Abstract
In all sensory modalities, intracortical inhibition shapes the functional properties of cortical neurons but also influences the responses to natural stimuli. Studies performed in various species have revealed that auditory cortex neurons respond to conspecific vocalizations by temporal spike patterns displaying a high trial-to-trial reliability, which might result from precise timing between excitation and inhibition. Studying the guinea pig auditory cortex, we show that partial blockage of GABAA receptors by gabazine (GBZ) application (10 μm, a concentration that promotes expansion of cortical receptive fields) increased the evoked firing rate and the spike-timing reliability during presentation of communication sounds (conspecific and heterospecific vocalizations), whereas GABAB receptor antagonists [10 μm saclofen; 10–50 μm CGP55845 (p-3-aminopropyl-p-diethoxymethyl phosphoric acid)] had nonsignificant effects. Computing mutual information (MI) from the responses to vocalizations using either the evoked firing rate or the temporal spike patterns revealed that GBZ application increased the MI derived from the activity of single cortical site but did not change the MI derived from population activity. In addition, quantification of information redundancy showed that GBZ significantly increased redundancy at the population level. This result suggests that a potential role of intracortical inhibition is to reduce information redundancy during the processing of natural stimuli.
Introduction
The ultimate goal of any processing taking place in sensory cortices is to generate neuronal mechanisms that are fast and reliable enough for identifying and discriminating between natural stimuli. One of the main challenges of sensory physiology is to decipher these mechanisms. In the auditory cortex (ACx), several groups have provided compelling evidence indicating that the temporal spike patterns occurring at presentation of communication sounds provide the bases of a neural code for discriminating between such sounds (Narayan et al., 2006; Schnupp et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2007; Recanzone, 2008; Huetz et al., 2006, 2009). In addition, comparisons between neurometric and psychometric functions suggest that the temporal spike patterns of ACx neurons allows discrimination performance at the neuronal level that approaches those observed at the behavioral level (Narayan et al., 2007; Walker et al., 2008).
A central question now is to determine which key factors control the ability of cortical neurons to discriminate between communication sounds. In all sensory cortices, it has long been suggested that the neuron functional properties result from the interplay between excitatory and inhibitory inputs (Gilbert and Wiesel, 1985; Ebner and Armstrong-James, 1990; Calford, 2002; Hirsch, 2003; Swadlow, 2003; Oswald et al., 2006). In the ACx, previous studies have described that application of GABAA antagonists enlarges the spectral receptive fields (Wang et al., 2000, 2002) or modifies temporal modulation transfer function (Kurt et al., 2006). However, only very few attempts have been made to determine whether the balance between excitation and inhibition changes the abilities of ACx neurons to discriminate between natural stimuli. Disrupting intracellular GABAA or GABAB inhibition, Rosen and Mooney (2003) were unable to block neuronal selectivity for the bird's own song in zebra finch HVC neurons. In contrast, blocking GABAA receptors in the bird functional analog of nonprimary ACx (the NCM nucleus) increased the phasic components of responses to conspecific songs (Pinaud et al., 2008). Furthermore, it was shown that alterations of GABAergic inhibition (by manipulating the level of brain-generated estradiol) influence both neuronal and behavioral discriminative performances (Tremere and Pinaud, 2011). To determine the effects of intracortical inhibition on the ACx discriminative abilities, we assessed to what extent individual cortical sites and ensembles of cortical sites are affected when a partial blockage of GABAA or GABAB receptors is made on the entire ACx. To achieve this, we topically applied GABAA [gabazine (GBZ)] or GABAB [saclofen and CGP55845 (p-3-aminopropyl-p-diethoxymethyl phosphoric acid) (CGP)] antagonists to the cortical surface while recording cells simultaneously in 16 locations of the primary ACx tonotopic map. Spectrotemporal receptive fields (STRFs) were first quantified to ensure that the drug concentration/time of application was able to induce reliable changes on the STRFs of the neurons. Then, we analyzed the response strength and the spike-timing reliability at presentation of conspecific and heterospecific vocalizations before and after drug application. We quantified the amount of mutual information (MI) carried by firing rate and temporal patterns in control and drug conditions at both the single site and population level. We found that reducing the intracortical inhibition increased the information conveyed by individual sites but left the population information unaffected, indicating that intracortical inhibition contributes to reducing redundancy between cortical sites.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Recordings were made in the primary ACx of adult pigmented guinea pigs of either sex. Animals, weighing 450–950 g (3–7 months old), came from our own colony housed in a humidity-controlled (50–55%) and temperature-controlled (22–24°C) facility on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:30 A.M.) with access to food and water ad libitum. All surgical procedures were performed in compliance with the guidelines determined by the national (JO 887-848) and European (86/609/EEC) legislations on animal experimentation, which are similar to those described in the Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Neuroscience Research of the Society of Neuroscience. Two to 3 d before each experiment, the animal's audiogram was determined by testing auditory brainstem responses (ABRs) under isoflurane anesthesia (2.5%), described previously by Gourévitch et al. (2009). The ABR was obtained by differential recordings between two subdermal electrodes (SC25; NeuroService) placed at the vertex and behind the mastoid bone. Averages of 500 responses were collected at nine frequencies (between 0.5 and 32 kHz) presented between 70 and 0 dB SPL to obtain the audiogram. All the animals used in the present study showed audiogram in the range reported previously for healthy guinea pigs (Robertson and Irvine, 1989; Gourévitch and Edeline, 2011).
Surgical procedures
The animal was anesthetized by an initial injection of urethane (1.2 g/kg, i.p.) supplemented by additional doses (0.5 g/kg, i.p.) when reflex movements were observed after pinching the hindpaw (usually once or twice during the recording session). A single dose of atropine sulfate (0.06 mg/kg, s.c.) was given to reduce bronchial secretions. After placing the animal in a stereotaxic frame, a craniotomy was performed above the left temporal cortex. A local anesthetic (2% xylocaine) was liberally injected in the wound. The opening was 8 mm wide starting at the intersection point between parietal and temporal bones and 8–10 mm in height. The dura above the ACx was removed under binocular control, and the CSF was drained through the cysterna magna to prevent the occurrence of edema. After the surgery, a pedestal of dental acrylic cement was built to allow atraumatic fixation of the animal's head during the recording session. The stereotaxic frame supporting the animal was placed in a sound-attenuating chamber (model AC1; IAC). At the end of the recording session, a lethal dose of pentobarbital (>200 mg/kg, i.p.) was administered to the animal.
Recording procedures
Data are from multiunit recordings in the primary ACx (area AI). Extracellular recordings were obtained from arrays of 16 tungsten electrodes (33 μm diameter, <1 MΩ) composed of two rows of eight electrodes separated by 1000 μm (350 μm between electrodes of the same row). A silver wire, used as ground, was inserted between the temporal bone and the dura mater on the contralateral side. The location of the primary ACx was estimated based on the pattern of vasculature observed in previous studies (Edeline and Weinberger, 1993; Manunta and Edeline, 1999; Wallace et al., 2000; Edeline et al., 2001). The raw signal was amplified 10,000 times (Medusa; Tucker-Davis Technologies) and was then processed by an RX5 multichannel data acquisition system (Tucker-Davis Technologies). The signal collected from each electrode was filtered (610–10,000 Hz) to extract multiunit activity (MUA). The trigger level was set for each electrode to select the largest action potentials from the signal and was the same before and after drug application. Online and offline examination of the waveforms suggests that the MUA collected here was made of action potentials generated by three to eight neurons in the vicinity of the electrode. At the beginning of each experiment, we set the position of the electrode array in such a way that the two rows of eight electrodes sampled neurons responding from low to high frequency when progressing in the rostrocaudal direction (see example of tonotopic maps recorded with such an array in the study by Gaucher et al., 2012, their Fig. 1).
Drug delivery
Topical application allows application of pharmacological agents on cortical areas and has been used previously in several brain regions (Riquimaroux et al., 1991, 1992; Jones and Barth, 2002; Caesar et al., 2003, 2008; Yu et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2009). Here, this technique was used because we aimed at assessing the consequences of GABA receptor blockage over the whole cortical map. During this manipulation, neuronal responses were simultaneously sampled from 16 different locations in the primary ACx. Several pilot studies were performed to determine (1) the GBZ concentration to be used and (2) the time course of the GBZ effect on ACx neurons. First, we looked for GBZ concentrations that can be applied in vivo without triggering epileptiform neuronal activity. Based on several in vivo studies (Darbin et al., 2006; Darbin and Wichmann, 2008; Tachibana et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2009), we opted for a 10 μm GBZ concentration. However, this low concentration can still lead to abnormal (seizure-like) activities when continuously applied to the surface of the cortex. For this reason, we applied the drug only for a brief period via a filter paper (∼0.7 × 2.5 mm) placed in close vicinity to the electrode array. Initially, two durations of drug application were used in our experiments: 2 and 4 min. However, we noted that the 2 min application protocol did not induce effects significantly different from spontaneous fluctuations or from application of saline. Moreover, in another set of experiments performed with multichannel silicon probe electrodes (16 channels; A-Style probe; NeuroNexus), we assessed the magnitude of GBZ effects as a function of the cortical layer. A 2 min application induced significant effects on the STRF parameters from layer I to layer IV but not in layers V–VI (Fig. 1A). In contrast, a 4 min application induced a strong effect on the STRF parameters in all cortical layers (Fig. 1B). For these reasons, results presented here are only those obtained with a 4 min application protocol. In these conditions, there was an increase in evoked activity starting in <10 min after drug application and remaining above the control level for >60 min (Fig. 1C). With this protocol, epileptiform activity was never observed. Because the two GABAB antagonists used in our study (saclofen and CGP) have molecular weights similar to GBZ, we assumed that their effects should have a similar time course. All drugs were freshly dissolved on the day of the experiment and kept at 38°C until application on the cortical surface.
Acoustic stimuli
Artificial stimuli.
Acoustic stimuli were generated in MATLAB (MathWorks), transferred to an RP2.1-based sound delivery system (Tucker-Davis Technologies) and sent to a Fostex speaker (FE87E). The speaker was placed at 2 cm from the guinea pig's right ear, a distance at which the speaker produced a flat spectrum (±3 dB) between 140 Hz and 36 kHz. Calibration of the speaker was made using noise and pure tones recorded by a microphone (4133; Brüel & Kjær) coupled to a preamplifier (2169; Brüel & Kjær) and a digital recorder (PMD671; Marantz). The STRFs were determined using 97 or 129 gamma-tone frequencies, covering six (0.14–9, 0.28–18, or 0.56–36 kHz) or eight (0.14–36 kHz) octaves, respectively, and presented at three intensities: (1) 85 or 75 dB SPL; (2) 65 or 55 dB SPL; and (3) 45 or 35 dB SPL. At a given intensity, each frequency was repeated eight times at a rate of 2.35 Hz in pseudorandom order. The duration of these tones over half-peak amplitude was 15 ms and the total duration of the tone was 50 ms, so there was no overlap between tones.
Natural stimuli.
A set of three conspecific and five heterospecific vocalizations was used to assess the neuronal response to communication sounds. The conspecific vocalizations were recorded from animals of our colony. Pairs of animals were placed in the acoustic chamber, and their vocalizations were recorded by a microphone (4133; Brüel & Kjær) coupled to a preamplifier (2169; Brüel & Kjær) and a digital recorder (PMD671; Marantz). A large set of vocalizations was loaded in the Audition software (Adobe Audition 3; Adobe Systems), and representative examples of a purr, a chutter, and a whistle were selected. Three heterospecific vocalizations were selected to approximately match the conspecific vocalizations in terms of both the spectral content and the frequency and amplitude modulations according to visual inspection of their spectrograms. The heterospecific vocalizations were not modified to equalize their modulation power spectra relative to the conspecific ones. These vocalizations were a dolphin chutter, a rabbit whistle, and a dove purr. The two other heterospecific vocalizations were selected to be strongly different from conspecific vocalizations and included a Marsh Warbler song and a low-frequency call from a wart-biter cricket. All heterospecific vocalizations were downloaded from the Macaulay Library of the Cornell laboratory of Ornithology (http://macaulaylibrary.org/index.do). Figure 2 shows the spectrograms and envelops of the three conspecific vocalizations and their heterospecific counterparts.
Experimental protocol
Inserting an array of 16 electrodes in the cortical tissue almost systematically induces a deformation of the cortex. At least a 30 min recovering time lapse was allowed for the cortex to return to its initial shape, and then the array was slowly lowered. STRFs were used to assess the quality of our recordings and to adjust the electrodes depth. The recording depth was 500–1000 μm, which corresponds to layer III and the upper part of layer IV according to Wallace and Palmer (2008). However, as a result of the convex shape of the guinea pig ACx, it is likely that all the electrodes were not exactly at the same depth.
When a clear tuning was obtained for at least 8 of the 16 electrodes, the stability of the tuning was assessed: we required that the recordings displayed at least three successive similar STRFs (each lasting 6 min) before starting the protocol. When the stability was satisfactory, the protocol started by presenting the acoustic stimuli in the following order: gamma tones to determine the STRF at three intensities (18 min in total), followed by 3 min of spontaneous activity, followed by conspecific and heterospecific vocalizations presented at 75 dB SPL. Each vocalization was repeated 20 times. Presentation of this entire stimulus set lasted 30 min and was followed by the 4 min period of drug application. The same set of stimuli was presented immediately after drug application in the same order and then 1 h after when the recordings were stable.
Data analyses
Quantification of responses to pure tones.
The STRFs derived from MUA were obtained by constructing poststimulus time histograms (PSTHs) for each frequency with 1 ms time bins. All spikes falling in the averaging time window (starting at stimulus onset and lasting 100 ms) were counted. Thus, STRFs are matrices of 100 bins in abscissa (time) multiplied by 97 or 129 bins in ordinate (frequency). All STRFs were smoothed with a uniform 5 × 5 bin window.
For each STRF, at a given intensity, the best frequency was defined as the frequency at which the highest firing rate was recorded. At each intensity, peaks of significant response were automatically identified using the following procedure: a positive peak in the multiunit-based STRF was defined as a contour of firing rate above the average level of the baseline activity (estimated from the first 10 ms of STRFs at all intensity levels) plus six times the SD of the baseline activity. For a given site and a given intensity, three measures were extracted from the peaks and one directly from the spike trains.
First, the “total bandwidth” was defined as the sum of all peak widths in octaves. Second, the “response duration” was the time difference between the first and last spikes of the significant peaks. Third, the “response strength” was the total number of spikes falling in the significant peaks. In addition, we computed the “temporal precision” of the spike trains corresponding to the STRF excitatory area. This was done by computing the shuffled autocorrelogram (SAC) (Joris 2003) for all pairs of spike trains evoked by the pure tones within the STRF bandwidth. The SAC was then fitted to the following equation (Elhilali et al., 2004; Chen et al., 2012):
where α is the firing reliability, λ is the firing rate, and σ2 is the spike-timing jitter variance. The parameter σ and α were obtained by fitting the model to the experimentally measured shuffled correlogram using constrained least-squares optimization. The σ parameter corresponds to the encoding window, denoted ET in the study by Chen et al. (2012). The encoding window was extracted for all sites for which a good fit (r > 0.6) could be obtained. Note that the integrating window (denoted IT) defined by these authors was computed exactly as our response duration.
Quantification of responses evoked by vocalizations.
The responses to vocalizations were quantified using three parameters: (1) the evoked response, which corresponds to the difference between evoked and spontaneous firing rates at presentation of the vocalizations; (2) the encoding window computed from the SAC using the same method as for the STRF (see above); and (3) the spike-timing reliability coefficient (CorrCoef), which was used to quantify the trial-to-trial reliability of the responses. This index was computed for each vocalization: it corresponds to the normalized covariance between each pair of action potential trains recorded at presentation of this vocalization and was calculated as follows:
where N is the number of trials, and σxixj is the normalized covariance at zero lag between spike trains xi and xj, where i and j are the trial numbers. Spike trains xi and xj were previously convolved with a 10-ms-width Gaussian window. This value was chosen according to the analysis of the temporal precision maximizing the MI. This analysis revealed that the maximal information was obtained for 8 ms in average (data not shown).
The CorrCoef was used because our simulations indicated that it was not influenced by fluctuations of firing rate. In these simulations, the firing rate influence on CorrCoef values was studied using homogenous Poisson spike-train simulations. In a first set of simulations, we computed the CorrCoef for 50 surrogate spike trains with various firing rates showing no spike-timing reliability (Poisson spike trains). The CorrCoef stayed ∼0 for every firing rate tested. In a second set of simulations, we generated a spike train with a relatively high value of CorrCoef (∼0.4); then we added spikes at random time points within this spike train to quantify putative variations of the CorrCoef in a situation mimicking the natural neuronal response. Here again, this index did not increase with random additions of action potentials. Thus, the CorrCoef index does not depend on the firing rate variations. These simulations also allowed computing the chance level of the CorrCoef value: 0.026 for a 0.01 confidence interval.
To discard recordings not responding to any vocalization, we set up a criterion of an evoked firing rate of at least 1.5 spikes/s or a CorrCoef of at least 0.15. A systematic and meticulous examination of our database has shown that recordings exhibiting only a phasic onset response generated a CorrCoef of at least 0.15 and that cells responding by tonic responses lacking temporal organization generated at least a firing rate of 1.5 spikes/s.
Quantification of correlations between pairs of neurons.
Another way to evaluate neuronal reliability at the level of pair of neurons is to perform an analysis of noise and signal correlations, as described by Averbeck et al. (2006). In a set of interesting studies (Latham and Nirenberg, 2005; Averbeck and Lee, 2006; Averbeck et al., 2006), these authors pointed out that it is possible to dissociate between noise correlations (i.e., the firing rate correlation for a pair of neurons in response to presentations of the same stimulus) versus signal correlation (i.e., the correlation between the mean responses of the same pair of neurons to different stimuli). Noise correlations, if present, reflect the existence of a connection between two recording sites, without making assumptions on the anatomical circuitry of this connection. They only represent a covariation of the variability of the recording site responses. Signal correlations reflect a co-tuning of a pair of recording sites. Noise and signal correlations between pairs of recordings sites were computed with exactly the same procedure for vocalizations and pure tones. To compute noise correlation, the firing rate of neuronal responses to each stimulus was first normalized by the average firing rate in response to this stimulus. Then the normalized firing rates in response to each presentation of each stimulus were taken for a pair of recordings, and the Pearson's correlation coefficient between them was computed. The values of the Pearson's coefficient for each pair of recordings were then averaged. To compute the signal correlation, we computed the Pearson's coefficient between the mean responses to each stimulus from a pair of recordings. We then averaged the result for all pairs of recordings.
Quantification of MI from the responses to vocalizations.
We used an indirect method (Schnupp et al., 2006) to quantify the amount of information (Shannon, 1948) contained in the responses to vocalizations obtained in control conditions and after application of GABAergic antagonists. This method allows quantifying how well the identity of the vocalization can be inferred from the neuronal responses. Here, “neuronal responses” refers to either the spike trains recorded under one single electrode in response to one stimulus (for the computation of the individual MI) or a concatenation of spike trains simultaneously recorded under several electrodes (for the computation of the population MI). In both cases, the following computation steps were the same. Neuronal responses can be represented using different timescales ranging from the whole response (firing rate) to a 1 ms precision (precise temporal patterns), which allows analyzing how much the spike timing contributes to the information. Because this method is exhaustively described by Schnupp et al. (2006), we will present here only the main principles.
The method relies on a pattern recognition algorithm that is designed to “guess which stimulus evoked a particular response pattern” by going through the following steps. From all the responses of a cortical site to the different stimuli, a single response (test pattern) is extracted and represented as a PSTH with a given bin size (different sizes were considered as discussed further below). Then, from the remaining responses (training set), a mean response pattern is computed for each stimulus class. The test pattern is then assigned to the stimulus class of the closest mean response pattern. This operation is repeated for all the responses, generating a confusion matrix in which each response is assigned to a given stimulus class. From this confusion matrix, the MI is given by Shannon's formula:
where x and y are the rows and columns of the confusion matrix or, in other words, the values taken by the random variables “presented stimulus class” and “assigned stimulus class.”
In our case, we used responses to the eight vocalizations and selected the first 264 ms of these responses to work on spike trains of the same duration, with the shortest vocalization being 264 ms long (conspecific whistle). In a scenario in which the responses do not carry any information, the assignments of each response to a mean response pattern will be equivalent to chance level (here 1/8 because we used eight different stimuli and each stimulus was presented the same number of times) and the MI would be close to zero. In the opposite case in which responses are very different between stimulus classes and very similar within a stimulus class, the confusion matrix would be diagonal and the MI would tend to log2(8) = 3 bits.
As already stated, this algorithm was applied at different bin sizes ranging from 264 to 1 ms. For each cortical site, two parameters were extracted from this analysis: (1) the value of MI for the largest bin size, 264 ms, which represents the value of MI when only the firing rate is considered (when no temporal patterns are considered); and (2) the value of MI obtained for a bin of 8 ms, which was a temporal resolution leading to the highest values of information in previous studies (Schnupp et al., 2006; Huetz et al., 2009).
The MI estimates are subject to non-negligible positive sampling biases. Therefore, as in the study by Schnupp et al. (2006), we estimated the expected size of this bias by calculating MI values for “shuffled” data, in which the response patterns were randomly reassigned to stimulus classes. The shuffling was repeated 100 times, resulting in 100 MI estimates of the bias (MIbias). These MIbias estimates are then used as estimators for the computation of the statistical significance of the MI estimate for the real (unshuffled) datasets: the real estimate is considered significant if its value is statistically different from the distribution of MIbias shuffled estimates. Significant MI estimates were computed for MI calculated from neuronal responses under one electrode.
To assess whether drug-induced alterations of the firing rate account for any change in the information carried by the temporal patterns, we performed spike count normalizations between pre-conditions and post-conditions as described by Tremere and Pinaud (2011). For each cortical site, spikes were randomly removed or added from the post-application cortical responses to exactly match the pre-application firing rate. For the spike additions, pseudorandom values were drawn from a Poisson distribution, and spikes were inserted at these random arrival times. For the spike removals, spike arrival times were randomly selected among all spikes evoked by a vocalization (all trials) and removed. For each cortical site, spike count normalization was performed 50 times, and the mean MI value was taken. As stated in Results, this normalization did not significantly affect the MI values at the precision under study (8 ms bin size).
Quantification of redundancy in the responses to vocalizations.
We computed the redundancy in neuronal responses according to the method described in previous studies (Narayanan et al., 2005; So et al., 2012). This method relied on the comparison between the information carried by a group of simultaneously recorded neuronal responses and the sum of information carried by each individual recording from the same group. In our study, the information carried by a group of recording was estimated by the population MI (MIpopulation), and the information carried by one recording was estimated by the individual MI (MIindividual). Formally, the redundancy (called Pensemble by Narayanan et al., 2005; So et al., 2012) was defined as follows:
where N is the number of recordings used to compute the MIpopulation. When Pensemble was positive, neuronal responses were considered synergistic. On the contrary, when Pensemble was negative, the neuronal responses were redundant.
In practice, we could record from 16 cortical sites simultaneously, so the Pensemble was computed using a maximum of 16 recordings, from which the non-responsive ones were excluded. To assess the influence of the group size of simultaneous recordings on the information carried by that group (MIpopulation), the number of sites used for computing MIpopulation varied from two to the maximal possible size (equivalent to 16 minus the non-responsive sites). Because the number of possible combinations could be extremely large (Cnk, where k is the group size and n is the number of responsive sites in a recording session), a threshold was fixed to reduce computation time: when the number of possible combinations exceeded 100, 100 combinations were randomly chosen. Otherwise, the Pensemble values were computed for all possible combinations.
In all the results, data are presented as mean ± SEM values, and all the statistical tests are paired t tests unless otherwise specified. We used a significance level of 0.05 with Bonferroni's corrections for multiple tests.
Results
Application of GBZ was tested on five animals, which provided 77 stable recordings throughout the recording session. Saclofen application was tested in seven animals, which provided 92 recordings. CGP was tested in nine animals, which provided 125 recordings (4 animals/60 recordings for 10 μm and 5 animals/65 recordings for 50 μm). Saline application was performed on a separate set of three animals, providing 73 recordings. In the following presentation of the results, we will first describe the effects of the three GABA antagonists on the STRF parameters and then on the responses to conspecific and heterospecific vocalizations.
Analyzing spontaneous activity (recorded during a 3 min silence period) revealed that GBZ primarily increased spontaneous activity (by 77%), whereas application of the two GABAB antagonists led to smaller effects: 41% for saclofen and 20% for CGP. Note that the increase for CGP is in the range of the changes observed in control conditions: saline application led to a 21% increase in spontaneous activity (see Discussion).
Effects of the GABA antagonists on STRFs
In most cases, application of the GABAA antagonist GBZ induced three striking effects on the STRFs of the neurons: there was a large increase in (1) the bandwidth of the suprathreshold tuning, (2) the duration of evoked response, and (3) the response strength. Figure 3A illustrates these effects: the best frequency of this recording was at 25 kHz at 75 dB SPL (Fig. 3A, pre), and it expanded in the low-frequency side both immediately after and 1 h after application. As indicated by the color code, the response strength also increased, as did the response duration. In contrast, the GABAB antagonists (saclofen and CGP) induced only small or no effects. Figure 3, C and D, presents two examples of STRFs obtained after application of CGP at 10 and 50 μm. In both cases, there was no change in response bandwidth, strength, and duration immediately after and 1 h after application. Figure 3B presents an example of STRFs obtained before and after saline application: there was a slight increase in response strength but no change in tuning bandwidth or response duration.
Across all the tested intensities, the group data confirmed that the striking modulation exerted by GABAA receptors on the STRF parameters (Fig. 4). On average, GBZ produced a significant increase in bandwidth (from 1 ± 0.09 to 1.7 ± 0.17 octave; p < 0.001), response duration (from 21.7 ± 2.25 to 39.9 ± 2.74 ms; p < 0.001), and response strength (from 12.2 ± 1.69 to 71.1 ± 12.9 spikes; p < 0.001). The encoding window derived from the response autocorrelogram was also significantly increased (from 6.55 ± 1.29 to 15.31 ± 1.75 ms; p < 0.001; data not shown). In contrast, saline application and application of GABAB antagonists did not induce significant effects on bandwidth and duration of evoked responses (Fig. 4). The only significant effects observed were a slight increase in response strength with CGP tested at 10 μm and a decrease with the same drug at a concentration of 50 μm. The encoding window was also unaffected by saline or GABAB antagonists (data not shown).
It could be suspected that the effects induced by GBZ differ with the stimuli intensity (i.e., can differ at suprathreshold intensities vs close to threshold intensities). Analyzing the effects of GBZ at three groups of intensities (≥75, 65, and ≤55 dB SPL) did not reveal major differences (data not shown). Response strength and the response duration were increased at the three intensities (p < 0.001 in all cases). The bandwidth was increased at the two highest intensities (p < 0.001) but not at the lowest ones (≤55 dB), suggesting that the tuning bandwidth is less influenced by inhibition close to threshold.
Altogether, these results clearly indicate that application of GABAA antagonists expanded the STRF in the spectral and temporal domains. At the concentration used here (up to 50 μm for CGP), antagonists of GABAB receptors have little or no effects on the STRF parameters. In fact, the changes detected after application of GABAB antagonists were in the range of those observed after saline application.
Effects of the GABA antagonists on responses to communication sounds
The responses to vocalizations were analyzed for the recordings exhibiting at least an evoked firing rate of 1.5 spikes/s or at least a value of CorrCoef of 0.15 (see Materials and Methods). For this subset of recordings (n = 44 of 80 recordings), the GBZ effects observed in the STRFs were identical to those described for the whole population (bandwidth, p < 0.002; response duration, p < 0.0001; response strength, p < 0.002).
Before drug application, the raster plots revealed the presence of reproducible spike patterns in the responses to vocalizations. For example, in Figure 5A, the recordings collected from electrodes 3, 4, 9, 10, 14, and 15 displayed reproducible patterns with presentations of a chutter call, whereas the recordings obtained from other electrodes did not. Over the whole population, there was an increase in firing rate during each vocalization relative to spontaneous activity (paired t test between spontaneous and evoked activity for each response, p < 0.0001), but recordings exhibiting clear increases in firing rate during the vocalizations did not always display temporal patterns. There was no systematic difference in terms of firing rate or CorrCoef between the responses to conspecific and heterospecific vocalizations. Some differences were detected (e.g., the CorrCoef was higher for the conspecific compared with the heterospecific purr, p < 0.05), but they could simply result from the fact that the modulation power spectra were not equalized between the two sets of stimuli.
As expected, based on the effects observed in the STRFs, GBZ application increased the responses evoked by vocalizations. Figure 5B shows that this increase reinforced preexisting temporal patterns (electrodes 9, 10, 14, and 15) and also revealed the presence of previously undetectable temporal patterns (electrodes 11–13). As illustrated in Figure 5C, the increase in firing rate obtained after GBZ application was often accompanied by an increase in CorrCoef, whereas CGP (GABAB antagonists) did not increase either the firing rate or the CorrCoef (Fig. 5D).
The group data, based on responses to all vocalizations, confirmed the effects described by these examples. As shown in the scattergrams of Figure 6A, GBZ application promoted large increases in firing rate (presented by the points above the diagonal line). Saline and CGP application induced significant effects, which were much smaller than those triggered by GBZ (Fig. 6A, inset; GBZ, 8.5 ± 0.4 vs 15.1 ± 0.6 spike/s, p < 0.0001; saline, 11.1 ± 0.6 vs 13.8 ± 0.6 spike/s, p < 0.0001; CGP at 10 μm, 7.5 ± 0.4 vs 9.2 ± 0.5 spike/s, p = 0.0002; CGP at 50 μm, 11.5 ± 0.5 vs 10.4 ± 0.6 spike/s, p = 0.2).
Similarly, scattergrams representing the effects on the CorrCoef index (Fig. 6B) indicate that only GBZ promoted an increase in trial-to-trial reliability. On average, after GBZ application, the CorrCoef was increased (0.11 ± 0.01 vs 0.18 ± 0.01; p = 0.0007), whereas it was unchanged after saline application (0.05 ± 0.005 vs 0.04 ± 0.005; p = 0.2). The CorrCoef was slightly decreased after application of CGP at 10 μm (0.09 ± 0.007 vs 0.06 ± 0.006; p < 0.0001), an effect that was not confirmed when a CGP concentration of 50 μm was used (0.16 ± 0.01 vs 0.15 ± 0.01; p = 0.10). Remember that CorrCoeff values above 0.026 are significantly above the chancel level computed from our simulations (see Materials and Methods).
As explained in Materials and Methods, the variability of covariations between pairs of recording sites can also be investigated by computing the noise correlations. GBZ application significantly increased noise correlations in the neuronal responses to vocalizations (from 0.31 ± 0.02 to 0.45 ± 0.03; p < 0.001; Fig. 7). This increase in noise correlations indicated that, for every pair of recording sites, the covariability of their responses tended to be reduced. This suggests that GBZ tended to increase the strength of a common influence on pairs of recording sites. Interestingly, GBZ not only decreases the individual variability (as shown by the increased CorrCoef) but also makes it more correlated between pairs of recording sites. Note that this result also held true when responses to pure tones were considered (from 0.1 ± 0.01 to 0.21 ± 0.01; p < 0.001; data not shown).
Surprisingly, the GBZ induced increase in trial-to-trial reliability was concomitant with an increase of the encoding window as measured from the SAC (10.78 ± 3.82 vs 20.72 ± 2.81 ms; p < 0.05), which was not the case with saline application (14.76 ± 2.51 vs 12.19 ± 1.75 ms; p = 0.34). This result points out that more complex effects than a simple increase in trial-to-trial reliability may be induced by GBZ.
In fact, from the rasters in Figure 5B, it appears that in many cases the strongest phasic excitatory responses were followed by strong suppression of activity potentially induced by cortical inhibition. These periods of inhibition seem to be more pronounced after GBZ applications. To determine whether this was true at the population level, we built averaged PSTHs before and after GBZ application using all the recordings. Figure 8 shows PSTHs for the three vocalizations in which significant responses could be averaged over the largest populations (n > 20 recordings). After GBZ application, the periods of strongest responses were followed by periods of more pronounced silence for the whistle and the songbird (Fig 8A,B, arrows). However, this effect was not systematic because it did not occur for the heterospecific whistle (Fig. 8C). To determine how general this effect was over the population of recordings and across the different vocalizations, we averaged all the significant peaks of responses. For each PSTH, we first detected significant peaks of responses by a firing rate superior to the spontaneous activity plus four times its SD. This threshold was used to delimit the beginning and ending of each peak. Only peaks of response lasting >20 ms and <80 ms were taken into account. Averaging all the significant excitatory peaks of responses obtained from these PSTHs (Fig. 8D) revealed that, after GBZ application, the activity tended to fall below the level obtained before the peak of the vocalization, whereas it remained at that level before application. Statistical comparison between the averaged 20 ms before and after the peak (Fig. 8E) confirmed that GBZ increases the post-peak inhibition (pre-peak, 25.3 to 25.1 spikes/s; post-peak, 24.7 to 20.2 spikes/1; p < 0.001), indicating that applying GABAA receptor antagonists can paradoxically reinforce phasic inhibition during natural stimuli.
Investigating the neural code before and after reducing intracortical inhibition
For each recording, we quantified the estimated MI for nine values of temporal resolution before and after drug application. These values ranged from the shortest vocalization duration (the conspecific whistle, 264 ms) to 1 ms. Figure 9 (left) presents the spike trains of two multiunit recordings before and after GBZ application. From these raster plots, it is apparent that GBZ increased the strength of evoked responses but also reinforced temporal organization of responses during the first 264 ms of vocalizations (left of the dotted line on the rasters; the vocalization duration is represented by the black lines below the rasters). As shown in Figure 9, A3 and B3, when MI was computed for increasing values of temporal resolution, the MI values after GBZ application were systematically higher than those before application. Two variables were extracted from these curves (see Materials and Methods). First, we selected for each recording the MI value obtained for the largest bin size (264 ms), which is the amount of information carried by the firing rate over this time period (MIFR). Second, we selected the MI value for a temporal resolution of 8 ms (MI8 ms) to quantify the amount of information carried by temporal patterns (the 8 ms resolution was chosen according to Schnupp et al., 2006; Huetz et al., 2009; see Materials and Methods). The values of MIFR and MI8 ms were compared before and after drug application.
Figure 10A shows the effects of pharmacological agents on the MI carried by individual cortical sites, when computed from either firing rate (Fig. 10A1, top, A2) or temporal patterns (Fig. 10A1, bottom, A3). Figure 10A1 shows the discriminative ability of a cortical site. In the control condition (Pre GBZ), the discriminative ability was low (MIFR = 0.49 bits; MI8 ms = 0.45 bits) but was increased after GBZ application (MIFR = 0.75 bits; MI8 ms = 1.17 bits). On average (Fig 10A2,A3), saline application did not change the value of MIFR and the value of MI8 ms (p > 0.5 and p > 0.6; respectively). In contrast, the values of MIFR and MI8 ms were significantly increased after GBZ application (p < 0.0001 in both cases; Fig. 10A2,A3). The increase in MI8 ms was not attributable to the increase in firing rate between pre-GBZ and post-GBZ application because an increase in MI8 ms value was still observed when using the normalization procedure (the pre and post responses were normalized because their firing rate were identical: without normalization, MI8 ms = 0.727 bits; with normalization, MI8 ms = 0.719 bits; for details, see Materials and Methods). These increases were not observed with saclofen and CGP. These results indicate that GBZ application increased the information carried by firing rate and spike patterns. Obviously, MI computed at a single cortical site (MIindividual) provides only a partial reflection of the encoding ability of the ACx. To gain more insights into how reducing intracortical inhibition affects the coding ability of the whole ACx, we computed the MI derived from simultaneously recorded cortical sites (MIpopulation; see Materials and Methods). Responsive sites were selected with two criteria: (1) significant responses to vocalization and (2) MI estimate that is significantly above the bias (see Materials and Methods). Confusion matrices obtained from a set of simultaneous recordings (Fig. 10B1) illustrate that the discriminative ability of a population is accurate, although false attributions could happen. Usually, confusion occurred between stimuli with similar acoustic parameters (for example, purr and dove call; Fig. 10B1, Pre GBZ). Again, the MIpopulation computed with temporal patterns was higher than the MIpopulation computed with firing rate. The MIpopulation computed from firing rate was not affected by saline application or by any of the agonists used in our study, including GBZ (Fig. 10B2, p > 0.1). Surprisingly, this result was the same even when MIpopulation was computed from temporal patterns (Fig. 10B3, p > 0.1). Thus, the MI computed from ensemble recordings is unaffected by GBZ application, whereas MIs computed from single cortical site of the same ensemble are increased. This paradoxical result suggests the following hypothesis: reducing inhibition locally increased the information measured at cortical sites but at the same time left unaffected the information carried by large ensemble recordings. Logically, this should imply that the redundancy between cortical sites was increased.
To directly assess this hypothesis, the MIpopulation was quantified for various sizes of recording ensembles (from 2 to 16 sites) and compared with the sum of the individual MIs of the same ensemble (see Materials and Methods). Figure 11A displays the evolution of the MIpopulation for one recording ensemble as a function of the size of this ensemble, before and after GBZ application. For small sizes of recording ensembles (n < 5), the information was not redundant, but for larger sizes, the information carried by each site started to become redundant (as indicated by the asymptotic shape of the two curves on Fig. 11A). Figure 11B shows the sum of MIindividual as a function of the number of sites considered, before and after GBZ application. As expected, the MIpopulation increased linearly with the number of cortical sites and always remained higher after GBZ application than before. Finally, Figure 11C shows the redundancy, quantified by the Pensemble (see Materials and Methods) as a function of the number of sites. Both before and after GBZ application, the Pensemble is negative and inversely proportional to the amount of sites considered, indicating that the responses at each cortical site were redundant. The difference between the red and blue curves points out the difference in redundancy before and after GBZ application. With this particular example, the difference was negative, implying that the information carried by this set of cortical sites was more redundant after GBZ application. The difference between the Pensemble before and after application was computed for all the recording ensembles and all tested drugs (Fig. 11D). GBZ application induced a clear increase in redundancy of the responses, as indicated by the fact that all but one curves were negative. On the contrary, saclofen and CGP application did not induce any clear effect on redundancy.
Potentially, redundancy could also be investigated through the analysis of signal correlation. This analysis showed that the mean Pearson's coefficient between the mean responses to vocalizations of neuron pairs was significantly increased by GBZ application (from 0.46 ± 0.04 to 0.77 ± 0.02; p < 0.001; Fig. 7). This increase in signal correlation indicated that every pair of recording sites tended to respond similarly to the same stimuli. This result confirmed that GBZ application induced a homogenization of the neuron responses, thus increasing their redundancy. Note that Pearson's coefficient can only be calculated for two-dimensional sets of data, hence preventing the computation of signal correlation between more than two recording sites. The signal correlation between responses to pure tones was also increased by GBZ application (from 0.2 ± 0.02 to 0.39 ± 0.03; p < 0.001; data not shown). This last result is not surprising given the fact that GBZ increased the bandwidth of the STRFs.
Together, these analyses demonstrate that decreasing the level of GABAA-mediated inhibition increases the information carried by the spike trains for individual cortical sites but also increased cortical response redundancy.
Discussion
Here, we show that, when a partial blockage of GABAA receptors is efficient enough to expand cortical STRFs, the responses of ACx neurons to communication sounds are increased and temporally more reliable. Consequently, the discrimination performance of each individual cortical site (indexed by the MI) is enhanced in terms of both firing rate and temporal spike patterns. However, under these conditions of reduced inhibition, the MI computed at the population level (i.e., when groups of 2–16 cortical sites are considered) is unaffected, and we propose that this is likely attributable to an increased redundancy of neural responses after GBZ application. These results suggest that, in normal conditions, cortical inhibition contribute to decreased cortical responses redundancy.
Methodological considerations
Topical application has been used previously in various cortical (Eder et al., 1997; Jones and Barth, 2002; Yu et al., 2008) and cerebellar (Caesar et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2009) areas because it presents several advantages. At variance with iontophoretic application, which allows application of a drug to a single location, topical application exerts an effect on the entire cortical network converging onto the recorded neurons. The drug concentration that actually reached the cortical neurons was unknown (as it is with iontophoretic application) but was certainly much lower than 10 μm given the short pulse application that we used. In our conditions, it is likely that all cortical layers were affected by the drug application as indicated by the recordings obtained simultaneously at different depths with a 16-channel silicon probe (Fig. 1). Special care was taken to ensure that blocking GABAA receptors did not trigger pathological activity: the low drug concentration and the short period of drug application allowed modulation of cortical inhibition without any sign of epileptiform activity. In fact, we first determined (in pilot experiments; see Materials and Methods) the concentration/duration of GBZ application that triggered epileptic activities (12 min application at 20 μm) and reduced the concentration/duration to a level that did not trigger epileptic activity (4 min application at 10 μm).
Obviously, this technique presents pitfalls because applying drugs directly on the cortical surface can produce mechanical instability during electrophysiological recordings. This might explain why saline application led to non-negligible changes in firing rate (relative to control: 20% in spontaneous activity; 11% in evoked activity). Also, in contrast with iontophoretic application (Manunta and Edeline, 1997, 1998), it was not always possible here to observe a recovery after GBZ application: when the recordings were stable over long time periods, the effects were often present up to 1 h after application (for an example of lasting effects, see Fig. 3A; for a 1 h post-recovery, see Fig. 5C). Despite these limitations, the results obtained on the STRF parameters correspond to what should be expected based on the known action of GABAA receptor antagonists on cortical neurons.
Caution must be exerted in the interpretation of our findings because multiunit activity was recorded in our experiments and individual neurons were not considered. As a consequence, the MI evaluated at the level of a single cortical site is already the reflection of a local computation performed by a small population of individual neurons. More importantly, the redundancy was computed between small groups of neurons located at various distances from each other on the tonotopic map. At the level of the tonotopic map, reducing cortical inhibition did not influence the population MI but increased the information redundancy. At the level of a single cortical site, MI was increased, but it cannot be assessed whether the redundancy between individual neurons was unaffected, increased, or decreased. Clearly, an additional level of analysis is lacking for complete understanding of how different cell types contribute to increase the local MI in a small cortical column when inhibition is attenuated. In the following discussion, we should keep in mind that the “local” effects discussed here are at the level of a single cortical site but they might stem from heterogeneous effects at the level of the individual neurons.
Comparison with studies describing the effects of GABA antagonists on ACx tuning curves
It has long been proposed that GABAergic inhibition sculpts ACx receptive fields (for review, see Suga, 1995; Wehr and Metherate, 2011; Yuan et al., 2011). The concept of lateral inhibition was initially promoted by the two-tone suppression (or forward masking) protocol (Brosch and Schreiner, 1997), but direct evidence only emerged when it was shown that iontophoretic application of bicuculline expands the frequency tuning curves of ACx neurons (Wang et al., 2000, 2002; but see Kurt et al., 2006). In contrast, whole-cell recordings (Wehr and Zador, 2003, 2005; Tan et al., 2004) pointed out the co-tuning of excitation and inhibition converging onto cortical cells and did not reveal a broader tuning of inhibitory currents (but see Wu et al., 2008 for opposite results). In the temporal domain, it was shown that GBZ application induced deterioration of phase-locking to high modulation frequencies (Kurt et al., 2006). With application of GABAA antagonist over the entire cortical map (which reduced the activity of the entire inhibitory network converging onto the recorded cells), we report here a concomitant bandwidth and duration expansion of STRFs, suggesting that GABAA receptors influence both spectral and temporal selectivity of ACx neurons.
The lack of effects of GABAB antagonists is surprising given that in vitro studies have documented the presence of GABAB currents on ACx neurons when activating cortical or thalamic inputs (Cox et al., 1992; Metherate and Ashe, 1994; Hefti and Smith, 2000; Cruikshank et al., 2002; Wehr and Zador, 2005). However, it is possible that GABAB-mediated inhibition has a time constant that is too slow (160–170 ms in the study by Cox et al., 1992) and an amplitude that is too low (4–5 mV) to influence the interplay between fast EPSPs and fast IPSPs underlying the short-latency responses of ACx neurons. It might be argued that the concentration used here (up to 50 μm) is not sufficient to modulate the strength and temporal reliability of auditory responses. However, (1) much lower concentrations (50 nm to 1 μm) were used in vitro to antagonize GABAB responses (Bessaïh et al., 2006; Mann et al., 2009) and (2) no effect on laminar current source density profiles was observed with a higher concentration (6 mm) of SCH50911 [(2S)(+)5,5-dimethyl-2-morpholineacetic acid], another GABAB antagonist (Happel et al., 2010).
Influence of GABAA-mediated inhibition on responses to vocalizations
Very few studies have tried to modulate cortical inhibition with presentation of communication sounds. In the zebra finch HVC nucleus, Rosen and Mooney (2003) disrupted either GIRK-mediated inhibition or GABAA inhibition intracellularly while quantifying the response to the bird's own song. Surprisingly, temporal spike patterns were suppressed by neither GABAA nor GABAB blockers. In the functional analogous of non-primary ACx (the NCM nucleus) of birds a blockade of GABAA receptors systematically increased the phasic components of responses to conspecific songs (Pinaud et al., 2008). Similar effects were detected in our study: phasic responses were reinforced without promoting the emergence of tonic responses (Figs. 5B,C, 8). Oddly, facilitation of phasic responses observed in the NCM occurred without modifications of frequency tuning (Pinaud et al., 2008), whereas the effects reported here in the cortical STRFs are in good agreement with the response modifications to communication sounds. By reducing the level of tonic inhibition, GBZ allowed the expression of subthreshold inputs at the borders of the receptive field leading to broader STRFs. This reduced tonic inhibition also allowed expression of subthreshold inputs in response to vocalizations, which can explain the emergence of new phasic peaks in the responses obtained after GBZ application (Fig. 5B,C). In addition, strong phasic responses were often followed by prolonged periods of silence, as if the stronger excitatory responses were followed by stronger postexcitation inhibitions (Figs. 8, 9). As depicted in Figure 12, A1 and B1, this can result from the fact that inhibitory neurons are also more excitable after GBZ application (the reduction of tonic inhibition also affects these cells). Thus, the result of our experiment opposed our initial predictions. Initially, two reasons led us to suspect that blocking GABAA inhibition could impair the temporal precision of neuronal responses. First, disrupting the precise timing of IPSPs and EPSPs converging on a given cell should promote non-reproducible temporal patterns and then lead to a loss of information contained in the temporal patterns. Second, reducing the level of tonic inhibition should increase spontaneous activity, which could also disorganize the temporal patterns by random insertion of spikes in evoked responses. However, these two effects were totally surpassed by a third one, namely by the reinforcement of the feedforward inhibition occurring after each excitatory input from the thalamus. As a consequence, after a salient acoustic event triggering a thalamic volley, the cortex responded with larger excitatory responses followed by a silent period until a new thalamic volley—activated by another acoustic event—triggered the new phasic cortical response. Compared with the control condition, the periods of strong activation followed by silent periods led to a higher spike-timing reliability. Because the exact timing of the sequence of strong activation/inhibition depends on the vocalization acoustic features, the partial blockage of GABAA receptors reinforced the differences between spike patterns elicited by different vocalizations at each cortical site. Thus, at a given cortical site, a partial blockage of intracortical inhibition led to higher levels of information carried by each neuron, simply by reinforcing the within-stimulus similarity (spike-timing reliability) and the between-stimuli differences (temporal response patterns).
Role of inhibition in population coding
Partial removal of intracortical inhibition that enhances information contained in spike trains of cortical neurons is a counterintuitive result. However, when groups of simultaneously recorded cortical sites were considered, the population information was not increased by partial blockage of inhibition. In fact, we have to keep in mind that, compared with neurons of subcortical relays, ACx neurons have difficulty in providing an accurate description of the rapidly fluctuating features of communication sounds (Joris et al., 2004; but see Elhilali et al., 2004; Huetz et al., 2011; for review, see Gaucher et al., 2013). Attenuating intracortical inhibition tends to promote a regimen of activity that is mostly controlled by thalamic inputs and might favor responses that are a closer reflection of the stimulus acoustic characteristics than in control situations. Thus, at presentation of a given vocalization, the redundancy between cortical sites is increased because many cortical sites provide similar response profiles (compare the responses of neurons 1 and 2 in Fig. 12A3 in control conditions vs after GBZ application in Fig. 12B3; Pola et al., 2003). This view echoes the results obtained by Chechik et al. (2006) who suggested that reduction of redundancy may be a general principle of sensory systems when progressing from the brainstem to the cortex. One can consider that the role of cortical inhibition is to promote neuronal representations that are less correlated with the stimulus acoustic features than those already present at the subcortical levels. This can be achieved by restricting the emission of action potentials to precise temporal windows, which differ from a cortical site to the next (and from one neuron to the next). This would corroborate the sparse code hypothesis according to which auditory neurons tend to respond to a small number of acoustic features with only a few action potentials (DeWeese et al., 2003; Hromádka et al., 2008) and in a noncorrelated way between each other (Chen et al., 2012). Moreover, it has been shown that alterations of GABAergic inhibition (for example, by estradiol) can affect both the neuronal and behavioral discriminative performance (Tremere and Pinaud, 2011), which suggests that the generation of cortical spike patterns is one of the key elements underlying the perceptual ability to discriminate between communication sounds (Wang et al., 2007; Walker et al., 2008; Shetake et al., 2011).
Conclusions
Over the past decades, several functional roles have been attributed to cortical inhibition. At the single-cell level, sharpening the functional properties of cortical neurons (frequency tuning in the auditory modality, orientation tuning in the visual system) has been, and is still, one of the most popular hypotheses (for review, see Suga, 1995; Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011), regardless that is achieved by a similar co-tuning of excitation and inhibition, a broad tuning, or no tuning at all of the inhibition (Cardin et al., 2007; Wu et al., 2008; Katzner et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2011). At the network level, a broad base of literature now indicates that inhibition is involved in the generation and the spatial coherence of fast (20–80 Hz) oscillations (Traub et al., 1996, 1997; Wang and Buzsáki, 1996; Hasenstaub et al., 2005; Atallah and Scanziani, 2009; Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009) and promotes layer selectivity of these oscillations in the somatosensory cortex (Adesnik and Scanziani, 2010). Between these two extremes of a multiscale analysis, our results point out that cortical inhibition can modulate the information content of neuronal responses at both the level of a single cortical site and the map level. Dissecting the mechanisms involved at these different levels of analysis should be a main challenge for future studies, particularly because inhibition dominates sensory responses in waking state (Haider et al., 2013).
Footnotes
This work was supported by grants from the National Research Agency (HearFin, Program Neuro2011; Hearing Loss, Program Neuro2006) (J.-M.E.). Q.G. was supported by a fellowship from the Ministry of State Education and Research. We thank J. J. Eggermont and G. Shaw for their generous help with the acquisition software and N. Leresche for advices on the GABAB antagonists. Special thanks to Nathalie Samson and Pascale Leblanc-Veyrac for taking care of the guinea pig colony and to Sabrina Davis for improving the language of this manuscript.
References
- Adesnik H, Scanziani M. Lateral competition for cortical space by layer-specific horizontal circuits. Nature. 2010;464:1155–1160. doi: 10.1038/nature08935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atallah BV, Scanziani M. Instantaneous modulation of gamma oscillation frequency by balancing excitation with inhibition. Neuron. 2009;62:566–577. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Averbeck BB, Lee D. Effects of noise correlations on information encoding and decoding. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:3633–3644. doi: 10.1152/jn.00919.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Averbeck BB, Latham PE, Pouget A. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:358–366. doi: 10.1038/nrn1888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bessaïh T, Bourgeais L, Badiu CI, Carter DA, Toth TI, Ruano D, Lambolez B, Crunelli V, Leresche N. Nucleus-specific abnormalities of GABAergic synaptic transmission in a genetic model of absence seizures. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:3074–3081. doi: 10.1152/jn.00682.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brosch M, Schreiner CE. Time course of forward masking tuning curves in cat primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77:923–943. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.2.923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caesar K, Thomsen K, Lauritzen M. Dissociation of spikes, synaptic activity, and activity-dependent increments in rat cerebellar blood flow by tonic synaptic inhibition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:16000–16005. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2635195100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caesar K, Offenhauser N, Lauritzen M. Gamma-aminobutyric acid modulates local brain oxygen consumption and blood flow in rat cerebellar cortex. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2008;28:906–915. doi: 10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calford MB. Mechanisms for acute changes in sensory maps. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2002;508:451–460. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-0713-0_51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Palmer LA, Contreras D. Stimulus feature selectivity in excitatory and inhibitory neurons in primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27:10333–10344. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1692-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Carlén M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Tsai LH, Moore CI. Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature. 2009;459:663–667. doi: 10.1038/nature08002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chechik G, Anderson MJ, Bar-Yosef O, Young ED, Tishby N, Nelken I. Reduction of information redundancy in the ascending auditory pathway. Neuron. 2006;51:359–368. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen C, Read HL, Escabí MA. Precise feature based time scales and frequency decorrelation lead to a sparse auditory code. J Neurosci. 2012;32:8454–8468. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6506-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox CL, Metherate R, Weinberger NM, Ashe JH. Synaptic potentials and effects of amino acid antagonists in the auditory cortex. Brain Res Bull. 1992;28:401–410. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(92)90039-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruikshank SJ, Rose HJ, Metherate R. Auditory thalamocortical synaptic transmission in vitro. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:361–384. doi: 10.1152/jn.00549.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darbin O, Wichmann T. Effects of striatal GABAA-receptor blockade on striatal and cortical activity in monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99:1294–1305. doi: 10.1152/jn.01191.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darbin O, Newton L, Wichmann T. A new probe to monitor the effects of drugs on local field potentials. J Neurosci Methods. 2006;155:291–295. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeWeese MR, Wehr M, Zador AM. Binary spiking in auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2003;23:7940–7949. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-21-07940.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebner FF, Armstrong-James MA. Intracortical processes regulating the integration of sensory information. Prog Brain Res. 1990;86:129–141. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63172-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edeline JM, Weinberger NM. Receptive field plasticity in the auditory cortex during frequency discrimination training: selective retuning independent of task difficulty. Behav Neurosci. 1993;107:82–103. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.1.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edeline JM, Dutrieux G, Manunta Y, Hennevin E. Diversity of receptive field changes in auditory cortex during natural sleep. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;14:1865–1880. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01821.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eder HG, Jones DB, Fisher RS. Local perfusion of diazepam attenuates interictal and ictal events in the bicuculline model of epilepsy in rats. Epilepsia. 1997;38:516–521. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1997.tb01134.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elhilali M, Fritz JB, Klein DJ, Simon JZ, Shamma SA. Dynamics of precise spike timing in primary auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24:1159–1172. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3825-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaucher Q, Edeline JM, Gourévitch B. How different are the local field potentials and spiking activities? Insights from multi-electrodes arrays. J Physiol Paris. 2012;106:93–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaucher Q, Huetz C, Gourévitch B, Laudanski J, Occelli F, Edeline JM. How do auditory cortex neurons represent communication sounds? Hear Res. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.03.011. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.03.011. Advance online publication. Retrieved May 21, 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Intrinsic connectivity and receptive field properties in visual cortex. Vision Res. 1985;25:365–374. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90061-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gourévitch B, Edeline JM. Age-related changes in the guinea pig auditory cortex: relationship with peripheral changes and comparison with tone-induced hearing loss. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;34:1953–1965. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07905.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gourévitch B, Doisy T, Avillac M, Edeline JM. Follow-up of latency and threshold shifts of auditory brainstem responses after single and interrupted acoustic trauma in guinea pig. Brain Res. 2009;1304:66–79. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haider B, Häusser M, Carandini M. Inhibition dominates sensory responses in the awake cortex. Nature. 2013;493:97–100. doi: 10.1038/nature11665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Happel MF, Jeschke M, Ohl FW. Spectral integration in primary auditory cortex attribuable to temporally precise convergence of thalamo-cortical and intracortical inputs. J Neurosci. 2010;30:11114–11127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0689-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasenstaub A, Shu Y, Haider B, Kraushaar U, Duque A, McCormick DA. Inhibitory postsynaptic potentials carry synchronized frequency information in active cortical networks. Neuron. 2005;47:423–435. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hefti BJ, Smith PH. Anatomy, physiology, and synaptic responses of rat layer V auditory cortical cells and effects of intracellular GABA(A) blockade. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:2626–2638. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.5.2626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirsch JA. Synaptic physiology and receptive field structure in the early visual pathway of the cat. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:63–69. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.1.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hromádka T, Deweese MR, Zador AM. Sparse representation of sounds in the unanesthetized auditory cortex. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:e16. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huetz C, Del Negro C, Lebas N, Tarroux P, Edeline JM. Contribution of spike timing to the information transmitted by HVC neurons. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:1091–1108. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04967.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huetz C, Philibert B, Edeline JM. A spike-timing code for discriminating conspecific vocalizations in the thalamocortical system of anesthetized and awake guinea pigs. J Neurosci. 2009;29:334–350. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3269-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huetz C, Gourévitch B, Edeline JM. Neural codes in the thalamocortical auditory system: from artificial stimuli to communication sounds. Hear Res. 2011;271:147–158. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacson JS, Scanziani M. How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron. 2011;72:231–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones MS, Barth DS. Effects of bicuculline methiodide on fast (>200 Hz) electrical oscillations in rat somatosensory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88:1016–1025. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joris PX. Interaural time sensitivity dominated by cochlea-induced envelope patterns. J Neurosci. 2003;23:6345–6350. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-15-06345.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joris PX, Schreiner CE, Rees A. Neural processing of amplitude-modulated sounds. Physiol Rev. 2004;84:541–577. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00029.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katzner S, Busse L, Carandini M. GABAA inhibition controls response gain in visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31:5931–5941. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5753-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurt S, Crook JM, Ohl FW, Scheich H, Schulze H. Differential effects of iontophoretic in vivo application of the GABA(A)-antagonists bicuculline and gabazine in sensory cortex. Hear Res. 2006;212:224–235. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latham PE, Nirenberg S. Synergy, redundancy, and independence in population codes, revisited. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5195–5206. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5319-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu BH, Li YT, Ma WP, Pan CJ, Zhang LI, Tao HW. Broad inhibition sharpens orientation selectivity by expanding input dynamic range in mouse simple cells. Neuron. 2011;71:542–554. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mann EO, Kohl MM, Paulsen O. Distinct roles of GABAA and GABAB receptors in balancing and terminating persistent cortical activity. J Neurosci. 2009;29:7513–7518. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6162-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manunta Y, Edeline JM. Effects of noradrenaline on frequency tuning of auditory cortex neurons. Eur J Neurosci. 1997;9:833–847. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01433.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manunta Y, Edeline JM. Effects of noradrenaline on rate-level function of auditory cortex neurons: is there a gating effect of noradrenaline? Exp Brain Res. 1998;118:361–372. doi: 10.1007/s002210050290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manunta Y, Edeline JM. Effects of noradrenaline on frequency tuning of auditory cortex neurons during wakefulness and slow-wave sleep. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:2134–2150. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00633.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metherate R, Ashe JH. Facilitation of an NMDA receptor-mediated EPSP by paired-pulse stimulation in rat neocortex via depression of GABAergic IPSPs. J Physiol. 1994;481:331–348. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1994.sp020443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narayanan NS, Kimchi EY, Laubach M. Redundancy and synergy of neuronal ensembles in motor cortex. J Neurosci. 2005;25:4207–4216. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4697-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narayan R, Graña G, Sen K. Distinct time scales in cortical discrimination of natural sounds in songbirds. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:252–258. doi: 10.1152/jn.01257.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narayan R, Best V, Ozmeral E, McClaine E, Dent M, Shinn-Cunningham B, Sen K. Cortical interference effects in the cocktail party problem. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1601–1607. doi: 10.1038/nn2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oswald AM, Schiff ML, Reyes AD. Synaptic mechanisms underlying auditory processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:371–376. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinaud R, Terleph TA, Tremere LA, Phan ML, Dagostin AA, Leão RM, Mello CV, Vicario DS. Inhibitory network interactions shape the auditory processing of natural communication signals in the songbird auditory forebrain. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:441–455. doi: 10.1152/jn.01239.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pola G, Thiele A, Hoffmann KP, Panzeri S. An exact method to quantify the information transmitted by different mechanisms of correlational coding. Network. 2003;14:35–60. doi: 10.1088/0954-898X/14/1/303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Recanzone GH. Representation of con-specific vocalizations in the core and belt areas of the auditory cortex in the alert macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 2008;28:13184–13193. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3619-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riquimaroux H, Gaioni SJ, Suga N. Cortical computational maps control auditory perception. Science. 1991;251:565–568. doi: 10.1126/science.1990432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riquimaroux H, Gaioni SJ, Suga N. Inactivation of the DSCF area of the auditory cortex with muscimol disrupts frequency discrimination in the mustached bat. J Neurophysiol. 1992;68:1613–1623. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.5.1613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson D, Irvine DR. Plasticity of frequency organization in auditory cortex of guinea pigs with partial unilateral deafness. J Comp Neurol. 1989;282:456–471. doi: 10.1002/cne.902820311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosen MJ, Mooney R. Inhibitory and excitatory mechanisms underlying auditory responses to learned vocalizations in the songbird nucleus HVC. Neuron. 2003;39:177–194. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00357-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnupp JW, Hall TM, Kokelaar RF, Ahmed B. Plasticity of temporal pattern codes for vocalization stimuli in primary auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2006;26:4785–4795. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4330-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shannon CE. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical J. 1948;27:379–423. [Google Scholar]
- Shetake JA, Wolf JT, Cheung RJ, Engineer CT, Ram SK, Kilgard MP. Cortical activity patterns predict robust speech discrimination ability in noise. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;34:1823–1838. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07887.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- So K, Ganguly K, Jimenez J, Gastpar MC, Carmena JM. Redundant information encoding in primary motor cortex during natural and prosthetic motor control. J Comput Neurosci. 2012;32:555–561. doi: 10.1007/s10827-011-0369-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sohal VS, Zhang F, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K. Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature. 2009;459:698–702. doi: 10.1038/nature07991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suga N. Sharpening of frequency tuning by inhibition in the central auditory system: tribute to Yasuji Katsuki. Neurosci Res. 1995;21:287–299. doi: 10.1016/0168-0102(94)00868-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swadlow HA. Fast-spike interneurons and feedforward inhibition in awake sensory neocortex. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:25–32. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tachibana Y, Kita H, Chiken S, Takada M, Nambu A. Motor cortical control of internal pallidal activity through glutamatergic and GABAergic inputs in awake monkeys. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;27:238–253. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05990.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan AY, Zhang LI, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE. Tone-evoked excitatory and inhibitory synaptic conductances of primary auditory cortex neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92:630–643. doi: 10.1152/jn.01020.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traub RD, Whittington MA, Stanford IM, Jefferys JG. A mechanism for generation of long-range synchronous fast oscillations in the cortex. Nature. 1996;383:621–624. doi: 10.1038/383621a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traub RD, Jefferys JG, Whittington MA. Simulation of gamma rhythms in networks of interneurons and pyramidal cells. J Comput Neurosci. 1997;4:141–150. doi: 10.1023/A:1008839312043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremere LA, Pinaud R. Brain-generated estradiol drives long-term optimization of auditory coding to enhance the discrimination of communication signals. J Neurosci. 2011;31:3271–3289. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4355-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker KM, Ahmed B, Schnupp JW. Linking cortical spike pattern codes to auditory perception. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20:135–152. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace MN, Palmer AR. Laminar differences in the response properties of cells in the primary auditory cortex. Exp Brain Res. 2008;184:179–191. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1092-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace MN, Rutkowski RG, Palmer AR. Identification and localisation of auditory areas in guinea pig cortex. Exp Brain Res. 2000;132:445–456. doi: 10.1007/s002210000362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang J, Caspary D, Salvi RJ. GABA-A antagonist causes dramatic expansion of tuning in primary auditory cortex. Neuroreport. 2000;11:1137–1140. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200004070-00045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang J, McFadden SL, Caspary D, Salvi R. Gamma-aminobutyric acid circuits shape response properties of auditory cortex neurons. Brain Res. 2002;944:219–231. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02926-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang L, Narayan R, Graña G, Shamir M, Sen K. Cortical discrimination of complex natural stimuli: can single neurons match behavior? J Neurosci. 2007;27:582–589. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3699-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X, Chen G, Gao W, Ebner T. Long-term potentiation of the responses to parallel fiber stimulation in mouse cerebellar cortex in vivo. Neuroscience. 2009;162:713–722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang XJ, Buzsáki G. Gamma oscillation by synaptic inhibition in a hippocampal interneuronal network model. J Neurosci. 1996;16:6402–6413. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-20-06402.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wehr M, Metherate R. Synaptic integration in auditory cortex. In: Winer JA, Schreiner CE, editors. The auditory cortex. New York: Springer; 2011. pp. 235–250. [Google Scholar]
- Wehr M, Zador AM. Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature. 2003;426:442–446. doi: 10.1038/nature02116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wehr M, Zador AM. Synaptic mechanisms of forward suppression in rat auditory cortex. Neuron. 2005;47:437–445. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu GK, Arbuckle R, Liu BH, Tao HW, Zhang LI. Lateral sharpening of cortical frequency tuning by approximately balanced inhibition. Neuron. 2008;58:132–143. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu H, Chen X, Sun C, Shou T. Global evaluation of contributions of GABA A, AMPA and NMDA receptors to orientation maps in cat's visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2008;40:776–787. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuan K, Shih JY, Winer JA, Schreiner CE. Functional networks of parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in cat auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31:13333–13342. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1000-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]