Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013 Jan 16;37(0):10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.018. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.018

Striatal ups and downs: Their roles in vulnerability to addictions in humans

Marco Leyton a,**, Paul Vezina b,*
PMCID: PMC3743927  NIHMSID: NIHMS436830  PMID: 23333263

Abstract

Susceptibility to addictive behaviors has been related to both increases and decreases in striatal function. Both profiles have been reported in humans as well as in animal models. Yet, the mechanisms underlying these opposing effects and the manner in which they relate to the behavioral development and expression of addiction remain unclear. In the present review of human studies, we describe a number of factors that could influence whether striatal hyper- or hypo-function is observed and propose a model that integrates the influence of these opposite responses on the expression of addiction related behaviors. Central to this model is the role played by the presence versus absence of addiction related cues and their ability to regulate responding to abused drugs and other rewards. Striatal function and incentive motivational states are increased in the presence of these cues and decreased in their absence. Alternations between these states might account for the progressive narrowing of interests as addictions develop and point to relevant processes to target in treatment.

Keywords: Basal ganglia, Conditioning, Dopamine, Drug addiction, Drug self-administration, Functional magnetic resonance imaging, Positron emission tomography, Sensitization, Striatum

1. Introduction

Two frequently contrasted theories propose that the development of addiction related behaviors reflects the hyper- versus hypo-activation of limbic reward systems. The debate is not new (e.g., Wikler, 1948, 1973; Vogel et al., 1948). Nor are the positions irreconcilable. Recent evidence raises the possibility that the expression of hyper- versus hypo-active incentive motivational states might reflect, in significant part, the presence versus absence of addiction related cues (Leyton and Vezina, 2012; see also Anagnostaras and Robinson, 1996; Anagnostaras et al., 2002; Stewart and Vezina, 1988, 1991; Vezina and Leyton, 2009). The present review focuses on the evidence for these alternating states in humans, the possibility that individuals may differ in their susceptibility to them, and the role that addiction related cues play in their expression. Although considered in the human clinical setting, many of the ideas discussed here have been tested over the last thirty years in some detail in preclinical drug sensitization experiments. The processes identified in these studies could have particular bearing for our understanding of the role played by addiction related cues in the generation of subjective and behavioral states in humans. We thus begin with a brief review of this literature before turning to a systematic treatment of the evidence in humans.

2. Preclinical studies in laboratory animals

Psychostimulant drugs like amphetamine, cocaine, and nicotine have long been known to produce their behavioral activating and motivating effects by stimulating the mesoaccumbens dopamine (DA) system. Many preclinical studies, mostly in rodents, have studied the effects of repeated exposure to these drugs on biochemistry and behavior. Of the different consequences of drug exposure assessed, two have emerged that have particular relevance for our understanding of excessive drug taking: the development of sensitization to the behavioral stimulant and incentive motivational effects of drugs and the formation of conditioned associations between these drug effects and various environmental stimuli. Although separate phenomena, these two consequences of drug exposure are known to interact as outlined below. It is the nature of this interaction that may be particularly informative for understanding how addiction related cues can influence the generation of subjective and behavioral states in humans.

An extensive preclinical literature now indicates that repeated intermittent exposure to psychostimulant drugs enhances not only the locomotor and brain DA activating effects they produce but more importantly the amount of work animals will emit to obtain and self-administer the drug (Mendrek et al., 1998; Vezina, 2004; Vezina et al., 2007). These effects are persistent (they are observed weeks to months after drug exposure in rodents; Hamamura et al., 1991; Paulson et al., 1991; Suto et al., 2004; Vezina et al., 2002), there is evidence that they increase in magnitude with the passage of time (Vanderschuren and Kalivas, 2000; Vezina, 2007), and they are observed following intermittent exposure (Robinson and Becker, 1986; Zimmer et al., 2012), a pattern often associated with initial exposure to the drug and initiation of drug use. Together, these findings support the proposal that sensitization of mesoaccumbens DA neuron reactivity may underlie the transition from sporadic experimentation to more frequent drug use and substance related problems (Robinson and Berridge, 1993, 2003).

An equally longstanding preclinical literature supports the importance of conditioned associations between stimulant drug effects and environmental contextual stimuli in drug seeking and self-administration (Stewart et al., 1984). The ability of drug paired stimuli to elicit conditioned locomotion (Stewart and Eikelboom, 1987) and forebrain DA release (Aragona et al., 2009; Di Ciano et al., 1998; Duvauchelle et al., 2000; Ito et al., 2000) is well established. Importantly, environmental stimuli previously paired with a psychostimulant drug slow extinction of responding for the drug (Tran-Nguyen et al., 1998) and reinstate drug seeking (de Wit and Stewart, 1981) in a manner that parallels their effects on DA transmission in the nucleus accumbens and amygdala (Weiss et al., 2000). The ability of these stimuli to reinstate drug seeking is long-lasting (Ciccocioppo et al., 2004) and becomes more intense with time (Grimm et al., 2001).

Because repeated systemic drug injections are administered in the presence of multiple environmental stimuli, the conditions are ripe for the simultaneous development of sensitization and conditioning of stimulant drug effects and for these two forms of plasticity to interact. While sensitization is known to develop non-associatively (Singer et al., 2009; Vezina and Stewart, 1990), there is evidence that its expression can come to be controlled by environmental stimuli previously paired or unpaired with the drug (Anagnostaras and Robinson, 1996; Anagnostaras et al., 2002; Stewart and Vezina, 1988, 1991; Vezina and Leyton, 2009). Thus, rats previously exposed to the drug in one environment exhibit sensitized behavioral responses in this environment while rats previously exposed to the drug elsewhere do not. Indeed, rats that previously received the drug elsewhere show levels of responding on tests for sensitization that are comparable to those of rats administered the drug for the first time. This control over the expression of behavioral sensitization may be mediated, at least for contextual stimuli, by activity in a ventral hippocampus - nucleus accumbens – ventral pallidum – ventral tegmental area neuronal loop that regulates DA neuron firing in the latter site (Lodge and Grace, 2008).

Much of the evidence for the ability of environmental stimuli to control the expression of sensitization comes from experiments measuring locomotion (above references) although similar effects have been reported for drug-induced nucleus accumbens DA over-flow (Guillory et al., 2006; Reid et al., 1996). Importantly, such conditioned environmental stimuli have also been shown to control the expression of enhanced amphetamine self-administration and drug-induced reinstatement in rats previously exposed to nicotine (Cortright et al., 2012), again underscoring the critical role these stimuli play in the expression of enhanced drug self-administration and drug seeking.

The above preclinical findings notwithstanding, there has been some debate as to their generalizability to the human clinical arena. For example, no change or even reduced rather than augmented striatal responses to drugs have been reported in a number of influential studies of psychostimulant exposure conducted in drug self-administering non-human primates and addicted human subjects (e.g., Bradberry, 2007; Volkow et al., 1997). This has led to the proposal that increased DA reactivity associated with drug sensitization is of limited value to the human condition as a mechanism for drug abuse and other forms of pathology. We assess the merits of this argument below by reviewing the results of a large number of studies aimed at deciphering the effects of drugs and drug associated cues in humans. A number of factors emerge that may have potential importance for understanding how motivated behaviors are generated. Central among these is the presence versus absence of addiction related cues and their ability to regulate responding to abused drugs and other rewards. This factor in particular can facilitate the integration of a previously disparate group of findings in the animal and human literatures alike.

3. Studies in humans: subjective and behavioral states

3.1. Effects of cues

In substance abusers, exposure to stimulant drug associated cues elicits a wide range of subjective, behavioral and physiological responses (Carter and Tiffany, 1999; Childress et al., 1988; O’Brien et al., 1990). That these responses are drug-like is consistent with their ability to elicit incentive motivational states associated with the drug (Stewart et al., 1984; Robinson and Berridge, 2003)1.

The elicited states include a narrowing of attentional focus toward the rewards and an increased propensity to pursue and approach them. The critical processes associated with activity in the striatum need not necessarily be conscious (Fischman, 1989; Tiffany, 1990; Lamb et al., 1991; Winkielman et al., 2005; Childress et al., 2008; Field et al., 2009; Perkins, 2009; Berridge, 2012; Waters et al., 2012); conscious craving may be more closely related to activity in cortical structures (Goldstein et al., 2009; de Lange et al., 2011). Nonetheless, self-reported craving is a commonly used proxy and ecological momentary assessments acquired with real-time electronic diaries confirm that exposure to drug cues, and their elicitation of craving states, commonly occur in the minutes and hours before new bouts of stimulant drug use (Epstein et al., 2009). Similarly, in laboratory studies, craving and reward-seeking behavior have been reported to increase following exposure to cues associated with amphetamine (Culbertson et al., 2010; Tolivar et al., 2010), cocaine (Childress et al., 1988, 1993), alcohol (George et al., 2001; Bragulat et al., 2008), cigarettes (Droungas et al., 1995; Carter and Tiffany, 2001; Wray et al., 2011), heroin (Fatseas et al., 2011; Zhao et al., 2012a), and natural rewards such as food (Jansen, 1998; Kelley and Berridge, 2002; Mahler and de Wit, 2010) and sex (Conaglen and Evans, 2006; Kim and Zauberman, in press).

Cues have more potent effects when subjects know that there will soon be an opportunity to use the drug (Carter and Tiffany, 2001; Dar et al., 2005; Juliano and Brandon, 1998). These, of course, are the usual circumstances under which the cues appear in the natural environment. A striking illustration of this phenomenon was recently reported in flight attendants. Smokers on either short (3–5.5 h) or long flights (8–13 h) developed cigarette cravings toward the end of the trip. Cravings at the end of the short flight were as strong as cravings at the end of the long flight and substantially higher than those seen at the shorter time-point during the long flight (Dar et al., 2010).

Drug related cues can also elicit behavioral effects. These include conditioned place preferences (Childs and de Wit, 2009, in press) and attentional biases (Cox et al., 2006; Hogarth et al., 2008; Field et al., 2009; Little et al., 2012), conditioned reinforcement (Foltin and Haney, 2000), accelerated initiation of drug use (Herman, 1974), as well as increased drug seeking (Panlilio et al., 2005; Hogarth et al., 2007) and self-administration (Herman, 1974; Droungas et al., 1995; Mucha et al., 1998; Hogarth et al., 2010).

3.2. Effects of drugs

As discussed above, a large animal literature indicates that the repeated administration of abused drugs can alter their effects. In humans, the best established change observed following repeated drug exposure has been transient tolerance to the subjective effects of stimulants (Brauer et al., 1996) and to the depressant effects of opiates and benzodiazepines (Hug, 1972)2. By comparison, the possibility that drug sensitization might occur in humans has been considered more controversial. Initial evidence came from observations made in the U.S. and Japan following the Second World War during episodes of heightened abuse of amphetamine-like drugs. Retrospective histories from this period suggested that repeated exposure to high doses of amphetamines (typically 100 mg or more) could lead to psychotic symptoms, including hallucinations and delusions (Connell, 1958; Ellinwood, 1967; Griffith et al., 1972; Sato, 1992; Sato et al., 1992). These effects could be reproduced in laboratory settings (Angrist & Gershon, 1970; Bell, 1973). The time course leading to the first psychotic episode was found to vary between individuals, an effect possibly related to dose, frequency of use, the co-abuse of other substances, and the presence of pre-existing vulnerability traits. Strikingly, periods of substance abuse were followed in some individuals by a long-lasting susceptibility to the re-emergence of psychotic symptoms after re-exposure to a much lower dose of the drug (Sato, 1992; Sato et al., 1992).

Although intriguing, the above reports did not provide direct experimental evidence that repeated drug exposure could produce sensitization-like phenomena in humans. This evidence has been reported only more recently. In six of seven controlled laboratory studies in which participants received a minimum of 20 mg (p.o.) of d-amphetamine per session, sensitization of the drug’s energizing effects was observed (Table 1). In the most recent study, evidence that this effect could come under environmental control was also seen. Subjects who received two doses of d-amphetamine in the same room reported a sensitized response to the second dose whereas those who received the second dose in a distinctively different room showed, if anything, evidence of tolerance (Childs and de Wit, in press).

Table 1.

Dose-dependent development of sensitized responding to repeated d-amphetamine in healthy human subjects.

Amphetamine dose No. of doses Sensitization? Reference
5.0 mg, p.o. 5 doses No – mood, drug tablets chosen Johanson and Uhlenhuth (1981)
10.0 mg, p.o. 6 doses No – speech rate, smoking, stimulant effects, liking Kelly et al. (1991)
20.0 mg, p.o. 2 doses No – subjective and psychomotor effects Wachtel and de Wit (1999)
~20 mg, p.o. (0.25 mg/kg) 2 doses Yes – energy, eye-blink, mood, speech rate Strakowski et al. (1996)
~20 mg, p.o. (0.25 mg/kg) 3 doses Yes – energy, eye-blink Strakowski and Sax (1998)
~20 mg, p.o. (0.25 mg/kg) 3 doses Yes – energy, euphoria Strakowski et al. (2001)
~20 mg, p.o. (0.30 mg/kg) 4 doses Yes – energy, eye-blink Boileau et al. (2006)
~20 mg, p.o. (0.30 mg/kg) 4 doses Yes – energy, euphoria O’Daly et al. (2011)
20.0 mg, p.o. 2 doses Yes – stimulation, craving Childs and de Wit (in press)

It is noteworthy that the time courses for sensitization- and tolerance-like phenomena are different3. Whereas sensitization is a long-lasting, possibly even permanent phenomenon (Robinson and Becker, 1986; Boileau et al., 2006), tolerance is more transient (Vogel et al., 1948; Hug, 1972; Brauer et al., 1996). Indeed, a major precipitant of drug overdose and mortality stems from the ability of drug seeking states to be elicited long after tolerance has dissipated (Merrall et al., 2010).

3.3. Effects of cues and cues + drugs in different subject populations

In the sections below, we review the effects of drug cues and cues plus drugs. These effects are examined separately in healthy subjects without addictions, in subjects at risk for addictions, and in subjects with substance use disorders. Distinguishing between these populations is necessary as drugs and drug cues elicit different effects in different individuals. Since the effects of both acute and repeated drug exposure can interact with the particular characteristics an individual presents, they can provide insights into the factors that regulate the development and expression of motivated behaviors directed at obtaining and self-administering drugs of abuse. Indeed, as often noted, only some of the individuals who experiment with drugs develop a substance use disorder (Tsuang et al., 1998; Zinkernagel et al., 2001; Anthony, 2002; Agrawal et al., 2012; Kendler et al., 2012). Factors that have been identified to influence progression to addiction include personality traits (Ayduk et al., 2000; Conrod et al., 2000; Tarter et al., 2003), early life histories (Hyman et al., 2006; Enoch et al., 2010), ever-changing socio-cultural norms (Nutt, 2012), individual differences in drug specific metabolic enzymes (Ferguson and Tyndale, 2011), and additional heritable factors with unclear mechanisms. An implication of these observations for the study of addiction and addiction related processes is the need to identify and characterize effects that might occur preferentially within a subset of individuals (see also Saunders and Robinson, this issue).

4. Subjects without addictions: striatal activations

4.1. Effects of cues

Exposure to reward related events consistently activates the striatum in healthy humans (Knutson and Cooper, 2005). This has been studied in greatest detail in relation to monetary reward. In these studies, numerous types of stimuli are presented. These include (i) familiar cues that subjects already know are associated with the presence or absence of rewards, (ii) previously neutral cues that subjects learn about during the study, (iii) cues indicating that a reward will be presented either after passive waiting or after the emission of an operant response, and (iv) the reward itself. Each of these features can affect the magnitude of the striatal response and whether it is observed primarily within the ventral or dorsal striatum (O’Doherty et al., 2004; Knutson and Cooper, 2005). The focus of the present review is on how the magnitude of these striatal responses is influenced by individual and group differences.

In addition to monetary reward, healthy human subjects have been reported to show striatal activations following exposure to cues associated with food (Small et al., 2001; Beaver et al., 2006; Hommer et al., in press; Demos et al., 2012; Tang et al., 2012), sex (Hamann et al., 2004; Cloutier et al., 2008; Demos et al., 2012), and alcohol (Seo et al., 2011)4. There is evidence that these fMRI measured cerebral blood flow (CBF) responses may have been accompanied by an increase in DA release (Box 1). For example, striatal DA release measured in healthy subjects by positron emission tomography (PET) can correlate with fMRI measured activations (Schott et al., 2008). More importantly, evidence of DA release has been observed in healthy human subjects playing video games (Koepp et al., 1998) and following exposure to cues previously paired with monetary reward (Zald et al., 2004; Schott et al., 2008; Martin-Soelch et al., 2011; cf, Hakyemez et al., 2008), food (Volkow et al., 2002; Small et al., 2003), alcohol (Yoder et al., 2009) and amphetamine (Boileau et al., 2007).

Box 1.

During the past few decades, two main tools have been developed to measure activity in living human brain. In functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, regional brain activity is assessed by measuring changes in cerebral blood flow (CBF). As with all living tissues, increased activity requires increased blood flow to supply needed oxygen. Magnetically captured fMRI signals respond to changes in blood flow by exploiting the paramagnetic and diamagnetic properties of deoxygenated and oxygenated hemoglobin, respectively. Temporal resolution ranges from 100 ms to 2 s depending on whether a single brain slice or the whole brain is sampled. Anatomical resolution ranges from <1 to 3 mm3, depending on the magnet size (Hernandez et al., 2001). This method lacks neurochemical specificity.

Positron emission tomography (PET) can also be used to measure brain activity, but it is based on different principles. Subjects are administered a radioactively labeled substance that can cross the blood brain barrier. The decaying tracer emits positrons that typically travel 0.2–7 mm before colliding with an electron. The collision produces gamma rays that travel in diametrically opposite directions leading to their simultaneous activation of coincidence detectors positioned around the subject’s head. The subsequently processed signals provide information about magnitude with temporal and spatial properties. Labeled water permits the measure of CBF. The use of labeled tracers such as [11C]raclopride (a D2/3 DA receptor antagonist) permits estimation of the availability of D2/3 DA receptors. When extracellular DA levels are increased, the availability of DA receptors for [11C]raclopride is reduced. Although temporal (20 to 30 min) and anatomical (cm3 range) resolution are modest, the method is well validated (Laruelle, 2000; Doudet and Holden, 2003).

The magnitude of the cue-induced DA response might vary with the expected certainty that a reward will be obtained. For example, in nonhuman primates, the largest increases in reward cue-induced DA cell firing are seen under conditions of maximal uncertainty (Fiorillo et al., 2003). Recent evidence raises the possibility that this effect of uncertainty can occur in humans as well: patients with Parkinson’s disease exhibit a larger placebo-induced DA response if they are informed that the chance of receiving L-DOPA medication is 75% as compared to 100% (Lidstone et al., 2010)5.

4.2. Effects of cues + drugs

As seen in laboratory animals, there is evidence for reciprocal interactions between drugs and reward related cues with each modulating the response to the other. In healthy human subjects, this has been observed most clearly in two studies where the dopaminergic effects of methylphenidate were augmented by the presence of salient appetitive cues (Volkow et al., 2002, 2004). In the first study, conducted in healthy food-deprived subjects (16–20 h abstinent), the combination of a low dose of methylphenidate (20 mg, p.o.) and food cues (visual, olfactory, taste) elicited greater striatal DA release and greater self-reported hunger than either alone (Volkow et al., 2002). Individual differences in DA release correlated with self-reported hunger and desire for food. In the second study, methylphenidate (20 mg, p.o.) elicited measureable striatal DA release only when it was paired with a salient mathematics task in which subjects could earn a monetary reward. The greater the DA release, the more interesting subjects reported the task to be (Volkow et al., 2004).

A third study provided the first explicit test of whether repeated drug administration could lead to DA sensitization in humans (Fig. 1). Healthy subjects were exposed to three doses of d-amphetamine (0.3 mg/kg, p.o.) on an every other day schedule. Following a two-week break, a fourth dose was given. The DA response to this fourth dose was significantly greater than that elicited by the first dose. A fifth dose, given a full year later, yielded an even larger effect (Boileau et al., 2006). Notably, all doses of d-amphetamine were administered in the same environment (the PET apparatus), rendering the results obtained consistent with environment-specific sensitization. While this study did not determine whether DA sensitization could also have been expressed if the amphetamine had been administered elsewhere, two recent studies conducted in non-dependent stimulant drug users are consistent with the proposal that the presence versus absence of drug associated stimuli can indeed influence the magnitude of drug-induced DA responses. In the first study, individual differences in cocaine-induced increases in extracellular DA were predicted by lifetime histories of stimulant drug use on the street: the greater the past drug use, the greater the DA response (Cox et al., 2009). In this study, participants prepared, manipulated, and self-administered the drug in their usual fashion. That is, cocaine associated cues were clearly present and engaged with. By comparison, in a second very similar study, healthy, non-dependent stimulant drug users were administered a disguised dose of d-amphetamine. In this case, individual differences in DA release were negatively correlated with drug use: the greater the past drug use, the smaller the DA response (Casey et al., 2012). Since similar effects have been well characterized in studies conducted in laboratory animals (Anagnostaras and Robinson, 1996; Anagnostaras et al., 2002; Stewart and Vezina, 1988, 1991; Vezina and Leyton, 2009), a tempting though speculative interpretation of these findings is that the presence versus absence of discrete and contextual drug associated cues modulated the response to the unconditioned drug stimulus. Thus, the presence of salient reward related cues might enable enhanced dopaminergic responding to a pharmacological challenge; the absence of such cues might prevent the expression of enhanced DA responses.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Amphetamine-induced DA sensitization in humans. Healthy male subjects received five doses of d-amphetamine (0.3 mg/kg, p.o.) while laying in a PET scanner. The first three doses were administered every second day. The fourth dose was given following a two-week abstinence period. The fifth dose was given one year later. The figure depicts the effects on striatal DA release, as indexed by changes in [11C]raclopride binding, at dose 1 ((A) n = 10), dose 4 ((B) n = 10), and dose 5 ((C) n = 7). The higher the color coded t value, the greater the change in [11C]raclopride binding. As illustrated in (D), linear regression analyses indicated that successive doses led to progressively larger responses. From Boileau et al. (2006).

4.3. Age related differences: implications for development

An emerging literature is drawing attention to differences in striatal responding to reward related stimuli in adolescents (13–15 years of age) relative to young adults (early 20s). For example, adolescents have been reported to exhibit larger striatal activation than adults when presented with a stimulus that signals the opportunity to respond for money (Geier et al., 2010) and in response to receipt of the reward (Ernst et al., 2005; Galvan et al., 2006). Moreover, among the adolescents, the greater the striatal response to these cues, the higher their sensation seeking personality trait scores and self-reported excitement (Bjork et al., 2008a)6. These age-related responses have been proposed to account for developmental differences in risk-taking and reward-seeking behaviors (Spear, 2011; Ernst and Fudge, 2009; Somerville et al., 2010). Indeed, there is evidence that these striatal effects have predictive validity. For example, among healthy undergraduates (n = 58), the larger the nucleus accumbens response to food cues, the more weight subjects gained at follow-up six months later; the larger the response to sex cues, the greater the amount of sexual activity (Demos et al., 2012).

5. Subjects at risk for addictions: striatal activations

Groups of individuals can be categorized according to their risk for addiction. Among the best established predictors are (i) a dense family history of substance use problems (Dawson et al., 1992; Merikangas et al., 1998; Stoltenberg et al., 1998), (ii) externalizing behavioral characteristics and impulsive, sensation seeking personality traits (Krueger, 1999; Kendler et al., 1997, 2003, Tarter et al., 2003), and (iii) subjective and behavioral responses to a drug challenge (Schuckit, 1980; de Wit and Phillips, 2012).

5.1. Effects of cues

A small literature describes responses to rewards and reward related cues in subjects at risk for substance use disorders (see Tables 2 and 3). For example, compared to healthy low risk controls, larger striatal responses have been observed in subjects at familial risk for alcoholism when performing the Iowa Gambling Task (Acheson et al., 2009) and following exposure to alcohol odors (Kareken et al., 2004; Oberlin et al., 2012). In comparison, in studies where unfamiliar or otherwise neutral monetary reward cues were presented, high-risk populations exhibit smaller striatal responses than healthy controls (Andrews et al., 2011; Schneider et al., 2012; Yau et al., 2012).

Table 2.

fMRI BOLD striatal activations observed in human subjects in the presence and absence of reward associated cues. Subjects were individuals with susceptibilities to, or with current addiction disorders.

Subjects Task/challenge Results Comment Reference
Cues Present
  • 30 cocaine dependent patients in treatment

  • 36 healthy controls

Autobiographical scripts of drug use, stress and neutral experiences Greater activation to drug scripts in male patients; greater activation to stress in female patients Hospitalized patients receiving treatment for two weeks Potenza et al. (2012)
  • 24 heroin dependent patients in short-term abstinence (1 month)

  • 20 healthy controls

Images of heroin use or neutral stimuli Greater activation in response to heroin images in patients In these short-term abstinent patients, the longer the abstinence, the greater the activation Li et al. (2012)
Heroin addicts
  • 17, short-term abstinence (1 month)

  • 17 long-term abstinence (1 year)

Images of heroin use or neutral stimuli Heroin images activated striatum in short-term abstinent subjects and deactivated it in long-term abstinent subjects Subjects able to sustain long-term abstinence might be able to inhibit responses to drug cues Lou et al. (2012)
  • 15 pathological gamblers

  • 16 healthy controls

Playing cards associated with monetary reward Higher activation in gamblers van Holst et al. (2012)
Heavy drinkers
  • with (n=17) and

  • without (n=13) family history of alcoholism

Alcohol odor Higher activation in subjects with higher antisocial traits No effect of family history Modest sample size; both groups consisted of heavy drinkers (18 drinks/week) with moderately high alcohol use problem scores Oberlin et al. (2012)
  • 20 cocaine dependent patients

  • 20 healthy controls

MID Higher activation to reward notification in patients; higher response predicted worse treatment outcome Treatment-seeking patients; the MID can be demanding, requiring sustained attention Jia et al. (2011)
  • 326 heavy drinkers

Alcohol taste cue Positive correlation with alcohol use problems No control group Claus et al. (2011)
  • 11 heavy drinkers

  • 12 light drinkers

Alcohol images Higher activation in heavy drinkers Small sample size Ihssen et al. (2011)
Heavy drinkers
  • with (n=14) and

  • without (n=12) family history of alcoholism

Alcohol odors No group differences Small sample size; both groups consisted of heavy drinkers (17–18 drinks/week) with moderately high alcohol use problem scores Kareken et al. (2010)
  • 15 subjects with family history of alcoholism

  • 19 healthy controls

Iowa Gambling Task Higher activation in family history positive group Modest sample size Acheson et al. (2009)
  • 15 heroin dependent patients

  • 12 healthy controls

Heroin versus neutral videos Higher activation in patients to heroin cues; lower responses to neutral cues Modest sample size Yang et al. (2009)
  • 12 opiate dependent patients

  • 17 healthy controls

Heroin versus pleasant versus neutral pictures Only patients showed significant response to heroin cues; only controls showed significant response to pleasant non-drug cues Modest sample size Zijlstra et al. (2009)
  • 23 alcohol dependent patients

  • 23 healthy controls

MID Higher activation to reward notification in patients Hospitalized patients receiving treatment Bjork et al. (2008b)
  • 37 heavy drinkers

Alcohol taste cue Positive correlation with craving and alcohol use problems No control group Filbey et al. (2008)
  • 16 detoxified alcohol dependent subjects

  • 16 healthy controls

Alcohol images + MID Alcoholics showed greater activation to anticipation of alcohol cues and lower activation to non-alcoholic cues Modest sample size Wrase et al. (2007)
  • 6 detoxified alcohol dependent patients

  • 6 healthy controls

Alcohol images No effect in striatum Small sample size; hospitalized patients receiving treatment Lingford-Hughes et al. (2006)
  • 12 pathological gamblers

  • 12 healthy controls

Card guessing game with electronic cards Lower activation in gamblers Small sample size; cues possibly not evocative of real cards Reuter et al. (2005)
  • 10 alcohol dependent subjects

  • 10 healthy controls

Alcohol sip + images Activation in alcoholics, not controls; positive correlation between cue-induced craving and accumbens activation in alcoholics only Small sample size Myrick et al. (2004)
  • 10 abstinent alcohol dependent subjects

  • 10 healthy controls

Alcohol images Greater activation in alcoholics; strong activations predicted relapse Small sample size Grüsser et al. (2004)
  • 4 alcohol dependent subjects

  • 4 healthy controls

Alcohol images Higher activation in alcoholics; strong activations predicted rapid relapse Small sample size Braus et al. (2001)
  • 17 cocaine abusers

  • 14 healthy controls

4-min videos of cocaine use, sex; neutral activity Higher activity in cocaine abusers Lower activity than controls in response to sex video Garavan et al. (2000)
Cues Absent
  • 30 cocaine addicted subjects

  • 28 former cocaine addicted subjects

  • 31 healthy controls

Unfamiliar Domino game in which subjects gain or lose chips Lower activation to gains in former cocaine addicted versus healthy and current addicted subjects; no difference between control and current addicted subjects Hyatt et al. (2012)
  • 14 pathological gamblers

  • 14 healthy controls

MID Lower activation in gamblers during prospect and anticipation Modest sample size Balodis et al. (2012)
  • 31 adolescents with problem substance use

  • 266 healthy controls

MID Lower activation in substance abusers during anticipation phase Schneider et al. (2012)
  • 20 children of alcoholics

  • 20 healthy controls

MID Lower activation in children of alcoholics during anticipation phase Only seen in subgroup that had not yet developed problem drinking Yau et al. (2012)
  • 16 pathological gamblers

  • 16 healthy controls

Delay discounting task Negative correlation with gambling severity Between group effects not reported Miedl et al. (2012)
  • 43 adolescent smokers

  • 43 healthy controls

MID Lower activation in smokers during anticipation phase Same response seen in subjects who smoked ≤ 10 cigarettes; thus, blunted response may be vulnerability trait Peters et al. (2011)
  • 30 subjects with family history of alcoholism

  • 19 healthy controls

MID Lower activation in family history positive subjects during anticipation phase; higher activation during the prospect phase Andrews et al. (2011)
  • 14 heavy drinkers

  • 14 light drinkers

Intravenous ethanol (BAC=0.08) Lower activation in heavy drinkers Modest sample size Gilman et al. (2012)
Parkinson’s disease patients
  • with (n=9)

  • without (n=9) impulse-control disorders

Balloon Analog Risk Task Lower activation in patients with impulse-control problems Modest sample size Rao et al. (2010)
  • 19 problem gamblers

  • 19 smokers

  • 19 healthy controls

Monetary gain following exposure to previously neutral cues No group differences de Ruiter et al. (2009)
  • 19 detoxed alcoholics

  • 19 healthy controls

MID Lower activation in alcoholics during anticipation phase In alcoholics, low striatal activation predicted high impulsivity scores Beck et al. (2009)
  • 13 children of alcoholics with high sensation seeking scores

  • 13 healthy adolescents

MID Greater activation in high sensation seekers during anticipation phase Modest sample size Bjork et al. (2008a)
  • 16 detoxified alcohol dependent subjects

  • 16 healthy controls

Alcohol images + MID Alcoholics showed lower activations to MID monetary anticipation High striatal responses also seen in response to alcohol cues Wrase et al. (2007)
  • 17 cocaine abusers

  • 14 healthy controls

4-min videos of cocaine use, sex; neutral activity Lower activity than controls in response to sex video Higher activity than controls in response to cocaine video Garavan et al. (2000)

BAC: blood alcohol concentration. MID: monetary incentive delay task.

Table 3.

PET [11C]raclopride striatal responses observed in human subjects in the presence and absence of reward associated cues. Subjects were individuals with susceptibilities to, or with current addiction disorders.

Subjects Task/challenge Results Comment Reference
Cues Present
  • 12 pathological gamblers

  • 12 healthy controls

Slot machine gambling task Higher DA response in severe gamblers High DA response predicted greater gambling problem severity Joutsa et al. (2012)
  • 13 pathological gamblers

  • 12 healthy controls

Undisguised amphetamine pills Higher DA release in gamblers High DA response predicted more desire to gamble and greater difficulty restraining from gambling Payer et al. (2012)
Obese subjects
  • with (n=10) or

  • without (n=8) binge eating disorder

Food smell and taste Higher DA release in binge eaters Modest sample size; DA release correlated with binge eating problems Wang et al. (2011)
Parkinson’s disease patients
  • with (n=11) or

  • without (n=7) impulse-control problems

Various reward related cues (images of appetizing food, sex, gambling, drugs etc.) Higher DA release in impulsive patients Modest sample size O’Sullivan et al. (2011)
  • 18 pathological gamblers

  • 16 healthy controls

Iowa Gambling Task High DA release predicted greater excitement No significant effect of group Linnet et al. (2011)
  • 16 pathological gamblers

  • 15 healthy controls

Iowa Gambling Task High DA release predicted poorer performance No significant effect of group Linnet et al. (2010)
Heavy drinkers at
  • high (n=13) or

  • low-risk (n=13) for alcoholism

Alcohol ingestion Higher DA response in subjects at high risk Risk defined by (i) SHAS scores and (ii) reward-seeking personality traits Setiawan et al. (2010)
Parkinson’s disease patients
  • with (n=7) or

  • without (n=7) pathological gambling

L-DOPA + gambling task with cards Higher DA release in gamblers Small sample size Steeves et al. (2009)
  • 20 cocaine dependent patients

Methylphenidate (0, 20 mg, p.o.) with or without cocaine videos Lower DA response with drug videos Cues did not predict receipt of reward; possible reward prediction error; no comparison group Volkow et al. (2008)
Parkinson’s disease patients
  • with (n=8) or

  • without (n=8) substance dependence

L-DOPA medication Higher DA release in substance dependent patients Small sample size Evans et al. (2006)
Cues Absent
  • 11 patients with schizophrenia and a substance use disorder

  • 15 healthy controls

Amphetamine (0, 0.3 mg/kg, iv) Lower DA release in patients Thompson et al. (in press)
  • 16 cannabis dependent patients

  • 16 healthy controls

Amphetamine 0, 0.3 mg/kg, iv) No group differences Earlier age of onset of cannabis use predicted lower DA response Urban et al. (2012)
  • 15 patients with bulimia nervosa

  • 14 healthy controls

Methylphenidate (0, 60 mg, po) Lower DA release in patients Low DA release predicted more frequent binge eating Broft et al. (2012)
  • 16 heroin dependent patients

  • 16 healthy controls

Methylphenidate (0, 60 mg, po) Lower DA release in patients Martinez et al. (2012)
  • 24 cocaine dependent patients

  • 24 healthy controls

Methylphenidate (0, 60 mg, po) Lower DA release in patients Low DA release predicted poor treatment outcome Martinez et al. (2011)
  • 16 subjects at high familial risk for addictions

  • 15 subjects at low familial risk matched on drug use

  • 17 healthy subjects,

d-amphetamine (0, 0.3 mg/kg, po) Lower DA release in high-risk subjects Low DA release predicted by lifetime history of drug use Casey et al. (2012)
  • 24 cocaine dependent patients

  • 24 healthy controls

Methylphenidate (0, 60 mg, po) Lower DA release in patients Low DA release predicted choice of lab cocaine over money Martinez et al. (2007)
  • 20 alcohol dependent patients

  • 20 healthy controls

Methylphenidate (0, 0.5 mg/kg, iv) Lower DA release in patients Volkow et al. (2007)
  • 15 alcoholic dependent patients

  • 15 healthy controls

Amphetamine (0, 0.3 mg/kg, iv) Lower DA release in patients Martinez et al. (2005)
  • 20 cocaine dependent patients

  • 23 healthy controls

Methylphenidate (0, 0.5 mg/kg, iv) Lower DA release in patients Volkow et al. (1997)

DA: dopamine. SHAS: Subjective High Assessment Scale.

5.2. Effects of cues + drugs

There is evidence that the effects of drugs and drug associated cues can interact in subjects at risk for addictions. In non-dependent heavy drinking cigarette chippers, for example, alcohol ingestion was found to increase the striatal response to cigarette cues (King et al., 2010). Conversely, there is evidence that cues can augment the effects of drugs. In subjects at elevated risk for addictions, striatal DA responses were augmented relative to low-risk subjects when the substance was ingested in the usual fashion (Setiawan et al., 2010) but diminished when the drug was administered in the absence of drug related cues (Casey et al., 2012). The blunted response reflected both a familial trait and an effect of past drug use: the greater the lifetime history of drug use, the smaller the DA response (Casey et al., 2012). The effects of familial trait and past drug use were independent. This was demonstrated in two ways. First, a control group was included consisting of stimulant drug using subjects matched on substance use to the high-risk subjects but lacking a family history of drug use problems. The high risk subjects with a family history of substance use disorders exhibited lower DA release than either this “low risk” drug using group or stimulant drug naïve subjects. Second, including drug use histories as a potential confounding variable in the statistical analyses did not diminish the contribution of family history. That is, both family and drug use histories produced the same effect but acted as independent contributors.

6. Subjects with substance use disorders: striatal activations

6.1. Effects of cues

Two recent meta-analyses independently concluded that the striatum is consistently activated by exposure to drug-related cues in subjects meeting diagnostic criteria for substance use disorders (Chase et al., 2011; Tang et al., 2012). These responses are stable (Schacht et al., 2011) and elevated, as compared to non-substance abusers. For example, compared to light social drinkers, dependent drinkers have been reported to exhibit greater alcohol cue-induced striatal activation (Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010; Ihssen et al., 2011): the greater the striatal response, the greater the cue-induced attentional biases (Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2011) and the more severe the obsessive-compulsive drinking symptoms (Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010). Similarly, in a large study of 326 heavy drinkers, the greater the alcohol cue-induced striatal activation, the greater the severity of alcohol use problems (Claus et al., 2011)7.

There is evidence that the above striatal activations may have been accompanied by an increase in DA release. Changes in PET tracer binding values indicative of striatal DA release have been observed following exposure to cues associated with cocaine (Volkow et al., 2006; Wong et al., 2006; Fotros et al., 2012) and heroin (Zijlstra et al., 2008). The greater the cue-induced DA release, the greater the craving (Volkow et al., 2006; Wong et al., 2006; Zijlstra et al., 2008; Fotros et al., 2012).

As seen in other populations, there is also evidence in those with substance use disorders that striatal activations are blunted rather than augmented when addiction related cues are absent. Compared to control subjects, blunted striatal activations occur in response to pictures of food in alcoholics (Ihssen et al., 2011) and to unfamiliar or otherwise neutral monetary reward cues in smokers (Peters et al., 2011) and detoxified alcoholics (Wrase et al., 2007; Beck et al., 2009; cf Bjork et al., 2008b).

6.2. Effects of cues + drugs

In subjects with substance use disorders, stimulant drug-induced striatal DA responses have been reported to be markedly reduced when compared to those observed in healthy controls (Volkow et al., 1997, 2007; Martinez et al., 2005, 2007, 2011, 2012; Wang et al., 2012; Thompson et al., in press; cf Urban et al., 2012; see Tables 2 and 3). These reductions may possibly aggravate the clinical picture. The lower the DA response, the greater the stimulant drug self-administration observed in separate sessions where the drug and its associated cues were made available (Martinez et al., 2007) and the worse the clinical outcome at follow-up (Martinez et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2012).

Notably, however, in all of the above studies, DA release was measured in the absence of drug cues. This raises the possibility that, even in late stage addiction, the reduced DA responses observed reflect, at least in part, either the absence of drug associated stimuli necessary to enable the expression of enhanced dopaminergic responding or the presence of drug unpaired stimuli capable of inhibiting this response (Vezina and Leyton, 2009). We are aware of only one study that has tested this hypothesis explicitly. In this study, cocaine dependent subjects were administered amphetamine on test sessions with or without drug cues present (videos of actors simulating drug use). Compared to the test session conducted without drug cues, the presence of drug cues actually diminished the DA response further (Volkow et al., 2008), an effect opposite in direction to what had been predicted by the authors. This observation nonetheless adds to the evidence that environmental cues can modulate the pharmacological effects of a stimulant drug challenge. Moreover, as the authors noted, since the cues did not genuinely predict that drug would become available, there may have been a reward prediction error associated with diminished DA release (Schultz et al., 1997; Yoder et al., 2009). This interpretation, though, remains speculative until more studies explicitly testing the proposition are reported. Other factors that might lead to decreased drug-evoked DA release in substance dependent populations include neurotoxic effects of extensive drug use (Little et al., 2003, 2009) and pre-existing risk traits (Casey et al., 2012). Methodological limitations may also be relevant. As noted by Narendran and Martinez (2008), reduced dopaminergic responding could also reflect decreases in D2 or D3 DA receptor affinity, decreases in the ratio of D3 to D2 DA receptors, or an increase in resting baseline DA levels. Preliminary attempts to address some of these possibilities, though, suggest that stimulant drug addicts, tested under the same conditions as in the above studies, have lower rather than higher resting levels of DA (Martinez et al., 2009) and higher rather than lower D3 DA receptor levels at least in D3 DA receptor rich brain regions such as the midbrain and globus pallidus (Boileau et al., 2012).

7. Subjects with non-substance addictions – gambling and binge eating disorders: striatal activations

Gambling (Frascella et al., 2010; Leeman and Potenza, 2012) and binge eating disorders8 (Davis et al., 2011; Gearhardt et al., 2011) have been proposed to be forms of addiction. Both groups are at elevated risk for substance use disorders, yet some of the affected individuals do not use drugs or alcohol extensively. Studies in these populations with non-substance addictions thus have the potential to shed light on mechanisms relevant to perturbed reward seeking behaviors in isolation from the effects produced by drugs themselves.

In fMRI studies, increased striatal activations have been observed in problem gamblers, as compared to non-gamblers, following exposure to playing cards associated with monetary reward (van Holst et al., 2012). In contrast, either blunted (Balodis et al., 2012; Miedl et al., 2012; cf Reuter et al., 2005) or normal striatal responses (de Ruiter et al., 2009) have been reported following exposure to unfamiliar or otherwise neutral monetary reward cues (see Tables 2 and 3).

The results of PET [11C]raclopride studies suggest that striatal DA responses follow the same pattern. For example, increased striatal DA responses have been observed to (i) a realistic gambling task in patients with severe pathological gambling (Joutsa et al., 2012), (ii) familiar gambling cues plus L-DOPA in patients with comorbid Parkinson’s disease and pathological gambling (Steeves et al., 2009), (iii) food stimuli presented to binge eaters (Wang et al., 2011), (iv) L-DOPA medication given to Parkinson’s patients exhibiting various impulse control problems (Evans et al., 2006; O’Sullivan et al., 2011), and (v) the undisguised administration of d-amphetamine pills to gamblers (Payer et al., 2012). By comparison, blunted striatal DA responses have been observed following stimulant drug challenges administered without drug cues in patients with bulimia nervosa (Broft et al., 2012). Of note, the augmented DA responses may aggravate the clinical picture. Pathological gamblers who show greater striatal DA release have higher clinical severity scores (Joutsa et al., 2012), greater difficulty restraining from gambling (Payer et al., 2012), and poorer performance scores on the Iowa Gambling Task (Linnet et al., 2010, 2011).

8. Conclusions: treating the striatum – boost or block?

Addictions are complex, multi-factorial, and heterogeneous in origin and expression. The factors discussed in the present review will not account for all facets of the disease. At the neurobiological level alone, addictions involve more brain regions than the striatum and more neurotransmitters than DA. Nonetheless, the current view describes processes that can account for much of the variability in the literature. It can also improve our understanding of the role of addiction related cues in disease etiology, course and outcome.

The studies reviewed above suggest that, in humans, repeated exposure to motivationally intense stimuli can lead to conditioned and sensitized behavioral and neurobiological responses. As exposure accrues, these cues can also come to modulate responses to the rewards themselves. Striatal hyperactivation can occur when rewards and reward related cues are present. Striatal hypoactivation can occur when reward-paired cues are absent. Exposure to rewards in the presence of reward associated cues can produce synergistic effects, a co-occurrence that to date has been more common on the street than in the laboratory. Finally, the results reviewed here suggest that these conditioned processes might exert their effects not only at early stages of substance use but continue to do so during later stages of addiction as well.

These cue modulated effects are of more than academic interest. First, the situation-dependent, conditioned control of incentive motivational systems may account in large part for increased drive to obtain some rewards and decreased drive to obtain others, features that are prominent as addictions develop. Second, if the proposed processes continue to have the same effects once addictions are established, then the model also has implications for treatment. For example, multiple attempts have been made to block a presumed hyperactive (sensitized) DA system. Although the strategy has not been exhausted, double-blind, placebo controlled clinical trials with chronic neuroleptic medications have not proven to be effective (Grabowski et al., 2000; Kampman et al., 2003; Smelson et al., 2004; Reid et al., 2005). Alternatively, sharply increasing DA transmission is most likely a relapse precipitant (de Wit, 1996; Barrett et al., 2006). Each of these strategies in isolation may lack clinical efficacy because patients with addictions experience alternating periods of increased and decreased striatal activation (Fig. 2). Promising strategies may be better afforded by approaches that selectively target enhanced responding to the drug and its control by drug associated stimuli (Kim et al., 2005; Barrett et al., 2008; Venugopalan et al., 2011; Loweth et al., 2013) or that retrain the patient to orient toward other cues and rewards, as is achieved in attentional bias training (Attwood et al., 2008; Fadardi and Cox, 2009; Schoenmakers et al., 2010; Zhao et al., 2012b) and contingent management therapies (Dutra et al., 2008; Volpp et al., 2009). Slow release DA indirect agonist preparations have shown modest, though inconsistent, efficacy in some populations (Castells et al., 2010; Mariani et al., 2012). Selective DA D3 receptor antagonists and DA modulators are in development, and might prove helpful (Mugnaini et al., 2012; cf, Dodds et al., 2012).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Model of striatal activation in addiction. Patients may experience periods of hyper- and hypo-activations of the striatum related to the presence versus absence of addiction related cues. In this model, chronic neuroleptic treatment would be predicted to decrease cue-induced activations of the striatum, a possibly helpful action, but also to aggravate the low striatal activity when addiction related cues are absent. Strategies that target the low or high striatal states without aggravating the other may be more effective.

Adapted from Leyton (2007).

Finally, recent evidence has raised the possibility that individual differences in susceptibility to assign incentive value to reward related cues might be a general and heritable trait, influencing vulnerability to addictions or demarcating a distinct neurobiological risk pathway (Flagel et al., 2011; Fotros et al., 2012; Mahler and de Wit, 2010; Saunders and Robinson, this issue). In the latter case, DA targeted treatments might benefit the hypothesized DA reactive subgroup preferentially. Consistent with the notion that striatal reactivity reflects a pre-existing trait, individual differences in various reward seeking and impulsivity traits are predicted by the magnitude of striatal fMRI BOLD (Beaver et al., 2006; Bjork et al., 2008a) and DA responses (Leyton et al., 2002; Boileau et al., 2003, 2006; Buckholtz et al., 2010a,b; Treadway et al., 2012). The DA signals appear to have behavioral significance. Decreasing DA transmission diminishes cocaine cue-induced craving (Berger et al., 1996; Leyton et al., 2005), attentional biases toward drug cues (Franken et al., 2004; Munafó et al., 2007; Hitsman et al., 2008), the tendency of reward paired cues to elicit preferential responding (Leyton et al., 2007), and the willingness to work for drug (Barrett et al., 2008; Venugopalan et al., 2011) and monetary rewards (Cawley et al., 2010). These observations are consistent with the view that it is increased rather than decreased DA transmission that precipitates individual bouts of drug use, an observation recently seen across levels of substance use and addiction (Venugopalan et al., 2011). Thus, identifying ways to modulate these DA responses in a therapeutically significant way remains, we would suggest, an important clinical goal.

Acknowledgments

This review was made possible by grants from the Canadian Institutes for Health Research (MOP-36429 and MOP-64426, ML) and the National Institutes of Health (DA09397, PV). We dedicate this review to Ann Kelley. Her boundless energy, her love of life, and deep knowledge and passion for her work made her a role model for all of us.

Footnotes

1

Stimuli associated with opiates and ethanol yield a more complex mix of drug-like and drug-opposite effects (Wikler, 1973; Eikelboom and Stewart, 1982; Stewart et al., 1984; O’Brien et al., 1998; Stewart, 2004). For discussions of how deficit states can augment incentive motivational states and the salience of appetitive cues, see Toates (1986), Hutcheson et al. (2001), and Berridge (2012). A role for dysphoric states in the maintenance of stimulant use has also been proposed in opponent process views of drug taking (Koob and Le Moal, 1997). These states are usually observed soon after prolonged and continuous exposure to drugs but their subsequent elicitation by conditioned cues has also been proposed to contribute to relapse (Siegel, 1979).

2

Pharmacological tolerance refers to a decrease in a drug’s potency or efficacy (i.e., maximal effect) with repeated exposure. Conversely, sensitization, also labeled reverse-tolerance, refers to an increase in drug potency or efficacy (sometimes indicated as a significant response to a previously ineffective dose). Both terms describe empirical observations; in and of themselves they do not connote mechanism.

3

Despite the different time courses for tolerance and sensitization, there can be temporal overlap since each of these adaptations can occur simultaneously in different systems, as in, for example, those regulating respiration versus those mediating incentive motivation.

4

Striatal activations can also occur following monetary losses (Kühn et al., 2011). In this study, participants were 154 14-year old video gamers. Frequent gamers (>9 h/week) exhibited a larger striatal response to monetary loss as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) compared to less frequent gamers. Of note, stimuli indicative of loss are highly salient for gamers. Among professional gamers, greater striatal activation also predicts faster moves, an effect possibly reflecting an enhanced ability of cues to engage approach mechanisms (Wan et al., 2011).

5

These conditions of uncertain reward delivery simulate a core aspect of gambling. Moreover, in rodents, uncertain reward delivery can increase a cue’s motivational potency (Robinson and Berridge, 2012) and lead to behavioral sensitization to an amphetamine challenge (Singer et al., 2012).

6

There are also conditions when lower striatal responses are observed, although the results reported thus far are complex and the relevant determining factors remain unclear. For example, lower striatal responses have been observed in adolescents versus adults evaluating a cue prior to being able to respond to it (Geier et al., 2010). Similarly, while adolescents show larger responses than adults to rewards (Ernst et al., 2005; Galvan et al., 2006), the gain in striatal response between large versus small rewards ($5 versus 20 cents) has been reported to be less (Bjork et al., 2004). One interpretation is that adolescents exhibit larger striatal responses to rewards and reward paired cues but smaller responses to more distal cues requiring more elaborate evaluative processes.

7

A recent case study illustrates how increases and decreases in striatal activity can co-vary with drug-seeking behavior and addiction. A severely alcoholic patient received sessions of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex. Regional brain activity and self-reported craving were measured simultaneously. As expected, alcohol cue-induced craving was associated with increased activity in the nucleus accumbens. Strikingly, TMS decreased both the craving and the cue-induced activation of the nucleus accumbens, effects that were maintained for three months (De Ridder et al., 2011).

8

Binge eating disorders share various common features with substance use disorders and pathological gambling. Dysregulated reward seeking, disturbed impulse-control, and various other addictions are commonly co-morbid. Obesity also has been proposed to be a form of behavioral addiction, although this idea is more controversial. For a discussion of these issues, see Ziauddeen et al. (2012).

References

  1. Acheson A, Robinson JL, Glahn DC, Lovallo WR, Fox PT. Differential activation of the anterior cingulate cortex and caudate nucleus during a gambling simulation in persons with a family history of alcoholism: studies from the Oklahoma Family Health Patterns Project. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2009;100:17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.08.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agrawal A, Verweij KJH, Gillespie NA, Heath AC, Lessov-Schlaggar CN, Martin NG, Slutske WS, Whitfield JB, Lynskey MT. The genetics of addiction—a translational perspective. Translational Psychiatry. 2012;17(2):e140. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anagnostaras SG, Robinson TE. Sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of amphetamine: modulation by associative learning. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1996;110:1397–1414. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.110.6.1397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anagnostaras SG, Schallert T, Robinson TE. Memory processes governing amphetamine-induced psychomotor sensitization. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:703–715. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00402-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Andrews MM, Meda SA, Thomas AD, Potenza MN, Krystal JH, Worhunsky P, Stevens MC, O’Malley S, Book GA, Reynolds B, Pearlson GD. Individuals family history positive for alcoholism show functional resonance imaging differences in reward sensitivity that are related to impulsivity factors. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;69:675–683. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Angrist BM, Gershon S. The phenomenology of experimentally induced amphetamine psychosis—preliminary observations. Biological Psychiatry. 1970;2:95–107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Anthony JC. Epidemiology of drug dependence. In: Davis KL, Charney D, Coyle JT, Nemeroff C, editors. Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; Philadelphia: 2002. pp. 1557–1574. [Google Scholar]
  8. Aragona BJ, Day JJ, Roitman MF, Cleaveland NA, Wightman M, Carelli RM. Regional specificity in the real-time development of phasic dopamine transmission patterns during acquisition of a cue–cocaine association in rats. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;30:1889–1899. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07027.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Attwood AS, O’Sullivan H, Leonards U, Mackintosh B, Munafo MR. Attentional bias training and cue reactivity in cigarette smokers. Addiction. 2008;103:1875–1882. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02335.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Ayduk O, Mendoza-Denton R, Mischel W, Downey G, Peake PK, Rodriguez M. Regulating the interpersonal self: strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79:776–792. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.5.776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Balodis IM, Kober H, Worhunsky PD, Stevens MC, Pearlson GD, Potenza MN. Diminished frontostriatal activity during processing of monetary rewards and losses in pathological gambling. Biological Psychiatry. 2012;71:749–757. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Barrett SP, Pihl RO, Benkelfat C, Brunelle C, Young SN, Leyton M. The role of dopamine in alcohol self-administration in humans: individual differences. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;18:439–447. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Barrett SP, Tichnauer M, Leyton M, Pihl RO. Nicotine increases alcohol self-administration in non-dependent male smokers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2006;81:197–204. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Beaver JD, Lawrence AD, van Ditzhuijzen J, Davis MH, Woods A, Calder AJ. Individual differences in reward drive predict neural responses to images of food. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:5160–5166. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0350-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Beck A, Schlagenhauf F, Wüstenberg T, Hein J, Kienast T, Kahnt T, Schmack K, Hägele C, Knutson B, Heinz A, Wrase J. Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66:734–742. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bell DS. The experimental reproduction of amphetamine psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1973;29:35–40. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1973.04200010020003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Berger SP, Hall S, Mickalian JD, Reid MS, Crawford CA, Delucchi K, Carr K, Hall S. Haloperidol antagonism of cue-elicited cocaine craving. Lancet. 1996;347:504–508. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(96)91139-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Berridge KC. From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;35:1124–1143. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.07990.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bjork JM, Knutson B, Fong GW, Caggiano DM, Bennett SM, Hommer DW. Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: similarities and differences from young adults. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:1793–1802. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4862-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bjork JM, Knutson B, Hommer DW. Incentive-elicited striatal activation in adolescent children of alcoholics. Addiction. 2008a;103:1308–1319. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02250.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bjork JM, Smith AR, Hommer DW. Striatal sensitivity to reward deliveries and omissions in substance dependent patients. NeuroImage. 2008b;42:1609–1621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Boileau I, Assad JM, Pihl RO, Benkelfat C, Leyton M, Diksic M, Tremblay RE, Dagher A. Alcohol promotes dopamine release in human nucleus accumbens. Synapse. 2003;49:226–231. doi: 10.1002/syn.10226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Gunn RN, Baker GB, Diksic M, Benkelfat C. Modeling sensitization to stimulants in humans: A [11C]raclopride/PET study in healthy volunteers. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:1386–1395. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Welfeld K, Booij L, Diksic M, Benkelfat C. Conditioned dopamine release in humans: A PET [11C]raclopride study with amphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(15):3998–4003. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4370-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Boileau I, Payer D, Houle S, Behzadi A, Rusjan PM, Tong J, Wilkins D, Selby P, George TP, Zack M, Furukawa Y, McCluskey T, Wilson AA, Kish SJ. Higher binding of the dopamine D3 receptor-preferring ligand [11C]-(+)-propyl-hexahydro-naptho-oxazin in methamphetamine polydrug users: a positron emission tomography study. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:1353–1359. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4371-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Bradberry CW. Cocaine sensitization and dopamine mediation of cue effects in rodents, monkeys, and humans: Areas of agreement, disagreement, and implications for addiction. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191:705–717. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0561-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Bragulat V, Dzemidzic M, Talavage T, Davidson D, O’Connor SJ, Karaken DA. Alcohol sensitizes cerebral responses to the odors of alcoholic drinks: an fMRI study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2008;32:1124–1134. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00693.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Brauer LH, Ambre J, de Wit H. Acute tolerance to subjective but not cardiovascular effects of d-amphetamine in normal, healthy men. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1996;16:72–76. doi: 10.1097/00004714-199602000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Braus DF, Wrase J, Grüsser S, Hermann D, Ruf M, Flor H, Mann K, Heinz A. Alcohol-associated stimuli activate the ventral striatum in abstinent alcoholics. Journal of Neural Transmission. 2001;108:887–894. doi: 10.1007/s007020170038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Broft A, Shingleton R, Kaufman J, Liu F, Kumar D, Slifstein M, Abi-Dargham A, Schebendach J, Van Heertum R, Attia E, Martinez D, Walsh BT. Striatal dopamine in bulimia nervosa: a PET imaging study. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2012;45(5):648–656. doi: 10.1002/eat.20984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Buckholtz JW, Treadway MT, Cowan RL, Woodward ND, Benning SD, Li R, Ansari MS, Baldwin RM, Schwartzman AN, Shelby ES, Smith CE, Cole D, Kessler RM, Zald DH. Mesolimbic dopamine reward system hypersensitivity in individuals with psychopathic traits. Nature Neuroscience. 2010a;13:419–421. doi: 10.1038/nn.2510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Buckholtz JW, Treadway MT, Cowan RL, Woodward ND, Li R, Ansari MS, Baldwin RM, Schwartzman AN, Shelby ES, Smith CE, Kessler RM, Zald DH. Dopaminergic network differences in human impulsivity. Science. 2010b;329:532. doi: 10.1126/science.1185778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Carter BL, Tiffany ST. Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction. 1999;94:327–340. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Carter BL, Tiffany ST. The cue-availability paradigm: the effects of cigarette availability on cue reactivity in smokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopathology. 2001;9:183–190. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.9.2.183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Casey KF, Benkelfat C, Cherkasova MV, Baker GB, Dagher A, Leyton M. Attenuated amphetamine-induced dopamine release in subjects at high familial risk for substance dependence. The 10th International Catecholamine Symposium.2012. [Google Scholar]
  36. Castells X, Casas M, Pérez-Maná C, Roncero C, Vidal X, Capellà D. Efficacy of psychostimulant drugs for cocaine dependence. The Cochrane Library. 2010;3:1–206. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD007380.pub3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Cawley EI, Park S, aan het Rot M, Sancton K, Benkelfat C, Young SN, Boivin D, Leyton M. Dopamine and light: effects on mood and motivational states in mildly seasonal women. 33rd Annual Meeting of the Canadian College of Neuropsychopharmacology.2010. [Google Scholar]
  38. Chase HW, Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Hogarth L. The neural basis of drug stimulus processing and craving: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;70:785–793. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Childress AR, Ehrman RN, Wang Z, Li Y, Sciortino N, Hakun J, Jens W, Suh J, Listerud J, Marquez K, Franklin T, Langleben D, Detre J, O’Brien CP. Prelude to passion: limbic activation by unseen drug and sexual cues. PLoS ONE. 2008;3(1):e1506. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Childress AR, Hole AV, Ehrman RN, Robbins SJ, McLellan AT, O’Brien CP. Cue reactivity and cue reactivity interventions in drug dependence. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph. 1993;137:73–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R, O’Brien CP. Classically conditioned responses in cocaine and opioid dependence: a role in relapse? In: Ray BA, editor. Learning Factors in Substance Abuse. Vol. 84. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; Rockville, MD: 1988. pp. 25–43. [Google Scholar]
  42. Childs E, de Wit H. Amphetamine-induced place preference in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;15:900–904. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Childs E, de Wit H. Contextual conditioning enhances the psychostimulant and incentive properties of d-amphetamine in humans. Addiction Biology. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00416.x. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ciccocioppo R, Martin-Fardon R, Weiss F. Stimuli associated with a single cocaine experience elicit long-lasting cocaine-seeking. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:495–496. doi: 10.1038/nn1219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Claus ED, Ewing SWF, Filbey FM, Sabbineni A, Hutchison KE. Identifying neurobiological phenotypes associated with alcohol use disorder severity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:2086–2096. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Cloutier J, Heatherton TF, Whalen PJ, Kelley WM. Are attractive people rewarding? Sex differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20:941–951. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Conaglen HM, Evans IM. Pictoral cues and sexual desire: an experimental approach. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2006;35:201–216. doi: 10.1007/s10508-005-9000-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Connell PH. Amphetamine Psychosis, Maudsley Monograph No 5. Chapman and Hall; London: 1958. [Google Scholar]
  49. Conrod PJ, Pihl RO, Stewart SH, Dongier M. Validation of a system of classifying female substance abusers on the basis of personality and motivational risk factors for substance abuse. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2000;14:243–256. doi: 10.1037//0893-164x.14.3.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Cortright JJ, Sampedro GR, Neugebauer NM, Vezina P. Previous exposure to nicotine enhances the incentive motivational effects of amphetamine via nicotine-associated contextual stimuli. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:2277–2284. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Cox SML, Benkelfat C, Dagher A, Delaney JS, Durand F, McKenzie SA, Kolivakis T, Casey KF, Leyton M. Striatal dopamine responses to intranasal cocaine self-administration in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;65:846–850. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Cox WM, Fadardi JS, Pothos EM. The addiction Stroop test: theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;32:443–476. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Culbertson C, Nicolas S, Zaharovits I, London ED, de la Garza R, II, Brody AL, Newton TF. Methamphetamine craving induced in an online virtual reality environment. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2010;96:454–460. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Dar R, Rosen-Korain N, Shapira O, Gottlieb Y, Frenk H. The craving to smoke in flight attendants: relations with smoking deprivation, anticipation of smoking, and actual smoking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119:248–253. doi: 10.1037/a0017778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Dar R, Stronguin F, Marouani R, Krupsky M, Frenk H. Craving to smoke in orthodox Jewish smokers who abstain on the Sabbath: a comparison to a baseline and a forced abstinence workday. Psychopharmacology. 2005;183:294–299. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0192-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Davis C, Curtis C, Levitan RD, Carter JC, Kaplan AS, Kennedy JL. Evidence that ‘food addiction’ is a valid phenotype of obesity. Appetite. 2011;57:711–717. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Dawson DA, Harford TC, Grant BF. Family history as a predictor of alcohol dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1992;16:572–575. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1992.tb01419.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. de Lange FP, van Gaal S, Lamme VAF, Dehaene S. How awareness changes the relative weights of evidence during human decision-making. PLoS ONE. 2011;9:e1001203. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Demos KE, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM. Individual differences in nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual behavior. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:5549–5552. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5958-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. De Ridder D, Vanneste S, Kovacs S, Sunaert S, Dom G. Transient alcohol craving suppression by rTMS of dorsal anterior cingulate: an fMRI and LORETA EEG study. Neuroscience Letters. 2011;496:5–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.03.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. de Ruiter MB, Veltman DJ, Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, Sjoerds Z, van den Brink W. Response perseveration and ventral prefrontal sensitivity to reward and punishment in male problem gamblers and smokers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1027–1038. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. de Wit H. Priming effects with drugs and other reinforcers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1996;4:5–11. [Google Scholar]
  63. de Wit H, Phillips TJ. Do initial responses to drugs predict future use or abuse? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2012;36:1565–1576. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. de Wit H, Stewart J. Reinstatement of cocaine-reinforced responding in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1981;75:134–143. doi: 10.1007/BF00432175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Di Ciano P, Blaha CD, Phillips AG. Conditioned changes in dopamine oxidation currents in the nucleus accumbens of rats by stimuli paired with self-administration or yoked-administration of d-amphetamine. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;10:1121–1127. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00125.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Dodds CM, O’Neil B, Beaver J, Makwana A, Bani M, Merlo-Pich E, Flecther PC, Koch A, Bullmore ET, Nathan PJ. Effect of the dopamine D3 receptor antagonist GSK598809 on brain responses to rewarding food images in overweight and obese binge eaters. Appetite. 2012;59:27–33. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Doudet DJ, Holden JE. Raclopride studies of dopamine release: dependence on presynaptic integrity. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;54:193–199. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00288-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Droungas A, Ehrman RN, Childress AR, O’Brien CP. Effect of smoking cues and cigarette availability on craving and smoking behavior. Addictive Behaviors. 1995;20:657–673. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(95)00029-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Dutra L, Stathopolous G, Basden SL, Leyro TM, Powers MB, Otto MWA. A meta-analytic review of psychosocial interventions for substance use disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165:179–187. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06111851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Duvauchelle CL, Ikegami A, Castaneda E. Conditioned increases in behavioral activity and accumbens dopamine levels produced by intravenous cocaine. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2000;114:1156–1166. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.6.1156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Eikelboom R, Stewart J. Conditioning of drug-induced physiological responses. Psychological Review. 1982;89:507–528. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Ellinwood EH. Amphetamine psychosis: I. Description of the individuals and process. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1967;144:273–283. [Google Scholar]
  73. Enoch MA, Hodgkinson CA, Yuan Q, Shen PH, Goldman D, Roy A. The influence of GABRA2, childhood trauma, and their interaction on alcohol, heroin, and cocaine dependence. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:20–27. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Epstein DH, Willner-Reid J, Vahabzadeh M, Mezghanni M, Lin J-L, Preston KL. Real-time electronic diary reports of cue exposure and mood in the hours before cocaine and heroin craving and use. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:88–94. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ernst M, Fudge JL. A developmental neurobiological model of motivated behavior: anatomy, connectivity and ontogeny of the triadic nodes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009;33:367–382. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Ernst M, Nelson EE, Jazbec S, McClure EB, Monk CS, Leibenluft E, Blair J, Pine DS. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. NeuroImage. 2005;25(4):1279–1291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Evans AH, Pavese N, Lawrence AD, Tai YF, Appel S, Doder M, Brooks DJ, Lees AJ, Piccini P. Compulsive drug use linked to sensitized ventral striatal dopamine transmission. Annals of Neurology. 2006;59:852–858. doi: 10.1002/ana.20822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Fadardi JS, Cox WM. Reversing the sequence: reducing alcohol consumption by overcoming alcohol attentional bias. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2009;101:137–145. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Fatseas M, Denis C, Massida Z, Verger M, Franques-Rénéric P, Auriacombe M. Cue-induced reactivity, cortisol response and substance use outcome in treated heroin dependent individuals. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;70:720–727. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Ferguson CS, Tyndale RF. Cytochrome P450 enzymes in the brain: emerging evidence of biological significance. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences. 2011;32:708–714. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2011.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Field M, Munafò MR, Franken IHA. A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135:589–607. doi: 10.1037/a0015843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Filbey FM, Claus E, Audette AR, Niculescu M, Banich MT, Du YP, Hutchison KE. Exposure to the taste of alcohol elicits activation of the mesocorticolimbic neurocircuitry. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1391–1401. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Fiorillo CD, Tobler PN, Schultz W. Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science. 2003;299:1898–1902. doi: 10.1126/science.1077349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Fischman MW. Relationship between self-reported drug effects and their reinforcing effects: studies with stimulant drugs. NIDA Research Monograph. 1989;92:1211–1230. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Flagel SB, Clark JJ, Robinson TE, Mayo L, Czuj A, Willuhn I, Akers CA, Clinton SM, Phillips PEM, Akil H. A selective role for dopamine in stimulus-reward learning. Nature. 2011;469:53–57. doi: 10.1038/nature09588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Foltin RW, Haney M. Conditioned effects of environmental stimuli paired with smoked cocaine in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2000;149:24–33. doi: 10.1007/s002139900340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Fotros A, Casey KF, Larcher K, Verhaeghe JAJ, Cox SM, Gravel P, Reader AJ, Dagher A, Benkelfat C, Leyton M. Cue-induced dopamine release in striatal and extra-striatal regions in cocaine dependent users: a high-resolution PET [18F]fallypride study. The 10th International Catecholamine Symposium.2012. [Google Scholar]
  88. Franken IH, Hendriks VM, Stam CJ, van den Brink W. A role for dopamine in the processing of drug cues in heroin dependent patients. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;14:503–508. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2004.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Frascella J, Potenza MN, Brown LL, Childress AR. Shared brain vulnerabilities open the way for nonsubstance addictions: carving addiction at a new joint? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1187:294–315. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05420.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, Penn J, Voss H, Glover G, Casey BJ. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6885–6892. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Garavan H, Pankiewicz J, Bloom A, Cho JK, Sperry L, Ross TJ, Salmeron BJ, Risinger R, Kelley D, Stein EA. Cue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157:1789–1798. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Geier CF, Terwilliger R, Teslovich T, Velanova K, Luna B. Immaturities in reward processing and its influence on inhibitory control in adolescence. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:1613–1629. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Gearhardt AN, White MA, Potenza MN. Binge eating disorder and food addiction. Current Drug Abuse Reviews. 2011;4:201–207. doi: 10.2174/1874473711104030201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. George MS, Anton RF, Bloomer C, Teneback C, Drobes DJ, Lorberbaum JP, Nahas Z, Vincent DJ. Activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior thalamus in alcoholic subjects on exposure to alcohol-specific cues. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58:345–352. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Gilman JM, Ramchandani VA, Crouss T, Hommer DW. Subjective and neural responses to intravenous alcohol in young adults with light and heavy drinking patterns. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:467–477. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Goldstein RZ, Craig AD, Bechara A, Garavan H, Childress AR, Paulus MP, Volkow ND. The neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13:372–380. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Grabowski J, Rhoades H, Silverman P, Schmitz J, Stotts A, Creson D, Rahn B. Risperidone for the treatment of cocaine dependence: randomized, double-blind trial. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2000;20:305–310. doi: 10.1097/00004714-200006000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Griffith JD, Cavanaugh J, Held J, Oates JA. Dextroamphetamine: evaluation of psychotomimetc properties in man. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1972;26:97–100. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1972.01750200001001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature. 2001;412:141–142. doi: 10.1038/35084134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Grüsser SM, Wrase J, Klein S, Hermann D, Smolka MN, Ruf M, Weber-Fahr W, Flor H, Mann K, Braus DF, Heinz A. Cue-induced activation of the striatum and medial prefrontal cortex is associated with subsequent relapse in abstinent alcoholics. Psychopharmacology. 2004;175:296–302. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1828-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Guillory AM, Suto N, You Z-B, Vezina P. Effects of conditioned inhibition on neurotransmitter overflow in the nucleus accumbens. Society for Neuroscience. 2006;32:483–493. Abstracts. [Google Scholar]
  102. Hakyemez HS, Dagher A, Smith SD, Zald DH. Striatal dopamine transmission during passive monetary reward task. NeuroImage. 2008;15:2058–2065. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Hamamura T, Akiyama K, Akimoto K, Kashihara K, Okumura K, Ujike H, Otsuki S. Co-administration of either a selective D1 or D2 dopamine antagonist with methamphetamine prevents methamphetamine- induced behavioral sensitization and neurochemical change, studied by in vivo intracerebral dialysis. Brain Research. 1991;546:40–46. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)91156-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Hamann S, Herman RA, Nolan CL, Wallen K. Men and women differ in amygdala response to visual sexual stimuli. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:411–416. doi: 10.1038/nn1208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Herman CP. External and internal cues as determinants of the smoking behavior of light and heavy smokers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1974;30:664–672. doi: 10.1037/h0037440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Hernandez L, Wager T, Jonides J. Introduction to functional neuroimaging. In: Cabeza R, Kingstone A, editors. Handbook of Functional Neuroimaging of Cognition. Chapter 1 The MIT Press; Cambridge (MA): 2001. [Google Scholar]
  107. Hitsman B, MacKillop J, Lingford-Hughes A, Williams TM, Ahmad F, Adams S, Nutt DJ, Munafó MR. Effects of acute tyrosine/phenylalanine depletion on the selective processing of smoking-related cues and the relative value of cigarettes in smokers. Psychopharmacology. 2008;196:611–621. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0995-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Hogarth L, Dickinson A, Wright A, Kouvaraki M, Duka T. The role of drug expectancy in the control of human drug seeking. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2007;33:484–496. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.33.4.484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Hogarth L, Dickinson A, Duka T. The associative basis of cue-elicited drug taking in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2010;208:337–351. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1735-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Hogarth L, Dickinson A, Janowski A, Nikitina A, Duka T. The role of attentional bias in mediating human drug-seeking behaviour. Psychopharmacology. 2008;201:29–41. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1244-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Hommer RE, Seo D, Lacadie CM, Chaplin TM, Mayes LC, Sinha R, Potenza MN. Neural correlates of stress and favorite-food cue exposure in adolescents: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Human Brain Mapping. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22089. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Hug CC. Characteristics and theories related to tolerance development. In: Mulé SJ, Brill H, editors. Chemical and Biological Aspects of Drug Dependence. CRC Press; Cleveland: 1972. pp. 307–358. [Google Scholar]
  113. Hutcheson DM, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW, Dickinson A. The role of withdrawal in heroin addiction: enhances reward or promotes avoidance? Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4:943–947. doi: 10.1038/nn0901-943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Hyatt CJ, Assaf M, Muska CE, Rosen RI, Thomas AD, Johnson MR, Hylton JL, Andrews MM, Reynolds BA, Krystal JH, Potenza MN, Pearlson GD. Reward-related dorsal striatal activity differences between former and current cocaine dependent individuals during an interactive competitive game. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e34917. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Hyman SM, Garcia M, Sinha R. Gender specific associations between types of childhood maltreatment and the onset, escalation and severity of substance use in cocaine-dependent adults. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2006;32:655–664. doi: 10.1080/10623320600919193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Ihssen N, Cox WM, Wiggett A, Fadardi JS, Linden DEJ. Differentiating heavy from light drinkers by neural responses to visual alcohol cues and other motivational stimuli. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21:1408–1415. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Ito R, Dalley JW, Howes SR, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Dissociation in conditioned dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core and shell in response to cocaine cues and during cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:7489–7495. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-07489.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Jansen A. A learning model of binge eating: cue reactivity and cue exposure. Behavior Research and Therapy. 1998;36:257–272. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00055-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Jia Z, Worhunsky PD, Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ, Stevens MC, Pearlson GD, Potenza MN. An initial study of neural responses to monetary incentives as related to treatment outcome in cocaine dependence. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;70:553–560. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Johanson CE, Uhlenhuth EH. Drug preference and mood in humans: repeated assessment of d-amphetamine. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1981;14:159–163. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(81)90237-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Joutsa J, Johansson J, Niemela S, Ollikainen A, Hirvonen MH, Piepponen P, Arponen E, Alho H, Voon V, Rinne JO, Hietala J, Kaasinen V. Mesolimbic dopamine release is linked to symptom severity in pathological gambling. NeuroImage. 2012;60:1992–1999. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Juliano LM, Brandon TH. Reactivity to perceived smoking availability and environmental cues: evidence with urge and reaction time. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1998;6:45–53. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.6.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Kampman KM, Pettinati H, Lynch KG, Sparkman T, O’Brien CP. A pilot trial of olanzapine for the treatment of cocaine dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2003;70:265–273. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(03)00009-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Kareken DA, Bragulat V, Dzemidzic M, Cox C, Talavage T, Davidson D, O’Connor SJ. Family history of alcoholism mediates the frontal response to alcoholic drink odors and alcohol in at-risk drinkers. NeuroImage. 2010;50:267–276. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Kareken DA, Claus ED, Sabri M, Dzemidzic M, Kosobud AEK, Radnovich AJ, Hector D, Ramchandani VA, O’Connor SJ, Lowe M, Li TK. Alcohol-related olfactory cues activate the nucleus accumbens and ventral tegmental area in high-risk drinkers: Preliminary findings. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2004;28:550–557. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000122764.60626.af. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Kelley AE, Berridge KC. The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance to addictive drugs. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:3306–3311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03306.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Kelly TH, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. The effects of repeated amphetamine exposure on multiple measures of human behavior. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1991;38:417–426. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90301-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Kendler KS, Chen X, Dick D, Maes H, Gillepsie N, Neale MC, Riley B. Recent advances in the genetic epidemiology and molecular genetics of substance use disorders. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:181–189. doi: 10.1038/nn.3018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Kendler KS, Davis CG, Kessler RC. The familial aggregation of common psychiatric and substance use disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey: a family history study. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;170:541–548. doi: 10.1192/bjp.170.6.541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale MC. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:929–937. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Kim BK, Zauberman G. Can Victoria’s Secret change the future? A subjective time perception account of sexual-cue effects on impatience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi: 10.1037/a0028954. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Kim J-H, Austin JD, Tanabe L, Creekmore E, Vezina P. Activation of group II mGlu receptors blocks the enhanced drug taking induced by previous exposure to amphetamine. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;21:295–300. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03822.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. King A, McNamara P, Angstadt M, Phan KL. Neural substrates of alcohol-induced smoking urge in heavy drinking nondaily smokers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:692–701. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Koepp MJ, Gunn RN, Lawrence AD, Cunningham VJ, Dagher A, Jones T, Brooks DJ, Bench CJ, Grasby PM. Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature. 1998;393:266–268. doi: 10.1038/30498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Knutson B, Cooper JC. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction. Current Opinion in Neurology. 2005;18:411–417. doi: 10.1097/01.wco.0000173463.24758.f6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science. 1997;278:52–58. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Krueger RF. The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:921–926. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Kühn S, Romanowski A, Schilling R, Mörsen C, Seiferth N, Banaschewski T, Barbot A, Barker GJ, Büchel C, Conrod PJ, Dalley JW, Flor H, Garavan H, Ittermann B, Mann K, Martinot J-L, Paus T, Rietschel M, Smolka MN, Ströhle A, Walaszek B, Schumann G, Heinz A, Gallinat J The IMAGEN Consortium. The neural basis of video gaming. Translational Psychiatry. 2011;15:e53. doi: 10.1038/tp.2011.53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Lamb RJ, Preston KL, Schindler C, Meisch RA, Davis F, Katz JL, Henningfield JE, Goldberg SR. The reinforcing and subjective effects of morphine in post-addicts: a dose–response study. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1991;259:1165–1173. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Laruelle M. Imaging synaptic neurotransmission with in vivo binding competition techniques: a critical review. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism. 2000;20:423–452. doi: 10.1097/00004647-200003000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Leeman RF, Potenza MN. Similarities and differences between pathological gambling and substance use disorders: a focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Psychopharmacology. 2012;219:466–490. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2550-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Leyton M. Conditioned and sensitized responses to stimulant drugs in humans. Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2007;31:1601–1613. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.08.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Leyton M, aan het Rot M, Booij L, Baker GB, Young SN, Benkelfat C. Mood-elevating effects of d-amphetamine and incentive salience: the effect of acute dopamine precursor depletion. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience. 2007;32:129–136. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Leyton M, Boileau I, Benkelfat C, Diksic M, Baker GB, Dagher A. Amphetamine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine, drug wanting, and novelty seeking: a PET/[11C]raclopride study in healthy men. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:1027–1035. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00366-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Leyton M, Casey KF, Delaney JS, Kolivakis T, Benkelfa tC. Cocaine craving, euphoria, and self-administration: a preliminary study of the effect of catecholamine precursor depletion. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2005;119:1619–1627. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.6.1619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Leyton M, Vezina P. On cue: striatal ups and downs in addictions. Biological Psychiatry. 2012;72:e21–e22. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Li Q, Wang Y, Zhang Y, Li W, Yang W, Zhu J, Wu N, Chang H, Zheng Y, Qin W, Zhao L, Yuan K, Liu J, Wang W, Tian J. Craving correlates with mesolimbic responses to heroin-related cues in short-term abstinence from heroin: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Research. 2012;1469:63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Lidstone SC, Schulzer M, Dinelle K, Mak E, Sossi V, Ruth TJ, de la Fuente-Fernández R, Phillips AG, Stoessl AJ. Effects of expectation on placebo-induced dopamine release in Parkinson Disease. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:857–865. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Lingford-Hughes AR, Daglish MRC, Stevenson BJ, Feeney A, Pandit SA, Wilson SJ, Myles J, Grasby PM, Nutt DJ. Imaging alcohol cue exposure in alcohol dependence using a PET 15O-H2-O paradigm: results from a pilot study. Addiction Biology. 2006;11:107–115. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2006.00001.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Linnet J, Peterson E, Doudet DJ, Gjedde A, Moller A. Dopamine release in ventral striatum of pathological gamblers losing money. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2010;122:326–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01591.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Linnet J, Peterson E, Gjedde A, Doudet DJ. Inverse association between dopaminergic neurotransmission and Iowa Gambling Task performance in pathological gamblers and healthy controls. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2011;52:28–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00837.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Little KY, Krolewski DM, Zhang L, Cassin BJ. Loss of striatal vesicular monoamine transporter protein (VMAT2) in human cocaine users. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:47–55. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Little KY, Ramssen E, Welchko R, Volberg V, Roland CJ, Cassin B. Decreased brain dopamine cell numbers in human cocaine users. Psychiatry Research. 2009;168:173–180. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Little M, Euser AS, Munafò MR, Franken IHA. Electrophysiological indices of biased cognitive processing of substance-related cues: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2012;36:1803–1816. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Lodge DJ, Grace AA. Amphetamine activation of hippocampal drive of mesolimbic dopamine neurons: a mechanism of behavioral sensitization. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:7876–7882. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1582-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Lou M, Wang E, Shen Y, Wang J. Cue-elicited craving in heroin addicts at different abstinent time: an fMRI pilot study. Substance Use & Misuse. 2012;47:631–639. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2011.646381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Loweth JA, Li D, Cortright JJ, Wilke G, Jeyifous O, Neve RL, Bayer KU, Vezina P. Persistent reversal of enhanced amphetamine intake by transient CaMKII inhibition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:1411–1416. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4386-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Mahler SV, de Wit H. Cue-reactors: individual differences in cue-induced craving after food or smoking abstinence. PloS ONE. 2010;5:e15475. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Mariani JJ, Pavlicova M, Bisaga A, Nunes EV, Brooks DJ, Levin FR. Extended-release mixed amphetamine salts and topiramate for cocaine dependence: a randomized controlled trial. Biological Psychiatry. 2012;72:950–956. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.05.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Martinez D, Carpenter KM, Liu F, Slifstein M, Broft A, Friedman AC, Kumar D, van Heertum R, Kleber HD, Nunes E. Imaging dopamine transmission in cocaine dependence: link between neurochemistry and response to treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;168:634–641. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10050748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Martinez D, Gil R, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Huang Y, Perez A, Kegeles L, Talbot P, Evans S, Krystal J, Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A. Alcohol dependence is associated with blunted dopamine transmission in the ventral striatum. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;58:779–786. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Martinez D, Greene K, Broft A, Kumar D, Liu F, Narendran R, Slifstein M, Van Heertum R, Kleber HD. Lower level of endogenous dopamine in patients with cocaine dependence: findings from PET imaging of D2/D3 receptors following acute dopamine depletion. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:1170–1177. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08121801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Martinez D, Narendran R, Foltin RW, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Brof tA, et al. Amphetamine-induced dopamine release: markedly blunted in cocaine dependence and predictive of the choice to self-administer cocaine. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:622–629. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Martinez D, Saccone PA, Liu F, Slifstein M, Orlowska D, Grassetti A, Cook S, Broft S, van Heertum R, Comer SD. Deficits in dopamine D2 receptors and presynaptic dopamine in heroin dependence: commonalities and differences with other types of addiction. Biological Psychiatry. 2012;71:192–198. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.08.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Martin-Soelch C, Szczepanik J, Nugen A, Barhaghi K, Rallis D, Herscovitch P, Carson RE, Drevets WC. Lateralization and gender differences in the dopaminergic response to unpredictable reward in the human ventral striatum. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;33:1706–1715. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07642.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Mendrek A, Blaha CD, Phillips AG. Pre-exposure of rats to amphetamine sensitizes self-administration of this drug under a progressive ratio schedule. Psychopharmacology. 1998;135:416–422. doi: 10.1007/s002130050530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Merrall ELC, Kariminia A, Binswanger IA, Hobbs MS, Farrell M, Marsden J, Hutchison SJ, Bird SM. Meta-analysis of drug-related deaths soon after release from prison. Addiction. 2010;105:1545–1554. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.02990.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Merikangas KR, Stolar M, Stevens DE, Goulet J, Preisig MA, Fenton B, Zhang H, O’Malley SS, Rounsaville BJ. Familial transmission of substance use disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1998;55:973–979. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.11.973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Miedl SF, Peters J, Büchel C. Altered neural reward representations in pathological gamblers revealed by delay and probability discounting. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69:177–186. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Mucha RF, Pauli P, Angrilli A. Conditioned responses elicited by experimentally produced cues for smoking. Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology. 1998;76:259–268. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Mugnaini M, Iavarone L, Cavallini P, Griffante C, Oliosi B, Savoia C, Beaver J, Rabiner EA, Micheli F, Heiderbreder C, Andorn AC, Pich EM, Bani M. GSK598809, occupancy of brain dopamine D3 receptors and drug craving: a translational study. Society for Neuroscience. 2012;38 doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.171. Abstracts. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Munafó MR, Mannie ZN, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ, McTavish SB. Effects of acute tyrosine depletion on subjective craving and selective processing of smoking-related cues in abstinent smokers. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2007;21:805–814. doi: 10.1177/0269881107077216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Myrick H, Anton RF, Li X, Henderson S, Drobes D, Voronin K, George MS. Differential brain activity in alcoholics and social drinkers to alcohol cues: relationship to craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:393–402. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Narendran R, Martinez D. Cocaine abuse and sensitization of striatal dopamine transmission: a critical review of the preclinical and clinical imaging literature. Synapse. 2008;62:851–869. doi: 10.1002/syn.20566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Nutt DJ. Minimizing the Harms of Legal and Illegal Drugs. UIT Cambridge Ltd.; Cambridge, England: 2012. Drugs Without the Hot Air. [Google Scholar]
  176. Oberlin BG, Dzemidzic M, Bragulat V, Lehigh CA, Talavage T, O’Connor SJ, Kareken DA. Limbic responses to reward cues correlate with antisocial trait density in heavy drinkers. NeuroImage. 2012;60:644–652. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. O’Brien CP, Childress AR, Ehrman R, Robbins SJ. Conditioning factors in drug abuse: can they explain compulsion? Journal of Psychopharmacology. 1998;12:15–22. doi: 10.1177/026988119801200103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. O’Brien CP, Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R. Integrating systemic cue exposure with standard treatment in recovering drug dependent patients. Addictive Behaviors. 1990;15:355–365. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(90)90045-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. O’Daly OG, Joyce D, Stephan KE, Murray RM, Shergill SS. An fMRI investigation of the amphetamine sensitisation model of schizophrenia in healthy male volunteers. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2011;68:545–554. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. O’Doherty J, Dayan P, Schultz J, Deichmann R, Friston K, Dolan RJ. Dissociable roles of the ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science. 2004;304:452–454. doi: 10.1126/science.1094285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. O’Sullivan SS, Wu K, Politis M, Lawrence AD, Evans AH, Bose SK, Djamshidan A, Lees AJ, Piccini P. Cue-induced striatal dopamine release in Parkinson’s disease-associated impulsive-compulsive behaviours. Brain. 2011;134:969–978. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Panlilio LV, Yasar S, Nemeth-Coslett R, Katz JL, Henningfield JE, Solinas M, Heishman SJ, Schindler CW, Goldberg SR. Human cocaine-seeking behavior and its control by drug-associated stimuli in the laboratory. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:433–443. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Paulson PE, Camp DM, Robinson TE. Time course of transient behavioral depression and persistent behavioral sensitization in relation to regional brain monoamine concentrations during amphetamine withdrawal in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1991;103:480–492. doi: 10.1007/BF02244248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Payer D, Boileau I, Lobo D, Chugani B, Behzadi A, Wilson A, Kish S, Houle S, Zack M. Investigating dopamine function with [11C]raclopride and [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET. Society of Biological Psychiatry. 2012;434 Abstracts. [Google Scholar]
  185. Perkins KA. Cues must increase smoking behavior to be clinically relevant. Addiction. 2009;104:1620–1622. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02670.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Peters J, Bromberg U, Schneider S, Brassen S, Menz M, Banaschewski T, Conrod PJ, Flor H, Gallinat J, Garavan H, et al. Lower ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation in adolescent smokers. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;168:540–549. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10071024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Potenza MN, Hong KA, Lacadie CM, Fulbright KK, Tuit KL, Sinha R. Neural correlates of stress-induced and cue-induced drug craving: influences of sex and cocaine dependence. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169:406–414. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Rao H, Mamikonyan E, Detre JA, Siderowf AD, Stern MB, Potenza MN, Weintraub D. Decreased ventral striatal activity with impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders. 2010;25:1660–1669. doi: 10.1002/mds.23147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Reid MS, Casadonte P, Baker S, Sanfilipo M, Braunstein D, Hitzemann R, Montgomery R, Majewska D, Robinson J, Rotrosen J. A placebo-controlled screening trial of olanzapine, valproate, and coenzyme Q10/L-carnitine for the treatment of cocaine dependence. Addiction. 2005;100:43–57. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.00990.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Reid MS, Ho LB, Berger SP. Effects of environmental conditioning on the development of nicotine sensitization: Behavioral and neurochemical analysis. Psychopharmacology. 1996;126:301–310. doi: 10.1007/BF02247381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Reuter J, Raedler T, Rose M, Hand I, Gläscher J, Büchel C. Pathological gambling is linked to reduced activation of the mesolimbic reward system. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:147–148. doi: 10.1038/nn1378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Robinson MJ, Berridge KC. Natural rewards, gambling and addiction: recomputation of incentive motivation for reward cues. Society for Neuroscience. 2012;38:605.3. Abstracts. [Google Scholar]
  193. Robinson TE, Becker JB. Enduring changes in brain and behavior produced by chronic amphetamine administration: a review and evaluation of animal models of amphetamine psychosis. Brain Research. 1986;396:157–198. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(86)80193-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews. 1993;18:247–291. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Addiction. Annual Review of Psychology. 2003;54:25–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Sato M. A lasting vulnerability to psychosis in patients with previous methamphetamine psychosis. Annals New York Academy of Sciences. 1992;654:160–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25965.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Sato M, Numachi Y, Hamamura T. Relapse of paranoid psychotic state in methamphetamine model of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1992;18:115–122. doi: 10.1093/schbul/18.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Schacht JP, Anton RF, Randall PK, Li X, Henderson S, Myrick H. Stability of fMRI striatal response to alcohol cues: a hierarchical modeling approach. NeuroImage. 2011;56:61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Schneider S, Peters J, Bromberg U, Brassen S, Medl SF, Banaschewski T, Barker GJ, Conrod PJ, et al. Risk taking and the adolescent reward system: a potential common link to substance abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169:39–46. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11030489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Schoenmakers TM, de Bruin M, Lux IF, Goertz AG, Van Kerkhof DH, Wiers RW. Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modification training in abstinent alcoholic patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2010;109:30–36. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Schott BH, Minuzzi L, Krebs RM, Elmenhorst D, Lang M, Winz OH, Seidenbecher CI, Coenen HH, Heinze HJ, Ziles K, Düzel E, Bauer A. Mesolimbic functional magnetic resonance imaging activations during reward anticipation correlate with reward-related ventral striatal dopamine release. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;24:14311–14319. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2058-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Schuckit MA. Self-rating of alcohol intoxication by young men with and without family histories of alcoholism. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1980;41:242–249. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1980.41.242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Seo D, Jia Z, Lacadie CM, Tsou KA, Bergquist K, Sinha R. Sex differences in neural responses to stress and alcohol context cues. Human Brain Mapping. 2011;32:1998–2013. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Setiawan E, Pihl RO, Casey KF, Dagher A, Benkelfat C, Leyton M. Increased alcohol-induced dopamine release in subjects at risk for alcohol dependence: a PET [11C]raclopride study. Canadian College of Neuropsychopharmacology Annual Meeting; 2010. Abstract. [Google Scholar]
  206. Siegel S. The role of conditioning in drug tolerance and addiction. In: Keehn JD, editor. Psychopathology in Animals: Research and Treatment Implications. Academic Press; New York: 1979. pp. 143–168. [Google Scholar]
  207. Singer BF, Scott-Railton J, Vezina P. Unpredictable saccharin reinforcement enhances locomotor responding to amphetamine. Behavioural Brain Research. 2012;226:340–344. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Singer BF, Tanabe LM, Gorny G, Jake-Matthews C, Li Y, Kolb B, Vezina P. Amphetamine-induced changes in dendritic morphology in rat forebrain correspond to associative drug conditioning rather than nonassociative drug sensitization. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;65:835–840. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.12.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Small DM, Jones-Gotman M, Dagher A. Feeding-induced dopamine release in dorsal striatum correlates with meal pleasantness ratings in healthy human volunteers. NeuroImage. 2003;19:1709–1715. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00253-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  210. Small DM, Zatorre RJ, Dagher A, Evans AC, Jones-Gotman M. Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain. 2001;124:1720–1733. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.9.1720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. Smelson DA, Williams J, Ziedonis D, Sussner BD, Losonczy MF, Engelhart C, Kaune M. A double-blind placebo-controlled pilot study of risperidone for decreasing cue-elicited craving in recently withdrawn cocaine dependent patients. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2004;27:45–49. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2004.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Somerville LH, Jones RM, Casey BJ. A time of change: behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain and Cognition. 2010;72:124–133. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  213. Spear LP. Rewards, aversions and affect in adolescence: emerging convergences across laboratory animal and human data. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;1:390–403. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  214. Steeves TDL, Miyasaki J, Zurowski M, Lang AE, Pellecchia G, Van Eimeren T, Rusjan P, Houle S, Strafella AP. Increased striatal dopamine release in Parkinsonian patients with pathological gambling: a [11C]raclopride PET study. Brain. 2009;132:1376–1385. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  215. Stewart J. Disentangling the sources of opioid withdrawal responses: comment on McDonald and Siegel. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2004;12:20–22. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.12.1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  216. Stewart J, de Wit H, Eikelboom R. The role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychological Review. 1984;91:251–268. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  217. Stewart J, Eikelboom R. Conditioned drug effects. In: Iversen LL, Iversen SD, Snyder SH, editors. Handbook of Psychopharmacology. Plenum Press; New York: 1987. pp. 1–57. [Google Scholar]
  218. Stewart J, Vezina P. Conditioning and behavioral sensitization. In: Kalivas PW, Barnes CD, editors. Sensitization in the Nervous System. Telford Press; Caldwell, New Jersey: 1988. pp. 207–224. [Google Scholar]
  219. Stewart J, Vezina P. Extinction procedures abolish conditioned stimulus control but spare sensitized responding to amphetamine. Behavioural Pharmacology. 1991;2:65–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  220. Stoltenberg SF, Mudd SA, Blow FC, Hill EM. Evaluating measures of family history of alcoholism: density versus dichotomy. Addiction. 1998;93:1511–1520. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.931015117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  221. Strakowski SM, Sax KW. Progressive behavioral response to repeated d-amphetamine challenge: further evidence for sensitization in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 1998;44:1171–1177. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00454-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  222. Strakowski SM, Sax KW, Rosenberg HL, DelBello MP, Adler CM. Human response to repeated low-dose d-amphetamine: evidence for behavioral enhancement and tolerance. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25:548–554. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00253-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  223. Strakowski SM, Sax KW, Setters MJ, Keck PE., Jr Enhanced response to repeated d-amphetamine challenge: evidence for behavioral sensitization in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 1996;40:872–880. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00497-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  224. Suto N, Tanabe LM, Austin JD, Creekmore E, Pham CT, Vezina P. Previous exposure to psychostimulants enhances the reinstatement of cocaine seeking by nucleus accumbens AMPA. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:2149–2159. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  225. Tang DW, Fellows LK, Small DM, Dagher A. Food and drug cues activate similar brain regions: a meta-analysis of functional MRI studies. Physiology and Behavior. 2012;106:317–324. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  226. Tarter RF, Kirisci L, Mezzich A, Cornelius JR, Pajer K, Vanyukov M, Gardner W, Blackson T, Clark D. Neurobiological disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1078–1085. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  227. Thompson JL, Urban N, Slifstein M, Xu X, Kegels LS, Girgis RR, Beckeman Y, Harkavy-Friedman JM, Gil R, Abi-Dargham A. Striatal dopamine release in schizophrenia comorbid with substance dependence. Molecular Psychiatry. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.109. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  228. Tiffany ST. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychological Review. 1990;97:147–168. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.2.147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  229. Toates F. Motivational Systems. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  230. Tolivar BK, McRae-Clark AL, Saladin M, Price KL, Simpson AN, DeSantis SM, Baker NL, Brady KT. Determinants of cue-elicited craving and physiological reactivity in methamphetamine-dependent subjects in the laboratory. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2010;36:106–113. doi: 10.3109/00952991003686402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  231. Tran-Nguyen LTL, Fuchs RA, Coffey GP, Baker DA, O’Dell LE, Neisewander JL. Time-dependent changes in cocaine-seeking behavior and extracellular dopamine levels in the amygdala during cocaine withdrawal. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;19:48–59. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00205-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  232. Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Cowan RL, Woodward ND, Li R, Ansari MS, Baldwin RM, Schwartzman AN, Kessler RM, Zald DH. Dopaminergic mechanisms of individual differences in human effort-based decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:6170–6176. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6459-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  233. Tsuang MT, Lyons MJ, Meyer JM, Doyle T, Éisen SA, Goldberg J, True W, Lin N, Toomey R, Eaves L. Co-occurrence of abuse of different drugs in men. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1998;55:967–972. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.11.967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  234. Urban NBL, Slifstein M, Thompson JL, Xu X, Girgis RR, Raheja S, Haney M, Abi-Dargham A. Dopamine release in chronic cannabis users: A [11C]raclopride positron emission tomography study. Biological Psychiatry. 2012;71:677–683. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  235. van Holst RJ, Veltman DJ, Büchel C, van den Brink W, Goudriaan AE. Distorted expectancy coding in problem gambling: is the addictive in the anticipation? Biological Psychiatry. 2012;71:741–748. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  236. Vanderschuren LJ, Kalivas PW. Alterations in dopaminergic and glu-tamatergic transmission in the induction and expression of behavioral sensitization. A critical review of preclinical studies. Psychopharmacology. 2000;51:99–120. doi: 10.1007/s002130000493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  237. Venugopalan VV, Casey KF, O’Hara C, O’Loughlin J, Benkelfat C, Fellows LK, Leyton M. Acute phenylalanine/tyrosine depletion reduces motivation to smoke cigarettes across stages of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:2469–2476. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  238. Vezina P. Sensitization of midbrain dopamine neuron reactivity and the self-administration of psychomotor stimulant drugs. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;27:827–839. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  239. Vezina P. Sensitization, drug addiction and psychopathology in animals and humans. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2007;31:1553–1555. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.08.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  240. Vezina P, Leyton M. Conditioned cues and the expression of stimulant sensitization in animals and humans. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:160–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  241. Vezina P, Lorrain DS, Arnold GM, Austin JD, Suto N. Sensitization of midbrain dopamine neuron reactivity promotes the pursuit of amphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:4654–4662. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04654.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  242. Vezina P, McGehee DS, Green WN. Exposure to nicotine and sensitization of nicotine-induced behaviors. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2007;31:1625–1638. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.08.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  243. Vezina P, Stewart J. Amphetamine administered to the ventral tegmental area but not to the nucleus accumbens sensitizes rats to systemic morphine: lack of conditioned effects. Brain Research. 1990;516:99–106. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90902-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  244. Vogel VH, Isbell H, Chapman KW. Present status of narcotic addiction. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1948;138:1019–1026. doi: 10.1001/jama.1948.02900140011003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  245. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gatley SJ, Hitzemann R, et al. Decreased striatal dopaminergic responsiveness in detoxified cocaine-dependent subjects. Nature. 1997;386:830–833. doi: 10.1038/386830a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  246. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Jayne M, Franceschi D, Wong C, Gatley SJ, Gifford AN, Ding YS, Pappas N. Nonhedonic food motivation in humans involves dopamine in the dorsal striatum and methylphenidate amplifies this effect. Synapse. 2002;44:175–180. doi: 10.1002/syn.10075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  247. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, et al. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6583–6588. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1544-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  248. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, Jayne M, Ma Y, Wong C. Dopamine increases in striatum do not elicit craving in cocaine abusers unless they are coupled with cocaine cues. NeuroImage. 2008;39:1266–1273. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  249. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Jayne M, Ma Y, Pradhan K, Wong C. Profound decreases in dopamine release in striatum in detoxified alcoholics: possible orbitofrontal involvement. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:12700–12706. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3371-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  250. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Telang F, Maynard L, Logan J, Gatley SJ, Pappas N, Wong C, Vaska P, Zhu W, Swanson JM. Evidence that methylphenidate enhances the saliency of a mathematical task by increasing dopamine in the human brain. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161:1173–1180. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  251. Vollstädt-Klein S, Loeber S, Richter A, Kirsch M, Bach P, von der Goltz C, Hermann D, Mann K, Kiefer F. Validating incenting salience with functional magnetic resonance imaging: association between mesolimbic cue reactivity and attentional bias in alcohol-dependent patients. Addiction Biology. 2011;17:807–816. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00352.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  252. Vollstädt-Klein S, Wichert S, Rabinstein J, Bühler M, Klein O, Ende G, Hermann D, Mann K. Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal striatum. Addiction. 2010;105:1741–1749. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03022.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  253. Volpp KG, Troxel AB, Pauly MV, Glick HA, Puig A, Asch DA, Galvin R, Zhu J, Wan F, DeGuzman J, Corbett E, Weiner J, Audrain-McGovern J. A randomized controlled trial of financial incentives for smoking cessation. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2009;360:699–709. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa0806819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  254. Wachtel SR, de Wit H. Subjective and behavioral effects of repeated d-amphetamine in humans. Behavioral Pharmacology. 1999;10:271–281. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199905000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  255. Wan X, Nakatani H, Ueno K, Asamizuya T, Cheng K, Tanaka K. The neural basis of intuitive best next-move generation in board game experts. Science. 2011;21:341–346. doi: 10.1126/science.1194732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  256. Wang GJ, Geliebter A, Volkow ND, Telang FW, Logan J, Jayne MC, Galanti K, Selig PA, Han H, Zhu W, Wong CT, Fowler JS. Enhanced striatal dopamine release during food stimulation in binge eating disorder. Obesity. 2011;19:1601–1608. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  257. Wang GJ, Smith L, Volkow ND, Telang F, Logan F, Tomasi D, Wong CT, Hoffman W, Jayne M, Alia-Klein N, Thanos P, Fowler JS. Decreased dopamine activity predicts relapse in methamphetamine abusers. Molecular Psychiatry. 2012;17:918–925. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  258. Waters AJ, Marhe R, Franken IH. Attentional bias to drug cues is elevated before and during temptations to use heroin and cocaine. Psychopharmacology. 2012;219:909–921. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2424-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  259. Weiss F, Maldonado-Vlaar CS, Parsons LH, Kerr TM, Smith DL, Ben-Shahar O. Control of cocaine-seeking behavior by drug associated stimuli in rats: effects on recovery of extinguished operant-responding and extracellular dopamine levels in amygdala and nucleus accumbens. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2000;97:4321–4326. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.8.4321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  260. Wikler A. Recent progress in research on the neurophysiological basis of morphine addiction. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1948;105:329–338. doi: 10.1176/ajp.105.5.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  261. Wikler A. Dynamics of drug dependence. Implications of a conditioning theory for research and treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1973;28:611–616. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1973.01750350005001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  262. Winkielman P, Berridge KC, Wilbarger JL. Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2005;31:121–135. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  263. Wong DF, Kuwabara H, Schretlen DJ, Bonson KR, Zhou Y, Nandi A, et al. Increased occupancy of dopamine receptors in human striatum during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2716–2727. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  264. Wrase J, Schlagenhauf F, Kienast T, Wüstenberg T, Bermpohl F, Kahnt T, Beck A, Ströhle A, Juckel G, Knutson B, Heinz A. Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics. NeuroImage. 2007;35:787–794. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  265. Wray JM, Godleski SA, Tiffany ST. Cue-reactivity in the natural environment of cigarette smokers: the impact of photographic and in vivo smoking stimuli. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2011;4:733–737. doi: 10.1037/a0023687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  266. Yang Z, Xie J, Shao YC, Xie CM, Fu LP, Li DJ, Fan M, Ma L, Li SJ. Dynamic neural responses to cue-reactivity paradigms in heroin dependent users: an fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:766–775. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  267. Yau WYW, Zubieta JK, Weiland BJ, Samudra PG, Zucker RA, Heitzeg MH. Nucleus accumbens response to incentive stimuli anticipation in children of alcoholics: relationships with precursive behavioral risk and lifetime alcohol use. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:2544–2551. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1390-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  268. Yoder KK, Morris ED, Constantinescu CC, Cheng TE, Normandin MD, O’Connor SJ, Kareken DA. When what you see isn’t what you get: alcohol cues, alcohol administration, prediction error, and human striatal dopamine. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2009;33:139–149. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00821.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  269. Zald DH, Boileau I, El-Dearedy W, Gunn R, McGlone F, Ditcher GS, Dagher A. Dopamine transmission in the human striatum during monetary reward tasks. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;28:4104–4112. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4643-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  270. Zhao LY, Tian J, Wang W, Qin W, Shi J, Li Q, Yuan K, Dong MH, Yang WC, Wang Y-R, Sun LL, Lu L. The role of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in the regulation of craving by reappraisal in smokers. PLoS One. 2012a;7:e43598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  271. Zhao M, Fan C, Du J, Jiang H, Chen H, Sun H. Cue-induced craving and physiological responses in recently and long-abstinent herion-dependent patients. Addictive Behaviors. 2012b;37:393–398. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.11.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  272. Ziauddeen H, Farooqi IS, Fletcher PC. Obesity and the brain: how convincing is the addiction model? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:279–286. doi: 10.1038/nrn3212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  273. Zijlstra F, Booij J, van den Brink W, Franken IHA. Striatal dopamine D2 receptor binding and dopamine release during cue-elicited craving in recently abstinent opiate-dependent males. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;18:262–270. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2007.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  274. Zijlstra F, Veltman DJ, Booij J, van den Brink W, Franken IHA. Neurobiological substrates of cue-elicited craving and anhedonia in recently abstinent opioid-dependent males. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2009;99:183–192. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  275. Zimmer BA, Oleson EB, Roberts DCS. The motivation to self-administer is increased after a history of spiking brain levels of cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1901–1910. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  276. Zinkernagel C, Naef MR, Bucher HC, Ladewig D, Gyr N, Battegay M. Onset and pattern of substance use in intravenous drug users of an opiate maintenance program. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2001;64:105–109. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(00)00221-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES