Skip to main content
Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN logoLink to Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN
. 2013 Sep;38(5):317–324. doi: 10.1503/jpn.120143

Eye movements during natural actions in patients with schizophrenia

Céline Delerue 1,, Mary Hayhoe 1, Muriel Boucart 1
PMCID: PMC3756115  PMID: 23552500

Abstract

Background

Visual scanning and planning of actions are reported to be abnormal in patients with schizophrenia. Most studies that monitored eye movements in these patients were performed under free-viewing conditions and used 2-dimensional images. However, images differ from the natural world in several ways, including task demands and the dimensionality of the display. Our study was designed to assess whether abnormalities in visual exploration in patients with schizophrenia generalize to active-viewing tasks in realistic conditions of viewing and to examine whether disturbances in action sequencing in these patients are reflected in their visual scanning patterns while executing natural tasks.

Methods

We monitored visual scan paths in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Participants performed several tasks in which they were asked to look at a realistic scene on a table (free-viewing) and perform 2 active-viewing tasks: a familiar task (sandwich-making) and an unfamiliar task (model-building). The scenes contained both task-relevant and task-irrelevant objects.

Results

We included 15 patients and 15 controls in our analysis. Patients exhibited abnormalities in the free-viewing condition. Their patterns of exploration were similar to those of controls in the familiar task, but they showed scanning differences in the unfamiliar task. Patients were also slower than controls to accomplish both tasks.

Limitations

Patients with schizophrenia were taking antipsychotic medications, so the presence of medication effects cannot be excluded.

Conclusion

People with schizophrenia present a basic psycho-motor slowing and seem to establish a less efficient planning strategy in the case of more complex and unfamiliar tasks.

Introduction

Eye movement disturbances in visual exploration have been reported in patients with schizophrenia, and visual scan path abnormalities have been proposed to serve as a trait marker for the disorder.1,2 Benson and colleagues3 have even suggested that simple eye movement tests, such as smooth pursuit, fixation stability or free-viewing tasks, can distinguish between schizophrenia and the control condition in case–control studies. Studies monitoring eye movements have found reduced visual scan paths on photographs of faces1,48 and various other stimuli, such as complex scenes;9 geometric shapes;10 Rorschach stimuli;11 and photographs of landscapes, fractals and meaningless patterns.12 In most of these studies, visual scanning was examined under free-viewing conditions,4,12 and in some,1,5,7 participants were asked to determine the facial expression. Moreover, these studies used 2-dimensional (2-D) images; however, 2-D images differ from the natural world in several ways, including task demands and the dimensionality of the display. One of the objectives of our study was to assess whether abnormalities in visual exploration in patients with schizophrenia generalize to active-viewing tasks under realistic viewing conditions.

Numerous studies have documented various forms of disturbances of action production, such as poverty of action, disorganization behaviour, stereotyped and incoherent actions, in patients with schizophrenia. Psychomotor slowing is considered an important clinical characteristic of schizophrenia, but its exact nature remains unclear, as it involves multiple aspects, including goal selection, inhibition, planning, sequencing and execution. Several studies have provided evidence of psychomotor slowing and planning deficits in patients with schizophrenia. Jogems-Kosterman and colleagues13 reported that patients with schizophrenia were slower than controls in copying tasks. This slowing was found in initiation time (i.e., before starting an action) and in movement time (i.e., during the execution period). Moreover, increased figure complexity, or decreased familiarity, lengthened the initiation time. Grootens and colleagues14 demonstrated that psychomotor planning deficits were already present in the early stages of schizophrenia and involved deficient planning but intact motor action. In figure-copying tasks, patients with recent-onset schizophrenia were substantially slower than controls in the initiation of motor actions as soon as they encountered a minor increase in complexity, such as unfamiliar figures or the planning of a motor sequence. Using a sequential pointing task, Delevoye-Turrell and colleagues15 found that patients with schizophrenia executed the sequences less fluently than controls, particularly when sequences of action were required, and that the patients’ fluency deficit increased with sequence complexity. In a computerized version of the Tower of London task, Pantelis and colleagues16 found that increased task complexity prolonged the duration of the execution period more strongly in patients with schizophrenia than in controls. This result suggests that patients need more time during the task for further planning and monitoring of their movements. Semkovska and colleagues17 and Stip and colleagues18 suggested that deficits in executive functions have a detrimental impact on typical activities of daily living. With a standardized behavioural scale of action sequences, these studies have assessed patients’ ability to perform daily activities, such as choosing a menu, shopping and cooking. Patients with schizophrenia showed more planning, sequencing, repetition and omission errors than controls. Despite many studies on action planning deficits in patients with schizophrenia, their ability to plan and to accomplish a sequence of actions has, to our knowledge, not yet been studied using eye movement recording. Therefore, we sought to examine whether disturbances in action sequencing in patients with schizophrenia are reflected in their visual scanning patterns while executing well-learned or less familiar natural actions.

We examined the pattern of eye movements in patients with schizophrenia using the sandwich-making task,19 as its pattern is well known in healthy observers. In studies on food preparation, there are periods of search, particularly before the actual task begins. During these periods, objects are located, and at least some of their positions are memorized, but without any manipulative action. Then, more commonly, both hands are engaged with the same object, and in a few cases, the 2 hands have separate roles. Very few irrelevant objects are fixated during the execution of the task in healthy participants. The gaze moves from one task-relevant object to the next, ignoring all other objects not involved in the task, even when they are salient objects in the environment. The authors19 concluded that, in real tasks, the eyes are driven much more by top–down information from the script of the activity, and very little by salient features, such as contrast. They also observed that the fixations are precisely linked in time to actions, such as placing or grasping an object. Gaze moves to locations where information critical for manipulation is obtained, and fixation precedes action by a short interval, usually less than a second.1921 Observers appear to use gaze to select the specific information required for a point in the task. This aspect of natural behaviour has been called a “just-in-time” strategy.22

Based on previous findings, especially reduced visual exploration, the fact that patients with schizophrenia exhibit substantial difficulties with planning and organization of action, and that these patients show an exaggerated susceptibility to distraction,23 we expected that patients would explore less, exhibit fewer fixations to the relevant objects and more fixations to irrelevant objects than controls, and that the familiar task would be better accomplished than the unfamiliar task.

Methods

Participants

We recruited medicated in- and outpatients fulfilling the DSM–IV diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia24 and age- and sex-matched healthy controls without a psychiatric diagnosis (Axis I and II) and without a family history of mental illness to participate in the experiment. Patients were recruited from the Department of General Psychiatry in Lille University Medical Center and from the Psychology Center in Béthune (France). Controls were recruited among students and members of the medical staff. The inclusion criterion for all groups was normal or corrected-to-normal vision (assessed by the Snellen chart). Exclusion criteria were recent history of substance abuse, ocular disease, epilepsy and other neurologic disorders, and failure to understand the instructions. We rated schizophrenia symptoms using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS).25 The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Lille University Hospital. All participants provided informed consent.

Stimuli

Different “familiar sandwich” scenes (Fig. 1A) were built containing 7 task-relevant objects required to make a butter and jelly sandwich and pour a glass of water, as well as 7 task-irrelevant objects. All objects were laid out on a table. When the participant was seated at the table, with all objects within reach, the plate close to the observer subtended about 20° of the visual angle and the butter and jelly subtended about 7°. All objects were located within a region covering 90°.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Examples of the 2 scene layouts used. (A) The “familiar sandwich” scene contained both task-relevant (bread, butter, jelly, knife, plate, glass, water bottle) and irrelevant objects (fork, tool, plug, tape measure, stapler, soda, spice jar). (B) The “unfamiliar construction set” scene contained both task-relevant (model pieces, screws, nuts) and irrelevant objects (2 other pieces from construction set that were not necessary, 2 different kinds of paperclips).

An “unfamiliar construction set” scene (Fig. 1B) was also built containing a display model and several pieces from a child’s construction set in 9 plastic containers on another table. Three containers held the model pieces that had to be manipulated; 2 contained screws and nuts, respectively; and 4 contained distractor pieces. All containers were within reach of the participants.

The 2 scenes were occluded by a white board showing the calibration points, which was removed once the calibration was completed. Only 1 scene was visible at a time.

Equipment

We monitored monocular (right) eye position using the iViewX HED (SensoMotoric Instruments) eye tracker with a scene camera. The video-based eye tracker is head-mounted and uses infrared reflection to provide an eye-in-head signal at a sampling rate of 50 Hz and an accuracy of about 1°. The scene camera mounted on the head was positioned so that its field of view was centred on the participant’s field of view. Calibration was performed using a 5-point grid. Following calibration, the eye tracker creates a cursor, indicating eye-in-head position, which is merged with the video from the scene camera. The scene camera moves with the head, so the eye-in-head signal indicates the gaze point. The eye tracker thus provides a video recording of eye position from the participant’s perspective, and the data analysis is based on the video, as there is no separate numerical data stream. We analyzed the video recordings on a frame-by-frame basis, recording the time of initiation and termination of each eye and hand movement, the location of the fixations and the nature of the hand actions. Saccades appeared in the large displacements of the cursor between video frames. The beginning and end of each saccade was identified and recorded using a video analysis tool. Fixations are defined visually when the cursor stays within a given location (less than a degree) defined by the noise level of the tracker. Thus, fixations are defined jointly by position and velocity. Blinks are detected by occlusion of the pupil, and the cursor is occluded during the blink.

Procedure

Participants started with a free-viewing task in which they were asked to look at a realistic scene (“sandwich scene”) on a table for 10 seconds. Then, participants performed 2 active-viewing tasks. The first was to make a butter and jelly sandwich and pour a glass of water (familiar task), and the second was to assemble 4 wooden slats from a child’s construction set using screws and nuts according to a display model (unfamiliar task). The order of tasks was randomized.

Before the experiments, the layout was occluded by a white board showing the 5 calibration points, enabling the participants to be calibrated on the plane of the working surface. The participant had to fixate the targets (coloured dots) while his/her eye positions were recorded by the system. Once the calibration was completed, the white board was removed, and the participant immediately started the task. A recalibration procedure occurred after each task. The entire session lasted about 30 minutes.

Data analysis

We tested 2 experimental conditions: free versus active viewing and familiar versus unfamiliar task. We measured the duration of individual fixations and the total gaze duration on specific objects in instances where several successive fixations were made on the same object. Gaze duration on both relevant and irrelevant objects was then determined. Eye movement variables were submitted to analyses of variance (ANOVAs) using the STATISTICA software from StatSoft (version 7.1). We examined possible confounding effects of medication, illness duration, positive and negative symptom categories (indexed by PANSS), patient categories, sex and handedness. For patients with schizophrenia, there were no significant correlations between medication, illness duration or symptom category on one hand and any of the scan path variables on the other. We computed associations between sex or handedness and scan path variables for both groups. No statistically significant correlations emerged.

Results

Participants

Fifteen patients with schizophrenia and 15 age- and sex-matched healthy controls took part in the experiment. Participant demographic and clinical characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls performing free-viewing and active-viewing tasks

Group; mean (SD)*

Characteristic Schizophrenia, n = 15 Control, n = 15
Age, yr 35.1 (7.1) 33.9 (8.7)
Sex, no. male:female 13:2 13:2
Handedness, no. right:left 12:3 12:3
Antipsychotic medication, mg chlorpromazine equivalent 469.8 (186.8)
Benzodiazepine medication, mg diazepam equivalent 154.5 (40.9)
Illness duration, yr 11.4 (8.2)
PANSS score
 Positive symptom 19.1
 Negative symptom 21.2
 General psychopathology 37.7
 Total 78.0
*

Unless otherwise indicated.

PANSS = Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale;25 SD = standard deviation.

Free versus active viewing

Fixation durations

The fixation duration refers to the mean duration across all fixations. A 2 (group: patients/controls) ×2 (task: free/active viewing) repeated-measures multivariate ANOVA showed a significant main effect of group (F1,28 = 23.3, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.45) and task (F1,28 = 49.6, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.64) on the fixation durations. There was also a significant interaction between group and task (F1,28 = 10.8, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.28). Patients exhibited longer fixation durations in the free-viewing condition (F1,28 = 56.5, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.67), but did not differ from controls in the active-viewing condition (F1,28 = 1.8, p = 0.19, ηp2 = 0.06; Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Mean fixation durations for patients and controls in the sandwich-making task as a function of viewing condition (free v. active viewing). *p < 0.001.

A contrast analysis for patients and controls showed that both groups had longer fixation durations in the active-viewing condition (patients: free v. active viewing, F1,28 = 7.1, p = 0.013, ηp2 = 0.20; controls, free v. active viewing, F1,28 = 53.2, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.66).

Gaze durations on relevant and irrelevant objects

A 2 (group: patients/controls) ×2 (task: free/active viewing) ×2 (objects: relevant/irrelevant) repeated-measures multivariate ANOVA showed a significant main effect of group (F1,28 = 13.8, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.33), task (F1,28 = 29.4, p < 0.001, p2 = 0.51) and objects (F1,28 = 115.3, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.80) on the gaze durations. There was no significant interaction between group, task and objects (F1,28 = 2.1, p = 0.15, ηp2 = 0.07), but there was a significant interaction between group and task (F1,28 = 14.1, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.34) and between task and objects (F1,28 = 164.7, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.85; Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Gaze durations for patients and controls in the sandwich-making task as a function of viewing condition (free v. active viewing). *p < 0.001. IO = irrelevant objects, RO = relevant objects.

Free-viewing condition

Patients with schizophrenia exhibited longer gaze durations than controls on objects in general in the free-viewing condition (relevant objects, F1,28 = 49.5, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.64; irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 15.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.35).

Patients and controls looked equally at relevant and irrelevant objects in the free-viewing condition (patients, relevant v. irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 3.2, p = 0.08, ηp2 = 0.10; controls, relevant v. irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 1.3, p = 0.26, ηp2 = 0.05).

Active-viewing condition

Patients did not differ from controls in the realistic task (relevant objects, F1,28 = 0.01, p = 0.91, ηp2 = 0.01; irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 2.8, p = 0.10, ηp2 = 0.10).

Patients and controls looked more at relevant objects in the active-viewing condition (patients, relevant v. irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 77.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.73; controls, relevant v. irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 88.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.76).

Free- versus active-viewing conditions

Patients looked more at relevant objects and less at irrelevant objects in the real task, as did controls (patients: relevant objects, free v. active viewing, F1,28 = 25.3, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.47; irrelevant objects, free v. active viewing, F1,28 = 52.6, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.65; controls: relevant objects, free v. active viewing, F1,28 = 85.2, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.75; irrelevant objects, free v. active viewing, F1,28 = 12.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.29).

Familiar versus unfamiliar task

Performance task durations

Total task duration

A 2 (group: patients/controls) ×2 (task: familiar/unfamiliar) repeated-measures multivariate ANOVA showed a significant main effect of group (F1,28 = 44.1, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.61) and task (F1,28 = 51.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.65) on the task durations. There was a significant interaction between group and task (F1,28 = 19.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.42). For accomplishing the familiar task, patients needed on average 1.54 minutes, whereas controls needed 1.19 minutes (F1,28 = 22.1, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.44). In the unfamiliar task, these values were 3.20 minutes for patients and 1.57 minutes for controls (F1,28 = 33.3, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.54). Patients took longer in the unfamiliar than the familiar task (F1,28 = 67.7, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.71), but there was no significant difference between the 2 tasks among controls (F1,28 = 3.7, p = 0.07, ηp2 = 0.12).

Pretask duration

We examined visual scanning after the scene was initially exposed by removing the calibration display and before the first reaching movements, which indicated that participants had begun the task. This period was called “pretask.” A 2 (group) ×2 (task) repeated-measures multivariate ANOVA showed a significant main effect of group (F1,28 = 88.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.76) and task (F1,28 = 24.5, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.47) on the pretask durations. There was a significant interaction between group and task (F1,28 = 5.2, p = 0.030, ηp2 = 0.16). Relative to controls, the pretask was significantly longer in patients both in the familiar task (F1,28 = 22.0, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.44) and in the unfamiliar task (F1,28 = 47.6, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.63), and the pretask duration was even longer in the more complex unfamiliar task (F1,28 = 26.1, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.48). Patients needed on average 4.9 seconds (compared with 3.1 s in the control group) to make the first move in the familiar task. In the unfamiliar task, these values were 7.6 seconds for patients and 4.0 seconds for controls. Patients were slower than controls in the pretask.

Gaze durations on relevant and irrelevant objects

To examine visual exploration on the objects, we determined 2 time periods for the 2 tasks: the pretask (i.e., period before the first reaching movement) and the “working” period (i.e., period during which participants accomplished the task).

A 2 (group: patients/controls) ×2 (task: familiar/unfamiliar) ×2 (period: pretask/working) ×2 (objects: relevant/irrelevant) repeated-measures multivariate ANOVA showed a significant main effect of group (F1,28 = 17.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.39), task (F1,28 = 11.9, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.30), period (F1,28 = 110.2, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.80) and objects (F1,28 = 244.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.90) on the gaze durations. Moreover, there was a significant interaction between group, task and period (F1,28 = 6.0, p = 0.021, ηp2 = 0.18; Fig. 4).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Gaze durations for patients and controls as a function of the task (familiar/unfamiliar) and on the display model in the unfamiliar task. *p < 0.01, **p < 0.001. IO = irrelevant objects, RO = relevant objects.

Familiar task (sandwich-making)

There was only 1 difference between the groups. Patients exhibited longer gaze durations than controls on irrelevant objects in the pretask period of the familiar task (F1,28 = 8.9, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.24).

In the 2 periods, both groups had longer gaze durations on relevant objects (patients: pretask, relevant v. irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 10.4 p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.27; working, relevant v. irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 107.8, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.79; controls: pretask, relevant v. irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 16.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.38; working, relevant v. irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 119.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.81).

Moreover, when patients and controls accomplished the task, they looked more at relevant objects (patients: relevant objects, pretask v. working, F1,28 = 13.6, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.33; controls: relevant objects, pretask v. working, F1,28 = 12.1, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.30) and less at irrelevant objects (patients: irrelevant objects, pretask v. working, F1,28 = 5.9, p = 0.021, ηp2 = 0.18; controls: irrelevant objects, pretask v. working, F1,28 = 4.4, p = 0.045, ηp2 = 0.14).

Unfamiliar task (model-building)

Patients presented the same scanning pattern as controls in the pretask period of the unfamiliar task. The 2 groups looked at relevant and irrelevant objects equally in this period. There was no significant difference between the groups in the pretask (all F < 1, all p > 0.05).

In the working period, patients had longer gaze durations on relevant objects and on irrelevant objects than controls (relevant objects, F1,28 = 9.9 p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.26; irrelevant objects, F1,28 = 96.9, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.78).

Moreover, patients and controls looked more at relevant objects when they accomplished the task (patients: relevant objects, pretask v. working, F1,28 = 94.6, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.77; controls: relevant objects, pretask v. working, F1,28 = 35.7, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.56), but there was 1 difference between the 2 groups on irrelevant objects. When they accomplished the task, patients looked more at irrelevant objects (pretask v. working, F1,28 = 39.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.58), whereas controls looked less at irrelevant objects during this same period (pre-task v. working, F1,28 = 5.5, p = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.16).

A separate ANOVA revealed that patients had to look more at the display model than controls to accomplish the task correctly (F1,28 = 17.6, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.39; Fig. 4).

Number of errors

If the participant used an irrelevant object, an error was recorded. The 2 groups did not make any mistakes in the familiar task, but patients made significantly more mistakes than controls in the unfamiliar task (patients, mean 3.3 errors; controls, mean 0.5 errors; F1,28 = 27.4, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.49).

“Look-ahead” fixations

During the execution of natural tasks, study participants sometimes fixate objects that are not yet manipulated but will be grasped a few seconds later. Such early looks are known as “look-ahead fixations.”26 They are identified as a lookahead if the participant looks back to the original object before subsequently returning (within 3 s) to manipulate the target of the look-ahead. They are not artifacts of “look-backs,” as participants do not look back at objects once they have finished with them, even if these objects remain in full view. Look-ahead fixations occur on average 3 seconds before the reach, and their frequency is influenced by task sequence, suggesting that they are purposeful and are thought to reflect planning of the next action.27

A 2 (group: patients/controls) ×2 (task: familiar/unfamiliar) repeated-measures multivariate ANOVA showed a significant main effect of group (F1,28 = 6.9, p = 0.013, ηp2 = 0.20) and task (F1,28 = 12.1, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.30) on the percentage of lookahead fixations. There was also a significant interaction between group and task (F1,28 = 4.4, p = 0.044, ηp2 = 0.14).

Patients exhibited fewer look-aheads in the unfamiliar task (F1,28 = 16.1, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.37), but did not differ from controls in the familiar task (F1,28 = 0.03, p = 0.87, ηp2 = 0.01). In the unfamiliar task, patients’ look-aheads accounted for 15.1% of the fixations compared with 21.2% in the control group. In the familiar task, these values were 22.7% for patients and 23.0% for controls.

A contrast analysis for patients and controls showed that patients made fewer look-ahead fixations in the unfamiliar task (F1,28 = 15.6, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.36), whereas there was no significant difference between the 2 tasks for controls (F1,28 = 0.9, p = 0.34, ηp2 = 0.03).

Discussion

Much of the work on fixation patterns in scenes has been performed using 2-D images. However, images differ from the natural world in several ways, including the nature of the task demands, the dimensionality of the display, and the familiarity and the complexity of the tasks. In natural behaviour, fixation patterns are highly task-dependent.19,21,26,2831 Each task has a characteristic and flexible pattern of eye movements that accompanies it, and this pattern is similar among individuals. To investigate the role of these factors in gaze patterns, we monitored eye movements in patients with schizophrenia and healthy participants under a free-viewing condition versus an active-viewing condition with realistic scenes as stimuli and in a familiar task (sandwich-making) versus an unfamiliar task (model-building) condition.

Consistent with previous eye movement studies using images as stimuli,12 our results show that patients with schizophrenia exhibited abnormalities in the free-viewing condition with longer fixation durations and fewer fixations than controls. Patients did not differ from controls in the active-viewing condition in which both groups looked more at relevant objects to accomplish the action, ignoring the distractors. This result can be related to studies on attentional control and cognitive flexibility in patients with schizophrenia.23 Patients are able to normalize their patterns of exploration when they are actively involved in more demanding tasks.32,33

The second part of our work was designed to study the planning deficits in eye movement patterns in patients with schizophrenia. Thus, we have monitored eye movements in both patients and controls under 2 realistic active-viewing conditions (familiar/unfamiliar). Our results show that patients were slower than controls in both tasks. Patients were also significantly slower than controls in the initiation of motor actions (pretask) in both tasks, and the pretask duration was even longer in the more complex unfamiliar task. These results are consistent with those of previous studies showing a general psychomotor slowing in patients with schizophrenia.13 Longer initiation times for more complex tasks suggest increased difficulties in preplanning the entire sequence before its initiation; however, when the task was more difficult, patients in our study did not seem to use more initiation time to memorize and plan the required actions. Similarly, using “grip” actions, Delevoye-Turrell and colleagues34 have shown that patients with schizophrenia are characterized by an abnormal allocation profile: more attention was allocated for execution than for planning. In our study, patients performed relatively worse in the unfamiliar task (i.e., patients made more mistakes than controls in copying the display model).

Problems in the planning of movements in patients with schizophrenia could result from the effects of the complexity and unfamiliarity that are greater in people with schizophrenia. These observations are consistent with those in studies demonstrating that complexity increased planning deficits.13,14,16 A study using the Tower of London task16 mentioned the possibility that patients with schizophrenia may not plan the required sequence of actions fully in advance; this hypothesis was supported by their finding that these patients needed more moves to complete the trials and produced fewer perfect solutions.

In our study, patients presented patterns of exploration quite similar to those of controls in the familiar task, but showed scanning differences in the unfamiliar task. One explanation could be that the sandwich task is a well-learned task — a familiar daily activity involving less demanding cognitive resources. In the unfamiliar task, patients looked more at distractors. It is known that patients with schizophrenia are more sensitive to distraction than healthy controls.23,35 In our study, it might be that a salient feature in the object (e.g., colour, contrast edges) automatically captured the patients’ attention and that they found it difficult to disengage their attention from that feature. Moreover, studies using the anti-saccade paradigm have demonstrated that patients show difficulty inhibiting a reflexive saccade.36 However, our results showed that patients also looked more at task-relevant objects than controls in the execution period. Furthermore, patients made fewer “look-ahead” fixations in the unfamiliar task. This suggests that people with schizophrenia tend to plan their actions less in advance in the case of more complex, unfamiliar tasks. Hayhoe and colleagues19 and Land and colleagues21 have observed that about one-third of reaching and grasping movements were preceded a few seconds earlier by a fixation on the object. In a similar construction task, Mennie and colleagues27 reported a 20% rate of look-aheads, a value similar to that found in our study. Thus, patients seem to establish a less efficient planning strategy than controls.

It has recently been reported that a patient with action disorganization syndrome resulting from lesions in the frontal cortex produced fewer anticipatory fixations than healthy participants in a tea-making task.37 Failure to complete behavioural routines is attributed to degradation of a stored action schema.38 For example, if patients have a degraded stored action schema, their eye movements may be less constrained by the task and more likely to be driven by salient objects in the environment.

Land and colleagues21 have challenged the idea that everyday tasks are automatic in the sense that they normally require no online monitoring or feedback from “higher-level” control systems, such as the Supervisory Attentional System (SAS) of the Norman and Shallice39 control-of-action model. When the action is novel or complex, the SAS is required for selection of a desired response sequence. It is thus involved particularly in tasks demanding initiation, planning, mental set-shifting, strategy allocation, monitoring and inhibition. Consequently, impairment in this system would be expected to result in the inability to formulate a goal, to plan and to choose between alternative sequences of behaviour to reach a particular goal. The results obtained with the Tower of London task strongly suggest that the SAS planning function is impaired in patients with schizophrenia.40 Our patients’ results in the unfamiliar task could be plausibly understood as a failure at the level of the SAS.

Our results are also in line with those of a study showing a motor-planning deficit (i.e., pointing to a target) when the planning involved an internal representation of a stimulus sequence.15 Hayhoe and colleagues41 provided evidence of the existence of sophisticated internal models of the structure of the environment. Cohen and Servan-Schreiber42 have suggested that a disturbance in the internal representation of contextual information might provide a common explanation for deficits in several attention-related tasks in patients with schizophrenia. In our study, this is suggested by the fact that patients needed to look more at the display model than controls did to accomplish the unfamiliar task. Patients had difficulty maintaining a mental representation of the task to perform. Our unfamiliar task probably involved higher working memory requirements, and our results could be explained by working memory deficit. Lee and Park43 have suggested that working memory deficits in patients with schizophrenia are robust and independent of study modality.

Furthermore, Cohen and Servan-Schreiber42 suggest that this behavioural deficit may be explained by a specific biological disturbance (i.e., a reduction in the effects of dopamine [DA] in the prefrontal cortex). Evidence implicates DA dysregulation related to prefrontal dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia.44,45 It is also well known that Parkinson disease is associated with a progressive dysfunction of the dopaminergic neurotransmission in the basal ganglia.46 Deficits in executive functions have been documented in this disease;47 patients have been shown to be impaired in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test,48 the Tower of London test49 and the Stroop test,50 thus suggesting planning deficits. They also exhibit visual scanning deficits, such as longer fixation times on faces.51 Therefore, DA level in the prefrontal cortex might play a critical role in cognitive processes in patients with schizophrenia.

Limitations

Although in our study antipsychotic dosage equivalents did not appear to be correlated to various eye movement variables, we cannot exclude an effect of medication. Kojima and colleagues52 have reported that neither the number of fixations nor scan path length were correlated with the chlorpromazine equivalent dosage in 50 patients with chronic schizophrenia, and this was confirmed by Matsushima and colleagues,53 Streit and colleagues7 and Loughland and colleagues,5 who found no relation between dysfunctional scan paths and medication in patients with schizophrenia, whereas Williams and colleagues54 reported that patients treated with risperidone showed greater attention to salient features, and Reilly and colleagues55 showed that pharmacological treatment might have an effect on eye movement control in saccadic tasks.

Another possible limitation is that we used only 1 trial per condition. Thus, measures of reliability come from average performance over fixations within a task and similarity among participants, as indicated by the standard error measures. However, the magnitude of the reported effects indicates that this is not a major concern.

Conclusion

Overall, patients with schizophrenia seem to present deficient planning but intact motor action in more complex, unfamiliar tasks, which could make higher demands on cognitive processing than the well-learned task. There is some evidence in favour of abnormal connectivity between brain areas in patients with schizophrenia. A future area of research could be to explore whether abnormal connectivity between frontal and parietal regions is associated with problems in the sequencing of planned motor actions.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to all of the patients who accepted to take part in the study and the psychiatrists who gave access to their patients and their staff. The study was funded by a PhD grant to C. Delerue by the region Nord-Pas de Calais and the Lille University Hospital. M. Hayhoe’s participation was supported by National Institutes of Health grant EY-05729.

Footnotes

Competing interests: As above for C. Delerue and M. Hayhoe. Otherwise, none declared.

Contributors: C. Delerue and M. Boucart designed the study. C. Delerue acquired the data. All authors analyzed the data, wrote and reviewed the article, and approved its publication.

References

  • 1.Loughland CM, Williams LM, Gordon E. Schizophrenia and affective disorder show different visual scanning behaviour for faces: A trait versus state-based distinction? Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:338–48. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01356-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Loughland CM, Williams LM, Harris AW. Visual scanpath dysfunction in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia probands: Evidence for a vulnerability marker? Schizophr Res. 2004;67:11–21. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(03)00094-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Benson PJ, Beedie SA, Shephard E, et al. Simple viewing tests can detect eye movement abnormalities that distinguish schizophrenia cases from controls with exceptional accuracy. Biol Psychiatry. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Green MJ, Williams LM, Davidson D. Visual scanpaths to threat related faces in deluded schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2003;119:271–85. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(03)00129-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Loughland CM, Williams LM, Gordon E. Visual scanpaths to positive and negative facial emotions in an outpatient schizophrenia sample. Schizophr Res. 2002;55:159–70. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00186-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Phillips ML, David AS. Visual scan paths are abnormal in deluded schizophrenics. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:99–105. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(96)00061-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Streit M, Wölwer W, Gaebel W. Facial-affect recognition and visual scanning behaviour in the course of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1997;24:311–7. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(96)00126-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Williams LM, Loughland CM, Gordon E, et al. Visual scanpaths in schizophrenia: Is there a deficit in face recognition? Schizophr Res. 1999;40:189–99. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00056-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gaebel W, Ulrich G, Frick K. Visuomotor performance of schizophrenic patients and normal controls in a picture viewing task. Biol Psychiatry. 1987;22:1227–37. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(87)90030-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Obayashi S, Msatsushima E, Ando H, et al. Exploratory eye movements during the benton visual retention test: characteristics of visual behavior in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003;57:409–15. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2003.01140.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Minassian A, Granholm E, Verney S, et al. Visual scanning deficits in schizophrenia and their relationship to executive functioning impairment. Schizophr Res. 2005;74:69–79. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bestelmeyer PE, Tatler BW, Phillips LH, et al. Global visual scanning abnormalities in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res. 2006;87:212–22. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jogems-Kosterman BJ, Zitman FG, Van Hoof JJ, et al. Psychomotor slowing and planning deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2001;48:317–33. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00097-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Grootens KP, Vermeeren L, Verkes RJ, et al. Psychomotor planning is deficient in recent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2009;107:294–302. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Delevoye-Turrell Y, Giersch A, Wing AM, et al. Motor fluency deficits in the sequencing of actions in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116:56–64. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pantelis C, Barnes TR, Nelson HE, et al. Frontal-striatal cognitive deficits in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Brain. 1997;120:1823–43. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.10.1823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Semkovska M, Bédard MA, Godbout L, et al. Assessment of executive dysfunction during activities of daily living in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;69:289–300. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stip E, Sablier J, Sephery AA, et al. Kitchen and schizophrenia: a crossroad activity of an ecological, occupational and neuropsychological assessment. Sante Ment Que. 2007;32:159–79. doi: 10.7202/016514ar. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hayhoe MM, Shrivastava A, Mruczek R, et al. Visual memory and motor planning in a natural task. J Vis. 2003;3:49–63. doi: 10.1167/3.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Johansson RS, Westling G, Bäckström A, et al. Eye-hand coordination in object manipulation. J Neurosci. 2001;21:6917–32. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-17-06917.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Land M, Mennie N, Rusted J. The roles of vision and eye movements in the control of activities of daily living. Perception. 1999;28:1311–28. doi: 10.1068/p2935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ballard DH, Hayhoe MM, Pelz JB. Memory representations in natural tasks. J Cogn Neurosci. 1995;7:66–80. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ducato MG, Michael GA, Thomas P, et al. Attentional capture in schizophrenia: failure to resist interference from motion signals. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2008;13:185–209. doi: 10.1080/13546800701706530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington: The Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kay SR, Opler LA, Fiszbein A. Positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) North Tonawanda (NY): Multi-Health Systems Inc.; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Pelz JB, Canosa R. Oculomotor behavior and perceptual strategies in complex tasks. Vision Res. 2001;41:3587–96. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00245-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mennie N, Hayhoe M, Sullivan B. Look-ahead fixations: anticipatory eye movements in natural tasks. Exp Brain Res. 2007;179:427–42. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0804-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Land MF. Eye movements and the control of actions in everyday life. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2006;25:296–324. doi: 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2006.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Land MF, Lee DN. Where we look when we steer. Nature. 1994;369:742–4. doi: 10.1038/369742a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Land MF, McLeod P. From eye movements to actions: how batsmen hit the ball. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1340–5. doi: 10.1038/81887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Patla AE, Vickers JN. Where and when do we look as we approach and step over an obstacle in the travel path? Neuroreport. 1997;8:3661–5. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199712010-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Delerue C, Laprévote V, Verfaillie K, et al. Gaze control during face exploration in schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett. 2010;482:245–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.07.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Tonoya Y, Matsui M, Kurachi M, et al. Exploratory eye movements in schizophrenia. Effects of figure size and the instruction on visual search. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;252:255–61. doi: 10.1007/s00406-002-0390-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Delevoye-Turrell YN, Thomas P, Giersch A. Attention for movement production: abnormal profiles in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006;84:430–2. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ducato MG, Thomas P, Monestes JL, et al. Attentional capture in schizophrenia and schizotypy: effect of attentional load. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2008;13:89–111. doi: 10.1080/13546800701707371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hutton SB, Ettinger U. The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: a critical review. Psychophysiology. 2006;43:302–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00403.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Forde EM, Rusted J, Mennie N, et al. The eyes have it: an exploration of eye movements in action disorganisation syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1895–900. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Humphreys GW, Forde EME. Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1998;15:771–811. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Norman DA, Shallice T. Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior. In: Davidson RJ, Schwartz GE, Shapiro D, editors. Consciousness and self-regulation Advances in research and theory. Vol. 4. New York: Plenum Press; 1986. pp. 1–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Marczewski P, Van der Linden M, Laroi F. Further investigation of the supervisory attentional system in schizophrenia: planning, inhibition, and rule abstraction. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2001;6:175–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hayhoe M, Mennie N, Sullivan B, et al. The role of internal models and prediction in catching balls. Proceedings of AAAI Fall Symposium Series; Menlo Park (CA). Nov. 4–6, 2005; [accessed 2013 Feb. 25]. Available: www.aaai.org/Library/Symposia/Fall/fs05-05.php. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Cohen JD, Servan-Schreiber D. Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychol Rev. 1992;99:45–77. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lee J, Park S. Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:599–611. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bunney WE, Bunney BG. Evidence for a compromised dorsolateral prefrontal cortical parallel circuit in schizophrenia. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2000;31:138–46. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00031-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Mattay VS, et al. Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:1078–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.11.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Evans AH, Lees AJ. Dopamine dysregulation syndrome in Parkinson’s disease. Curr Opin Neurol. 2004;17:393–8. doi: 10.1097/01.wco.0000137528.23126.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Dubois B, Pillon B. Cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol. 1997;244:2–8. doi: 10.1007/pl00007725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lees AJ, Smith E. Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1983;106:257–70. doi: 10.1093/brain/106.2.257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Owen AM, James M, Leigh PN, et al. Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1992;115:1727–51. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.6.1727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Dubois B, Pillon B, Malapani C, et al. Subcortical dementia and Parkinson’s disease: What are the cognitive functions of the basal ganglia? In: Wolters EC, Scheltens P, editors. Mental dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Current issues in neurodegenerative diseases. The Netherlands: ICG Publications; 1994. pp. 194–210. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Clark US, Neargarder S, Cronin-Golomb A. Visual exploration of emotional expressions in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1901–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kojima T, Matsushima E, Nakajima K, et al. Eye movements in acute, chronic, and remitted schizophrenics. Biol Psychiatry. 1990;27:975–89. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(90)90035-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Matsushima E, Kojima T, Ohbayashi S, et al. Exploratory eye movements in schizophrenic patients and patients with frontal lobe lesions. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1992;241:210–4. doi: 10.1007/BF02190255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Williams LM, Loughland CM, Green MJ, et al. Emotion perception in schizophrenia: an eye movement study comparing the effectiveness of risperidone vs. haloperidol. Psychiatry Res. 2003;120:13–27. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(03)00166-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Reilly JL, Lencer R, Bishop JR, et al. Pharmacological treatment effects on eye movement control. Brain Cogn. 2008;68:415–35. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN are provided here courtesy of Canadian Medical Association

RESOURCES