Abstract
Insufficient energy compensation after a preload (meal, snack, or beverage) has been associated with excess energy intake, but experimental studies have used heterogeneous methodologies, making energy compensation difficult to predict. The aim of this systematic review was to analyze the relative contributions of two key variables, preload physical form and intermeal interval (IMI), to differences in energy compensation. Forty-eight publications were included, from which percent energy compensation (%EC) data were extracted for 253 interventions (121 liquid, 69 semisolid, 20 solid, and 43 composite preloads). Energy compensation ranged from −370% (overconsumption, mostly of liquids) to 450% (overcompensation). A meta-regression analysis of studies reporting positive energy compensation showed that IMI (as the predominant factor) together with preload physical form and energy contributed significantly to %EC differences, accounting for 50% of the variance, independently from gender and BMI. Energy compensation was maximized when the preload was in semisolid/solid form and the IMI was 30–120 min. These results may assist in the interpretation of studies assessing the relative efficacy of interventions to enhance satiety, including functional foods and weight management products.
Keywords: intermeal interval, physical form, satiety, weight management
Introduction
Energy compensation is defined as the adjustment of energy intake provoked by the previous ingestion of a given stimulus (preload), whether a meal, a snack, or a beverage.1 Insufficient energy compensation both in the short and the long term has been associated with increased energy intakes and positive energy balance, leading to obesity.2,3 Understanding the various influences on energy compensation may assist in controlling energy intake and inform obesity prevention strategies.
Studies focusing on the short-term regulation of energy intake typically employ a preload paradigm, where the effects of a food attribute on subsequent eating can be evaluated.4 This design allows direct observation of energy intake in controlled environments, overcoming the issue of misreporting of intake.4,5 In some cases, the time elapsed until the spontaneous request for the next meal has been used as a surrogate for satiety1,6; however, this time interval reflects, in part, the hormonal and physiological changes related to preload nutrient oxidation.7,8 Time is also an important cognitive element influencing intake, with humans typically consuming more of a preload when it is known that there will be no access to other foods for a particular amount of time.6 On the other hand, when the intermeal interval (IMI) is fixed, the duration of the IMI may be critical in determining the extent of energy compensation9 and be subject to the influence of preload characteristics, such as its physical form (i.e., liquid or solid).2,10
One question that remains unanswered is which time interval following consumption of a preload is the most appropriate for measuring energy compensation and whether this interval is dependent on preload attributes, such as its physical form.8,11 Physiologically, 20 min appears to be the minimum interval for the first post-absorptive effects of the preload to influence energy levels,12 but this is further influenced by food type and subject characteristics.9 Assuming that the rate at which nutrients are delivered from the stomach into the small intestine in healthy subjects is approximately 2–4 kcal/min (8.4–16.8 kJ/min),13,14 then a period of 60–125 min would be required to empty 250 kcal.15 This is corroborated by self-selected IMIs (time until request of a meal) of 1 h and 2 h after carbohydrate-rich and fat-rich preloads (1 MJ, 240 kcal).16 It is postulated that short intervals will allow the detection of gastric and orosensory effects, while post-absorptive ileal and colonic effects will require longer intervals.1,9,17
In general, the effect of any preload manipulation will tend to diminish as the IMI extends9; however, very few studies have analyzed this effect systematically and they have tended to omit the effects of covariates such as the preload's physical form and energy density. In addition, preload studies have traditionally used a wide variety of IMIs (between 5 min and up to 4 h), with considerable variation in the results (probably associated with the different physiological mechanisms involved during eating).1 Many of the studies do not justify the selection of time interval and there appears to be only limited, and inconsistent, empirical data reporting on the differential satiating effects of foods by time.9,18–20
Beyond time and physical form, evidence for a differential effect of gender and BMI on energy compensation has also been controversial.9,11,21–24 When reported, gender effects have been associated with differences in hormonal levels25 and food-related neural processes.26,27 On the other hand, evidence suggests that energy intake regulation is impaired in older adults compared with younger individuals and perceptions of hunger and satiety may also be decreased.28–31
It is known that satiety hormones regulate the IMI in lean subjects7,16,32 but how these processes differ in overweight individuals is still unclear.8,33
The aim of this systematic review of the literature was to quantify the effects of the time interval (IMI) between preload and next meal on energy compensation levels in adults, when analyzed under laboratory conditions. The ways in which this relationship may be modulated by the food's physical form and by other preload attributes such as weight, energy content, and energy density are also investigated. Data on males and females, as well as lean and overweight/obese subjects, were collected, to compare results across subgroups.
It was hypothesized that changes in the IMI would be associated with changes in energy compensation, so that shorter IMIs would be associated with more precise energy compensation, responding to volume effects (stomach stretch receptors), modulated by the food's physical form and, to some extent, energy load and energy density.19,20,34 Compensation was also expected to increase with increasing energy load (post-ingestive effects) and higher volume (lower energy density). Finally, confirmation was sought regarding the reported differences in energy compensation between men and women and between lean and overweight subjects. Preload nutrient composition was not included in the regression analyses due to the initial complexity of the data, but this will be explored in future analyses.
Methods
Search strategy
Data from preload studies published in English in paper and electronic format between January 1990 and January 2011 were extracted by four independent investigators (CR, EAR, KG, and NV). It was considered that this time span would allow the identification of sufficient data given the recent popularity of preload designs (e.g., to validate satiety claims for new and reformulated products). Articles were identified by searching the following databases at four different time periods between October 2008 and January 2011: PubMed (US National Library of Medicine National Institutes of Health), PsycINFO (EBSCO Industries, UK), ISI Web of Knowledge (Thomson Reuters, UK), Google Scholar (Google, UK), and Science Direct (Elsevier B.V., UK). Key words used were “energy compensation,” “preload,” “satiation,” “satiety,” “intermeal interval,” “appetite measures,” and combinations of these. The initial searches were complemented with further searches from specific journal websites and by cross-referencing from literature lists in published articles.35 About 40% of the identified abstracts were screened by at least two investigators. From these searches, three initial databases were created; these were then merged into one final database in which 100% of the extracted entries were confirmed by at least two investigators (Figure 1).
Inclusion criteria for publications
Publications were included in the database if they reported oral consumption data for healthy volunteers, ages 18–65 years, with no history of clinically diagnosed eating disorders. Data on percent energy compensation (%EC) versus a no preload or water control or, in its absence, a low-energy control of no more than 150 kcal had to be included. This was done to broaden the inclusion criteria for solid preloads, which were heavily underrepresented in initial searches. The 150 kcal cut-off value was chosen as representative of a small meal.36,37 Most studies reported energy compensation versus only one control preload. In the few studies where more than one control condition was used, compensation data for only one control comparison were entered into the database to avoid too highly correlated measures of energy compensation.
Studies were included if energy compensation values were provided or if energy intake and preload attributes (i.e., grams, kcal, or kJ) were reported, such that energy compensation could be manually computed. In addition, the IMI had to be reported and be no less than 5 min so the study could be considered a preload design. For the purpose of this review, IMI was defined as the amount of time, in minutes, between the start of the preload and the start of the test meal.6 Thus, the time spent by the subjects consuming the preload was included as part of the IMI. Studies with alternative sweeteners or nutrient replacers were included if they met the above inclusion criteria. When appropriate, the alternative sweetener preload condition was used as control preload. Comparisons of alternative sweetener conditions versus water were not included in the review.
Studies conducted in free-living conditions, where subjects spontaneously requested the meals, or in controlled studies, where the IMIs were either not fixed or varied across preload and control foods, were excluded. Studies utilizing nasogastric or other enterally or parenterally infused preloads were also excluded, because orosensory cues were bypassed in these studies, their results may not be comparable to those of studies in which oral cues were present. Studies involving alcohol-containing preloads were also excluded to avoid confounding due to the appetite-stimulating effects of alcohol.38
Data extraction and construction of the database
Mean energy compensation values were extracted from the published source in numerical form from the text or, when available in graphic form only, a numerical value was obtained using an image-to-data tool (http://www.tushar-mehta.com) and subsequently entered into a spreadsheet. Additional information extracted included participants' sex (male, female, or both); BMI group (lean, overweight, obese, or mixed groups); preload weight (g); preload energy (kcal; kJ); preload energy density (kcal/g; kJ/g, including beverages); intermeal time (minutes), and a description of the preload food (physical form, ingredient composition), as well as of the control preload (ingredient and energy content). In regard to physical form, preloads were categorized as liquids (e.g., beverages, broth-type soups, water); semisolids (e.g., yogurts, jelly, fruit puree); solids (e.g., sandwiches, bread, salad vegetables); and composite meals. Composite meals were defined as any semisolid or solid preload that was accompanied by a drink served in a separate container (e.g., sandwich with beverage). Foods composed of solid and liquid parts, such as chunky vegetable soups or chicken and noodle soups, were classed as semisolids.
Whenever not reported, or if reported in another way, %EC was calculated as follows39:
In this equation, EI represents energy intake at the next meal under the control or preload conditions, excluding the energy of the preload itself and of any subsequent meals eaten; EP represents the energy of the preload (or the difference in preload energy when this was >0).
Values of 100% indicate perfect compensation. Values <100% indicate partial compensation, of which values <0% indicate eating additional energy beyond the preload energy content. For example, 50% EC after consumption of a 200 kcal yogurt preload versus water indicates that subjects consumed 100 kcal less at the next meal after eating the preload than after consuming water, while −50% EC indicates that subjects consumed 100 kcal more after the yogurt than after water (referred to as “overeating” in this review). Values above 100% indicate that the preload suppressed subsequent intake to an extent greater than the energy content of the preload (referred to as “overcompensation”). For example, a %EC value of 150% indicates participants consumed 300 kcal less at the next meal after a 200 kcal preload than after the water.
The total weight of the preload included small (50–100 mL) volumes of water when these were administered to diminish aftertaste or reduce mouth dryness, served together or within 5 min of the preload, as it was considered that this amount of water could mix with the preload volume in the stomach. For composite meals, the weight of the beverages (in g) was included in the total weight of the preload and incorporated in the energy density calculation. For instance, in a study21 providing preloads that consisted of toast (43 g, 100 kcal) and beverage (591 g, 248 kcal), the energy density of the preload was calculated as total kcal/total g, or 348 kcal/634 g, i.e., 0.55 kcal/g. In studies where subjects were asked to taste and consume a small set of foods before the preload (e.g., to rate pleasantness), energy and weight of the taste test foods were included in the preload energy and weight. In cases where preload weights and energy were not reported (<2% of cases) the weight and energy of the taste foods or any preload component was estimated using the USDA National Nutrient Database release 24 (http://www.nal.usda.gov/fnic/foodcomp/search/).
Data management and statistics
The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Version 18 for Windows software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, USA) was used to analyze the data.
The dependent variable was %EC. The predictor variables were as follows: IMI (minutes), preload weight (g); preload energy (kcal); preload energy density (kcal/g); and preload physical form (liquid, semisolid, solid, and composite meal). All variables were treated as continuous data, except for the physical form of the preload, which was treated as a binary dummy variable for each physical form category with liquid as the reference category. BMI was treated as a binary dummy variable to categorize studies with lean subjects only versus studies including lean and overweight/obese subjects. The latter were grouped to allow sufficient sample size for the non-lean studies, which were underrepresented (see Results).
To improve normality, the %EC variable was transformed using the function SQRT (squared root), after collapsing all negative and zero values into zero (as it was considered that a negative %EC and a zero %EC represents a similar behavioral outcome; i.e., the individual either did not respond to the preload energy or actually overate in response to it); the result is referred to hereafter as the energy compensation index (ECI). An ECI value of 10 corresponds to 100% (precise) energy compensation. To illustrate effects across the whole range of data (including negative and null compensation, as well as positive) the untransformed variable, %EC, was used in the descriptive analyses.
Spearman's rank correlation was applied to test the strength and direction of the association between %EC and each of the independent variables (which were also non-normally distributed). The Kruskal-Wallis test was used to compare median %EC values across physical form groups (i.e., liquids, semisolids, solids, and composite meals), and the Mann-Whitney test for differences within physical form category pairs (i.e., median %EC in liquid versus semisolids; liquids versus solids; liquids versus composite meals; semisolids versus solids; semisolids versus composite meals; and solids versus composite meals).
Differences in ECI across physical form groups by gender, BMI category (lean versus other), and IMI category (interventions using IMI of <30 min versus 30–120 min versus >120 min) were explored with ANOVA, including gender and BMI as fixed factors. Zero ECI values (corresponding to null and negative compensation) were excluded from this analysis to achieve homogeneous residual dispersion and, thus, meet the ANOVA assumptions. This exclusion still allows the functional relation between energy compensation and the other variables to be determined when assuming that the preload is effective, thus, representing a means of controlling weight. The Bonferroni correction was applied for post-hoc multiple comparisons. To account for differences in sample size and the presence of repeated measures design across interventions, a weight was applied to each intervention using the following formula: n(1/rep), where “n” is the sample size for the experiment and “rep” is the number of conditions (interventions) each subject was tested on within that experiment, excluding the control condition.40
Meta-regression was used to investigate the relative impact of predictor variables on %EC (as ECI) over time in all interventions reporting positive %EC (n = 234). Initial visualization of the data indicated that %EC values decreased sharply with the first time intervals; this suggested that log10(IMI) would be a more appropriate predictor variable, so this was used in the regression analysis in place of IMI.
The relative impact of log10(IMI), preload weight, preload energy, and preload physical form on ECI changes was investigated with and without adjusting for gender and BMI group and also including interaction effects between log10(IMI) and physical form. Energy density (ED) is a function of preload energy and preload weight, so ED was investigated in a separate model that did not incorporate weight and energy. This model was also tested twice (with and without adjusting for gender and BMI). All regression analyses were performed by weighted least squares with weights as described for ANOVA.
To further explore the effect of the repeated measures design on the dependent variable (ECI), a sensitivity analysis was conducted by adjusting for the variable indicating the number of repeated measures (range, 1–9) in the regression analyses. The level of statistical significance for all analyses was set at α = 0.05.
Testing of the models against newly generated data
The predictive capacity of the regression models was tested against data from two interventions reporting positive %EC values for liquids and semisolids as a representation of the most common preload types, published after the original review was completed.41,42 For this, the published data on IMI, preload weight and energy content, or energy density, and physical form category were entered into each model to calculate the ECI, from which predicted %EC was derived. This was then compared with the %EC values reported for a milk preload41 and with the %EC values for a sucrose-sweetened jelly, a sucrose drink, a glucose-fructose drink, and two (one sweet, one acidic) protein-containing jellies and drinks.42 It was verified that the reported %EC for all eight preloads was based on the %EC calculation method used in this review, and when this was not the case, it was recalculated and reported in the results. Although the jelly preloads were strictly semisolids, their accompaniment by water led them to be considered “composite meals” to match the criteria used in the review.
Results
Database characteristics
The final database contained 253 preload interventions published across 48 independent publications between 1984 and 2011. The preload interventions were 48% liquids11,22,24,29,43–68; 27% semisolid,9,28,39,43,51–57,69–71 17% solids or semisolids accompanied by a drink (composite meals),21,47,51,53,56,62,69,72–78 and 8% exclusively solid preloads.52,57,64,69,73,79–82 The majority of the studies were in lean subjects9,11,21,22,24,28,39,43,44,46,47,49,50,54–57,59,61–63,65–69,71,72,74–82 (78% versus 22% for non-lean29,45,48,51–53,58,60,64,70,73). Studies with males only9,11,21,22,24,28,44,45,47–49,51,53,54,56,59–63,66–68,70,72,74,76,78,80 were overrepresented (48%) compared with studies with only females9,11,21,22,24,43,50,51,55,56,60,65,66,69,70,81,82 or including both genders29,39,46,52,57,58,64,71,73,75,77,79 (35 and 17% respectively). Only a very small number of interventions70 (6%) specifically enrolled restrained subjects, with the majority involving subjects identified as nonrestrained eaters (Table 1).
Table 1.
VariableReferences | Preload examples | No. of interventions | Percentagea |
---|---|---|---|
Physical form | |||
Liquid preloads11,22,24,29,43–,68 | Custom-made beverages (e.g., milkshake with added nutrients), fruit juice, liquid yogurts, milk, soft drinks, smooth soup (e.g., with particles <1 mm), broth, sweetened water | 121 | 48 |
Semisolid preloads9,28,39,43,51–53,55–57,69–71 | Yogurts, jelly, pureed food, chunky soups, pasta soups, thickened milkshakes, pate-type food | 69 | 27 |
Composite meal preloads21,47,51,53,56,62,69,72–78 | Any non-liquid served with water or a drink (e.g., cheese, crackers, and fruit juice; meat casserole with glass of water) | 43 | 17 |
Solid preloads52,57,64,69,73,79–82 | Vegetables, salad, bread, sandwiches, whole fruit, cheese, meat, pasta | 20 | 8 |
Sex | |||
Male participants9,11,21,22,24,28,44,45,47–49,51,53,54,56,59–63,66–68,70,72,74,76,78,80 | Data for male participants only | 120 | 48 |
Female participants9,11,21,22,24,43,50,51,55,56,60,65,66,69,70,81,82 | Data for female participants only | 90 | 35 |
Both sexes29,39,46,52,57,58,64,71,73,75,77,79 | Grouped data for males and females | 43 | 17 |
BMI and restrained eating | |||
Lean participants9,11,21,22,24,28,39,43,44,46,47,49,50,54–57,59,61–63,65–69,71,72,74–82 | Data for non-overweight, non-obese participants | 197 | 78 |
Non-lean participants29,45,48,51–53,58,60,64,70,73 | Includes groups of lean plus overweight or obese subjects | 56 | 22 |
Restrained eaters70 | Defined by validated eating behavior questionnaire (10 lean and 5 obese subjects) | 15 | 6 |
Total interventions | 253 | 100% |
Percentage of number of interventions from total in database.
The median %EC for all 253 foods was 62% (interquartile range [IQR], 74). The %EC values ranged from −379% (overeating, i.e., %EC < 0) to 450% (overcompensation, i.e., %EC > 100). Overeating was restricted predominantly to liquid preloads consumed up to 150 min before the next meal (Figure 2), and was observed in 11% of all liquid preloads (i.e., 13 of 121). The median preload energy, weight, and energy density across all preloads were 263 kcal (IQR, 197), 396 g (IQR, 200), and 0.63 kcal/g (IQR, 0.60), respectively (Table 2).
Table 2.
Preload type | %EC | IMI (min) | Weight (g) | Energy (kcal) | ED (kcal/g) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
All foods (n = 253) | |||||
Range | −379 to 450 | 5 to 240 | 24 to 1,225 | 37 to 1,175 | 0.18 to 3.02 |
Median | 62 | 60 | 396 | 263 | 0.63 |
IQR | 74 | 90 | 200 | 197 | 0.60 |
Liquids (n = 121) | |||||
Range | −305 to 375 | 5 to 225 | 95 to 800 | 37 to 800 | 0.18 to 1.66 |
Median | 43a | 60 | 415 | 200 | 0.45 |
IQR | 64 | 90 | 200 | 150 | 0.4 |
Semisolids (n = 69) | |||||
Range | 2 to 318 | 5 to 225 | 100 to 750 | 39 to 800 | 0.32 to 1.47 |
Median | 99b | 30 | 350 | 357 | 0.75 |
IQR | 66 | 60 | 150 | 220 | 0.56 |
Composite meals (n = 43) | |||||
Range | −379 to 178 | 5 to 210 | 54 to 1,225 | 48 to 1,175 | 0.21 to 1.91 |
Median | 62 | 100 | 410 | 274 | 0.63 |
IQR | 60 | 104 | 363 | 176 | 0.51 |
Solids (n = 20) | |||||
Range | −36 to 450 | 5 to 240 | 24 to 693 | 50 to 658 | 0.33 to 3.02 |
Median | 83 | 30 | 260 | 307 | 1.33 |
IQR | 85 | 195 | 190 | 347 | 1.82 |
The median %EC in liquids differs from semisolids with p < 0.001, and from solids with p < 0.05.
The median %EC in semisolids differs from composite meals with p < 0.01.
When grouped by physical form, the median %EC values were as follows: 99% for semisolids (IQR, 66); 83% for solids (IQR, 85); 62% for composite meals (IQR, 60); and 43% for liquids (IQR, 64). These differences were significant when tested with the Kruskal Wallis test (X2 = 38.07; p < 0.001). Significant differences were detected between %EC medians in liquids versus semisolids and solids; and in semisolids versus composite meals (Table 2). Liquids had the lowest energy load, highest volume, and lowest ED, while solids had the highest ED, as expected. The median IMI was 60 min for all food interventions (IQR, 90 min). IMI ranged widely within all physical form categories (Table 2).
Association between energy compensation and IMI and preload physical form
Percent energy compensation correlated significantly with IMI for all food interventions (n = 253, p < 0.001). The association was strongest for semisolid foods but was robust across physical form subgroups (Figure 2).
For the studies reporting positive %EC only, the ECI correlated well with the IMI (r = −0.430, p < 0.001, n = 234) and even better with log10(IMI) (r = −0.475, p < 0.001), indicating that log10(IMI) was probably a better predictor variable than IMI. Time interval alone as log10(IMI) explained 23% of the variance in ECI in a simple regression model. When explored by gender and BMI subgroups, ECI correlated significantly with log10(IMI) in females (r = –0.513, p < 0.001), males (r = –0.499, p < 0.001), lean subjects (r = –0.395, p < 0.001), and in the non-lean group (r = –0.731, p < 0.001). These models, however, do not take into account possible modulating effects of other variables such as physical form.
Differences in ECI across physical form subgroups were significant for the whole sample (F3 = 14.66, p < 0.001). Post-hoc subgroup comparisons revealed that ECI for liquids (mean ± SEM 7.12 ± 0.27) was significantly lower than for semisolids (9.52 ± 0.41, p < 0.001) and solids (10.86 ± 0.64, p < 0.001); but not different from composite meals (7.81 ± 0.47, p > 0.05); while composite meals differed significantly from semisolids (p < 0.05) and solids (p < 0.01). There was no interaction between physical form and gender (p = 0.08) or physical form and BMI (p = 0.30).
IMI was divided in three meaningful categories (with cut-offs at 30 and 120 min representing relatively short and long IMIs commonly used in the literature) and found to have a significant interaction with preload physical form (p = 0.014). Hence, a subgroup analysis by categories of IMI was conducted by ANOVA, which revealed a significant main effect of physical form within interventions using IMIs < 30 min (F3 = 3.92, p < 0.05) and IMIs 30–120 min (F3 = 8.97, p < 0.001) but not in studies with longer IMIs (>120 min). Post-hoc tests revealed differences in liquids versus semisolids for studies using IMIs < 30 min, which were maintained when IMI extended to 30–120 min. For this time interval, liquid preloads differed also from solids. Beyond 120 min, there were no differences across physical forms (Figure 3).
Correlation between energy compensation and preload weight, energy content, and energy density
No significant correlations were detected between %EC and preload weight for the whole dataset or for physical form subgroups (Figure 4a).
A significant correlation was identified between %EC and preload energy content for all food interventions (p < 0.01) (Figure 4b). This correlation was strongest for semisolid foods (rho = –0.549, p < 0.001) and was still significant for solid foods (rho = –0.541) and composite meal preloads (rho = –0.331; both p < 0.05) but not liquid preloads (p > 0.05).
A significant correlation was identified for %EC with preload energy density for all food interventions (p < 0.05), and this was driven mainly by the semisolid preloads (rho = –0.544; p < 0.001) (Figure 4c). The remaining correlations were non-significant (liquids; composite meals and solid preloads all p > 0.05).
After transformation, ECI still correlated significantly with preload energy content (r = –0.166; p < 0.01), but not with preload weight or energy density.
Relative contributions of time and preload characteristics to changes in percent energy compensation
Meta-regression analysis was used to investigate the relative impact of IMI and preload characteristics on the ECI variable (n = 234). Both models used liquid as the reference physical form. In the first model, ECI was regressed on log10(IMI), preload weight and energy content and in the second one, on log10(IMI) and energy density. Adjusting for BMI and gender did not significantly affect the results; therefore, these two covariates were dropped in subsequent analyses. Interactions between log10(IMI) and physical form were also included in the model initially but appeared to be non-significant and were subsequently dropped. The results of each model analysis are shown in Table 3. In the first model, the variables with the largest effect in decreasing order of standardized coefficient's size, were log(IMI), semisolid physical form, solid form, energy content, preload weight, and, lastly, composite meal form. All the variables made a significant contribution to the model (p < 0.05), which explained 50% of the variance in ECI (R2 = 0.499, F6 = 37.67, p < 0.001) (Table 3). The effect of time should be interpreted in a multiplicative way in that the same effect of about −0.5 log10(IMI) on ECI would occur between 1 and 10 min,10 and 100 min, and 100 and 1,000 min.
Table 3.
Variablea | B | SE for B | 95% confidence interval for B | Standardized β coefficients | P value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1b | |||||
(Constant) | 14.462 | 0.737 | (13.009–15.914) | <0.001 | |
LogIMI (min) | −4.207 | 0.424 | (−5.042–−3.371) | −0.514 | <0.001 |
Weight (g or mL) | 0.002 | 0.001 | (0.000–0.005) | 0.139 | 0.017 |
Energy (kcal) | −0.004 | 0.001 | (−0.007–−0.002) | −0.235 | <0.001 |
Semisolid = 1 (Other = 0) | 2.284 | 0.389 | (1.518–3.050) | 0.296 | <0.001 |
Solid = 1 (Other = 0) | 3.198 | 0.585 | (2.046–4.350) | 0.287 | <0.001 |
Comp. meal = 1 (Other = 0) | 1.016 | 0.428 | (0.173–1.860) | 0.120 | 0.018 |
Model 2c | |||||
(Constant) | 15.466 | 0.711 | (14.065–16.868) | <0.001 | |
LogIMI (min) | −4.285 | 0.399 | (−5.071–−3.499) | −0.523 | <0.001 |
Energy density (kcal/g) | −1.548 | 0.341 | (−2.220–−0.877) | −0.257 | <0.001 |
Semisolid = 1 (Other = 0) | 2.134 | 0.382 | (1.382–2.886) | 0.276 | <0.001 |
Solid = 1 (Other = 0) | 4.283 | 0.653 | (2.998–5.569) | 0.384 | <0.001 |
Comp. meal = 1 (Other = 0) | 1.105 | 0.419 | (0.280–1.930) | 0.130 | 0.009 |
The liquid form was used as the reference physical form in each model.
ECI = 14.462 − (4.207*logIMI) + (0.002*g) − (0.004*kcal) + (2.284 if semisolid) + (3.198 if solid) + (1.016 if composite meal).
ECI = 15.466 − (4.285*logIMI) − (1.548*kcal/g) + (2.134 if semisolid) + (4.283 if solid) + (1.105 if composite meal).
Abbreviations: SE, standard error.
Results for the model including preload ED were similar, with log(IMI) being the strongest contributing variable, followed by solid physical form, semisolid form, and then ED. Overall, this model also explained 50% of the variance in ECI (R2 = 0.502, F5 = 45.94, p < 0.001) (Table 3).
Adjusting for the variable indicating the number of repeated measures in the regression analyses had virtually no change on the direction or magnitude of the coefficients and the p-values (only a small increase in the ECI of 0.195 per repeated measure (p = 0.046) was detected in the model with energy and weight, with no significant effects detected for the model with ED).
Predictive capacity of the models
Based on the two models generated in this review, the predicted %EC for the preloads examined by Maersk et al.41 and by Akhavan et al.42 were within the 95% confidence interval for the reported %EC; the two exceptions were the acid protein jelly and the acid protein drink preloads, for which the predicted values were slightly below the lower end of the reported %EC confidence interval (Table 4).
Table 4.
Reference | No. of subjects | Intervention and control preloadsa | IMI (min) | Reported mean %EC ± s.e.m. | Reported 95%CI for mean | Predicted %EC (based on ECI) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Maersk et al. (2012)41 | 24 obese subjects | 500 mL (950 kJ, 227 kcal) of semi-skimmed milk against 500 mL water | 240 | 14.7% ± 29.0% | (−42.1, 71.5) | 21% (model 1) 21% (model 2) |
Akhavan et al. (2011)42 | 15 lean men | 300 mL (1,340 kJ, 320.1 kcal) of sucrose-sweetened jelly against 300 mL of sucralose-sweetened water (<0.5 kcal) | 60 | 32.2 ± 19.2% (30.2%)b | (−5.4, 69.8) | 57% (model 1) 59% (model 2) |
300 mL (1,340 kJ, 320.1 kcal) of sucrose drink against same control | 60 | 35.3 ± 21.6% (33.1%)b | (−7.0, 77.6) | 42% (model 1) 44% (model 2) | ||
300 mL (1,340 kJ, 320.1 kcal) of glucose-fructose drink against same control | 60 | 35.8 ± 23.7% (33.6%)b | (−10.7, 82.3) | 42% (model 1) 44% (model 2) | ||
Akhavan et al. (2011)42 | 14 lean men | 300 mL (1,255 kJ, 300 kcal) of sweet whey jelly against 300 mL water | 60 | 82.1 ± 16.6% | (49.6, 114.6) | 58% (model 1) 61% (model 2) |
300 mL (1,255 kJ, 300 kcal) of acid whey jelly against 300 mL water | 60 | 112.8 ± 19.9% | (73.8, 151.8) | 58% (model 1) 61% (model 2) | ||
300 mL (1,255 kJ, 300 kcal) of sweet whey drink against 300 mL water | 60 | 78.7 ± 22.0% | (35.6, 121.8) | 43% (model 1) 45% (model 2) | ||
300 mL (1,255 kJ, 300 kcal) of acid whey drink against 300 mL water | 60 | 89.2 ± 19.9% | (50.2, 128.2) | 43% (model 1) 45% (model 2) |
The sugar-sweetened beverage preload in Maersk et al. was not used as it reports an average EC below zero (overeating). The preloads in Akhavan et al. were served followed by 100 mL of water, which was added to the weight to calculate predicted %EC.
%EC was calculated following this review's methodology (incorporates full weight and EC for both intervention and control preloads).
Abbreviations: ECI, energy compensation index; s.e.m., standard error of the mean.
Discussion
The findings of this review demonstrate that under controlled laboratory conditions, the IMI is a strong determinant of energy compensation differences. Moreover, when examined in the context of BMI, gender, preload physical form, and preload weight, energy, and energy density, the IMI is the strongest contributing variable, followed by the physical form of the preload and its energy density. The regression models generated are able to explain up to 50% of the variance in %EC. Nearly half of the interventions consisted of liquid preloads, while solid preloads were underrepresented (<10%). The majority of studies employed lean individuals (frequently men), while less than 25% of interventions involved overweight and/or obese participants. In agreement with this review's main hypothesis, energy compensation decreased in all types of preloads the longer the IMI. Compensatory behavior appeared to decrease faster over time with semisolids and solids than with liquids, but results for solid preloads need to be interpreted carefully due to the small number of interventions identified. Liquid preloads were associated with incomplete energy compensation (%EC < 100) more frequently than other preloads, as documented previously.10,19,21 Of the 253 interventions examined, 144 (57%) reported incomplete compensation (i.e., energy compensation levels of 75% or less), and these were mainly associated with liquid preload results. Thus, among the liquid preload interventions, 73% (89 of 121) reported incomplete compensation. In contrast, 58% of the composite meal interventions, 35% of the solid, and 33% of the semisolid reported incomplete compensation of 75% or less. This suggests that studies with semisolid or solid preloads are more likely to result in close to, or precise, compensation (i.e., 76–100%) than studies including liquids alone, or as part of a meal. Further, overeating (%EC < 0) was detected in 11% of the liquid preloads (13 of 121 interventions) but in only 2 of the 43 composite meal interventions and in none of the semisolid interventions. Although the frequency of overeating with solid preloads was high (15%; 3 of 20), the sample of solid interventions may have been underrepresented. Overcompensation (%EC > 100), however, occurred in all preload types when the IMI was very short (<30 min).
Role of preload physical form on energy compensation
Overall, the results confirm the reported weak satiating effects of liquid preloads and their effects on energy compensation.2,10,19,83–86 Semisolids incorporate two properties that have been associated with satiety and that are not present in combination in any other physical form; these are the relatively large water content and the textural effects associated with chewing and affecting eating rate.83,85 These properties may explain the enhanced satiating capacity of semisolids over other preloads at short time intervals. In contrast, beverages have a high water content, but are consumed faster than semisolids,85,87 while solids are associated with greater chewing effort and longer oral exposure,88 which has been linked with higher satiety than liquids.62,82,89 The effects of eating rate and oral exposure relate, in part, to the concept of the taste system as a nutrient system (i.e., the sensory properties of food having an effect on satiety), as recently demonstrated in controlled studies.90,91 Sensory but also cognitive aspects (i.e., odor, visual texture, food size, and the perception of food as something to quench thirst versus hunger) have been implicated in different oral, hormonal, and gastrointestinal responses affecting the satiating properties of liquids, semisolids, and solids.6,85–88,92,93 It is, therefore, important to be cautious when categorizing the product's physical form, especially for foods at the interface of categories (e.g., yogurts, soups, and custards).
Analyses with studies reporting positive energy compensation
Results from the regression analyses support the hypothesis that the IMI is a major explanatory variable for changes in ECI, while the relationship between the log of time and ECI is shifted but not modified by physical form (based on the significant main effect of physical form and non-significant interactions between time and physical form). Accordingly, in these models, the regression lines for different types of physical forms are parallel; however, as the time variable has been transformed to a logarithmic scale to allow better fit of a linear model, the actual relationship with time is non-linear. This result is consistent with findings from free-living studies, which show a positive correlation between IMIs and the energy content of the meal.37 As the contribution to changes in %EC was significant for both time and physical form, and together they contribute to a large amount of the variability in %EC, it can be concluded that both factors are important to consider when designing laboratory studies on energy compensation. For example, while IMIs shorter than 30 min appear to produce large variability in individual compensatory responses, especially for liquid preloads, the effects of physical form and energy dissipate beyond 2 h when the energy content of the stomach has decreased and hunger signals become stronger.37 Thus, studies employing shorter or longer time gaps may not allow the detection of significant responses to texture manipulations or physical form contrasts due to excess variability or small size effects. To avoid such limitations, a possible useful starting point when designing a preload study would be to consider intervals of 30–120 min as potentially maximizing energy compensation. Within this timeframe, the most appropriate interval will depend on the study hypothesis, combined with specific preload characteristics (i.e., if physical form can be chosen, semisolid or solid should be considered first to maximize compensation). Beyond the variables studied in this review, the potential role of other factors should also be considered, such as the preload macronutrient composition, energy needs of the subjects, and nature (i.e., restricted meal versus buffet) of the test meal.1,48,94
The effects of physical form varied across time intervals (i.e., from <30 min to >120 min), which suggests that such physical effects are subject to the effects of time and confirming that time is a main factor (Figure 3). Thus, in the first 30 min after ingestion, semisolids and solids are the physical forms of food that are able to elicit the most precise compensation; however, semisolids quickly become less satiating beyond this time, and by 2 h, all foods elicit similar levels of incomplete compensation.
Preload ED was a weaker predictor of changes in %EC than time and physical form, but its contribution was still significant. From model 1, described above, it appears that the contribution of ED toward energy compensation may depend more on the energy content of the preloads and slightly less on the volume. A significant correlation was found between both %EC and ECI with preload energy, but not with preload weight. However, contrary to our expectations, and to the well-known hypothesis that mass/volume is a more important determinant of energy intake than energy content,95 the higher energy preloads were associated with less, not more, compensation. Some of the preload studies analyzed did not match the preload foods for palatability. As palatability correlates positively with energy density,89 and has been associated with increased consumption through sensory stimulation,96,97 interactions between palatability and energy density may have occurred, which could have confounded the results. In addition, the significant correlation between energy content and energy density present in our dataset (rho = 0.71, p < 0.01) may partly explain these results. Energy density incorporates both energy content and weight/volume simultaneously, either of which can influence %EC to a different extent. Employing a combination variable instead of weight and energy may have eliminated part of the confounding.
It is important to note two traits of the prediction models described here. First, the direction of the change in %EC (as ECI) is as predicted. That is, the longer the IMI, and the higher the ED, the smaller the %EC observed. This last result is probably associated with volume effects being stronger shortly after eating,98 particularly for liquid and semisolid foods, which, at a lower ED, would elicit stronger stomach stretch receptor signals than solids,99 in addition to different cognitive and sensory processes.86 Second, the magnitude of the predicted change in %EC is similar whether the model includes weight and energy content or ED. These two facts suggest that these models, albeit with a degree of error, appear robust in estimating changes in %EC. To confirm this, the models were tested against newly generated data from two recent publications, which were not included in the review; in seven of the eight interventions examined, the predicted value fell within the 95%CI of the reported value (differences in the jelly preload disappeared when it was categorized as semisolid).
Although both models can, in principle, be applied to predict changes in energy compensation under laboratory conditions for hypothetical preloads, in practical terms, model 1 is more specific because it defines both weight and energy content. For instance, a dairy product preload served 90 min before a test meal, weighing 150 g, with an energy content of 150 kcal, would be expected to elicit, on average, between 60% and 70% compensation if consumed as a semisolid (e.g., fromage frais or yogurt) but only 30–35% compensation if provided as a liquid yogurt beverage. Similarly, a 28 g (132 kcal) granola bar consumed with 200 mL water, would be expected to elicit precise compensation up to 20 min, based on model 1. These predictions have an associated error that depends on the level of unexplained dispersion (1- R2), i.e., variability in responses to preload interventions that is not explained by the linear regressions.
No clear effects of BMI category or sex were observed using this dataset. While %EC appeared to decrease faster in the non-lean group than in the lean group when using the whole dataset, such effects disappeared when analyzed in the context of other parameters (e.g., physical form, time). Some studies used preloads that were adjusted to the subject-specific energy requirements (including gender and BMI), while others did not. It is possible that these methodological differences, plus the inclusion of subjects with a wide range of BMIs in the non-lean category (as opposed to only overweight or obese, underrepresented in our dataset) could have masked some of the effects of BMI. A larger sample size for studies with exclusively overweight and obese subjects may overcome this problem.
Model validity and study limitations
The meta-regression analysis in this review has some important limitations. First, it was based on study-level summary results (i.e., average %EC) that did not include any information on the variability of the study-level estimates of energy compensation, which would have allowed fixed and random effects meta-analyses to be applied.100 Moreover, some of the summary-level results were obtained on the same group of subjects (with different preloads); therefore, some of the energy compensation summary values are likely to be correlated. These two issues were addressed by weighing the studies so that the weight of a study in the multiple regression analysis is directly proportional to sample size and inversely proportional to the number of times a study was repeated on the same subjects. It is recognized that the influence of a study estimate on the results of a meta-regression analysis depends on its standard errors and that, ideally, meta-regression analysis of such a dataset would be performed on individual participants' data to account for the within-subject variability; however, data on individual variability was not directly available for most of the preload interventions included in the analysis. The lack of such data also prevented us from taking into account the dependence of some of the effect size estimates in the meta-regression. These limitations of the model, which were partially addressed by the weighting, might have caused a decrease in the standard errors of the final estimates but would be unlikely to modify the direction and interpretation of the relationships between energy compensation, IMI, and preload physical form. The sensitivity analysis further confirmed that a repeated measures design had a very minimal impact on the results. Nonetheless, because there was a small significant increase in the ECI as the repeated sessions increased, this is probably an important variable to consider when planning future studies (i.e., to minimize learning effects that may lead subjects to compensate more as the number of exposures increases).
The liquid and semisolid preloads were overrepresented, which means the models are less accurate for solids and composite meals. Also, the majority of the studies included were in lean, adult subjects <65 years, making extrapolation of findings to the obese population and to other age groups less accurate. Only a very small number of interventions specifically enrolled restrained subjects, with the majority involving non-restrained (and presumably not disinhibited) eaters, such that these findings are not generalizable to individuals displaying dietary restriction or disinhibition. Finally, the models are able to explain only 50% of the variability in energy compensation for studies employing time gaps of up to 4 h and using a fixed IMI rather than spontaneous IMI. More research is needed to investigate what other factors are associated with changes in compensation, and in which direction these occur, as well as how these vary over the course of the day, or for longer periods of time as a reflection of free-living conditions. While these will offer complexity that needs to be addressed through future analyses, they are nonetheless relevant, with examples including the following: the macronutrient composition of the preload, including the protein and fiber content and glycemic properties; the preload palatability; whether the preload energy content was fixed versus proportional to the energy needs of the subject; and the format of the subsequent meal, i.e., buffet-style or restricted item.
Conclusion
Overall, the results of this study show that energy compensation under laboratory conditions is affected by a combination of time factors and the physical form of the preload. In studies employing the preload paradigm with semisolid, solid, and composite meal preloads, and in some cases liquid preloads, participants were able to compensate all or part of the energy consumed in the preload, or even overcompensate, when the time interval to the next meal was short (<30 min). However, this compensatory behavior became weaker with longer IMIs and less sharp, depending on the preload's physical form. On average, compensation drops below 60%, irrespective of physical form, when the IMI exceeds 2 h. To our knowledge, these are the first empirical data demonstrating a significant, robust effect of time over other preload attributes in energy compensation studies. Using the models described here, it was possible to quantify the relative impact of various factors on energy compensation when present simultaneously in the same study design; this provides new information beyond the effects of the individual variables on their own. Based on a large number of studies, it can be concluded that the preload paradigm is a useful methodology to assess the effects of particular food attributes on energy compensation in the short term and under controlled environmental conditions. These results may assist in the design and interpretation of experimental studies investigating the relative efficacy of new interventions to enhance satiety, such as functional foods or commercial weight-management products.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Professor Kees de Graaf for advice on the TM Excel add-in application and Miss Rhiannon Williams for initial contributions to the literature searches and data analysis.
Funding
This study was sponsored by the Department of Clinical Sciences, University of Chester and by the UK Medical Research Council (Programme U105960389).
Author contributions
All authors contributed to the writing of the manuscript. E.A-R. designed the study, contributed to the literature searches, data extraction, and independent reviewing; performed the statistical analyses; and wrote a first draft of the manuscript. LP advised on the study design, statistical analyses, and interpretation of results. CR, KG, and NV contributed to the literature searches, data extraction, independent reviewing, and initial data analysis. AD and SJ contributed to the study design and interpretation of results.
Declaration of interest
The authors have no relevant interests to declare.
References
- 1.Blundell J, de Graaf C, Hulshof T, et al. Appetite control: methodological aspects of the evaluation of foods. Obes Rev. 2010;11:251–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00714.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Almiron-Roig E, Chen Y, Drewnowski A. Liquid calories and the failure of satiety: how good is the evidence? Obes Rev. 2003;4:201–212. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-789x.2003.00112.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jebb SA, Siervo M, Fruhbeck G, et al. Variability of appetite control mechanisms in response to 9 weeks of progressive overfeeding in humans. Int J Obes (Lond) 2006;30:1160–1162. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kissileff HR. Effects of physical state (liquid-solid) of foods on food intake: procedural and substantive contributions. Am J Clin Nutr. 1985;42(5 Suppl):956–965. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/42.5.956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Livingstone MBE, Robson PJ, Welch RW, et al. Methodological issues in the assessment of satiety. Scand J Nutr. 2000;44:98–103. [Google Scholar]
- 6.de Graaf C, de Jong LS, Lambers AC. Palatability affects satiation but not satiety. Physiol Behav. 1999;66:681–688. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00335-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Chapelot D, Aubert R, Marmonier C, et al. An endocrine and metabolic definition of the intermeal interval in humans: evidence for a role of leptin on the prandial pattern through fatty acid disposal. Am J Clin Nutr. 2000;72:421–431. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/72.2.421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Blom WA, de Graaf C, Lluch A, et al. Postprandial ghrelin responses are associated with the intermeal interval in time-blinded normal weight men, but not in obese men. Physiol Behav. 2009;96:742–748. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rolls BJ, Kim S, McNelis AL, et al. Time course of effects of preloads high in fat or carbohydrate on food intake and hunger ratings in humans. Am J Physiol. 1991;260(4 Pt 2):R756–R763. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1991.260.4.R756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mattes R. Fluid calories and energy balance: the good, the bad, and the uncertain. Physiol Behav. 2006;89:66–70. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Soenen S, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. No differences in satiety or energy intake after high-fructose corn syrup, sucrose, or milk preloads. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:1586–1594. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/86.5.1586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Booth DA, Toates FM, Platt SV. Control systems for hunger and its implications in animals and man. In: Novin D, Wyrwicka W, Bray GA, editors. Hunger: Basic Mechanisms and Clinical Implications. New York: Raven Press; 1976. pp. 127–142. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jones KL, O'Donovan D, Russo A, et al. Effects of drink volume and glucose load on gastric emptying and postprandial blood pressure in healthy older subjects. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol. 2005;289:G240–G248. doi: 10.1152/ajpgi.00030.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Brener W, Hendrix TR, McHugh PR. Regulation of the gastric emptying of glucose. Gastroenterology. 1983;85:76–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Brand JG, Cagan RH, Naim M. Chemical senses in the release of gastric and pancreatic secretions. Annu Rev Nutr. 1982;2:249–276. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nu.02.070182.001341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Melanson KJ, Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Saris WH, et al. Blood glucose patterns and appetite in time-blinded humans: carbohydrate versus fat. Am J Physiol. 1999;277(2 Pt 2):R337–R345. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1999.277.2.R337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Reid M, Hetherington M. Relative effects of carbohydrates and protein on satiety – a review of methodology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1997;21:295–308. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(96)00024-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Chungchunlam SM, Moughan PJ, Henare SJ, et al. Effect of time of consumption of preloads on measures of satiety in healthy normal weight women. Appetite. 2012;59:281–288. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mattes RD, Campbell WW. Effects of food form and timing of ingestion on appetite and energy intake in lean young adults and in young adults with obesity. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:430–437. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2008.11.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Almiron-Roig E, Flores SY, Drewnowski A. No difference in satiety or in subsequent energy intakes between a beverage and a solid food. Physiol Behav. 2004;82:671–677. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Almiron-Roig E, Drewnowski A. Hunger, thirst, and energy intakes following consumption of caloric beverages. Physiol Behav. 2003;79:767–773. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00212-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ranawana DV, Henry CJ. Are caloric beverages compensated for in the short-term by young adults? An investigation with particular focus on gender differences. Appetite. 2010;55:137–146. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.05.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rolls BJ, Fedoroff IC, Guthrie JF. Gender differences in eating behavior and body weight regulation. Health Psychol. 1991;10:133–142. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.10.2.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Davy BM, Van Walleghen EL, Orr JS. Sex differences in acute energy intake regulation. Appetite. 2007;49:141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dye L, Blundell JE. Menstrual cycle and appetite control: implications for weight regulation. Hum Reprod. 1997;12:1142–1151. doi: 10.1093/humrep/12.6.1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Smeets PA, de Graaf C, Stafleu A, et al. Effect of satiety on brain activation during chocolate tasting in men and women. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83:1297–1305. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/83.6.1297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Uher R, Treasure J, Heining M, et al. Cerebral processing of food-related stimuli: effects of fasting and gender. Behav Brain Res. 2006;169:111–119. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rolls BJ, Dimeo KA, Shide DJ. Age-related impairments in the regulation of food intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 1995;62:923–931. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/62.5.923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Van Walleghen EL, Orr JS, Gentile CL, et al. Habitual physical activity differentially affects acute and short-term energy intake regulation in young and older adults. Int J Obes (Lond) 2007;31:1277–1285. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Roberts SB, Fuss P, Heyman MB, et al. Control of food intake in older men. JAMA. 1994;272:1601–1606. doi: 10.1001/jama.1994.03520200057036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hays NP, Roberts SB. The anorexia of aging in humans. Physiol Behav. 2006;88:257–266. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.05.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cummings DE, Frayo RS, Marmonier C, et al. Plasma ghrelin levels and hunger scores in humans initiating meals voluntarily without time- and food-related cues. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2004;287:E297–E304. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00582.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Callahan HS, Cummings DE, Pepe MS, et al. Postprandial suppression of plasma ghrelin level is proportional to ingested caloric load but does not predict intermeal interval in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89:1319–1324. doi: 10.1210/jc.2003-031267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.DellaValle DM, Roe LS, Rolls BJ. Does the consumption of caloric and non-caloric beverages with a meal affect energy intake? Appetite. 2005;44:187–193. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mattes RD. Dietary compensation by humans for supplemental energy provided as ethanol or carbohydrate in fluids. Physiol Behav. 1996;59:179–187. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02007-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nutrition & Diet Resources UK. Weight loss you can see. A food portion picture guide for a calorie-controlled diet. Ref. 9262 2011; Available at: http://www.ndr-uk.org Accessed 18 April 2013.
- 37.de Castro JM. Physiological, environmental, and subjective determinants of food intake in humans: a meal pattern analysis. Physiol Behav. 1988;44:651–659. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90331-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Caton SJ, Bate L, Hetherington M. Acute effects of an alcoholic drink on food intake: aperitif versus co-ingestion. Physiol Behav. 2007;90:368–375. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Zandstra EH, Mathey MF, Graaf C, et al. Short-term regulation of food intake in children, young adults and the elderly. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2000;54:239–246. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hedges LV, Tipton E, Johnson MC. Robust variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Res Synth Methods. 2010;1:39–65. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Maersk M, Belza A, Holst JJ, et al. Satiety scores and satiety hormone response after sucrose-sweetened soft drink compared with isocaloric semi-skimmed milk and with non-caloric soft drink: a controlled trial. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2012;66:523–529. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2011.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Akhavan T, Luhovyy BL, Anderson GH. Effect of drinking compared with eating sugars or whey protein on short-term appetite and food intake. Int J Obes. 2011;35:562–569. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2010.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Rolls BJ, Hetherington M, Burley VJ. Sensory stimulation and energy density in the development of satiety. Physiol Behav. 1988;44:727–733. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90053-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rolls BJ, Bell EA, Waugh BA. Increasing the volume of a food by incorporating air affects satiety in men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2000;72:361–368. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/72.2.361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Abou Samra R, Wolever TM, Anderson GH. Enhanced food intake regulatory responses after a glucose drink in hyperinsulinemic men. Int J Obes. 2007;31:1222–1231. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Almiron-Roig E, Grathwohl D, Green H, et al. Impact of some isoenergetic snacks on satiety and next meal intake in healthy adults. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2009;22:469–474. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-277X.2009.00978.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Anderson GH, Catherine NL, Woodend DM, et al. Inverse association between the effect of carbohydrates on blood glucose and subsequent short-term food intake in young men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002;76:1023–1030. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/76.5.1023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Anderson GH, Tecimer SN, Shah D, et al. Protein source, quantity, and time of consumption determine the effect of proteins on short-term food intake in young men. J Nutr. 2004;134:3011–3015. doi: 10.1093/jn/134.11.3011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bertenshaw EJ, Lluch A, Yeomans MR. Satiating effects of protein but not carbohydrate consumed in a between-meal beverage context. Physiol Behav. 2008;93:427–436. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.de Graaf C, Hulshof T, Weststrate JA, et al. Short-term effects of different amounts of protein, fats, and carbohydrates on satiety. Am J Clin Nutr. 1992;55:33–38. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/55.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Flood JE, Rolls BJ. Soup preloads in a variety of forms reduce meal energy intake. Appetite. 2007;49:626–634. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Flood-Obbagy JE, Rolls BJ. The effect of fruit in different forms on energy intake and satiety at a meal. Appetite. 2009;52:416–422. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Himaya A, Louis-Sylvestre J. The effect of soup on satiation. Appetite. 1998;30:199–210. doi: 10.1006/appe.1997.0138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Holt SH, Sandona N, Brand-Miller JC. The effects of sugar-free vs sugar-rich beverages on feelings of fullness and subsequent food intake. Int J Food Sci Nutr. 2000;51:59–71. doi: 10.1080/096374800100912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hulshof T, De Graaf C, Weststrate JA. The effects of preloads varying in physical state and fat content on satiety and energy intake. Appetite. 1993;21:273–286. doi: 10.1006/appe.1993.1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Kissileff HR, Gruss LP, Thornton J, et al. The satiating efficiency of foods. Physiol Behav. 1984;32:319–332. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90147-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Lavin JH, French SJ, Ruxton CH, et al. An investigation of the role of oro-sensory stimulation in sugar satiety? Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2002;26:384–388. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Monsivais P, Perrigue MM, Drewnowski A. Sugars and satiety: does the type of sweetener make a difference? Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:116–123. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/86.1.116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Muurahainen NE, Kissileff HR, Lachaussee J, et al. Effect of a soup preload on reduction of food intake by cholecystokinin in humans. Am J Physiol. 1991;260(4 Pt 2):R672–R680. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1991.260.4.R672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Perrigue MM, Monsivais P, Drewnowski A. Added soluble fiber enhances the satiating power of low-energy-density liquid yogurts. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:1862–1868. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2009.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Rolls BJ, Castellanos VH, Halford JC, et al. Volume of food consumed affects satiety in men. Am J Clin Nutr. 1998;67:1170–1177. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/67.6.1170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Rolls BJ, Fedoroff IC, Guthrie JF, et al. Foods with different satiating effects in humans. Appetite. 1990;15:115–126. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(90)90044-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Rolls BJ, Kim S, Fedoroff IC. Effects of drinks sweetened with sucrose or aspartame on hunger, thirst and food intake in men. Physiol Behav. 1990;48:19–26. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90254-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Spiegel TA, Shrager EE, Stellar E. Responses of lean and obese subjects to preloads, deprivation, and palatability. Appetite. 1989;13:45–69. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(89)90026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Spiegel TA, Hubert CD, Fried H, et al. Contribution of gastric and postgastric feedback to satiation and satiety in women. Physiol Behav. 1997;62:1125–1136. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00269-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Spitzer L, Rodin J. Effects of fructose and glucose preloads on subsequent food intake. Appetite. 1987;8:135–145. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(87)80006-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Woodend DM, Anderson GH. Effect of sucrose and safflower oil preloads on short term appetite and food intake of young men. Appetite. 2001;37:185–195. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Bertenshaw EJ, Lluch A, Yeomans MR. Dose-dependent effects of beverage protein content upon short-term intake. Appetite. 2009;52:580–587. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Rolls BJ, Bell EA, Thorwart ML. Water incorporated into a food but not served with a food decreases energy intake in lean women. Am J Clin Nutr. 1999;70:448–455. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/70.4.448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Rolls BJ, Kim-Harris S, Fischman MW, et al. Satiety after preloads with different amounts of fat and carbohydrate: implications for obesity. Am J Clin Nutr. 1994;60:476–487. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/60.4.476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.de Graaf C, Hulshof T. Effects of weight and energy content of preloads on subsequent appetite and food intake. Appetite. 1996;26:139–151. doi: 10.1006/appe.1996.0012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Abou Samra R, Anderson GH. Insoluble cereal fiber reduces appetite and short-term food intake and glycemic response to food consumed 75 min later by healthy men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:972–979. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/86.4.972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Johnson J, Vickers Z. Effects of flavor and macronutrient composition of food servings on liking, hunger and subsequent intake. Appetite. 1993;21:25–39. doi: 10.1006/appe.1993.1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Mazlan N, Horgan G, Stubbs RJ. Energy density and weight of food effect short-term caloric compensation in men. Physiol Behav. 2006;87:679–686. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Monsivais P, Carter BE, Christiansen M, et al. Soluble fiber dextrin enhances the satiating power of beverages. Appetite. 2011;56:9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Porrini M, Santangelo A, Crovetti R, et al. Weight, protein, fat, and timing of preloads affect food intake. Physiol Behav. 1997;62:563–570. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00162-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Rolls BJ, Laster LJ, Summerfelt A. Hunger and food intake following consumption of low-calorie foods. Appetite. 1989;13:115–127. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(89)90109-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Stewart SL, Black RM, Wolever TMS, et al. The relationship between the glycaemic response to breakfast cereals and subjective appetite and food intake. Nutr Res. 1997;17:1249–1260. [Google Scholar]
- 79.Erdmann J, Topsch R, Lippl F, et al. Postprandial response of plasma ghrelin levels to various test meals in relation to food intake, plasma insulin, and glucose. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89:3048–3054. doi: 10.1210/jc.2003-031610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Porrini M, Crovetti R, Riso P, et al. Effects of physical and chemical characteristics of food on specific and general satiety. Physiol Behav. 1995;57:461–468. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)00242-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Potier M, Fromentin G, Calvez J, et al. A high-protein, moderate-energy, regular cheesy snack is energetically compensated in human subjects. Br J Nutr. 2009;102:625–631. doi: 10.1017/S0007114509236026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS. Salad and satiety: energy density and portion size of a first-course salad affect energy intake at lunch. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104:1570–1576. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2004.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Mattes RD, Rothacker D. Beverage viscosity is inversely related to postprandial hunger in humans. Physiol Behav. 2001;74:551–557. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00597-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Mourao DM, Bressan J, Campbell WW, et al. Effects of food form on appetite and energy intake in lean and obese young adults. Int J Obes (Lond) 2007;31:1688–1695. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Zijlstra N, Mars M, de Wijk RA, et al. The effect of viscosity on ad libitum food intake. Int J Obes (Lond) 2008;32:676–683. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Cassady BA, Considine RV, Mattes D. Beverage consumption, appetite, and energy intake: what did you expect? Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:587–593. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.025437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.de Wijk RA, Zijlstra N, Mars M, et al. The effects of food viscosity on bite size, bite effort and food intake. Physiol Behav. 2008;95:527–532. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.07.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Kissileff HR, Klingsberg G, Van Itallie TB. Universal eating monitor for continuous recording of solid or liquid consumption in man. Am J Physiol. 1980;238:R14–R22. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1980.238.1.R14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Drewnowski A. Energy density, palatability, and satiety: implications for weight control. Nutr Rev. 1998;56:347–353. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.1998.tb01677.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Yeomans MR. Flavour-nutrient learning in humans: an elusive phenomenon? Physiol Behav. 2012;106:345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Yeomans MR, Chambers L. Satiety-relevant sensory qualities enhance the satiating effects of mixed carbohydrate-protein preloads. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;94:1410–1417. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.011650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.de Wijk RA, Polet IA, Engelen L, et al. Amount of ingested custard dessert as affected by its color, odor, and texture. Physiol Behav. 2004;82:397–403. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Weijzen PL, Liem DG, Zandstra EH, et al. Sensory-specific satiety and intake: the difference between nibble- and bar-size snacks. Appetite. 2008;50:435–442. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Wiessing KR, Xin L, McGill AT, et al. Sensitivity of ad libitum meals to detect changes in hunger. Restricted-item or multi-item testmeals in the design of preload appetite studies. Appetite. 2012;58:1076–1082. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Rolls BJ. The relationship between dietary energy density and energy intake. Physiol Behav. 2009;97:609–615. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Blundell JE, Macdiarmid JI. Passive overconsumption. Fat intake and short-term energy balance. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1997;827:392–407. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb51850.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Yeomans MR. The role of palatability in control of human appetite: implications for understanding and treating obesity. In: Kirkham TC, Cooper SJ, editors. Appetite and Body Weight. Integrative Systems and the Development of Anti-Obesity Drugs. Burlington, MA: Academic Press; 2007. pp. 247–269. [Google Scholar]
- 98.Spiegel TA, Kaplan JM, Alavi A, et al. Effects of soup preloads on gastric emptying and fullness ratings following an egg sandwich meal. Physiol Behav. 1994;56:571–575. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90303-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Geliebter A. Gastric distension and gastric capacity in relation to food intake in humans. Physiol Behav. 1988;44:665–668. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90333-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, et al. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chichester: Wiley; 2009. [Google Scholar]