Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2013 Jul 24;110(32):13168–13173. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301696110

Word learning is mediated by the left arcuate fasciculus

Diana López-Barroso a,b,1, Marco Catani c, Pablo Ripollés a,b, Flavio Dell'Acqua c,d,e, Antoni Rodríguez-Fornells a,b,f, Ruth de Diego-Balaguer a,b,f
PMCID: PMC3740909  PMID: 23884655

Abstract

Human language requires constant learning of new words, leading to the acquisition of an average vocabulary of more than 30,000 words in adult life. The ability to learn new words is highly variable and may rely on the integration between auditory and motor information. Here, we combined diffusion imaging tractography and functional MRI to study whether the strength of anatomical and functional connectivity between auditory and motor language networks is associated with word learning ability. Our results showed that performance in word learning correlates with microstructural properties and strength of functional connectivity of the direct connections between Broca’s and Wernicke’s territories in the left hemisphere. This study suggests that our ability to learn new words relies on an efficient and fast communication between temporal and frontal areas. The absence of these connections in other animals may explain the unique ability of learning words in humans.

Keywords: white matter, dorsal stream, ventral stream, diffusion tensor imaging


Language is a unique human ability that has been suggested to depend on the evolution of direct connections between the temporal and frontal cortex for the integration of auditory and motor representation of words (1). Auditory-motor integration, involving the mapping of sound into articulation, has been proposed to be important not only for auditory perception (2) and speech processing (3, 4), but also for learning new words (1, 3, 5). The auditory-motor integration theory of speech is based in part on the observation that patients with lesions affecting the arcuate fasciculus (AF) connecting temporal and frontal cortices, are impaired not only in phonological and word repetition but also in verbal short-term memory tasks (6, 7). Moreover, stimulation of the premotor cortex with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation disrupts the capacity to discriminate between phonemes (8). Similarly, learning new words is impaired if participants are engaged in the articulation of irrelevant sounds while they try to learn new ones (9). These studies are supportive of a link between auditory and motor processes in word learning.

Auditory and motor areas communicate directly through the AF, a pathway that shows a progressively high degree of complexity along the phylogenetic scale (1013). Axonal tracing studies in monkey have shown that the AF connects to more dorsal regions of the temporo-parietal cortex (11, 14), whereas diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies in humans show a greater level of connectivity to auditory regions of the temporal lobe (10, 12). On the basis of these anatomical differences between human and other species, it has been suggested that the evolutionary expansion of auditory-motor connections allowed humans to develop a system for auditory working memory critical for learning complex phonological sequences (1, 15). There is, however, no experimental evidence so far that links the anatomy of the AF with word learning ability.

In the present study, we aimed to seek direct evidence of the role of the AF in auditory-motor integration and word learning. For that purpose, we explored the pattern of structural and functional connectivity between auditory and motor areas in both hemispheres as assessed by DTI-tractography and functional MRI (fMRI) as participants were learning words from fluent speech (Fig. 1A).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Task and methods used in the study. (A) Schematic illustration of the word learning task consisting of a learning phase and a recognition test. The learning phase consisted in the auditory presentation of an artificial language stream composed by trisyllabic words separated by brief pauses. The test phase consisted in the presentation of isolated words (“words” or “nonwords”) and participants were required to recognize each one as correct or incorrect based in the previously heard artificial language. (B) Example of tractography reconstruction of language pathways in the left hemisphere for one of the subjects rendered onto the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template.

In our study we have used a recently described model of the AF (10, 16) in which communication between temporal and frontal language areas is mediated by two parallel networks: a direct pathway (i.e., the long segment of the AF), which connects the posterior part of both superior [Brodmann area (BA) 22] and middle (BA 37) temporal gyrus (Wernicke’s territory) with the inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44 and 45), the middle frontal gyrus (BA 46) and the premotor cortex (BA 6) (Broca’s territory); an indirect pathway composed of an anterior segment connecting Broca’s territory with the inferior parietal cortex (BA 39 and 40) (Geschwind’s territory) and a posterior segment connecting Geschwind’s with Wernicke’s territories (Fig. 1B). Our primary hypothesis was that word learning ability correlates with structural and functional connectivity of tracts directly connecting auditory and motor areas (i.e., long segment). Alternatively, previous evidence suggests that a ventral pathway running through the extreme capsule also connects the language areas (1719). This path connects the inferior frontal gyrus (BA 45/47) with the superior temporal gyrus (BA 22), inferior parietal (BA 39) and occipital cortices (BA 17, 18, and 19) (12, 18, 20), and it has been claimed to be important for spoken-word recognition (21). During auditory word learning, the ventral pathway may allow the categorization of words as familiar once they have been learned, a point that could be also crucial for successful learning. This pathway corresponds to the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), which was dissected bilaterally (Fig. 1B and Fig. S1).

Results

Virtual dissections of the AF and the IFOF were performed in 21 right-handed healthy subjects (mean age = 25.6 ± 3.9 y, nine females) and surrogate measures of volume (i.e., number of streamlines) and microstructural properties of fibers possibly related to the degree of myelination, axonal architecture, and diameter [i.e., fractional anisotropy (FA) and radial diffusivity (RD)] were measured along the reconstructed streamlines (22) (see Materials and Methods). The analysis was performed separately for the three segments of the AF (Fig. 1B and SI Materials and Methods) and the IFOF (Fig. 1B, Fig. S1, and SI Materials and Methods) in both left and right hemispheres.

The word learning task involved two phases (Fig. 1A and Materials and Methods). In the learning phase participants were asked to memorize artificially created words composed of three syllables presented in the form of a fluent speech stream (23). The words were novel to the participants and had no semantic content. The learning task was performed in the MRI scan during the fMRI experiment. Immediately after the learning phase, participants were behaviorally tested and asked to recognize auditory presented words (recognition phase) (see Materials and Methods for further details). Word learning performances were measured using hit rates taking into account false alarms, calculating the d-prime (d′) index (24), which reflects individual’s ability to discriminate words presented in the artificial language from nonpresented words (nonwords) (SI Materials and Methods). One-sample t test against d′ = 0 (no discrimination) showed significant learning: t(24) = 2.74, P < 0.01.

Relationship Between Word Learning Performance and Tract Properties of the AF and IFOF.

After Bonferroni’s correction for multiple comparisons, a statistically significant negative correlation was found between word learning performance and radial diffusivity (r = −0.6; P < 0.005) in the left long segment (Fig. 2 and Table S1). Correlations with the anterior and posterior segments were not statistically significant (Fig. 2 and Table S1). There were no significant correlations with all three segments in the right hemisphere (Fig. 2 and Table S1). These findings suggest that microstructural properties of the dorsal direct connections between temporal and frontal language regions in the left hemisphere correlate with individual abilities to learn new words. In addition, a lateralization index was calculated by counting the number of reconstructed streamlines within each segment of the AF for each hemisphere (Materials and Methods, SI Materials and Methods, and Fig. S2). In agreement with previous reports (16), the long segment was left lateralized, the anterior segment right lateralized, and the posterior segment symmetrically distributed (Fig. S2 and Table S2). No significant correlation was observed between the lateralization of each segment of the AF and learning performance (Table S2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Link between the microstructure of the AF and word learning performance. The scatter plots show the correlations between word learning scores (d′) and RD in the long, anterior, and posterior segments of the AF for the left and right hemispheres. The index of correlation and the P value are provided on each plot box. An asterisk indicates the significant correlation (P < 0.0062). Nonsignificant correlations are marked as “n.s.” d′ = 0 designates no learning. Positive values reflect that participants discriminate words and nonwords accurately. Negative values indicate discrimination is achieved but individuals segmented incorrectly, classifying nonwords as words of the artificial language.

No statistically significant correlations were found between any of the DTI-derived measures and word learning scores for the IFOF of either hemisphere (Table S3). Analysis of the lateralization indices showed a symmetrical distribution for the number of streamlines of the IFOF and a left lateralization of its FA (Fig. S2 and Table S2). No significant correlation was found between the lateralization index of the IFOF and learning performance (Table S2).

Relationship Between Word Learning Performance and Functional Connectivity.

Functional connectivity analysis was performed using the temporal correlation of the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response between the three regions of the perisylvian network connected by the direct and indirect segments of the AF on both hemispheres (Materials and Methods). Results showed that individual variations in the strength of the functional connectivity between the left temporal (Wernicke’s territory) and frontal (Broca’s territory) areas correlated with word learning performance (r = 0.42, P < 0.036) (Fig. 3A). The strength of connectivity between right frontal and temporal regions or between the regions connected by the indirect segments in both hemispheres was not significantly correlated with performance (all P > 0.1). In addition, laterality analysis of functional connectivity between regions connected by each AF segment was also calculated (Materials and Methods and SI Materials and Methods). There was no significant correlation between the functional lateralization indices and word learning performance (all P > 0.09) (Table S4).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

fMRI analysis. (A) Scatter plot showing the correlation between word learning (d′) and the amount of functional connectivity between Broca’s and Wernicke’s territories in the left hemisphere during the word-learning task. Correlation index and the P value are provided on the plot box. The averaged functional connectivity (0.57) was significantly different from zero (P < 0.001). (B) Language versus baseline contrast in sagittal views superimposed on the MNI template (P < 0.05 FDR-corrected). Only activations in the left hemisphere are shown in the figure. See Table S5 for peak activation coordinates.

Relationship Between Structural and Functional Connectivity.

Tract-specific measurements of single segments and strength of the functional connectivity between perisylvian areas were used to analyze possible correlations between anatomical and functional connectivity. We found no significant correlations between diffusion parameters and measures of functional connectivity (all P > 0.1).

Discussion

The present study combines tractography and fMRI in direct support of the role of the AF in word learning. Our main results suggest that both structural and functional measures of connectivity between temporal and frontal language territories in the left hemisphere predict word learning abilities. The fact that correlations for the indirect pathways were not significant suggests that the direct connections are crucial for auditory-motor integration in word learning. The direct connections of the AF could mediate fast interaction between auditory and motor areas in the left hemisphere, thus facilitating those feed-forward and feed-back exchanges of information that are required to phoneme categorization (25) as well as to create the motor codes of the new phonological sequences (4, 5). Moreover, this sensory-motor circuit may provide the substrate for articulatory-based processes that allow keeping information active during working memory (26, 27).

In addition, word learning did not correlate with any of the microstructural measures of the ventral IFOF, indicating that auditory-motor integration through the dorsal pathway is a critical function at these earliest stages of language learning. The ventral pathway may nevertheless play a greater role at later stages of learning, when auditory representations of learned words are consolidated by taking into account invariance properties after multiple encounters (28), as well as once conceptual representations have been attached to the new words learned (29). Furthermore it is possible that word learning through the ventral pathway could be mediated by other tracts that were not included in our analysis (e.g., the uncinate fasciculus) (30).

A basic aspect of human language is its left-side lateralization (31). In our study, the correlation between language learning and the direct segment of the AF was exclusively found in the left hemisphere. This left lateralized relation was not relative to the characteristics of the right AF because lateralization indexes showed no correlation with word learning performance. Previous work found correlations between verbal memory for known words and the AF bilaterally (16). Compared with our study, the previous study used a task that required a semantic recall strategy for real words, which is known to be associated with bilateral activation of fronto-temporal networks (32). In contrast, no semantic component was present in our task, which was designed to have a purely phonological component and therefore to test a more simple auditory-motor integration strategy.

The present findings add unique insights into the relationship between brain morphology and language proficiency, previously explored by neuroimaging studies looking at the anatomy of single cortical regions. For example, the gray matter density of Broca’s area has been found to correlate with the level of language proficiency (33) and the number of years of phonetic training in expert phoneticians (34). Similarly, white matter density in left parietal regions (35, 36) and the superior temporal gyrus (36) predicts speech sound learning. Interestingly, a recent study has shown that changes in the cortical thickness of Broca’s area and left superior temporal gyrus can occur even only after 3 mo of intensive language training (37). On the other hand, the aforementioned findings, together with our results, contrast with retrospective studies showing an association between gray matter density in the posterior parietal cortex and vocabulary size of the first (38) and second language (39). These findings suggest that learning and storing new words rely on different anatomical structures, the former on temporal-frontal regions, the latter on inferior parietal areas.

The correlation we found with structural measures of connectivity suggests that radial diffusivity is a sensitive index of white matter microstructural features underlying efficient cognitive processing. Several factors, like axon diameter, fiber packing density, number of axons, fiber cohesiveness, and myelin thickness may contribute to radial diffusivity signal (22). Importantly, thicker myelin and larger diameter axons are correlated with increased conduction of action potentials through long association fibers (40). A decrease in radial diffusivity [i.e., the average of the diffusivities along the two minor axes of the tensor model (41)], is likely to reflect increased myelination or increased axonal diameter (42). Hence, the correlation reported in our study suggests that a possible explanation for the interindividual variability in word learning is a result of differences in the anatomy of direct connections between left auditory and motor areas. One could speculate that fibers with larger axons or greater myelination facilitate faster conduction of impulses, leading to better synchronization and information transfer between distant regions.

In addition, our results show that functional connectivity is an independent predictor of word learning performance. Several studies have focused on the physiological processes underpinning the fluctuations that are commonly observed in the BOLD response as measured by fMRI (4345). The BOLD signal is sensitive to changes in the oxygenation of the blood in a given area, which is associated to its increased neuronal activation (46). In relation to neural activity, BOLD response fluctuations reflect more the input and local processing of an area rather than its output (i.e., spiking activity) (45). Conversely, DTI is more sensitive to differences in white matter microstructure, such as axonal density, myelination, or fiber diameter (22). These microstructural properties of fibers are likely to facilitate a faster signal conduction but they may not be related to integrative processes performed at the cortical level (47, 48). Indeed, our results suggest that, although both functional and anatomical measures of connectivity are correlated to language-learning performances, a correlation between them should not be assumed.

Finally, the AF, taken as a possible anatomical substrate of auditory-motor integration, might represent a key step for language development. Comparative studies indicate that apes and monkeys have homologs for Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas in terms of gray matter (49, 50) connected by a dorsal and a ventral pathway (49). However, the trajectory of the AF is different between species. Middle and inferior temporal gyri terminations from the inferior frontal gyrus are more prominent in humans than in macaques and chimpanzees (12, 13, 49). In addition, the AF appears to show a greater modification in human evolution than the ventral fiber system of the extreme capsule (51). Indeed, although humans and chimpanzees show a dominant connection between Broca’s and Wernicke’s territories through the AF, macaques show a predominant connection through the ventral pathway (13). This latter path, involved in auditory object identification, is the one processing calls and vocalizations in these lower species (51). Interestingly, monkeys, which have a smaller AF compared to humans (11, 14), are able to perform complex tactile and visual memory tests but these skills are lost in the auditory modality (52). These findings suggest that the ability to integrate auditory-motor information confers some advantages in the manipulation of the representation of acoustic stimuli, thus allowing its storage in the long-term memory.

Ontogenetically, the AF develops slower than other associative pathways, including the ventral pathway (19). The AF’s terminations connecting with Broca’s area show progressive development during childhood, still under development at the age of 7 y (53, 54). However, the connections with premotor cortex, those responsible to auditory-motor integration, can be tracked in newborns (19, 54), suggesting a role in early language acquisition. Very early prelinguistic damage to the AF (55) induces delayed expressive language development and residual language difficulties remain even when within-normal levels of expressive language are reached later on through the available connectivity of the ventral pathway. Similar lesions after the age of 5 y (56) lead to preserved oral language, although with poor performance in nonword repetition, verbal working memory, and reading. In healthy adults, the use of articulatory suppression to block the use of the dorsal pathway interferes with word learning as well, and performance under this condition correlates with individual differences in the microstructural properties of the ventral pathway (9). Taken together, the above studies indicate that lesions to the AF induce language-learning difficulties. Even when the ventral system can compensate for language learning, optimal performance cannot be reached after complete damage of the AF.

It is noteworthy that despite the differences in terminology, each of the three segments composing the AF in the model adopted in the present study have some correspondence to what has been previously described in the monkey brain using axonal tracing (11, 12). The anterior segment corresponds to the third branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF III); the long segment corresponds to the arcuate sensu strictu; and the posterior segment includes fibres of the middle longitudinal fasciculus. In this study we have used DTI instead of more advanced methods, such as high angular resolution diffusion imaging or diffusion spectrum imaging. These methods lead to greater number of false-positive streamlines in the AF reconstruction that need to be anatomically validated (57). Thus, for AF, tractography based on DTI still remains a more conservative and reliable method (for a more in depth critique, see ref. 55, 58).

In conclusion, the present results support the idea that the direct segment of the AF is crucial for word learning, most probably because of its relevance to auditory-motor integration. This study also sheds light on the origin of the large individual variability observed when learning a new language and gives support to the idea that anatomical brain connectivity constrains the nature of the information processed across brain regions (59). The differences between humans and nonhuman primates in language learning might be related to the evolution of the AF, a pathway that has been shown to be structurally different in nonhuman primates, and that facilitates processing and manipulation of complex auditory information in humans.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Twenty-seven adult participants (mean age = 24.7 ± 4.6, 12 women) were recruited for the experiment. Participants were native Spanish speakers with no history of auditory problems. All participants in this study were classified as right-handed after completing the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (mean 80.4 ± 20.4) (60). The ethical committee of the University of Barcelona approved the protocol and written consent was obtained from all participants. Participants were paid for their participation. Six participants were removed for the tractography analysis because of MRI acquisition problems and two participants were removed from the behavioral correlations because no behavioral data were obtained from them. The final sample resulted in 20 subjects for the behavioral-tractography correlations, 25 for the behavioral-functional connectivity correlation, and 21 for the tractography-functional correlations.

Language Learning Task Design.

An artificial language-learning task was administered during fMRI scanning (Fig. 1A) through two runs. Eight different languages were built with each participant listening to two different languages. Languages were counterbalanced between subjects and were built by creating words composed of three syllables. Words were synthesized using the MBROLA speech synthesizer software (61) concatenating diphones at 16 kHz from the Spanish male database (http://tcts.fpms.ac.be/synthesis/). Languages were composed by nine different novel words built following Spanish phonotactic constrains. Each word had a 696-ms duration and brief pauses of 25 ms were inserted between them to introduce a prosodic cue marking words boundaries (62). Words were presented in pseudorandom order (i.e., the same word was not repeated in succession) and in the form of a fluent speech stream. The material was similar to the one used in de Diego-Balaguer et al. (23).

The task involved a learning phase and a test phase (Fig. 1A). The learning phase consisted of four active blocks lasting 30 s, and included 42 word presentations each. Active blocks were alternated with resting blocks of 20 s duration where participants were asked to simply fixate their gaze on a cross that remained in the middle of the screen.

Participants were told that they would hear a nonsense language and that their task was to pay attention to it because they would be asked to recognize words of this language after listening. Immediately after the learning phase, participants were behaviorally tested (recognition phase). Isolated words were presented auditorily and participants were required to judge whether each word had appeared in the previously learned language stream. Test items could be previously presented words, or nonwords formed with the same three syllables but in an incorrect order (Fig. 1A). Each test item appeared three times in each recognition phase. Responses were recorded using an MR-compatible response box containing two response keys (forefinger and middle-finger). Participants were required to press the left or right button, respectively, to judge whether the item was a word presented before or not. The experiment was run using the Presentation Software (www.neurobs.com). Stimuli were played through MR-compatible headphones.

MRI Acquisition.

Images were acquired on a 3T MRI scanner (Siemens Magnetom Trio) with 40-mT/m gradients, using an acquisition sequence fully optimized for DT-MRI of white matter, with the following parameters: voxel size of 2.0 × 2.0 × 2.0 mm, matrix 128 × 128, 64 slices with 2-mm-thick and no gap, NEX 1, TE 88 ms, b-value 1,000 s/mm2, eight runs of 12 diffusion-weighted directions and seven nondiffusion-weighted volumes, using a spin-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence coverage of the whole head. DTI images were acquired before the fMRI sequence. High-resolution structural images [T1-weighted sequence: slice thickness = 1 mm; no gap; number of slices = 240; repetition time (TR) = 2,300 ms; echo time (TE) = 3 ms; matrix = 256 × 256; field-of-view (FOV) = 244 mm] were also acquired. Subsequently, functional images were obtained by using a single-shot T2*-weighted gradient-echo EPI sequence (slice thickness = 4 mm; no gap; number of slices = 32, order of acquisition interleaved; TR = 2,000 ms; TE = 29 ms; flip angle = 80°; matrix = 128 × 128; FOV = 240 mm).

DTI-MRI Tractography.

Preprocessing of DTI data.

Diffusion data were processed using Explore DTI (www.exploredti.com). Data were first preprocessed for eddy current distortions and head motion. For each subject the b-matrix was then reoriented to provide a more accurate estimate of diffusion tensor orientations (63). Remaining artifacts caused by subject motion and cardiac pulsation were excluded from the analysis using RESTORE (64) to reject and correct outliers from diffusion-weighted imaging data. Diffusion tensor was estimated using a nonlinear least-squares approach (65) and FA and RD maps were calculated. Whole-brain tractography was performed using a b-spline interpolation of the diffusion tensor field and Euler integration to propagate streamlines following the directions of the principal eigenvector with a step size of 0.5 mm (66). Tractography was started in all brain voxels with FA > 0.2. Tractography was stopped where FA < 0.2 or when the angle between two consecutive tractography steps was larger than 35°. This whole-brain approach ensures that tractography reconstruction is less dependent on the regions of interest (ROI) delineation. Finally, tractography data and diffusion tensor maps were exported into Trackvis (67) for manual dissection of the tracts.

Tractography dissections.

Virtual dissections of the AF and the IFOF were carried out as described in previous studies (10, 16, 68) (see SI Materials and Methods for further details). Dissections were performed for each subject in the native space and in both hemispheres. The color fiber-orientation maps were resliced in axial, coronal, and sagittal planes and displayed in conjunction with tractography results to allow an approximation to the neuroanatomical location of the tract reconstructions. Because brain regions may have significant interindividual differences, ROIs were manually defined (i.e., the dissector placed the ROI according to individual anatomical landmarks, rather than coordinates or atlas-based constraints that may not apply to each single subject). The approach does not constrain tracts to start and end within the defined regions (SI Materials and Methods).

Tracts-specific measurements.

The number of streamlines (Ns), FA, and RD were extracted and averaged along the entire segmented tract (22). Ns is considered as a surrogate measure of tract volume; FA is considered as a measure linked to axonal architecture and myelination; RD describes miscroscopic water movements perpendicular to the fibers (69) and has been postulated to reflect myelin content along the axon. Demyelination (42) and severe tissue injury (70) have been associated with increased RD.

Statistical analysis.

The statistical analysis was performed using the SPSS software. Pearson’s correlation analysis was performed between the learning performance (d′ index) and the DTI measures (Ns, FA, RD) along each segment (long, anterior, posterior) of the AF as well as for the IFOF pathway from both hemispheres (Fig. 1B). Correlations were considered significant at P < 0.0062 after Bonferroni’s correction for multiple comparisons. Values that were greater than two SDs from the mean were considered outliers and were removed from the analysis (see Tables S1 and S3 for the final sample of each correlation analysis). A lateralization index was calculated for the Ns and FA according to the following formula: [(left) − (right)]/[(left) + (right)] (SI Materials and Methods) (16).

fMRI.

Preprocessing.

Data were preprocessed using Statistical Parameter Mapping software (SPM8, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College, London, United Kingdom, www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Preprocessing included realignment, coregistration between the mean functional and the structural T1, segmentation, normalization, and smoothing with an 8-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Normalization was performed using DARTEL (71).

Statistical analysis was based on a least-squares estimation using the general linear model by convolving a box-car regressor waveform with a canonical hemodynamic response function. Two main conditions of interest were modeled, language and rest. Parameters from head movement, obtained from realignment, were also included in the model. Main effects for each condition were calculated and the contrast of interest (language vs. rest) was estimated for each participant. A second level RFX analysis was performed by using a one-sample t test on the main contrast derived from the single subject data. Results are shown at a P < 0.05 false-discovery rate (FDR)-corrected (Fig. 3B and Table S5).

Functional connectivity (fcMRI) and statistical analysis.

Three ROIs were manually defined based on the anatomical gyri around the territories connected by the direct and the indirect segments of the AF (SI Materials and Methods and Fig. S3). The predominant tissue in each of the ROIs defined for functional connectivity analyses was gray matter (around 65%) (see SI Materials and Methods and Table S6 for further details). First-level subject-specific language vs. rest contrasts were masked with the defined ROIs, and for each subject, the peak coordinate within each ROI was selected. Eight-millimeter radius 3D-seeds were placed at the selected coordinates and mean voxel time-course series was extracted. These mean time courses were low-pass filtered and had the linear trend removed. MATLAB toolbox for functional connectivity (72) includes a function that calculates the relationship between two regions, after taking into account the influence of any other area. This toolbox was used to calculate correlations between the time series of each pair of territories. Finally, this correlation was transformed to a normal distribution using Fisher’s z transform (73).

Functional-behavioral correlations were performed for the areas in the frontal, temporal, and parietal cortices that the direct and the indirect segments of the AF connect (Broca, Wernicke, and Geschwind territories, respectively). Each subject’s z-transformed left and right correlations were regressed with behavioral performance (d′ scores).

Supplementary Material

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments

We thank Virginia Penhune for her helpful discussions on the results. The present project was funded by the FP7 ERC StG_313841 TuningLang (to R.d.D.-B.); the Spanish Government Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness Grants PSI2011-23624 (to R.d.D.-B.) and PSI2011-29219 (to A.R.-F.); predoctoral Grant 2010FI_B1 00169 from the Catalan government (to D.L.-B.); predoctoral Formación de Profesorado Universitario Grant AP2010-4179 from the Spanish Government (to P.R.); and in part by Guy’s and St Thomas’ Charity and the Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health at South London and Maudsley National Health Service Foundation Trust and Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London.

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

*This Direct Submission article had a prearranged editor.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1301696110/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Schulze K, Vargha-Khadem F, Mishkin M. Test of a motor theory of long-term auditory memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(18):7121–7125. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1204717109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Liberman AM, Mattingly IG. The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition. 1985;21(1):1–36. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90021-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hickok G, Poeppel D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(5):393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rauschecker JP, Scott SK. Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: Nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(6):718–724. doi: 10.1038/nn.2331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rodríguez-Fornells A, Cunillera T, Mestres-Missé A, de Diego-Balaguer R. Neurophysiological mechanisms involved in language learning in adults. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364(1536):3711–3735. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Benson DF, et al. Conduction aphasia. A clinicopathological study. Arch Neurol. 1973;28(5):339–346. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1973.00490230075011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Damasio H, Damasio AR. The anatomical basis of conduction aphasia. Brain. 1980;103(2):337–350. doi: 10.1093/brain/103.2.337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Meister IG, Wilson SM, Deblieck C, Wu AD, Iacoboni M. The essential role of premotor cortex in speech perception. Curr Biol. 2007;17(19):1692–1696. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Lopez-Barroso D, et al. Language learning under working memory constraints correlates with microstructural differences in the ventral language pathway. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(12):2742–2750. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Catani M, Jones DK, ffytche DH. Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Ann Neurol. 2005;57(1):8–16. doi: 10.1002/ana.20319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schmahmann JD, et al. Association fibre pathways of the brain: Parallel observations from diffusion spectrum imaging and autoradiography. Brain. 2007;130(Pt 3):630–653. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Valabregue R, Catani M. Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the frontal lobe association tracts. Cortex. 2012;48(1):82–96. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rilling JK, et al. The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11(4):426–428. doi: 10.1038/nn2072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Petrides M, Pandya DN. Projections to the frontal cortex from the posterior parietal region in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1984;228(1):105–116. doi: 10.1002/cne.902280110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Aboitiz F. Gestures, vocalizations, and memory in language origins. Front Evol Neurosci. 2012;4:2. doi: 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Catani M, et al. Symmetries in human brain language pathways correlate with verbal recall. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104(43):17163–17168. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702116104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Saur D, et al. Ventral and dorsal pathways for language. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(46):18035–18040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805234105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Martino J, Brogna C, Robles SG, Vergani F, Duffau H. Anatomic dissection of the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus revisited in the lights of brain stimulation data. Cortex. 2010;46(5):691–699. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Perani D, et al. Neural language networks at birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(38):16056–16061. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1102991108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Catani M, Howard RJ, Pajevic S, Jones DK. Virtual in vivo interactive dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain. Neuroimage. 2002;17(1):77–94. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.DeWitt I, Rauschecker JP. Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(8):E505–E514. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113427109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Beaulieu C. The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system—A technical review. NMR Biomed. 2002;15(7-8):435–455. doi: 10.1002/nbm.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.de Diego Balaguer R, Toro JM, Rodriguez-Fornells A, Bachoud-Lévi A-C. Different neurophysiological mechanisms underlying word and rule extraction from speech. PLoS ONE. 2007;2(11):e1175. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.MacMillan N, Creelman C. Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. Lawrence Elbaum, Mahwah, NJ; 2005. 2nd ed. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Chevillet MA, Jiang X, Rauschecker JP, Riesenhuber M. Automatic phoneme category selectivity in the dorsal auditory stream. J Neurosci. 2013;33(12):5208–5215. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1870-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Buchsbaum BR, D’Esposito M. The search for the phonological store: From loop to convolution. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20(5):762–778. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cunillera T, et al. Time course and functional neuroanatomy of speech segmentation in adults. Neuroimage. 2009;48(3):541–553. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Romanski LM, Tian B, Fritz J, Mishkin M, Rauschecker JP. Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(12):1131–1136. doi: 10.1038/16056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mestres-Missé A, Càmara E, Rodríguez-Fornells A, Rotte M, Münte TF. Functional neuroanatomy of meaning acquisition from context. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20(12):2153–2166. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Catani M, et al. A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2013 doi: 10.1093/brain/awt163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Geschwind N. The organization of language and the brain. Science. 1970;170(3961):940–944. doi: 10.1126/science.170.3961.940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Johnson SC, Saykin AJ, Flashman LA, McAllister TW, Sparling MB. Brain activation on fMRI and verbal memory ability: Functional neuroanatomic correlates of CVLT performance. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2001;7(1):55–62. doi: 10.1017/s135561770171106x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Stein M, et al. Structural plasticity in the language system related to increased second language proficiency. Cortex. 2012;48(4):458–465. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Golestani N, Price CJ, Scott SK. Born with an ear for dialects? Structural plasticity in the expert phonetician brain. J Neurosci. 2011;31(11):4213–4220. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3891-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Golestani N, Paus T, Zatorre RJ. Anatomical correlates of learning novel speech sounds. Neuron. 2002;35(5):997–1010. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00862-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Golestani N, Molko N, Dehaene S, LeBihan D, Pallier C. Brain structure predicts the learning of foreign speech sounds. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(3):575–582. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhk001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Mårtensson J, et al. Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. Neuroimage. 2012;63(1):240–244. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Lee H, et al. Anatomical traces of vocabulary acquisition in the adolescent brain. J Neurosci. 2007;27(5):1184–1189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4442-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mechelli A, et al. Structural plasticity in the biligual brain. Nature. 2004;431:757. doi: 10.1038/431757a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fields RD. White matter in learning, cognition and psychiatric disorders. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31(7):361–370. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Beaulieu C. In: Diffusion MRI. Jones DK, editor. New York: Oxford Univ Press; 2010. pp. 92–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Song S-K, et al. Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water. Neuroimage. 2002;17(3):1429–1436. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ogawa S, Lee TM. Magnetic resonance imaging of blood vessels at high fields: In vivo and in vitro measurements and image simulation. Magn Reson Med. 1990;16(1):9–18. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910160103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kwong KK, et al. Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1992;89(12):5675–5679. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.12.5675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Logothetis NK, Pauls J, Augath M, Trinath T, Oeltermann A. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature. 2001;412(6843):150–157. doi: 10.1038/35084005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Ogawa S, Lee TM, Nayak AS, Glynn P. Oxygenation-sensitive contrast in magnetic resonance image of rodent brain at high magnetic fields. Magn Reson Med. 1990;14(1):68–78. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910140108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Demerens C, et al. Induction of myelination in the central nervous system by electrical activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93(18):9887–9892. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.18.9887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Zatorre RJ, Fields RD, Johansen-Berg H. Plasticity in gray and white: Neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(4):528–536. doi: 10.1038/nn.3045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Petrides M, Pandya DN. Distinct parietal and temporal pathways to the homologues of Broca’s area in the monkey. PLoS Biol. 2009;7(8):e1000170. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Schenker NM, et al. A comparative quantitative analysis of cytoarchitecture and minicolumnar organization in Broca’s area in humans and great apes. J Comp Neurol. 2008;510(1):117–128. doi: 10.1002/cne.21792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Rilling JK, Glasser MF, Jbabdi S, Andersson J, Preuss TM. Continuity, divergence, and the evolution of brain language pathways. Front Evol Neurosci. 2011;3:11. doi: 10.3389/fnevo.2011.00011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Fritz J, Mishkin M, Saunders RC. In search of an auditory engram. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102(26):9359–9364. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503998102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Brauer J, Anwander A, Friederici AD. Neuroanatomical prerequisites for language functions in the maturing brain. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(2):459–466. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Brauer J, Anwander A, Perani D, Friederici AD. Dorsal and ventral pathways in language development. Brain Lang. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Yeatman JD, Feldman HM. Neural plasticity after pre-linguistic injury to the arcuate and superior longitudinal fasciculi. Cortex. 2013;49(1):301–311. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Rauschecker AM, et al. Reading impairment in a patient with missing arcuate fasciculus. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(1):180–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Dell’Acqua F, Catani M. Structural human brain networks: Hot topics in diffusion tractography. Curr Opin Neurol. 2012;25(4):375–383. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328355d544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Catani M, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Slater D, Dell’Acqua F. Connectomic approaches before the connectome. Neuroimage. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Johansen-Berg H. Structural plasticity: Rewiring the brain. Curr Biol. 2007;17(4):R141–R144. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Dutoit T, Pagel V, Pierret N, Bataille F, Vrecken O. Proceedings of The Fourth International Conference on Spoken Language Processing '96 (3) (Philadephia) 1996. The MBROLA project: Towards a set of high-quality speech synthesizers free of use for non-commercial purposes; pp. 1393–1396. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Peña M, Bonatti LL, Nespor M, Mehler J. Signal-driven computations in speech processing. Science. 2002;298(5593):604–607. doi: 10.1126/science.1072901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Leemans A, Jones DK. The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data. Magn Reson Med. 2009;61(6):1336–1349. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Chang L-C, Jones DK, Pierpaoli C. RESTORE: Robust estimation of tensors by outlier rejection. Magn Reson Med. 2005;53(5):1088–1095. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Jones DK, Basser PJ. “Squashing peanuts and smashing pumpkins”: How noise distorts diffusion-weighted MR data. Magn Reson Med. 2004;52(5):979–993. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Basser PJ, Pajevic S, Pierpaoli C, Duda J, Aldroubi A. In vivo fiber tractography using DT-MRI data. Magn Reson Med. 2000;44(4):625–632. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200010)44:4<625::aid-mrm17>3.0.co;2-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Wang R, Benner T, Sorensen A, Wedeen V. Proceedings of the International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine (Berlin) 2007. Diffusion toolkit: A software package for diffusion imaging data processing and tractography; p. 3720. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Catani M, Thiebaut de Schotten M. A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections. Cortex. 2008;44(8):1105–1132. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Klawiter EC, et al. Radial diffusivity predicts demyelination in ex vivo multiple sclerosis spinal cords. Neuroimage. 2011;55(4):1454–1460. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Naismith RT, et al. Radial diffusivity in remote optic neuritis discriminates visual outcomes. Neurology. 2010;74(21):1702–1710. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181e0434d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. 2007;38(1):95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Zhou D, Thompson WK, Siegle G. MATLAB toolbox for functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2009;47(4):1590–1607. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Barber AD, et al. Motor “dexterity”?: Evidence that left hemisphere lateralization of motor circuit connectivity is associated with better motor performance in children. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(1):51–59. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting Information

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES