Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Aug 12.
Published in final edited form as: J Rehabil Res Dev. 2012;49(2):209–220. doi: 10.1682/jrrd.2010.12.0239

New portable tool to screen vestibular and visual function—National Institutes of Health Toolbox initiative

Rose Marie Rine 1,*, Dale Roberts 2, Bree A Corbin 1, Roberta McKean-Cowdin 3, Rohit Varma 4, Jennifer Beaumont 5, Jerry Slotkin 5, Michael C Schubert 6
PMCID: PMC3741057  NIHMSID: NIHMS499206  PMID: 22773523

Abstract

As part of the National Institutes of Health Toolbox initiative, we developed a low-cost, easy-to-administer, and time-efficient test of vestibular and visual function. A computerized test of dynamic visual acuity (cDVA) was used to measure the difference in visual acuity between head still and moving in yaw. Participants included 318 individuals, aged 3 to 85 years (301 without and 17 with vestibular pathology). Adults used Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) optotypes; children used ETDRS, Lea, and HOTV optotypes. Bithermal calorics, rotational chair, and light box testing were used to validate the cDVA. Analysis revealed that the cDVA test is reliable for static (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC] >/= 0.64) and dynamic (ICC >/= 0.43–0.75) visual acuity. Children younger than 6 years old were more likely to complete cDVA with Lea optotypes, but reliability and correlation with ETDRS was better using HOTV optotypes. The high correlation between static acuity and light box test scores (r = 0.795), significant difference of cDVA scores between those with and without pathology (p </= 0.04), and the good to excellent sensitivity (73%) and specificity (69%) establish that the cDVA is a valid and reliable measure of visual acuity when the head is still and moving, as well as a good proxy of vestibular function to yaw rotation.

Keywords: dynamic visual acuity, gaze stability, NIH Toolbox, optotype, static visual acuity, vestibular, vestibular rehabilitation, vestibular test, vision, vision test

INTRODUCTION

The goal of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Toolbox initiative is to develop a valid set of instruments that measure motor, sensory, cognitive, and emotive functions for large-scale epidemiological and clinical trials and studies in subjects aged 3 to 85 years [1]. An important mandate of this goal was to develop instruments that could be administered with minimal training by nonclinical persons. For the sensory domain, a joint effort was invested by the vestibular and vision function teams to develop a single tool that would be useful for testing both senses. A select group of clinicians and basic scientists* served on separate vestibular and vision teams to identify appropriate tests for their respective sensory area, followed by joint meetings to identify areas of testing overlap between the two teams. The documentation of an increasing need for tests that can identify vestibular and/or visual impairments throughout the lifespan supports the development of tools to meet that need.

The vestibular system is an integral component of human sensory perception, including the perception of angular and linear motion. It provides the brain with both sensory afference and motor efference. Putative vestibular impairments affect 35 percent of U.S. adults over 40 years old [2], with approximately 8 million U.S. adults reporting chronic balance problems [3]. Aside from imbalance, consequences of vestibular deficits also include gaze instability and increased risk for falls [4]. Similar problems exist in pediatric populations. Recently, O’Reilly et al. completed a review of pediatric patients seen over a 4-year period with a chief complaint of dizziness or imbalance [5]. Of those seen by specialists in otolaryngology, 35 percent had confirmed vestibular disorders. Casselbrant et al. reported that balance and vestibular problems persist in children with a history of chronic middle ear effusion after the resolution of the effusion [6]. Together, growing evidence suggests that a significant percentage of adults and children have vestibular deficits, and the incidence in children is underestimated [78]. Part of the challenge in identifying the population with vestibular hypofunction (VH) is the existence of a significant time delay from the onset of symptoms to appropriate referral. This is in part caused by the lack of screening tools that can be used by nonspecialists to identify those individuals who should be referred for specialized testing and treatment.

Vision is a complex sensation that provides a personal representation of an individual’s surrounding environment. With the aging of the U.S. population, evaluation and treatment of age-related changes in vision-related diseases will be of growing importance in the next several decades. It is estimated that more than 150 million people in the U.S. general population wear corrective lenses to compensate for visual impairment caused by refractive error [9], which may include myopia, hyperopia, astigmatism, and presbyopia. Other relatively common disease-related causes of visual impairment include age-related macular degeneration, cataracts, diabetic retinopathy, and glaucoma. Studies to prevent and evaluate treatment for these conditions will be of increasing value because the number of people with vision impairment from age-related eye disease is expected to double in the next three decades [9].

Both the vestibular and vision teams were given the task of developing a test that was (1) limited in number of tests and/or demands on the study participants, (2) inexpensive, (3) time efficient, (4) portable, (5) a valid indicator of gaze stability and vision acuity, (6) able to be performed in subjects aged 3 to 85 years, and (7) capable of automated scoring and storing of data. The exclusion criteria for tests to be selected were (1) reliance on self-report of symptoms, (2) high cost, (3) requirement of examiner expertise, or (4) lack of psychometric strength (i.e., sensitivity, specificity, validity). Based on a comprehensive review of available tests and existing literature, the vestibular team developed two separate tests: one that isolates the vestibular system’s contribution to gaze stability (vestibular ocular reflex [VOR]) and one that isolates the vestibular system’s contribution to postural control. For the purpose of this article, we will only discuss VOR measurement.

The test chosen for VOR examination was a computerized test of dynamic visual acuity (cDVA) [1011]. For the vision team, computerized visual acuity testing was also deemed the best way to measure visual acuity. Therefore, the vestibular and vision teams agreed to merge their efforts into a single test of static visual acuity (SVA). While current versions of cDVA are available, they are expensive, require examiner expertise, and have no pediatric correlate. Consequently, efforts in software and hardware development were made to build a new test for use with children and adults that incorporated SVA and dynamic visual acuity (DVA) to evaluate peripheral VH for the vestibular team and visual resolution for the vision team.

BACKGROUND

Dynamic Visual Acuity

The semicircular canals and otolith organs of the peripheral vestibular end organ are essential for maintaining gaze and gait stability during head motion. This is achieved by efference from the VOR and the vestibulospinal reflexes. Damage to semicircular canals and/or otolith organs causes impaired visual acuity during head rotation (DVA); impaired balance and postural control; and symptoms of vertigo, falling, nausea, and disorientation [1018]. In addition to these symptoms, children with VH also experience a progressive motor delay with deleterious consequences on social development and educational performance [7,19]. However, lack of availability of low-cost, easy-to-administer tools to screen for VH has limited its identification. The NIH Toolbox cDVA was developed to address this shortcoming.

The DVA test measures the ability to see clearly during head rotation. Scores reflect the difference in visual acuity between stable head and moving head [1011]. At velocities <100 °/s, the ability to maintain gaze during head rotation is enabled by visual pursuit and the VOR [20]. For head velocities >100 °/s, the VOR is the primary contributor for detecting head rotation and maintaining stable gaze [21]. Herdman et al. were the first to develop a cDVA by flashing the letter E optotype whenever head velocities were between 120 and 180 °/s [10]. They presented optotypes in progressively smaller sizes, five at each acuity level, until the subject incorrectly identified five optotypes at a single line of acuity. A rate sensor mounted on a headband worn by the subject monitored active head velocity. The investigators reported good reliability, excellent sensitivity and specificity (94%), and significant effect of age in both nondisabled controls and patients with VH. In a separate study, Schubert et al. examined the use of a vertical DVA test and also reported excellent reliability (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC] = 0.94) for patients with VH, but poor sensitivity in identifying unilateral VH (UVH) loss (ICC = 0.23) [22]. This sensitivity improved to detect bilateral VH (BVH; ICC = 0.55), with the test having excellent specificity to rule out vestibular pathology of any type (ICC = 0.90). Each of these aforementioned tests was computerized but limited to testing adults (mean ± standard deviation [SD] = 50.6 ± 17.0 years) and did not have a validated SVA test.

Rine and Braswell developed a noncomputerized, clinical version of the horizontal DVA test for use with children [23]. Images included Lea symbols (circle, square, house, or apple; Figure 1(a)), posted on a 15-line vision optotype chart [24]. Children aged 3 to 15 years with (n = 11) and without (n = 76) BVH participated. Subjects initially completed the SVA test [25]. For the DVA test, the child’s head was passively moved in the horizontal plane, using a metronome to assure a velocity of 2 Hz. These investigators reported good to excellent reliability (ICC = 0.84) for horizontal DVA and excellent sensitivity and specificity (97%). However, this study was limited because a significant amount of examiner training was required and the sample was limited to those children with BVH. While UVH is more common than BVH in adults, we are unaware of the incidence of UVH in children or its effect on DVA in children.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Vision optotypes used included (a) Lea, (b) Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study, and (c) HOTV.

Static Visual Acuity

SVA tests measure impairments in visual resolution that can be caused by blurring of the retinal image, neural processing disorders, or damage to neurons in the retina or other parts of the visual pathway. The gold standard for visual acuity testing in adults is the Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) optotype [2627]. The ETDRS optotype was developed to evaluate changes in vision following panretinal photocoagulation in patients with diabetic retinopathy [27]. The test is administered in a standardized way, using visual acuity optotypes illuminated on a light box, resulting in quantitative visual acuity test results. It was designed to allow for comparison of data across research studies and is recommended by the National Academy of Sciences and the American National Standards Institute. The ETDRS optotypes include the same number of letters per row with equal spacing of letters and rows (0.1 logMAR between each row), with the individual rows balanced for letter difficulty. The ETDRS optotyes use 10 letters: C, D, H, K, N, O, R, S, V, and Z (Figure 1(b)). Scoring is completed on a logarithmic scale.

SVA testing in children (5–12 years old) has traditionally used the letters H, O, T, and V (Figure 1(c)), which replaced the ETDRS optotype letters in an attempt to improve testability in younger age groups [28]. Protocols using picture optotypes have also been used with high success rates (e.g., testability) but have been shown to overestimate visual acuity in some children with visual impairment [29]. Several investigators have reported that completion rates are better using symbols, as in the Lea optotypes [25,30], but a concern has been its sensitivity to test amblyopia. The use of HOTV optotypes was standardized in the Amblyopia Treatment Study, during which the protocol was found to have high testability and test-retest reliability in children aged 3 to 7 years [31]. The Amblyopia Treatment Study procedure was modified for an electronic visual acuity (EVA) tester to simplify the testing procedure and decrease testing variability [32]. HOTV testability in developmentally normal children was found to be 100 percent in 5- to 12-year-olds but was not tested in younger children. An EVA testing system has also been developed (electronic ETDRS [E-ETDRS] optotype) that uses a testing algorithm that runs on the EVA tester [3235]. In a study comparing E-ETDRS optotypes with the HOTV optotype, investigators found that children had better test results for HOTV optotypes compared with E-ETDRS optotypes (median difference = 0.06 logMAR or 3 letters on an optotype with 5 letters per line, p = 0.0001) when including children with normal eyes, eyes with refractive error, or amblyopic eyes [28].

The purpose of this project was to develop an inexpensive, portable, and psychometrically sound measure of visual acuity during still head and head in motion. Specifically, the objectives were to (1) determine the optimal optotype for use with young children, (2) establish the reliability of the DVA and SVA tests, and (3) determine the validity of the DVA and SVA tests.

METHODS

Subjects

We enrolled 318 subjects (17 with and 301 without vestibular pathology) in the study. To provide sufficient sample size to allow for repeat testing to determine optimal optotype in children, a larger portion of the sample was aged 12 or younger (Table 1). VH was confirmed by bithermal caloric (adults: >20% asymmetry) or rotational chair (children: 2 SDs below the normative age-matched data collected in this laboratory) tests. All children completed rotary testing. The types of pathology in this group were BVH secondary to infection (n = 10) or mild head trauma (n = 1). Only adults with suspect pathology completed bithermal caloric testing to confirm UVH: five caused by viral assault and one deafferented because of removal of vestibular schwannoma. Screening confirmed exclusion criteria, which included preexisting central nervous system disorder, cervical spine pathology, and any oculomotor impairment.

Table 1.

Subject demographics, n (%).

Demographic All (n = 318) Nondisabled (n = 301) Vestibular Pathology (n = 17)
Female 164 (51.6) 154 (51.2) 10 (58.8)
Age (yr)
 3 21 (6.6) 21 (7.0) 0 (0)
 4 34 (10.7) 34 (11.3) 0 (0)
 5 21 (6.6) 21 (7.0) 0 (0)
 6 23 (7.2) 22 (7.3) 1 (5.9)
 7 30 (9.4) 28 (9.3) 2 (11.8)
 8 27 (8.5) 27 (9.0) 0 (0)
 9 27 (8.5) 25 (8.3) 2 (11.8)
 10 18 (5.7) 17 (5.6) 1 (5.9)
 11 19 (6.0) 18 (6.0) 1 (5.9)
 12 20 (6.3) 18 (6.0) 2 (11.8)
 13–17 24 (7.5) 22 (7.3) 2 (11.8)
 ≥18 54 (17.0) 48 (16.0) 6 (35.3)
  18–35 16 (5.0) 16 (5.3) 0 (0)
  36–65 27 (8.5) 22 (7.3) 5 (29.4)
  66–85 11 (3.5) 10 (3.3) 1 (5.9)
Race/Ethnicity
 American Indian 2 (0.6) 2 (0.7) 0 (0)
 Asian 8 (2.5) 8 (2.7) 0 (0)
 Black or African American 12 (3.8) 12 (4.0) 0 (0)
 White 257 (80.8) 248 (82.4) 9 (52.9)
 More Than One Race 11 (3.5) 9 (3.0) 2 (11.8)
 Unknown/Not Reported 28 (8.8) 22 (7.3) 6 (35.3)
Hispanic 23 (7.2) 21 (7.0) 2 (11.8)

Instrumentation

Custom software was written in Python and C++. Hardware included a 2 GHz Intel Dual central processing unit laptop with 2 GB of RAM (IBM Thinkpad; Armonk, New York). The laptop was connected to a 1440 × 900 resolution monitor that displayed the optotypes. The operator used the built-in laptop display. A single-axis rate sensor (O-Navi; Vista, California) used to detect horizontal head rotation was attached to a soft bicycle light strap and secured to the head (Figure 2). The rate sensor triggered the software to flash an optotype only when head velocity met or exceeded 180 °/s. During SVA testing, optotypes were presented one at a time and continuously until the subject identified the optotype (no time limit). During cDVA, the optotype flashed on the screen for 83 ms only when the head moved 180 °/s. Optotype size ranged from 20/10 to 20/800. The software calculated an acuity score (in logMAR) assuming 100 optotypes were viewed using the formula: 1.7 − (0.02 × number correct) for SVA and DVA tests. We calculated the cDVA score from these measures by subtracting SVA from cDVA for each side.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Dynamic visual acuity headgear. Rate sensor attached to headband.

Data Collection

Subjects sat 12.5 ft away from the viewing screen at their eye level. The SVA test begins with an initial “quick” screening at size 20/50 and presents a single random letter per size (in steps of 0.1 logMAR), either going smaller until one is identified incorrectly or larger until one is identified correctly. The test then presents five letters per line size, beginning with the smallest size answered correctly during screening. If any letter is missed at the largest size seen, the next largest level is added. If three or more letters are correct at the smallest size seen, the next smallest level is added. The subject always sees all five letters for each line size. Letters from the largest level in the test are always shown first, working down to the smallest level. Testing stops when three or more letters are missed at the smallest level or the subject reaches the 20/10 level. The DVA test follows the same procedure, except it avoids the screening portion and starts three line sizes larger than the SVA screening. However, we performed training trials to assure correct head movement. The operator enters the subject’s responses. The software records the history of all optotypes displayed and the participants’ responses.

Testing was performed first with the head still (SVA) and again with the head moving (DVA) to the right (two trials) and then to the left (one trial). The first trial with rotation to the right was considered training for all subjects. We discarded scores from this trial and used them to control for learning effect [1011].

Subjects self-generated the horizontal head rotations and were instructed to “move your head side to side as if saying no.” Children completed training trials for DVA testing until they obtained 80 percent accuracy (4/5 correct optotypes). Young children responded verbally or by pointing to a card with the optotypes on it (at a size of 20/100). Subjects (n = 299) completed each test sequence twice to examine reliability.

For all subjects over 12 years old, we used the ETDRS optotype. To determine the optimal optotype set for use with children, we used three optotype sets (Lea, HOTV, and ETDRS); for subjects aged 3 to 6 years, we used both Lea symbols and HOTV optotypes, and for subjects aged 7 to 12 years, we completed tests using all three optotypes. Presentation sequence of optotypes for these groups was performed using a random block design.

To encourage participation by the youngest children, the examiners used reward stickers. Often, young children tend to avoid objects placed on their head, so the examiners offered “hats” (e.g., crown, pirate hat, or themed baseball cap) that could be attached to the head gear (Figure 3). Children were able to take their hat with them when testing was complete.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Hat incentive for children. To encourage participation and wearing rate sensor, paper hats were attached to headband. Rate sensor was concealed to minimize any anxiety and curiosity.

ETDRS light box acuity testing was completed by all subjects ≥7 years old to examine validity of the computerized SVA test. We asked subjects to read each letter of an ETDRS optotype placed 12.5 ft away, backlit with a light box. The test was complete when the subject missed three out of five optotypes at a line size (in logMAR) or read the entire optotype [27]. The SVA score was the line size (logMAR) just above this.

Statistical Analysis

We estimated test-retest reliability using ICCs within each of the age groups. To evaluate agreement between acuity scores based on different optotypes and tests, we computed ICCs between the pairs of measures (Lea, HOTV, ETDRS) within each age group and collapsed across age groups. We evaluated usability of each measure among children by examining the number able to successfully complete each test. We examined the associations of each measure with the relevant gold standard. For children, we used data from the rotational chair as a criterion reference; for adults, we used electronystagmography (ENG) as the reference to validate those subjects with vestibular pathology. In addition, we used a prior-established cDVA [36] to establish validity (sensitivity and specificity) in adults. For children, we used rotary test results to establish validity. To compare cDVA scores between those who did and did not pass the gold standard examination (evaluate construct validity), we used two-sample t-tests. We calculated effect sizes (mean difference divided by pooled SD) for all group comparisons to aid interpretation.

RESULTS

Demographics and Descriptives

Of the sample, 52 percent was female and 7 percent was Hispanic (Table 1). Of the 318 total participants, a small sample (n = 17) had a diagnosed vestibular pathology. Most of the individuals with vestibular pathology (14/17; 82%) were ≥9 years old.

Completion rates differed in the youngest groups (Table 2). For SVA testing, children 3 to 4 years old more successfully completed the test using the Lea optotypes instead of the HOTV optotypes.

Table 2.

Success rates for completion of testing by age and optotype. All other age groups achieved 100 percent completion rates.

Age (yr) Static Visual Acuity (%)
Dynamic Visual Acuity (%)
Lea HOTV Lea HOTV
3 81 76 70 40
4 94 86 85 50

Reliability

The reliability of SVA testing for each optotype depended on the optotype used but ranged from excellent to good (ICC = 0.89, 0.91, and 0.84 for Lea, HOTV, and ETDRS, respectively; Table 3). Reliability for cDVA was acceptable for the HOTV and ETDRS optotypes but not the Lea optotype (ICC = 0.55 for HOTV, 0.65 for ETDRS, 0.43 for Lea; Table 3). The difference in reliability of the optotypes was more pronounced in DVA testing (Table 3).

Table 3.

Reliability of visual acuity measures.

Measure ICC (95% CI)
All Age 3–4 (n = 37 to 50) Age 5–6 (n = 43 to 44) Age 7–12 (n = 133 to 141) Age 13–17 (n = 24) Age 18–76 (n = 50 to 54)
Static Visual Acuity
 Lea 0.89 (0.86, 0.92) 0.64 (0.35, 0.82) 0.75 (0.53, 0.87) 0.92 (0.89, 0.94)
 HOTV 0.91 (0.88, 0.93) 0.73 (0.47, 0.87) 0.61 (0.32, 0.80) 0.93 (0.90, 0.95)
 ETDRS 0.84 (0.79, 0.88) 0.85 (0.80, 0.89) 0.74 (0.50, 0.88) 0.81 (0.62, 0.91)
Dynamic Visual Acuity
 Lea (left) 0.38 (0.25, 0.50) 0.39 (0, 0.70) 0.45 (0.10, 0.70) 0.27 (0.11, 0.42)
 Lea (right) 0.29 (0.15, 0.42) 0.09 (0, 0.46) 0.50 (0.16, 0.73) 0.22 (0.05, 0.37)
 Lea (average) 0.43 (0.31, 0.54) 0.32 (0, 0.63) 0.52 (0.19, 0.74) 0.34 (0.18, 0.48)
 HOTV (left) 0.43 (0.30, 0.54) 0.57 (0.21, 0.79) 0.63 (0.35, 0.81) 0.28 (0.12, 0.43)
 HOTV (right) 0.46 (0.34, 0.57) 0.46 (0.06, 0.73) 0.18 (0, 0.51) 0.46 (0.32, 0.58)
 HOTV (average) 0.55 (0.44, 0.64) 0.63 (0.30, 0.83) 0.51 (0.18, 0.74) 0.48 (0.34, 0.60)
 ETDRS (left) 0.53 (0.42, 0.63) 0.38 (0.23, 0.52) 0.56 (0.22, 0.78) 0.44 (0.07, 0.70)
 ETDRS (right) 0.69 (0.61, 0.76) 0.42 (0.27, 0.55) 0.73 (0.48, 0.87) 0.76 (0.53, 0.89)
 ETDRS (average) 0.65 (0.56, 0.73) 0.48 (0.34, 0.60) 0.75 (0.51, 0.88) 0.58 (0.25, 0.79)

Note: Age range is provided due to varied number of subjects that completed retest within each optotype.

CI = confidence interval, ETDRS = Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study, ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

The following correlation of SVA scores between optotypes existed: Lea and HOTV = moderate to excellent (r = 0.58–0.98; overall r = 0.88); Lea and ETDRS = fair to excellent (r = 0.41–0.96; overall r = 0.90), and HOTV and ETDRS = good to excellent (r = 0.72–0.97; overall r = 0.93).

Validity

Sensitivity and specificity of cDVA was fair to excellent (calculations based on ETDRS optotypes only [n = 210]; Table 4). For children, scores achieved by those with and without VH were significantly different (p ≤ 0.001; effect size = 0.7; Figure 4). In adults, those who failed the previously established cDVA differed significantly from those who did not (p ≤ 0.001; effect size = 1.9; Figure 4). The agreement between the computerized version of SVA and light box visual acuity scores was good (r = 0.795; p ≤ 0.001; Figure 5).

Table 4.

Sensitivity and specificity (using Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study optotype) of computerized test of dynamic visual acuity (cDVA).

cDVA Pathology (n)
No Yes Total
Pass 135 4 139
Fail 60 11 71
Total 195 15 210

Note: Sensitivity = 73%; specificity = 69%.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Comparison of computerized testing of dynamic visual acuity (cDVA) score between those with and without pathology. cDVA score (dynamic visual acuity score – static visual acuity score, in logMAR) differed significantly between those with and without pathology in both younger and older age groups.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Correlation of computerized static visual acuity (SVA) and light box testing scores. SVA scores (in logMAR) attained on computerized test and light box test were highly correlated (r = 0.795; p < 0.001).

DISCUSSION

The cDVA created for the NIH Toolbox initiative is a valid measure of SVA, DVA, and VH. For children, cDVA reliability is moderate when using the ETDRS optotype and fair when using the Lea and HOTV optotypes. This might be because of the minimal variation in scores, since most subjects with pathology were >9 years old and thus were tested using only the ETDRS optotype. cDVA is a functional measure of VOR and will not replace the current standard of clinical vestibular function testing (i.e., bithermal caloric or rotary tests with ENG). This is in part because of the test being performed using active head rotations, which are known to recruit strategies to assist gaze stability, different from the VOR [37]. However, our version of the cDVA would be a useful means to screen visual acuity and VOR, particularly in large population-based studies.

Similar to prior reports [25,30], we had more success using the Lea optotypes for children <5 years old. This was particularly evident in DVA testing, which may be because of the increased difficulty of the dual task of moving the head and identifying a symbol. However, scores from HOTV had a higher correlation with the ETDRS optotype. These results suggest that the ideal optotype for children <6 years old is the HOTV optotype for SVA. However, if cDVA is of primary interest to investigators, the Lea optotype is optimal for success rates with very young children. Some of the challenges we experienced that influenced testability in children were that they were not comfortable or secure identifying letters, they were concerned they had made an error (or at least felt they did and did not want to continue), or they became bored with the task. All children were comfortable identifying the symbols and thus did not feel challenged until the optotype became small. With a dual discomfort level (letters and size with HOTV and ETDRS optotypes), children often wanted to stop all testing, with participation in the dynamic component adding a third challenge. Therefore, depending on the purpose of the data, cDVA in children should use the optotype that affords the most success (e.g., Lea optotype). Note that for this study, children completed testing of two or three different optotypes. This would not be the case in the typical clinical setting. In our experience, success in testing young children is maximized by keeping an upbeat, positive approach and using plenty of encouragement, applauding all responses.

We were able to complete the cDVA within 5 min for most subjects. Occasionally, testing in younger children required more demonstration, breaks, and rests and could thus require 7 min. This may be greatly influenced by the skill of the investigator working with children even though the protocol is computerized.

Both vestibular and visual functions are expected to be critical components of the functional status of individuals of all ages. This is primarily because of the growing evidence of vestibular and visual impairments in adults and children [23,5,7,38]. As we age, vestibular afferent neurons die and function similarly becomes impaired [39]. Refractive error is also a common condition in the general population, and the level of visual impairment caused by age-related diseases is expected to increase in the general population over the next several decades as the age distribution shifts toward greater numbers of individuals in older age groups. A readily available, inexpensive tool that enables screening of visual acuity and vestibular function by individuals with minimal training is important for healthcare and research.

LIMITATIONS

The limited number of subjects with vestibular pathology younger than 6 years old limits the generalization of sensitivity and specificity reported to those older than 6 years old. This, and the lack of gold standard testing of vestibular function in adults without vestibular pathology, may have affected results presented here. Furthermore, most subjects had visual acuity at or above 20/20. Further study is warranted that includes individuals with vestibular pathology across all ages, with gold standard testing of vestibular function of those with and without vestibular pathology, and those with visual acuity impairments to comprehensively establish the validity of this test.

CONCLUSIONS

Screening for vestibular function and visual acuity requires specialized training and expensive equipment that is not typically portable. Based on results presented here, the cDVA developed for use in the NIH Toolbox yields reliable and valid measures of both SVA and DVA. The test is inexpensive, portable, and can be completed by individuals aged 3 to 85 years. No special expertise in visual or vestibular testing is needed, and the test can be completed within 5 min. This makes it an optimal tool to screen for vestibular functional status by clinicians in various settings and investigators alike. Continuing research is underway to establish a normative database for individuals aged 3 to 85 years.

Acknowledgments

Funding/Support: This material was based on work supported in part by the Blueprint for Neuroscience Research and the Basic Behavioral and Social Science Opportunity Network (OppNet), NIH (grant HHS-N-260-2006-00007-C).

Abbreviations

BVH

bilateral vestibular hypofunction

cDVA

computerized test of dynamic visual acuity

DVA

dynamic visual activity

E-ETDRS

electronic Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study

ENG

electronystagmography

ETDRS

Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study

EVA

electronic visual acuity

ICC

intraclass correlation coefficient

NIH

National Institutes of Health

SD

standard deviation

SVA

static visual acuity

UVH

unilateral vestibular hypofunction

VH

vestibular hypofunction

VOR

vestibular ocular reflex

Footnotes

*

Vestibular Team: R. M. Rine, M. Schubert, S. Whitney, N. Shepard, D. Wrisley, G. Jacobsen, J. Carey, J. Slotkin, H. Hoffman. Vision Team: R. Varma, R. McKean-Cowdin, K. Cruickshanks, R. Hays, C. Johnson, M. Maguire, R. Massof, C. Owsley, M. Repka, S. Vitale, M. Wall.

Financial Disclosures: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Additional Contributions: Dr. Rine is now with Specialty Therapy Source, Jacksonville, Florida.

Institutional Review: We obtained informed consent for all subjects 18 years and older, parental consent for all minors, and assent for all 7 to 17 year olds (as approved by the institutional review boards of the University of North Florida, Johns Hopkins University, and University of Southern California).

Participant Follow-Up: The authors do not plan to inform participants of the publication of this study due to a lack of contact information. However, participants were informed that the results would be submitted for publication and encouraged to check the study Web site for an updated list of publications.

Author Contributions:

Study concept and design: R. M. Rine, M. C. Schubert, D. Roberts, J. Slotkin, R. McKean-Cowdin, R. Varma.

Acquisition of data: R. M. Rine, M. C. Schubert, B. A. Corbin, R. McKean-Cowdin.

Analysis and interpretation of data: R. M. Rine, M. C. Schubert, J. Beaumont, R. Varma.

Drafting of manuscript: R. M. Rine, M. C. Schubert, D. Roberts, J. Slotkin, R. McKean-Cowdin, R. Varma, B. A. Corbin, J. Beaumont.

Critical revision of manuscript for important intellectual content: R. M. Rine, M. C. Schubert, J. Slotkin, R. McKean-Cowdin, R. Varma.

Statistical analysis: J. Beaumont.

Administrative, technical, or material support: D. Roberts, B. A. Corbin, M. C. Schubert.

Study supervision: J. Slotkin, R. M. Rine, M. C. Schubert, R. Varma.

References

RESOURCES