Abstract
Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in ten early blind humans, we found robust occipital activation during two odor-processing tasks (discrimination or categorization of fruit and flower odors), as well as during control auditory-verbal conditions (discrimination or categorization of fruit and flower names). We also found evidence for reorganization and specialization of the ventral part of the occipital cortex, with dissociation according to stimulus modality: the right fusiform gyrus was most activated during olfactory conditions while part of the left ventral lateral occipital complex showed a preference for auditory-verbal processing. Only little occipital activation was found in sighted subjects, but the same right-olfactory/left-auditory-verbal hemispheric lateralization was found overall in their brain. This difference between the groups was mirrored by superior performance of the blind in various odor-processing tasks. Moreover, the level of right fusiform gyrus activation during the olfactory conditions was highly correlated with individual scores in a variety of odor recognition tests, indicating that the additional occipital activation may play a functional role in odor processing.
Introduction
When early deprived of its natural inputs a sensory cortex starts receiving and processing inputs in the remaining modalities [1]–[7]. In humans affected by early blindness (EB), several regions within the occipital cortex (OC) are recruited during the processing of auditory [8]–[13] and tactile stimuli [14]–[23] as well as during various cognitive tasks such as mental imagery [24]–[25], working memory [26]–[27] verbal processing and memory [28]–[32]. More recently, a stronger recruitment of the occipital cortex was also found during odor detection in blind compared to sighted participants [33], indicating further that the so-called “visual” cortex in the blind acquires non-visual functions. While these functional studies of brain activity elucidate some heightened auditory or tactile skills in blind individuals (e.g., enhanced spatial localization) [9]–[10], [13], [34]–[38], there were relatively few attempts to compare, in the same blind subjects, the brain activity elicited by stimuli processed in different sensory modalities [20], [23], [39]. For that reason, it is still unclear to what extent distinct (non-visual) sensory modalities are segregated in the reorganized occipital cortex of blind subjects. In addition, little is known about the cerebral network that supports performance in higher-level odor-processing in early blind individuals (e.g., odor discrimination, categorization or identification) since the few available studies [33, 40) were acquired during passive olfactory stimulation or during odor detection. We previously demonstrated that EB individuals were better than sighted subjects at discriminating, categorizing and identifying odors [41] see also [42]–[43], raising the question of the neural substrate that underlies this improved performance. In order to address the functional plasticity associated with olfactory expertise in the blind, we tested whether the occipital cortex of EB subjects was recruited during higher-level odor-processing tasks and if so, whether olfactory and auditory-verbal processing was dissociated in this cortex. We also tested to what extent the degree of occipital cortex recruitment, if present, would be predictive of individual differences in the behavioral performance of odor-processing tasks.
Materials and Methods
Ethics Statement
All participants provided their written informed consent prior to the study according to the Declaration of Helsinki (BMJ 1991; 302: 1194). The experimental protocol of the study was approved by the Biomedical Ethics Committee of the school of Medicine of the Université catholique de Louvain.
Subjects
The study was carried out in ten early blind subjects (EB, range 23–57 years, mean ± SD: 39.5±11.07) and ten sighted control participants (SC) who were blindfolded during the experiments and matched for age, sex, handedness and educational level (range 22–57 years, mean ± SD: 39.3±11.08, p>0.05). All subjects were right-handed males (see Table 1 for additional details regarding the subjects). They were healthy, without recorded history of neurological or psychiatric problems, and without olfactory disorders. Clinical hyposmia was excluded in all EB and 8 of their matched SC subjects in a previous behavioural study [41], [44] whereas all subjects rated their olfactory function as “normal” in self-reported ratings (i.e. being asked to evaluate their olfactory function as either “decreased”, “normal” or “over average). EB subjects were all affected by total blindness (without residual light perception) as a result of bilateral ocular or optic nerve lesions at birth or within the first 2 years of life, well before the completion of visual development [45]. None of the subjects reported any form of visual remembering. In addition, they were all autonomous and well integrated socially.
Table 1. Profile of the blind subjects.
Subjects | Age (years) | Educational level | Onset of blindness | Diagnosis | Performance (**) |
EB1 | 23 | Some college | Birth | Persistent hyperplastic primaryvitreous involving both eyes | NA |
EB2 | 28 | High school | Birth | Genetic (*) | 74 (°) |
EB3 | 31 | College degree | Birth | Leber congenital amaurosis | 66 |
EB4 | 42 | High school | Birth | Retinopathy of prematurity | 68 |
EB5 | 57 | College degree | <18 months | Bilateral retinoblastoma | 73 (°) |
EB6 | 31 | College degree | Birth | Anterior chamber cleavagesyndrome (Peters syndrome) | 73 |
EB7 | 43 | High school | Birth | Unknown, postmature birth (*) | 66 |
EB8 | 40 | College degree | Birth | Premature birth | 76 |
EB9 | 52 | College degree | Birth | Severe retinal dystrophy (*) | 72 |
EB10 | 48 | High school | <24 months | Bilateral retinoblastoma | 68 |
Note: EB: early blind; all subjects were male and right handed; (*): no additional details available.
(**) Behavioral performance in a variety of higher-level odor processing tasks (global percentage of correct answers) before fMRI (see text and Table S1 for details and scores of each task). (°) Behavioral performance in age-matched control was not available for this subject. As subjects EB5 and EB10 had very poor vision from birth and underwent a bilateral eye enucleation by the age of 18–24 months, they were considered early blind. They did not remember any visual experience.
Behavioral measurements
We assessed the subject's ability to discriminate, categorize and identify a variety of odour samples before the fMRI study. The material used to test the individual ability in odour-recognition, previously described in detail [41], consisted of 30 commercially available fragrances of flower, fruit, plant or domestic element (http://www.sentosphere.fr). Subjects were examined in sitting position in a well-ventilated dimly lit experimental room. For bi-rhinal stimulations, odorants were presented to the subject one by one with the bottle placed at 2 cm in front of nostrils during 3 s. with a 5 s. time interval between individual odours. A first discrimination task required the subject to smell each stimulus of pairs of stimuli, which had been constituted pseudo-randomly, and to determine whether the second odour was the same (half of the pairs) or different with respect to the first one. A second task of a free-identification was then used, in which subjects had to smell each odorant and name it. No feed-back was provided to the subject about the accuracy of his response before the end of a last task of categorization in which the subject was required to categorize each stimulus in one out of four (provided) semantic categories: fruit, flower, plant or other. The quotation was made on a 0/1 basis, with the total number of correct responses providing the score for each task. These scores were further averaged to provide the global odour-recognition performance (see Table S1 for a list of individual scores).
Equipment and Stimuli
Olfactory stimulation
We used four chemical odorants (Sigma Aldrich®, Germany) as fruit and flower odors: lemon (3,7-Dimethyl-2,6-octadienal dimethyl acetal), banana (Isoamyl acetate), lavender (1-Octen-3-yl acetate), and rose (2-Phenylethyl alcohol). A computer-controlled, MRI-compatible odor delivery system [46] was used as olfactory stimulator that allowed birhinal and timed delivery of each odor in synchrony with MRI sequences and participant’s inspiration phase.
Auditory stimulation
We used eight pre-recorded auditory-verbal stimuli (fruit and flower names): kiwi, cherry, tulip, hyacinth, peach, grape, begonia and orchid. An MRI compatible, high-definition piezoelectric sound delivery system was used as audio-system (the so-called SDS device, the fMRI.pl group, http://www.fmri.pl).
Tasks and Procedure
We used a block design paradigm with four experimental conditions (duration: 21 seconds) alternating with resting state periods (12 seconds) in eight runs of 408 seconds each. The fMRI acquisitions took place during two separate sessions of 4 runs each during the same week. Each condition was briefly announced via headphones during the preceding resting period. The order of conditions was pseudo-randomized and counterbalanced across subjects. Subjects provided their responses using two response button pads, one held in each hand. SC subjects were blindfolded during the experiments.
We used two different tasks in each modality: a categorization task that required an access to semantic information and a discrimination task that supposed a lower-level processing.
Odor categorization
Four olfactory stimulations were presented successively in one active block. At each trial, subjects had to categorize the stimulus as a flower or as a fruit. Odor discrimination: Four olfactory stimulations were presented successively and subjects had to determine whether each stimulus was the same or different from the preceding one. Word categorization: Eight auditory-verbal stimuli (fruit or flower names) were presented via headphones in one active block and subjects had to categorize each stimulus as a flower or as a fruit. Word discrimination: Eight stimuli were presented and subjects had to determine whether each word was the same or different from the preceding one. Subjects had to provide their response directly after each stimulus presentation (and before the presentation of the following stimulus). The stimulus sequences were pseudo-randomized.
Breathing control
To synchronize the olfactory stimulation with the inspiration phase, auditory signals were delivered to the subjects to control the breathing (see Figure 1 and Figure S1 for a display of the experimental design and the set-up). Two pure tones, a high frequency (264 Hz) and a low frequency (132 Hz) of 860 ms each, were used to warn the subject when to start an inspiration (breathing in) and an expiration (breathing out). Each stimulation block began with an inspiration occurring 1 second after the beginning of an olfactory stimulation, which stopped two seconds later. Then, the residual odor was flushed out from the system using clean air while the subject was breathing out. There were four inspiration phases in each epoch. During the auditory-verbal conditions, subjects were controlling their breathing the same way in order to keep the experimental parameters as comparable as possible across conditions.
Training
Before the fMRI experiment, each subject underwent two one-hour familiarization sessions to make sure that all subjects could control and adapt their breathing rhythm, perform the task and that the difficulty level of the task was equivalent between groups, i.e. did not constitute a confounding factor. The difficulty level of all fMRI conditions was intentionally set at a low level to insure that all subjects would be able to perform accurately the tasks during the fMRI acquisitions. At the end of the training phase all subjects were able to control their breathing as instructed and to discriminate and categorize the stimuli with a mean accuracy superior to 80%.
3D-MRI and fMRI Acquisition
Structural brain imaging was obtained in all subjects in the bicommissural (AC-PC) orientation [47] on a 3 Tesla MRI unit (Achieva, Philips Healthcare®, Best, The Netherlands) using a 3D fast T1-weighted gradient echo sequence with an inversion prepulse (Turbo field echo (TFE), TR [repetition time] = 9 ms, TE [echo time] = 4.6 ms, flip angle = 8 degree, 150 slices, 1 mm thickness, in plane resolution = 0.81×0.95 mm). The field of view was 220×197 mm, and the SENSE factor (parallel imaging) was 1.5. We used an 8 channels phased array head coil. Foam pads restrained the head.
Blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) fMRI data were acquired using a 2D single shot T2*-weighted gradient echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 3000 ms, TE = 27 ms) with 48 axial slices (thickness = 2.4 mm), in the AC-PC orientation. The matrix was 112×112×48 and the field of view was 220 mm2. The in-plane resolution was 2.12 mm2. The fMRI paradigm consisted in 8 runs of alternating epochs of experimental conditions and rest (21 s per active epoch, 7 brain volume repetitions, alternating with 12 s [4 repetitions] resting periods). Each condition was assessed 3 times in a separate run, in counterbalanced order. The first inspiration phase was synchronized with the acquisition of the first slice of the epoch.
Data Analysis
Data analysis was performed using the BrainVoyager QX 2.2 software package (Brain Innovation™, Maastricht, The Netherlands) using standard preprocessing procedures. fMRI data preprocessing included head motion correction, slice scan time correction, and high-pass filtering (cutoff frequency: 3 cycles/run) using temporal smoothing in the frequency domain to remove drifts and to improve the signal to noise ratio. No data included in the study exceeded motion of 2 mm in any given axis or had spike-like motion of more than 1 mm in any direction during a given fMRI session. Functional and anatomical data sets for each subject were coregistered and the resulting matching brain images were fit to standardized Talairach space [47]. Single-subject functional data were spatially smoothed (4 mm FWHM) in order to reduce intersubject anatomical variability and analyzed using one multiple regression model (General Linear Model, GLM [48]) consisting of predictors, which corresponded to the four experimental conditions, and in which the beta weights quantify the potential contribution of the predictors in explaining each voxel time course. The predictor time courses of the regression model were computed on the basis of a linear model of the relation between neural activity and hemodynamic response, assuming a rectangular neural response convolved with standard hemodynamic response function (HRF) during phases of active conditions [48]–[50].
A random-effects (RFX) group analysis [51] was then performed at the whole-brain level, using a mask for the cortex (gray matter) using one-sample and two-sample t tests with a threshold of p<0.001, uncorrected, in combination with a cluster size threshold adjustment to achieve a corrected p<0.05. This was done based on the [52] Monte Carlo simulation approach, extended to 3D data sets using the threshold size plug-in Brain Voyager QX [53]. In order to find the areas activated in each modality (olfactory and auditory-verbal processing), two basic contrasts of interests (compared to rest) were explored, both within and between groups: {olfactory conditions} (contrast weight : [1 1 0 0]) (areas involved in two aspects of active odor processing, i.e. odor discrimination and categorization), {auditory-verbal conditions} [0 0 1 1 ] (areas involved in the same tasks using auditory-verbal stimuli) and their interactions were tested at the whole brain level. In addition, to measure the link between performance and brain activity, a covariance analysis was performed on {olfactory conditions} brain activation maps with the individual odor-recognition score (averaged score for discrimination, free identification and categorization, see Table S1) used as the covariate. In order to control for group size, only 16 subjects (8 SC where this score was available and their matched EB subjects) were included in the covariance analysis. In all the areas with {olfactory conditions} where brain activity was found to be higher in EB group compared to SC group, correlation analyses were also performed between the beta weights of {olfactory conditions} and the performance values (odor-recognition scores), in order to identify the key area(s) explaining performance disparity between EB and SC subjects.
Results
Behavioral Results
The behavioural study showed that EB participants were significantly better to discriminate, categorize and identify odours than SC subjects (ANOVA’s: all p’s <0.05 in the group comparisons: p<0.005, <0.001, <0.01 for discrimination, identification and categorization respectively) and, as a consequence, had higher global odour-recognition scores (p<0.001). The mean scores for correct responses in EB and SC groups were 28.4±0.9 and 25.4±2.2 for odor discrimination, 13.0±1.9 and 6.4±1.5 for free identification and 22.4±1.9 and 19.0±2.8 for categorization. Individual scores for each task are shown in Table S1. The recorded in-scan responses confirmed that all subjects could perform adequately and easily the tasks (above 80% in all the subjects in all four conditions).
Functional Imaging Results
Olfactory and auditory-verbal activation patterns
In the entire group (both EB and SC plotted together), the contrast {Olfactory conditions vs. Rest} showed the expected bilateral activation in the olfactory cortex: in the entorhinal cortex (Brodmann area [BA] 34), the amygdala, the orbito-frontal cortex (OFC, BA10 and BA11) and the insula/OFC (BA13-47) (see Figure S2 for a display of brain activation related to olfactory processing). The contrast {Auditory conditions vs. Rest} showed bilateral activation in the primary and secondary auditory cortices within the superior and transverse temporal gyri (BA 41, 42, 22) (see Figure S3 for a display of brain activation related to auditory-verbal processing). No group difference was observed in any of these olfactory and auditory regions (all p’s>0.05 uncorrected), though there was a trend toward stronger activation in SC subjects in the left auditory cortex only (p = 0.06 uncorrected) (Figures S2 and S3).
During both olfactory and auditory conditions the occipital cortex was largely activated in EB subjects and to a much lesser extent in SC subjects (see Figure S4). These activation foci were mostly located within the ventral part of the occipital cortex and included the fusiform gyrus, the lingual gyrus and inferior and middle occipital gyri. In addition, we observed an opposite hemispheric lateralization for the olfactory and auditory processing. In the two groups, the left hemisphere was dominant for the auditory-verbal processing while the right hemisphere appeared more activated than the left one during the olfactory processing.
Group comparison and modality-specific activation
The group comparisons, performed at the whole brain level in the olfactory and auditory modalities, showed that the occipital cortex was more activated in EB than in SC subjects in both modalities. In the olfactory modality, only one activation focus was found in the right fusiform gyrus (FG, BA 19, x = 24, y = −67, z = −13, 404 voxels) (Figure 2). In the auditory modality, a single activation focus was found in the left middle occipital gyrus (MOG, BA 19, x = −46, y = −73, z = −5, 22 voxels). This activation focus was located in the posterior part of the ventral lateral occipital complex (LOC) [54]. No activation focus was found in the occipital cortex for the reverse contrast (SC minus EB) in any of the sensory modalities. This result is in line with the hemispheric lateralization previously found in the main effects related to sensory modality (see Figure S4 for a display of related brain activation in each group). Within-group analyses performed at the whole-brain level in EB subjects (p<0.001 uncorrected) revealed activation foci in the occipital cortex of the blind that overlapped those obtained in the group comparisons (see Figure 3, tables S2, S3, S4). The contrasts {olfactory minus auditory} and {auditory minus olfactory} in EB subjects revealed two activation foci: one specific to the olfactory modality in the right fusiform gyrus (x = 29, y = −64, z = −13, 65 voxels) and one specific to the auditory modality in the left MOG (x = −55, y = −61, z = −2, 130 voxels).
Covariance and correlation analyses: relationship between olfactory performance and brain activation
The activation map resulting from the analysis of covariance in 16 (8 EB) subjects with the individual global odor-recognition score (composite score : odor discrimination, odor categorization and odor identification) used as the external covariate of interest is displayed in Figure 4, coding the covariation between overall performance in odor recognition and brain activity across the entire brain volume during {Olfactory conditions vs. Rest}. Amongst the brain regions activated in the contrast {Olfactory conditions vs. Rest} either in EB or in SC subjects, only one brain region showed a positive and significant covariation between the individual performance in odor recognition and brain activity. This brain area was located in the right fusiform gyrus (FG, BA 19, x = 24, y = −64, z = −13, 380 voxels) and largely overlapped the activation focus obtained in the group comparison (EB minus SC) for olfactory processing (see Figure 4).
A complementary correlation analysis performed in these 16 subjects (8 SC and their matched EB subjects) showed a strong relationship between the performance index (odor recognition score) and the beta weights of the fusiform gyrus as identified in the contrast {olfactory conditions in EB minus olfactory conditions in SC} (r = 0,80, p<0.0001, see graph in Figure 4). It is worth noting that the covariance and correlation analyses based on the discrimination, free identification and categorization subscores separately yielded similar results (r = 0,64, p<0.01, r = 0,77, p<0.0001, r = 0,65, p<0.01).
Task-related activation patterns
Using a threshold of p<0.001 (uncorrected), the task comparisons, {categorization minus discrimination} and {discrimination minus categorization} did not show any activation focus in the occipital cortex of EB subjects in any of the two modalities (grouped together or tested separately).
Discussion
Functional Dissociation in the Occipital Cortex of the Early Blind
Here, we found a dissociation between olfactory and auditory-verbal processing in the occipital cortex (OC) of early blind (EB) subjects; the right fusiform gyrus was most activated during the olfactory conditions and part of the left middle occipital gyrus, located in the posterior part of the ventral lateral occipital complex [54]–[55] showed a preference for auditory-verbal processing (Figure 2). In addition, there was a strong correlation between the level of right fusiform gyrus activation during the olfactory conditions and the individual performance in a variety of odor identification tests.
Methodological Implications
Observing a double dissociation makes it unlikely that the observed differences between olfactory and auditory-verbal conditions were due to a general factor, such as arousal level, attention or difficulty level, which would have provided at most a single dissociation. The same tasks (stimulus discrimination and categorization) were used and the stimuli used in the two modalities belonged to the same semantic categories (fruits and flowers). The olfactory- and auditory-verbal-specific activation foci were spatially well segregated in different hemispheres and brain structures. We can therefore exclude any potential effect of the semantic category or inter-individual variations in the location and extent of the olfactory and auditory-verbal activation foci to explain the observed dissociation. These findings constitute some of the very first evidences that stimuli that are different in their nature recruit different parts of the occipital cortex in the same EB individuals; olfactory and auditory-verbal processing recruits distinct neural networks in this cortex.
Olfactory and Auditory-verbal Processing in the Occipital Cortex of the Early Blind
In a recent fMRI study, [33] reported occipital cortex (OC) activation in congenitally blind subjects when they paid attention to their olfactory sense to detect odors, but until now, no study had investigated whether distinct parts of the OC of blind subjects were specifically involved in higher order stimulus processing in olfaction as compared to other senses. In EB subjects, a recruitment of the OC during the processing of non visual information (including verbal stimuli) was consistently reported in many studies, although most of them focused either on the auditory or the tactile modality [11], [14], [17], [28], [30], [32], [37]. It has been proposed that the crossmodal recruitment of OC in EB subjects could explain their enhanced perceptual and memory abilities in the auditory and tactile modalities [1], [10], [13], [30], [34]–[35], [38]. Similarly, we show that the OC activation observed in EB subjects during olfactory processing predict their improved performance in various odor identification tasks (Figure 4, Table S1). Only the subjects who had the best scores among the sighted group showed a trend to recruit the right fusiform gyrus, indicating that this brain area could potentially play a role in odor recognition in sighted subjects as well. In the absence of vision, the olfactory sense becomes more crucial to identify persons and places and to evaluate the quality of food before bringing it to the mouth [42]. Because EB people rely more on olfaction, they develop superior abilities to process odors [43]–[44]. The “visual” cortex would provide the neural basis to facilitate the emergence of this type of practice-related behavioral compensation in blind subjects [4].
Cross-modal Plasticity and Sensory Specialization in the Occipital Cortex
For long the question of the specific/nonspecific nature of the brain activation observed in the OC of EB subjects has been debated. On the one hand, several occipital regions in blind subjects seemed quite indifferently recruited in various experimental conditions (tasks and stimuli) which led some authors to propose that the OC recruitment was nonspecific and served a general purpose [2], [19], [56]. On the other hand, several studies brought evidence for the existence of functional specializations in the OC of blind subjects [23]–[24], [30], [57]–[60]. To reconcile these points of view one could hypothesize that both theories are partly correct. Demonstrating the existence of a modular organization in some parts of the OC in EB does not necessarily exclude that other regions in the OC may support supra-modal or general factors that are involved in various perceptual and cognitive processes. The visual cortex is indeed quite complex and includes numerous specialized modules in sighted subjects [61]–[62] and probably in EB subjects as well.
Here, we showed for the first time a clear dissociation between olfactory and auditory-verbal modalities. This dissociation was found independently to the task performed (e.g. stimulus discrimination and categorization) which suggests that it was mainly driven by the stimulus processing. This finding constitutes new evidence in favour of the existence of functional specialization in the OC of EB subjects and sheds light on how nonvisual modalities are distributed in their reorganized OC. In the absence of visual inputs, nonvisual sensory modalities extend their cerebral networks into the OC to improve perceptual processing and remain, at least partly, segregated in this region. This also indicates that the stimulus nature is a key factor to understand the functional specialization of the OC of the blind [58]. The few studies that compared in the same blind subjects the brain activation elicited by the processing of auditory versus tactile stimuli did not find any clear dissociation between these two modalities in the OC [20], [23]. However, the stimuli used in these studies shared close physical properties: both were mechanical waves perceived either via the auditory (sounds) or the tactile (vibrations) modality. By contrast, the odor stimuli (chemical stimuli) used here were clearly different in nature from odor names. Furthermore, unlike the olfactory stimuli, auditory-verbal stimuli involved explicitly a verbal processing though the olfactory conditions may also have involved strategies based on verbalization. Finally, olfaction is the only sense for which there is no thalamic relay before reaching the primary sensory cortex [63].
Cross-modal Plasticity and Preserved Functional Segregation of the Ventral and Dorsal Streams
Several studies provided evidence in favour of preserved functional specialization within the so-called “visual” streams in EB humans. This was clearly demonstrated for the dorsal stream, where several regions retained their designated functional role in spatial and motion processing regardless to visual experience [22]–[23], [25]–[26], [57], [59]–[60], [64]–[66], and to a lesser extent for the ventral stream [17], [24], [67]. Interestingly, a similar preservation of the functional specialization was recently found in the auditory cortex of early deaf cats [68]–[69]. In the present study, most activation foci observed in the OC of EB subjects in both modalities were found in brain areas usually considered part of the “visual” ventral stream in sighted subjects [62], [70]. Given the nonspatial nature of the two tasks used in the present study, this brought further support to the growing evidence showing that the general functional role of regions in the ventral stream might be retained in blindness, but changes sensory modality [23].
Cross-modal Plasticity and Hemispheric Specialization in the Occipital Cortex
In the present study, we found opposite brain lateralization for olfactory and auditory-verbal processing in both groups: olfactory processing activated more the right hemisphere while auditory-verbal processing was dominant in the left side of the brain (see Figure S4). This type of lateralization was previously reported in sighted subjects both for olfactory [71]–[73] and verbal processing [74]–[75]. In EB subjects however, this lateralization appeared more pronounced in the OC, while this cortex was little activated in SC subjects. This strengthened lateralization in the OC of the blind leads us to suggest that parts of this cortex become a prominent component of the specialized olfactory and auditory-verbal cerebral networks after early visual deprivation. It is worth noting that a similar left-sided OC lateralization has been recently reported in EB subjects for verbal processing [31]–[32], [76].
Conclusions
Here we demonstrate that, in the absence of visual input, nonvisual sensory modalities colonize the “visual” cortex and that olfactory and auditory-verbal processing remains segregated in this cortex. Furthermore, the ventral stream seems to develop its designated functional role in processing stimulus identity independently of visual experience. We also demonstrate that the brain activity level in the right fusiform gyrus predicted performance in olfactory identification tasks indicating the specific role of this region in the processing of odors. Additional neuroimaging studies should further investigate in early blind subjects to what extent different sensory modalities are segregated in the occipital cortex and how nonvisual inputs promote development of functional modules within the ventral stream.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
The authors gratefully thank all the volunteers for their participation to the study. We wish to thank Profs. Josef Rauschecker and Brigitte Röder for their helpful comments on the experimental design and Mr. Benoît Gérard for technical development.
Funding Statement
The “Fonds spécial de Coopération Universitaire au Développement” of the Université catholique de Louvain (Belgium) supported Isabel Cuevas and Paula Plaza. Anne G. De Volder is Senior Research Associate and Elodie Lerens is Research Fellow at the Belgian National Funds for Scientific Research (FNRS, http://www.frs-fnrs.be/, Belgium). Laurent Renier is a Postdoctoral Researcher supported by the Brussels Institute for Research and Innovation (INNOVIRIS, http://www.innoviris.be/site/, Belgium). This study was supported by FRSM grant #3.4502.08 (Belgium). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Rauschecker JP (1995) Compensatory plasticity and sensory substitution in the cerebral cortex. Trends Neurosci 18: 36–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Kujala T, Alho K & Näätänen R (2000) Cross-modal reorganization of human cortical functions. Trends Neurosci 23: 115–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Bavelier D & Neville HJ (2002) Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 443–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Pascual-Leone A, Amedi A, Fregni F, Merabet LB (2005) The plastic human brain cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 28: 377–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Merabet LB, Pascual-Leone A (2010) Neural reorganization following sensory loss: the opportunity of change. Nat Rev Neurosci 11 (1): 44–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Kupers R, Pietrini P, Ricciardi E, Ptito M (2011a) The nature of consciousness in the visually deprived brain. Front Psychol 2: 19 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00019. Epub 2011 Feb 14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Renier LA, De Volder AG, Rauschecker JP (2013) Cortical plasticity and preserved function in early blindness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013, in press, Feb 20. pii: S0149–7634(13)00032–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.025. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 8. Kujala T, Alho K, Paavilainen P, Summala H, Näätänen R (1992) Neural plasticity in processing of sound location by early blind: an event-related potential study. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 84 (5): 469–472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Rauschecker JP, Korte M (1993) Auditory compensation for early blindness in cat cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 13 (10): 4538–4548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Röder B, Teder-Sälejärvi W, Sterr A, Rösler F, Hillyard SA, Neville HJ (1999) Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans. Nature 400: 162–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Weeks R, Horwitz B, Aziz-Sultan A, Tian B, Wessinger CM, et al. (2000) A positron emission tomography study of auditory localization in the congenitally blind. J Neurosci 20: 2664–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Gougoux F, Zatorre RJ, Lassonde M, Voss P, Lepore F (2005) Functional neuroimaging study of sound localization: visual cortex activity predicts performance in early-blind individuals. PLoS Biol 3: 324–333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Stevens AA, Snodgrass M, Schwartz D, Weaver K (2007) Preparatory activity in occipital cortex in early blind humans predicts auditory perceptual performance. J Neurosci 27: 10734–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Sadato N, Pascual-Leone A, Grafman J, Ibañez V, Deiber MP, Dold G, et al. (1996) Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects. Nature 380: 526–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Cohen LG, Celnik P, Pascual-Leone A, Corwell B, Falz L, Dambrosia J, et al. (1997) Functional relevance of cross-modal plasticity in blind humans. Nature 389: 180–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Büchel C, Price C, Frackowiak RSJ, Friston K (1998) Different activation patterns in the visual cortex of late and congenitally blind subjects. Brain 121: 409–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Pietrini P, Furey ML, Ricciardi E, Gobbini MI, Wu WH, Cohen L, et al. (2004) Beyond sensory images: Object-based representation in the human ventral pathway. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101(15) 5658–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Ptito M, Moesgaard SM, Gjedde A, Kupers R (2005) Cross-modal plasticity revealed by electrotactile stimulation of the tongue in the congenitally blind. Brain 128: 606–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Burton H, McLaren DG, Sinclair RJ (2006) Reading Embossed Capital Letters: An fMRI Study in Blind and Sighted Individuals. Hum Brain Mapp 27: 325–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Weaver KE & Stevens AA (2007) Attention and sensory interactions within occipital cortex in the early blind: An fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 315–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Stilla R, Hanna R, Hu X, Mariola E, Deshpande G, Sathian K (2008) Neural processing underlying tactile microspatial discrimination in the blind: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Vis 8: 1–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Kupers R, Chebat DR, Madsen KH, Paulson OB, Ptito M (2010) Neural correlates of virtual route recognition in congenital blindness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA; 107 1: 2716–2721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Renier LA, Anurova I, De Volder AG, Carlson S, VanMeter J, Rauschecker JP (2010) Preserved functional specialization for spatial processing in the middle occipital gyrus of the early blind. Neuron 68: 138–148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. De Volder AG, Toyama H, Kimura Y, Kiyosawa M, Nakano H, Vanlierde A, et al. (2001) Auditory Triggered Mental Imagery of Shape Involves Association Areas in Early Blind Humans. NeuroImage 14: 129–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Vanlierde A, De Volder AG, Wanet-Defalque MC, Veraart C (2003) Occipito-parietal cortex activation during visuo-spatial imagery in early blind humans. NeuroImage 19 (3) 698–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Bonino D, Ricciardi E, Sani L, Gentili C, Vanello N, Guazzelli M, et al. (2008) Tactile spatial working memory activates the dorsal extrastriate cortical pathway in congenitally blind individuals. Arch Ital Biol 146: 133–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Burton H, Sinclair RJ & Dixit S (2010) Working memory for vibrotactile frequencies: Comparison of cortical activity in blind and sighted Individuals. Hum Brain Mapp 31: 1686–1701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Röder B, Stock O, Bien S, Neville H, Rösler F (2002) Speech processing activates visual cortex in congenitally blind humans. Eur J Neurosci 16(5), 930–6. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 29. Burton H, Snyder AZ, Conturo TE, Akbudak E, Ollinger JM, Raichle ME (2002) Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a fMRI study of Braille reading. J Neurophysiol 87 (1): 589–607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Amedi A, Raz N, Pianka P, Malach R, Zohary E (2003) Early visual cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind. Nat Neurosci 6: 758–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Reich L, Szwed M, Cohen L, Amedi A (2011) Ventral Visual Stream Reading Center Independent of Visual Experience. Curr Biol 8 21(5): 363–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Bedny M, Pascual-Leone A, Dodell-Feder D, Fedorenko E, Saxe R (2011) Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108(11): 4429–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Kupers R, Beaulieu-Lefebvre M, Schneider FC, Kassuba T, Paulson OB, Siebner HR, et al. (2011b) Neural correlates of olfactory processing in congenital blindness. Neuropsychologia 49(7): 2037–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Pascual-Leone A, Torres F (1993) Plasticity of the sensorimotor cortex representation of the reading finger in Braille readers. Brain 116 (Pt 1): 39–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Lessard N, Pare M, Lepore F, Lassonde M (1998) Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects. Nature; 395 (6699): 278–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Grant AC, Thiagarajah MC, Sathian K (2000) Tactile perception in blind Braille readers: a psychophysical study of acuity and hyperacidity using gratings and dot patterns. Perception & Psychophysics 62: 301–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Gougoux F, Lepore F, Lassonde M, Voss P, Zatorre RJ, Belin P (2004) Pitch discrimination in the early blind. Nature 430 (6997): 309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Voss P, Lassonde M, Gougoux F, Fortin M, Guillemot JP, Lepore F (2004) Early-and late-onset blind individuals show supra-normal auditory abilities in far-space. Curr Biol 14: 1734–1738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Röder B, Rösler F, Hennighausen E, Näcker F (1996) Event-related potentials during auditory and somatosensory discrimination in sighted and blind human subjects. Cogn Brain Res. 4: 77–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Cuevas I, Plaza P, Rombaux P, Mouraux A, Delbeke J, Collignon O, et al. (2011) Chemosensory event-related potentials in early blind humans. B-ENT 7(1): 11–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Cuevas I, Plaza P, Rombaux P, De Volder AG, Renier L (2009) Odour discrimination and identification are improved in early blindness. Neuropsychologia 47: 3079–3083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Cuevas I, Plaza P, Rombaux Ph, Collignon O, De Volder AG, Renier L (2010a) Do early blind people develop a better sense of smell? A psychophysical study. J vis imp blindness 104 (6) 169–80. [Google Scholar]
- 43. Beaulieu-Lefebvre M, Schneider FC, Kupers R, Ptito M (2011) Odor perception and odor awareness in congenital blindness. Brain Res Bull 2011 84(3): 206–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Rombaux P, Huart C, De Volder AG, Cuevas I, Renier L, Duprez T, et al. (2010) Increased olfactory bulb volume and olfactory function in early blind subjects. Neuroreport 21(17): 1069–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Wiesel TN (1982) Postnatal development of the visual cortex and the influence of environment. Nature, 299 (5884): 583–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Cuevas I, Gérard B, Plaza P, Lerens E, Collignon O, Grandin C, et al. (2010b) Development of a fully-automated system for delivering odors in MRI environment. Behav Res Methods 42: 1072–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. (Thieme Medical, New York).
- 48.Friston KJ, Jezzard P, Turner R (1994) Analysis of function MRI time series. Hum. Brain Mapp 1, 153–71.
- 49.Lange N, Zeger SL (1996) Non-linear Fourier analysis of magnetic resonance functional neuroimage time series. Appl Stat; 46 (1), 1–29.
- 50.Boynton GM, Engel SA, Glover GH, Heeger DJ (1996) Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J Neurosci 16 (13), 4207–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 51. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Price CJ, Buchel C, Worsley KJ (1999) Multisubject fMRI Studies and Conjunction Analyses. NeuroImage 10: 385–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Forman SD, Cohen JD, Fitzgerald M, Eddy WF, Mintun MA, Noll DC (1995) Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn Reson Med 33(5): 636–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Goebel R, Esposito F, Formisano E (2006) Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with Brainvoyager QX: From single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self-organizing group independent component analysis. Human Brain Mapping 27, 392–401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 54. Malach R, Reppas JB, Benson RR, Kwong KK, Jiang H, Kennedy WA, et al. (1995) Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92: 8135–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Kourtzi Z & Kanwisher N (2000) Cortical regions involved in perceiving object shape. J Neurosci 20: 3310–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Lewis LB, Saenz M & Fine I (2010) Mechanisms of cross-modal plasticity in early-blind subjects. J Neurophysiol 104: 2995–3008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Ricciardi E, Vanello N, Sani L, Gentili C, Scilingo EP, Landini L, et al. (2007) The effect of visual experience on the development of functional architecture in hMT+. Cereb. Cortex 17: 2933–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Mahon BZ, Anzellotti S, Schwarzbach J, Zampini M, Caramazza A (2009) Category-specific organization in the human brain does not require visual experience. Neuron 63: 397–405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ptito M, Matteau I, Gjedde A., Kupers R (2009) Recruitment of the middle temporal area by tactile motion in congenital blindness. Neuroreport 20(6), 543–547. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 60. Bedny M, Konkle T, Pelphrey K, Saxe R (2010) Pascual-Leone (2010) A Sensitive period for a multimodal response in human visual motion area MT/MST. Curr Biol 20(21): 1900–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Grill-Spector K, Malach R (2004) The human visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 27: 649–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO & Brewer AA (2007) Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron 56: 366–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Bensafi M, Zelano C, Johnson B, Mainland J, Khan R, Sobel N (2004) Olfaction: From Sniff to Percept. In The Cognitive Neurosciences, third edition, 259–80. (MIT press, Cambridge).
- 64.Poirier C., Collignon O., De Volder, A.G Renier, L Vanlierde, A Tranduy, D., etal. (2006) Auditory motion perception activates visual motion areas in early blind subjects. NeuroImage; 31(1), 279–85. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 65. Saenz M, Lewis LB, Huth AG, Fine I, Koch C (2008) Visual Motion Area MT+/V5 Responds to Auditory Motion in Human Sight-Recovery Subjects. J Neurosci 28: 5141–5148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Fiehler K & Rösler F (2010) Plasticity of multisensory dorsal stream functions: evidence from congenitally blind and sighted adults. Restor Neurol Neurosci 28(2): 193–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Amedi A, Stern WM, Camprodon JA, Bermpohl F, Merabet L, Rotman S, et al. (2007) Shape conveyed by visual-to-auditory sensory substitution activates the lateral occipital complex. Nat Neurosci 10: 687–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Lomber SG, Meredith MA, Kral A (2010) Cross-modal plasticity in specific auditory cortices underlies visual compensations in the deaf. Nat Neurosci Nov 13(11): 1421–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Meredith MA, Kryklywy J, McMillan AJ, Malhotra S, Lum-Tai R, Lomber SG (2011) Crossmodal reorganization in the early deaf switches sensory, but not behavioral roles of auditory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. May 24 108(21): 8856–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Haxby JV, Grady CL, Horwitz B, Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M, Carson RE, et al. (1991) Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 88: 1621–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M, Evans AC, Meyer E (1992) Functional localization and lateralization of human olfactory cortex. Nature 360: 339–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Brand G, Millot JL, Henquell D (2001) Complexity of olfactory lateralization processes revealed by functional imaging: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25(2): 159–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Royet JP & Plailly J (2004) Lateralization of olfactory processes. Chem Senses 29: 731–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Morrell LK & Salamy JG (1971) Hemispheric asymmetry of electrocortical responses to speech stimuli. Science 174: 164–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Geffen G (1976) Development of hemispheric specialization for speech perception. Cortex 12(4): 337–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Striem-Amit E, Cohen L, Dehaene S, Amedi A (2012) Reading with sounds: sensory substitution selectively activates the visual word form area in the blind. Neuron 76: 640–652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Savic I, Gulyas B, Larsson M, Roland P (2000) Olfactory functions are mediated by parallel and hierarchical processing. Neuron 26: 735–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Small DM, Gerber JC, Mak YE, Hummel T (2005) Differential neural responses evoked by orthonasal versus retronasal odorant perception in humans. Neuron 47, 593–605. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 79. Springer JA, Binder JR, Hammeke TA, Swanson SJ, Frost JA, Bellgowan PS, et al. (1999) Language dominance in neurologically normal and epilepsy subjects: a functional MRI study. Brain 122 (Pt 11): 2033–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Liégeois F, Connelly A, Cross JH, Boyd SG, Gadian DG, Vargha-Khadem F, et al. (2004) Language reorganization in children with early-onset lesions of the left hemisphere: an fMRI study. Brain 127(Pt 6): 1229–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.