details (including intervention type, duration and principal components) and outcome details (including adherence assessment measure, data and definition). A list of intervention components was independently extracted from the articles verbatim by two reviewers. Grouping of similar components was undertaken by one reviewer and verified by a second reviewer.

Accuracy of data collected was verified by comparison of the forms completed by the two independent reviewers. In cases of discrepancy, consensus was agreed through discussion and, where necessary, referral to a third independent reviewer (DB). For studies with missing data or ambiguities, the corresponding author was contacted for clarification.

Quality assessment

A quality assessment of all included studies was made using the Cochrane risk of bias tool.18 The risk of bias was assessed in five domains deemed relevant to the included studies: random sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding of outcome assessment, incomplete outcome data and selective reporting. Performance bias (blinding of participants and personnel) was not included as the nature of the interventions meant that blinding of participants and personnel was impossible in almost all studies. None of the included studies were found to contain additional sources of potential bias not represented by the five included domains. The risk of bias for each study, in each of the five domains, was classified as low, uncertain or high, as recommended in the guidelines.18 The quality assessment process was undertaken independently by two reviewers, with consensus on the final risk classifications reached through discussion.

Data analysis

The meta-analysis was conducted using STATA (V.12.1). Given the broad inclusion criteria, we anticipated including studies from different populations with different diseases and which used different CBCTs. We therefore explored heterogeneity via calculation of the I2 statistic, which describes the percentage of total variation across studies that is due to heterogeneity rather than chance.21 22 A random effects model (DerSimonian-Laird method) was employed to calculate a pooled effect size (Hedges’ g) and 95% CI for the included studies.23 Calculation of the effect size as Hedges’ g (standardised difference in means) enabled adherence outcome measures of differing definition and measure to be combined, transforming this data into a common metric. When SD was missing, we estimated SE of mean difference based on reported p values, means and the number of patients. ORs were converted to standardised mean differences by using the formula SMD=lnOR×√3/π.23

Funnel plots were produced where appropriate to explore potential publication biases. STATA (V.12.1) was used to conduct Egger's test24 to test funnel plot asymmetry. We used the trim-and-fill method25 26 to estimate a summary effect size after adjusting for asymmetric funnel plots.

Variables of interest in influencing the effect size and informing intervention design were determined a priori and the following subgroup analyses undertaken using a random effects meta-regression: intervention components, setting, delivery personnel, delivery method and intervention exposure, disease area and risk of bias. The type of outcome measure used to assess adherence (objective compared with subjective) was added as a post hoc subgroup analysis to further explore heterogeneity. Objective outcome measures included electronic monitoring and pill counts, while subjective measures included all forms of self-report. Differences between subgroups were tested using the STATA ‘metareg’ command for random-effects univariate meta-regression analysis.

Results

Study selection, characteristics and quality

Figure 1 shows the number of papers excluded at each stage of the review. Of the 442 abstracts screened, 84 studies passed the abstract screening stage with moderate agreement between the two reviewers (κ=0·57). Conflict in classifying an intervention as a CBCT accounted for 31% of discrepancies and was heavily influenced by a paucity of information in the abstracts. At the full-text screening stage, agreement between the two independent reviewers was much higher, with a κ value of 0.91, indicating almost perfect agreement. After examining 84 full-text articles, we included 26 (31%) in the meta-analysis.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow diagram for selection of studies.

The main characteristics of the 26 included studies are summarised in table 1. The studies provided a total sample size of 5216 participants and were primarily undertaken in the USA, followed by the UK,27–29 Australia30 31 and the Netherlands.32 33 Dates of publication ranged from 1990 to 2012 with only two studies (7.7%) predating 2000.28 34 Ten (38.5%) were published within the last 5 years (2008–2013). The most common condition for which medications were prescribed was HIV, accounting for 14 (53.8%) studies. Other studies concerned treatments for a range of conditions including asthma,32 34 35 diabetes27 31 and hypertension.30 36

Table 1.

Characteristics of included studies in meta-analysis

Study Study setting Disease area Intervention description* Identified intervention components Components received by control group Sample size Intervention delivery style (and personnel) Intervention length (average)
Bailey et al34 Hospital clinic, USA Asthma Comprehensive programme integrating a skill-orientated self-help workbook with one-to-one counselling and adherence-enhancing strategies Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care; education via a standardised set of pamphlets and routine physician encouragement 225 Telephone calls and in person (specialist) 240 min (4×60 min sessions) over unknown period
Berger et al40 Telephone calls to patients at home, USA Multiple sclerosis Software-supported intervention based on transtheoretical model of change and MI Motivational Interviewing (MI) Standard care plus could telephone help line 367 Telephone calls (researcher) 9 sessions of unknown duration delivered over 3 months
Brown et al29 Hospital clinic, UK Epilepsy Formation of III via completion of a self-administered questionnaire Implementation Intention Interventions (III) Standard care plus self-report questionnaires 69 Questionnaire completion (not in person) One-off intervention of unknown duration
DiIorio et al41 Community clinic, USA HIV One-to-one counselling sessions based on MI MI Standard care; usual adherence education provided in the clinic 17 In person (routine HCP) 5×35 min sessions delivered over 12 months
DiIorio et al42 Hospital clinic, USA HIV MI as individual counselling sessions MI Standard care; usual (extensive) education provided at the clinic 213 Mostly in person with some telephone calls (routine HCP) 5 sessions of 35 min over 12 months
Farmer et al27 Community-based clinic, UK Type 2 diabetes Brief intervention to elicit beliefs, resolve barriers and form ‘if-then’ plans If-then planning (III) Standard care plus additional clinic visits for blood tests 211 In person (clinic nurse) One-off session lasting 30 min.
George et al30 Community pharmacies, Australia and Tasmania Hypertension Community pharmacy intervention of one-to-one sessions, monitoring and medication review MI Standard care 343 In person (routine HCP) 3 sessions of unknown duration over 6 months
Golin et al39 Community clinic, USA HIV Multicomponent MI-based intervention MI General HIV information provided via audio tape, two one-to-one sessions and two mail shots 117 In person (specialist) 2 sessions of unknown duration over 2 months
Hovell et al51 Hospital clinic, USA Tuberculosis Adherence coaching involving interviewing, contingency contracting and shaping procedures Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care; routine advice at appointments 188 Telephone calls and in person (researcher) 12 sessions of 15–30 min over 6 months
Konkle-Parker et al38 Community-based clinics and patients’ own homes, USA HIV Adherence intervention guided by the Information-Motivation-Behavioural Skills (IMB) model MI Standard care; usual clinic appointments 36 Telephone calls and in person (nurse practitioner) 8 sessions over 24 weeks. Average overall duration 1 h 30 min
Maneesriwongul et al37 Hospital outpatients clinic and telephone calls to patients at home, Thailand HIV MI with counselling MI Standard care; education and provision of leaflets at point of prescribing 60 Telephone calls and in person (researcher) 3 sessions approximately 30 min over a 4 week period
Murphy et al52 Community-based clinic, USA HIV Multicomponent and multidisciplinary intervention including behavioural strategies and cognitive behavioural therapy Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care; regular appointments with enquiries about adherence and an additional 30 min appointment for those with problems where medication schedule is written down for them 33 In person (specialist) 5 sessions of unknown duration over 7 weeks
Ogedegbe et al36 Community clinic, USA Hypertension Practice-based MI counseling MI Standard care; usual appointments plus additional visits for MEMS downloads 160 In person (researcher) 4 sessions lasting 30–40 min delivered over 12 months
Pradier et al50 Hospital clinic, France HIV Educational and counselling intervention founded in the principles of motivational psychology and client-centred therapy Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care; routine follow-up appointments 202 In person (routine HCP) 3 sessions of 45–60 min over 3 months
Put et al35 Hospital clinic, Belgium Asthma Behavioural change intervention involving psycho-education with behavioural and cognitive techniques Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard (no details provided) 23 In person (researcher) 360 min (6×60 min sessions) over 3 months
Remien et al49 Community-based clinic, USA HIV Couples-based intervention grounded in social action theory Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care; education at point of prescribing and follow-up to check adherence and investigate/address underlying causes of any non-adherence 196 In person (routine HCP) 4 sessions of 45–60 min over 5 weeks
Safren et al44 Community clinic, USA HIV Single session minimal treatment intervention using cognitive behavioural, MI and problem solving techniques MI Minimal contact intervention; daily diary used to record no. of pills prescribed and taken each day 53 In person (routine HCP) One-off intervention of unknown duration
Sheeran and Orbell28 Visits to patient's own home, UK Vitamin Supplements Formation of III via completion of a self-administered questionnaire Implementation Intention Intervention (III) Completion of same questionnaire but without formation of implementation intention 78 Questionnaire completion (not in person) One-off intervention of unknown duration
Simoni et al48 Community-based clinic and telephone calls to patients at home, USA HIV Peer-led medication-related social support intervention Multiple-components; non-specific techniques Standard care; education programme and social and health referrals as necessary 114 Group sessions and individual telephone calls (peers) 18 sessions of unknown duration over 3 months
Smith et al47 Community-based research office, USA HIV Self-management intervention based on feedback of adherence performance and principles of social cognitive theory Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care; usual medication counselling, educational leaflets, scheduling support reminder lists and discussion of adherence strategies 17 In person (routine HCP) Four sessions of unknown duration over 12 weeks
Solomon et al43 Telephone calls to patient's own home, USA Osteoporosis Telephone-based counselling programme rooted in MI MI Standard care plus seven information mailings on osteoarthritis care 2087 Telephone calls (health educator) 8 sessions of 14 min over 12 months
Tuldrà et al46 Hospital clinic, Spain HIV Psycheducative intervention based on Self-efficacy theory Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care; normal clinical follow-up 77 Unknown (routine HCP) 7 sessions of unknown duration
van Es et al32 Hospital clinic, Netherlands Asthma Intervention programme to stimulate a positive attitude, increase social support and enhance self-efficacy Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care; routine check-ups 67 In person (routine HCP) 7 sessions of 30–90 min over 12 months
Wagner et al45 Community clinic, USA HIV Cognitive behavioural intervention with motivational components, based on the information-motivation-behavioural skills (IMB) model Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care practices for improving adherence; education, tailoring regimen, offering a pillbox, adherence checks and enquiries about side effects 135 In person (routine HCP) 5 sessions of 30–45 min over 48 weeks
Weber et al33 Community, psychotherapy clinic, Netherlands HIV Cognitive behavioural intervention delivered by a psychotherapist Multiple components; non-specific techniques Standard care (no details provided) 53 In person (specialist) 11 sessions of 45 min over 12 months
Williams et al31 Telephone calls and visits to patient's own home, Australia Diabetes Multifactorial intervention consisting of self-monitoring of blood pressure, medicine review, educational DVDs and MI to support blood pressure control and optimal medication adherence MI Standard care (no details provided) 75 In person and phone calls (specialist) 5 sessions, one of 89 min and 4 of an average of 11.75 min, over 3 months

*See online supplementary table A for a detailed breakdown of intervention components.

Just over half of the included studies (53.8%) described an intervention with a clearly defined CBCT; MI was most commonly used and this was the case for 11 (42.3%) studies.30 31 36–44 A further three (11.5%) studies used Implementation Intention Interventions (III, also known as if-then planning) as a clearly defined CBCT. For 12 (46.2%) studies, a clearly defined CBCT, such as MI, could not be identified;32–35 45–52 these studies are identified in table 1 as ‘multiple components; non-specific techniques’. Instead, this group comprised multiple components such as ‘providing education’ or ‘increasing patient knowledge’, which was reported in nine (75%) studies in this group. Other components included ‘increasing self-efficacy’ and ‘developing or improving problem solving skills’, each reported in six (50) studies and ‘identifying and resolving adherence barriers’ and ‘increasing social support’, each of which was also reported in six studies (50%). All studies within this group included one or more components that aimed to alter the patient's thoughts, feelings, motivation or confidence towards adherence and that could therefore be classified as a CBCT. Detailed information regarding the identified intervention components extracted from each study are provided as an online supplementary table. The majority of interventions had multiple components. Many studies combined CBCT with more traditionally used educational (eg, increasing patient knowledge) and behavioural (eg, regimen simplification and provision of dosing aids) components.

Interventions were most commonly delivered in person, from community-based settings and by routine healthcare providers such as nurses, pharmacists and general medical practitioners. ‘Non-routine’ healthcare providers were considered to be those such as psychologists or psychotherapists, who would not ordinarily be involved in the patient's care.

The intervention period ranged from four (15·4%) studies reporting singular sessions, to six (23·1%) studies reporting multiple sessions over 12 months. The median (IQ) number of sessions over which interventions were delivered was 5 (3–7.3). The majority of interventions were delivered over a period of 6 months or less, which was the case for 17 studies (65.4%). Intervention exposure as the total number of minutes spent delivering the intervention could be estimated for 16 studies. In the remaining 10 studies, these data were not available. Intervention exposure ranged from 30 min to 8 h and 15 min. The median (IQR) intervention exposure was 175 (118–263) min.

The comparison group was ‘standard care’ for all studies; for 13 studies (50%), standard care involved some form of technique to improve adherence such as education, encouragement or provision of adherence aids and in these studies, recipients of the intervention received further techniques such as MI.

Supplementary figures S1 and S2 show the results of the risk of bias assessment. Only five (19.2%) studies27 36 41 48 49 scored ‘low risk’ in all five bias categories. Nineteen (73.1%) studies were scored as moderate overall risk, because 2–4 of the categories were classed as ‘low risk’. Two (7.7%)40 44 studies were classed as ‘high overall risk’, because only one risk category was scored as low. The most common source of bias was a lack of blinding of the outcome assessment; this is because the measure of adherence was frequently self-report. Self-report measures of adherence are commonly used but subject to patient bias. In the majority of cases, the patients were not blind to their treatment group allocation, and thus use of self-report measures leaves scope for bias.

Meta-analysis

Twenty-six RCTs were pooled to assess the effects of CBCTs on medication adherence. Three studies showed non-significant negative effects on medication adherence, but the remaining 23 studies all showed improvements in medication adherence with receipt of intervention. The effect size calculated for each study is summarised in table 2.

Table 2.

Study outcomes for studies included in meta-analysis

Extracted data
Study Sample size (intervention, control) Adherence definition (assessment measure) Intervention group Control group p Value Effect size (Hedges’ g)
(95% CI)
Bailey et al34 225 (124, 101) Percentage of patients scored as adherent on all 6 items of a self-report scale (based on Morisky's self-reported scale) Mean=91.9 Mean=61.7 0.001 0.44 (0.18 to 0.71)
Berger et al40 367 (172, 195) Percentage of patients discontinuing treatment by study endpoint (patient interview) Mean=98.8 Mean=91.3 0.001 0.35 (0.14 to 0.55)
Brown et al29 69 (36, 33) Percentage of prescribed doses taken over a month (electronic monitoring) Mean (SD)=93.4 (12.3) Mean (SD)=79.1 (28.1) 0.66 (0.18 to 1.14)
DiIorio et al41 17 (8, 9) Mean number of missed medicines in the last 30 days (self-report questionnaire) Mean (SD)=0.13 (0.35) Mean (SD)=0.98 (1.48) 0.73 (−0.21 to 1.67)
DiIorio et al42 213 (107, 106) Percentage of doses taken during the intervention period (electronic monitoring) Mean=64 Mean=55 0.09 0.23 (−0.04 to 0.50)
Farmer et al27 211 (126, 85) Percentage of days during a 12-week period in which medication was taken correctly (electronic monitoring) Mean (SD)=77.4 (26.3) Mean (SD)=64.0 (30.8) 0.04 0.47 (0.20 to 0.75)
George et al30 343 (170, 173) Percentage of participants classed as adherent (Morisky self-report scale) Mean=72.2 Mean=63.8 0.09 0.18 (−0.03 to 0.39)
Golin et al39 117 (59, 58) Percentage of prescribed doses taken in the month prior to the study endpoint (CAS) Mean (SD)=76 (27) Mean (SD)=71 (27) 0.18 (−0.18 to 0.54)
Hovell et al51 188 (92, 96) Cumulative number of doses taken over 9 months (patient interview) Mean (SD)=179.93 (57.01) Mean (SD)=150.98 (73.75) 0.44 (0.15 to 0.72)
Konkle-Parker et al 38 36 (21,15) Percentage of patients taking >90% of their medications in the last 3–4 weeks (prescription refill data) Mean (SD)=0.93 (0.23) Mean (SD)=0.92 (0.27) 0.04 (−0.61 to 0.69)
Maneesriwongul et al37 60 (30, 30) Mean percentage of doses taken over the last 4 weeks (self-report using a visual analogue scale) Mean (SD)=97.1 (3.3) Mean (SD)=89.8 (5.6) 1.55 (0.98 to 2.12)
Murphy et al52 33 (17, 16) Percentage of doses taken during the intervention period (self-report questionnaire) Mean (SD)=0.86 (0.33) Mean (SD)=0.83 (0.36) 0.09 (−0.58 to 0.75)
Ogedegbe et al36 160 (79, 81) Percentage of days during a 2-month period in which medication was taken correctly (electronic monitoring) Mean=56.9 Mean=42.9 0.027 0.35 (0.04 to 0.66)
Pradier et al50 202 (123, 121) Percentage of patients deemed to be adherent (taking 100% of doses; self-report questionnaire) Mean=75 Mean=61 0.04 0.34 (0.02 to 0.65)
Put et al35 23 (12, 11) Frequency of non-adherent behaviour over the last 3 months (self-report questionnaire) Mean (SD)=6.9 (1.2) Mean (SD)=8.1 (3.1) 0.50 (−0.30 to 1.30)
Remien et al49 196 (106, 109) Percentage of doses taken during the previous 2 weeks (electronic monitoring) Mean (SD)=76 (27) Mean (SD)=60 (34) 0.52 (0.25 to 0.79)
Safren et al44 53 (28, 25) Percentage of prescribed doses taken over the last 2 weeks (self-report questionnaire) Mean (SD)=93 (22) Mean (SD)=94 (10) −0.06 (−0.59 to 0.47)
Sheeran and Orbell28 78 (38, 40) Number of once daily doses missed over a 3 week period (self-report questionnaire) Mean=2.68 Mean=4.85 0.05 0.45 (0.00 to 0.89)
Simoni et al48 114 (57, 57) Percentage of doses taken over the last 7 days (electronic monitoring) Mean (SD)=32.3 (42.5) Mean (SD)=29.1 (39.7) 0.08 (−0.29 to 0.44)
Smith et al47 17 (8, 9) Percentage of participants taking ≥80% of their weekly doses (electronic monitoring) OR=7.8 (2.2 to 28.1) 1.08 (0.41 to 1.74)
Solomon et al43 2087 (1046, 1041) Median % medication possession ratio (prescription refill data) Median=49
IQR=7 to 88
Median=41
IQR=2 to 86
0.07 0.08 (−0.01 to 0.17)
Tuldrà et al46 77 (36, 41) Percentage of patients with monthly adherence ≥95% (self-reported number of pills taken) Mean=94 Mean=69 0.008 0.62 (0.16 to 1.07)
van Es et al32 67 (58, 54) Adherence score on a self-report scale based on how often medication was taken (never-always) Mean=7.7 Mean=6.7 0.05 0.48 (0.00 to 0.96)
Wagner et al45 135 (154, 76) Percentage of doses taken during the intervention period (electronic monitoring) Mean=83.5 Mean=86.4 0.57 −0.08 (−0.35 to 0.20)
Weber et al33 53 (29, 24) Percentage of patients with monthly adherence ≥95% (electronic monitoring) Mean=70.8 Mean=50 0.014 0.69 (0.14 to 1.24)
Williams et al31 75 (36, 39) Percentage of doses taken during the intervention period (pill counts Mean=58.4 Mean=66 0.162 −0.32 (−0.77 to 0.13)

Random effects meta-analysis showed evidence that CBCTs are associated with improved medication adherence. Figure 2 shows the forest plot for the 26 studies and exemplifies the tendency towards positive adherence effects with intervention. A pooled estimate of effect size (95% CI; reported as Hedges’ g) of 0.34 (0.23 to 0.46) was calculated when all studies were combined, although heterogeneity was high (I2=68%, 95% CI 52% to 79%).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Forrest plot for studies included in meta-analysis.

The funnel plot produced was indicative of publication bias (as shown in figure 3) and thus further explored using Egger's test, which confirmed statistically significant funnel plot asymmetry (p=0.005). The trim-and-fill technique was used to recompute an effect size which accounted for this asymmetry, yielding a more conservative effect size estimate of 0.21 (0.08 to 0.33; as shown in online supplementary figure S3). This effect size suggests that CBCTs elicit small but statistically significant improvements in medication adherence (p=0.001) relative to standard care. According to data from six studies that used the percentage of prescribed dose taken, the pooled SD of this outcome was 30.7%. Then a standardised mean difference of 0.205 (0.084 to 0.326) will correspond to a difference of 6.3% (2.6% to 10%) between the intervention and the control groups in the percentage of dose taken.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Funnel plot for studies included in meta-analysis.

Subgroup analyses via meta-regression

Table 3 summarises the results of the subgroup analyses to explore variations in effect sizes for the predetermined variables. The regression coefficient is the difference in pooled Hedges’ g between the two subgroups compared. A coefficient >0 indicates that studies in subgroup-A reported greater treatment effects than those in subgroup-B.

Table 3.

Summary of subgroup analyses

Variable Subgroup-A vs subgroup-B Number of studies (number of participants) in each subgroup Coefficient (95% CI) p Value
Intervention setting Hospital vs community 9 (1124) vs 17 (4092) 0.27 (0.01 to 0.54) 0.043
Disease area HIV vs other conditions 14 (1323) vs 12 (3893) 0.05 (−0.23 to 0.33) 0.72
Intervention components MI vs no MI component 11 (3538) vs 15 (1678) −0.17 (−0.44 to 0.09) 0.193
Intervention delivery method Entirely in person vs other methods 15 (1663) vs 11 (3553) −0.03 (−0.31 to 0.25) 0.841
Entirely over the telephone vs other methods 3 (2679) vs 23 (2537) −0.16 (−0.59 to 0.26) 0.442
Both in person and telephone vs other 7 (775) vs 19 (4441) −0.05 (−0.27 to 0.37) 0.744
Intervention delivery personnel Specialist vs Routine HCP 5 (503) vs 12 (1567) −0.01 (−0.46 to 0.26) 0.561
Total intervention exposure ≤3 h vs >3 h 9 (3061) vs 7 (887) 0.07 (−0.35 to 0.50)  0.728
Control group type Explicit active controls vs usual care (no adherence enhancing strategies) 13 (3683) vs 13 (1533) 0.09 (−0.18 to 0.37) 0.493
Risk of bias Outcome assessment blinding vs no outcome assessment blinding 15 (3555) vs 11 (1661) 0.05 (−0.24 to 0.33) 0.736
Outcome measures Objective vs subjective measured outcomes 14 (3850) vs 12 (1366) −0.16 (−0.44 to 0.11) 0.225

MI, Motivational Interviewing.

The classification of studies into subgroups was largely intuitive. However, as a continuous rather than a categorical variable, ‘total intervention exposure’ was less amenable to intuitive dichotomisation. In such instances, it is standard practice to create two subgroups by distributing a roughly equal number of studies to each group. An arbitrary cut-off point of 3 h was therefore used to split the data into two subgroups.

Interventions delivered from hospital settings were associated with greater treatment effects compared with interventions in community or other settings (difference 0.27, 95% CI 0.01 to 0.54, p=0.043). Differences in effect sizes between subgroups were statistically non-significant in all other cases. However, the subgroup analyses may have failed to detect important differences between subgroups because of the small number of studies included.

As the variable ‘intervention exposure’ was a continuous variable, an additional post hoc analysis was undertaken. This allowed the variable to be analysed in its ‘natural’ continuous state rather than in two subgroups. This exploratory analysis was undertaken to ensure that the arbitrary cut-off point of 3 h had not adversely influenced the data. A coefficient value (95% CI) of 0.001 (−0.001 to 0.002) suggested that there was no association between intervention exposure and effect size. A non-significant p value of 0.540 confirmed this and demonstrates results comparable to the subgroup analysis for this variable.

Discussion

Principal findings

Receipt of a cognitive-based behavioural adherence intervention was associated with small but statistically significant improvements in medication adherence. Heterogeneity was high and notable publication bias was identified. However, techniques have been used to account for this bias, resulting in a more conservative summary effect size of 0.21 (95% CI 0.08 to 0.33; p=0.001).

In half of the included studies, the standard care received by the control group explicitly involved some form of ‘adherence enhancing strategy’ such as provision of education, monitoring or review. Such strategies form the mainstay of current medication adherence interventions, and so our research suggests that CBCTs may be able to elicit adherence benefits beyond the techniques used in current practice.

The majority of interventions were complex and multifaceted, and thus subgroup analysis to explore whether this is associated with greater effect could not be undertaken. The subgroup analyses performed revealed that the effect size was greater when interventions were delivered in a hospital setting than in a community setting, but were not influenced by other variables such as the type of CBCT, delivery method and personnel or duration. Further work is necessary to explore the effect of settings on effect size.

Comparison with other studies

In 2003, Peterson et al53 conducted a meta-analysis of educational and behavioural interventions to improve medication adherence in a range of illnesses. The included studies were all RCTs delivered over similar time periods to those included in our study. The educational components and behavioural components such as changes in dosing schedule and reminders examined by Peterson et al closely mirror those utilised in the studies from our meta-analysis which used control groups with ‘active standard care’. Peterson et al reported a correlation coefficient (r) equivalent to a Cohen's d effect size of 0.16 (0.08, 0.24). For our study, the effect size for all studies, when adjusting for publication bias and reported as Hedges’ g, was 0.20 (0.08, 0.33). This suggests that inclusion of CBCTs strengthens the adherence improvements gained, if only marginally. Moreover, Peterson et al report publication bias observed from a funnel plot of their included studies, but have not made allowances for this bias via recomputed effect sizes. Their Cohen's d value of 0.16 is quite likely exaggerated by the noted publication bias and thus implies that the true difference in effect size between the two meta-analyses may be greater.

An effect size (Hedges’ g) of 0.25 (95% CI 0.07 to 0.42) for studies using MI was calculated, compared with an effect size of 0.41 (95% CI 0.278 to 0.541) for non-MI interventions. After adjusting for bias, the estimated Hedges’ g was 0.137 (95% CI −0.067 to 0.341) for studies using MI and 0.356 (95% CI 0.223 to 0.489) for studies using non-MI interventions. These estimated effect sizes closely match the effect size calculated when MI is used as a behavioural intervention in other healthcare domains14 and thus represent novel evidence for the wider application of MI techniques beyond the treatment of substance abuse and gambling. The overlapping CIs of the effect sizes calculated for MI-based and non-MI based interventions suggest that MI-based interventions are unlikely to be superior in their efficacy compared with those based on other CBCTs.

Strengths and weaknesses of our work

This study represents the first meta-analysis of MI and other CBCTs as medication adherence interventions and has been undertaken with methodological rigour and in accordance with published guidance.18 A notable strength of this work is the robust methodological techniques that have been applied to provide an estimate of effect size which accounts for publication biases, and thus greater confidence can be placed in the estimate. The work is also strengthened by restriction to RCTs.

While moderate agreement in abstract screening may be lower than ideal, this is largely attributable to the paucity of detail reported in abstracts and complexities in intervention definitions, which are known to be problematic in this domain.11–13 The conservative approach to abstract screening prevented study exclusion if disagreement was associated with insufficient information and thus prevented exclusion in error. Heterogeneity between the included studies was high with an I2 value of 68% (95% CI 52% to 79%) and thus raises the question as to whether the studies were sufficiently comparable to warrant pooling in a meta-analysis. While we defined our inclusion criteria to ensure that studies were as similar as possible (ie, all using a CBCT), heterogeneity was expected as other factors such as the populations and disease states studied were more difficult to control for. Interestingly, the largest study had a small standardised group difference compared with most of the other studies which contributed substantially to the heterogeneity.43 Aside from these between-study differences, the actual interventions were variable, as were the definitions of adherence and assessment tools used.

The differences between subgroups were statistically non-significant in terms of disease area, intervention components, delivery methods, delivery personnel, intensity, usual care and risk of bias. However, the statistical power was limited by the small number of studies included in the subgroup analyses. The analyses may therefore have failed to detect some important subgroup differences. Moreover, for variables such as the intervention exposure, meaningful conclusions are difficult to draw. While the analyses infer that intervention exposure did not influence effect size, it is important to remember that a whole host of variables are at large. It is possible that briefer interventions used different techniques or were delivered to different types of recipients compared with longer interventions, and therefore comparisons may not be wholly meaningful. Further work may be necessary to explore whether otherwise identical interventions (same technique, same population, same delivery personnel and so forth) differ in effect size when delivered with different exposure.

Despite these numerous between-study differences, the core of each intervention was the use of a CBCT to improve medication adherence, which was comparable across all studies, and thus we would argue that data pooling irrespective of heterogeneity was both intuitive and meaningful.

We have established that receipt of a cognitive-based behavioural medication adherence intervention is likely to elicit small improvements in medication adherence, but the clinical relevance and impact of this improvement remain unknown. Based on the mean adherence rates in the control groups, mean SDs and the effect size calculated, it has been possible to estimate the increase in percentage of doses taken for the intervention groups. Based on the adjusted Hedges’ g value of 0.205 (0.084 to 0.326), receipt of a CBCT improved adherence (% of doses taken) by 6.29% (2.58% to 10%). For some medications, a 6% increase in the percentage of doses taken may not be of clinical relevance. However, for other medications such as antiretroviral therapy for HIV, which requires very high levels of adherence or antiepileptic therapies with narrow therapeutic windows, a 6% increase in adherence may have notable clinical relevance. While many included studies provided data on clinical outcomes, pooling of these data from a diverse range of studies was not possible.

Implications

Motivational and CBCTs can seemingly be delivered effectively by routine healthcare professionals, with efficacy applicable to a range of diseases. Efficacy was not related to intervention exposure. Interestingly, the results also suggest that these interventions can be delivered via telephone or face-to-face with comparable efficacy. These are valuable traits for an adherence intervention which could be adaptable to a wide range of settings and amenable to tailoring to meet individual needs.

The flexibility and adaptability of these techniques coupled with their frequent simplicity means that practitioners may wish to consider incorporation of these techniques into their consultations when faced with the need to facilitate medication-related behaviour changes.

Recommendations and conclusions

Further investigation of these techniques as medication adherence interventions is warranted in order to further elucidate the characteristics most strongly associated with efficacy. Studies to determine patient and healthcare practitioner acceptability of these techniques are also necessary to establish their role in routine healthcare.

Supplementary Material

Author's manuscript
Reviewer comments

Acknowledgments

CE would like to acknowledge Estelle Payerne for her invaluable contributions as the second reviewer for abstract screening and data extraction. CE would also like to acknowledge David Wright and John Wood for their comments on the protocol design, data analysis and methods of dissemination, and Steven Watson for his ongoing technical support in using the meta-analysis software and comments regarding data interpretation.

Footnotes

Contributors: DB and CE were responsible for the overall study co-ordination. CE was responsible for the study conception and protocol design, under the supervision of DB with contributions from FS. All literature searching, abstract screening, study selection and data extraction was undertaken independently by CE and EP with referral to DB as a third reviewer as necessary. Assessment of methodological quality was also undertaken by CE and EP. CE was responsible for all data analysis with guidance from DB and FS. Statistical tests, asymmetry tests and trim and fill methods were undertaken by FS. CE wrote the first draft of the paper with guidance from DB and advice from FS. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding: This research has been supported by a PhD studentship awarded by the Dean of Science at the University of East Anglia.

Competing interests: None.

Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Data sharing statement: No additional data are available.

References

  • 1.World Health Organisation Adherence to long term therapies: evidence for action. Geneva: World Health Organisation, 2003 [Google Scholar]
  • 2.The National Collaborating Centre for Primary Care The National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE) clinical guideline 76, full guidance. Medicines adherence; involving patients in decisions about prescribed medicines and supporting adherence. London: The National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Simpson SH, Eurich DT, Majumdar SR. A meta-analysis of the association between adherence to drug therapy and mortality. BMJ 2006;333:15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Haynes RB, Ackloo E, Sahota N, et al. Interventions for enhancing medication adherence. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2008;2:CD000011. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD000011.pub3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Clifford S, Garfield S, Eliasson L, et al. Medication adherence and community pharmacy: a review of education, policy and research in England. Pharm Pract (Internet) 2010;8:77–88 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Horne R, Weinman J, Barber N, et al. Concordance, adherence and compliance in medicine taking. London: National Co-ordinating Centre for NHS Service Delivery and Organisation Research & Development; 2005, Report for the National Co-ordinating Centre for NHS Service Delivery and Organisation R & D (NCCSDO) [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Britt E, Hudson SM, Blampied NM. Motivational interviewing in health settings: a review. Patient Educ Counsel 2004;53:147–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Miller WR, Rollnick SR. Motivational interviewing: preparing people to change behaviour. New York: Guilford Press, 1991 [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing: preparing people for change. The Guilford Press, 2002 [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dunn C, Deroo L, Rivara FP. The use of brief interventions adapted from motivational interviewing across behavioral domains: a systematic review. Addiction 2001;96:1725–42 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Knight KM, McGowan L, Dickens C, et al. A systematic review of motivational interviewing in physical health care settings. Br J Health Psychol 2006;11:319–32 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Burke BL, Arkowitz H, Menchola M. The efficacy of motivational interviewing: a meta-analysis of controlled clinical trials. J Consult Clin Psychol 2003;71:843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rubak S, Sandbæk A, Lauritzen T, et al. Motivational interviewing: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Gen Pract 2005;55:305. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hettema J, Steele J, Miller WR. Motivational interviewing. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2005;1:91–111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lundahl B, Burke BL. The effectiveness and applicability of motivational interviewing: a practice friendly review of four meta analyses. J Clin Psychol 2009;65:1232–45 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lundahl BW, Kunz C, Brownell C, et al. A meta-analysis of motivational interviewing: twenty-five years of empirical studies. Res Soc Work Pract 2010;20:137 [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Rollnick S, Allison J, Ballasiotes S, et al. Variations on a theme: motivational interviewing and its adaptations. Motivational interviewing: preparing people for change. 2nd edition. New York, 2002:270–83 [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Higgins JPT, Green S, eds. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from www.cochrane-handbook.org (accessed 4 Jul 2011)
  • 19.Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ 2009;339:332–6 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 1977:159–74 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Higgins J, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta analysis. Stat Med 2002;21:1539–58 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Higgins J, Thompson S, Deeks J, et al. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analysis. BMJ 2003;327:557–60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, et al. Introduction to meta-analysis. Wiley Online Library, 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, et al. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997;315:629–34 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Duval S, Tweedie R. A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. J Am Stat Assoc 2000;95:89–98 [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 2000;56:455–63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Farmer A, Hardeman W, Hughes D, et al. An explanatory randomised controlled trial of a nurse-led, consultation-based intervention to support patients with adherence to taking glucose lowering medication for type 2 diabetes. BMC Fam Pract 2012;13:30–8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Sheeran P, Orbell S. Implementation intentions and repeated behaviour: augmenting the predictive validity of the theory of planned behaviour. Eur J Soc Psychol 1999;29:349–69 [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Brown I, Sheeran P, Reuber M. Enhancing antiepileptic drug adherence: a randomized controlled trial. Epilepsy Behav 2009;16:634–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.George J, McNamara K, Jackson S, et al. The HAPPY trial: a randomised controlled trial of a community pharmacy-based intervention for improving patient adherence to antihypertensive medicines. Int J Pharm Pract 2010;Conference: 2010 Royal Pharmaceutical Society Conference London United Kingdom. Conference Start: 20100905 Conference End: 20100906. Conference Publication: (var.pagings) 18:22–3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Williams A, Manias E, Walker R, et al. A multifactorial intervention to improve blood pressure control in co-existing diabetes and kidney disease: a feasibility randomized controlled trial. J Adv Nur 2012;68:2515–25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.van Es SM, Nagelkerke AF, Colland VT, et al. An intervention programme using the ASE-model aimed at enhancing adherence in adolescents with asthma. Patient Educ Couns 2001;44:193–203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Weber R, Christen L, Christen S, et al. Effect of individual cognitive behaviour intervention on adherence to antiretroviral therapy: prospective randomized trial. Antivir Ther 2004;9:85–96 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bailey WC, Richards JM, Jr, Brooks CM, et al. A randomized trial to improve self-management practices of adults with asthma. Arch Intern Med 1990;150:1664–8 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Put C, van den Bergh O, Lemaigre V, et al. Evaluation of an individualised asthma programme directed at behavioural change. Eur Respir J 2003;21:109–15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ogedegbe G, Chaplin W, Schoenthaler A, et al. A practice-based trial of motivational interviewing and adherence in hypertensive African Americans. Am J Hypertens 2008;21:1137–43 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Maneesriwongul W, Prajanket O-O, Saengcharnchai P. Effects of motivational interviewing or an educational video on knowledge about HIV/AIDS, health beliefs and antiretroviral medication adherence among adult Thais with HIV/AIDS. Pac Rim Int J Nurs Res 2012;16:124–37 [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Konkle-Parker DJ, Erlen JA, Dubbert PM, et al. Pilot testing of an HIV medication adherence intervention in a public clinic in the Deep South. J Am Acad Nurse Pract 2012;24:488–98 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Golin CE, Earp J, Tien H-C, et al. A 2-arm, randomized, controlled trial of a motivational interviewing-based intervention to improve adherence to antiretroviral therapy (ART) among patients failing or initiating ART. JAIDS 2006;42:42–51 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Berger BA, Liang H, Hudmon KS. Evaluation of software-based telephone counseling to enhance medication persistency among patients with multiple sclerosis. J Am Pharm Assoc 2005;45:466–72 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.DiIorio C, Resnicow K, McDonnell M, et al. Using motivational interviewing to promote adherence to antiretroviral medications: a pilot study. J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care 2003;14:52–62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.DiIorio C, McCarty F, Resnicow K, et al. Using motivational interviewing to promote adherence to antiretroviral medications: a randomized controlled study. AIDS Care 2008;20:273–83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Solomon DH, Iversen MD, Avorn J, et al. Osteoporosis telephonic intervention to improve medication regimen adherence: a large, pragmatic, randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med 2012;172:477–83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Safren SA, Otto MW, Worth JL, et al. Two strategies to increase adherence to HIV antiretroviral medication: life-steps and medication monitoring. Behav Res Ther 2001;39:1151–62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wagner GJ, Kanouse DE, Golinelli D, et al. Cognitive-behavioral intervention to enhance adherence to antiretroviral therapy: a randomized controlled trial (CCTG 578). AIDS 2006;20:1295–302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tuldrà A, Fumaz CR, Ferrer MJ, et al. Prospective randomized two-arm controlled study to determine the efficacy of a specific intervention to improve long-term adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy. JAIDS 2000;25:221–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Smith SR, Rublein JC, Marcus C, et al. A medication self-management program to improve adherence to HIV therapy regimens. Patient Educ Couns 2003;50:187–99 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Simoni JM, Huh D, Frick PA, et al. Peer support and pager messaging to promote antiretroviral modifying therapy in Seattle: a randomized controlled trial. JAIDS 2009;52:465–73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Remien RH, Stirratt MJ, Dolezal C, et al. Couple-focused support to improve HIV medication adherence: a randomized controlled trial. AIDS 2005;19:807–14 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Pradier C, Bentz L, Spire B, et al. Efficacy of an educational and counseling intervention on adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy: French prospective controlled study. HIV Clin Trials 2003;4:121–31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hovell MF, Sipan CL, Blumberg EJ, et al. Increasing Latino adolescents’ adherence to treatment for latent tuberculosis infection: a controlled trial. Am J Public Health 2003;93:1871–7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Murphy DA, Lu MC, Martin D, et al. Results of a pilot intervention trial to improve antiretroviral adherence among HIV-positive patients. J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care 2002;13:57–69 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Peterson AM, Takiya L, Finley R. Meta-analysis of trials of interventions to improve medication adherence. Am J Health Syst Pharm 2003;60:657–65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Author's manuscript
Reviewer comments

Articles from BMJ Open are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES