Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2011 Feb 23;31(8):2965–2973. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4920-10.2011

Altered Neural Responses to Sounds in Primate Primary Auditory Cortex during Slow-Wave Sleep

Elias B Issa 1, Xiaoqin Wang 1,
PMCID: PMC3758555  PMID: 21414918

Abstract

How sounds are processed by the brain during sleep is an important question for understanding how we perceive the sensory environment in this unique behavioral state. While human behavioral data have indicated selective impairments of sound processing during sleep, brain imaging and neurophysiology studies have reported that overall neural activity in auditory cortex during sleep is surprisingly similar to that during wakefulness. This responsiveness to external stimuli leaves open the question of how neural responses during sleep differ, if at all, from wakefulness. Using extracellular neural recordings in the primary auditory cortex of naturally sleeping common marmosets, we show that slow-wave sleep (SWS) alters neural responses in the primate auditory cortex in two specific ways. SWS reduced the sensitivity of auditory cortex such that quiet sounds elicited weak responses in SWS compared with wakefulness, while loud sounds evoked similar responses in SWS and wakefulness. Furthermore, SWS reduced the extent of sound-evoked response suppression. This pattern of alterations was not observed during rapid eye movement sleep and could not be easily explained by the presence of slow rhythms in SWS. The alteration of excitatory and inhibitory responses during SWS suggests limitations in auditory processing and provides novel insights for understanding why certain sounds are processed while others are missed during deep sleep.

Introduction

During sleep, the senses are dulled, yet little is known about underlying auditory cortical function during this state. The loss of conscious awareness in sleep is often compared with the state induced by anesthesia, but this may only be a superficial comparison as evoked potential (Nielsen-Bohlman et al., 1991; Bastuji et al., 2002) and imaging (Portas et al., 2000) studies in humans and, more recently, single neuron recordings in nonhuman primates (Issa and Wang, 2008) have shown a surprising degree of activation in response to sounds in auditory cortex during sleep in a manner that qualitatively resembles that during wakefulness rather than anesthesia. The finding that acoustic information is still reaching auditory cortex during sleep is also inconsistent with the prevailing view that a thalamic gate insulates the cortex from the external sensory environment (Steriade, 2003), as had been suggested by neural recordings across auditory (Edeline et al., 2000), visual (Livingstone and Hubel, 1981), and somatosensory (Mariotti et al., 1989) thalamus during sleep. This raises the possibility of a cortical mechanism such as reduced spiking thresholds or recurrent excitation that increases sensitivity to thalamic inputs. Steriade and colleagues foresaw the possibility for amplification of cortical responses over thalamic responses in their seminal work on slow oscillations (Steriade et al., 1993, 2001). They suggested that “up” (depolarized) periods of slow-brain rhythms could make neurons unusually excitable during slow-wave sleep (SWS) counteracting or even exceeding the effect of “down” (hyperpolarized) periods (Steriade et al., 2001). Such a mechanism has since been shown to modulate sensory responses in SWS (Massimini et al., 2003) and other nonsleep states (Stern et al., 1997; Poulet and Petersen, 2008). However, it remains unclear how sound-evoked activity in auditory cortex would be modified during SWS given, among other factors, the interplay of slow oscillations and reduced feedforward thalamic input (Hennevin et al., 2007).

Here, we measured auditory cortical responses in SWS across a continuum of sound levels to examine how SWS may modify auditory processing across the dynamic range of hearing. While neurons became less responsive to quiet sounds during SWS compared with wakefulness, they exhibited robust responses at loud sound levels. Furthermore, we observed that sound-evoked suppression was limited in SWS. These observations suggest that acoustic information is not faithfully transferred through auditory cortex in SWS and is instead processed with a limited dynamic range of excitation and inhibition.

Materials and Methods

Subjects and surgery.

Neural data were collected from primary auditory cortex (A1) in four hemispheres of three common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) (one male, two female) that were also used in our previous study (Issa and Wang, 2008). Animals were adapted to sit in a primate restraint chair for two weeks before a surgery implanting two stainless steel head posts for fixation of the head. Details of implant surgery can be found in previous studies (Lu et al., 2001; Wang et al., 2005). After surgery, animals were adapted to experiments during the daytime for at least one month before being switched to nighttime sessions for collection of sleep data. Within the first week of sleep recordings, animals adapted to sleeping in the laboratory in an upright, head-fixed position while sounds were playing. All experimental procedures were approved by the Johns Hopkins University Animal Use and Care Committee.

Physiological recordings.

Nightly sessions lasted for 6–8 h (8:00 P.M.–4:00 A.M.). Animals naturally slept in the chair while sounds were played, passing through 5–10 sleep cycles. A 1.1 mm diameter stainless steel screw was implanted in the caudal temporal lobe for measuring the electroencephalogram (EEG). The EEG was bandpass filtered (1 Hz-500 Hz), amplified (10,000×), and displayed on a digital oscilloscope for online monitoring. The EEG and video monitoring (sensitive in the infrared range) of the face and tail were used to assess an animal's behavioral state. Behavioral state was readily identified using standard human sleep scoring techniques (Carskadon and Rechtschaffen, 2000), and details are provided in a previous study (Issa and Wang, 2008). Here, it is important to note that sounds did not awaken the animals even when played at the loudest intensities used in this study. Furthermore, sounds of different intensities were interleaved so that any effect on sleep quality would have affected all sounds equally and been averaged out. Extracellular activity was measured using 2–5 MΩ tungsten microelectrodes (A-M Systems), bandpass filtered (300 Hz-7 kHz), and amplified (10,000×). The electrode signal was also bandpass filtered from 1–300 Hz to obtain the local field potential (LFP). The EEG signal measured from the stainless steel screw was used to assess the behavioral state of the animal, allowing for consistency across sessions. The low frequencies in the LFP signal (recorded from the microelectrode) were used in analyses that compared single neuron activity with ongoing low-frequency rhythms (see Fig. 7). Spike waveforms were sorted online (MSD, Alpha-Omega Engineering) using a template-matching algorithm to identify single units. We regularly obtained well isolated single-units with signal-to-noise ratios >15 dB (median signal-to-noise ratio = 22 dB). Neurons were held for an average of 90 min to allow comparison of neural responses across different states [marmoset sleep cycle ≈ 50 min (Crofts et al., 2001)]. Details of the identification of A1 are given previously (Issa and Wang, 2008).

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

Influence of slow rhythms on neural activity in awake, SWS, and REM. A, An example neuron whose background activity showed EEG state-dependent firing rate modulation (left bars). Trials (n = 35) were divided into quartiles based on EEG amplitude. Trials in the lower quartile (n = 9) and the upper quartile (n = 9) were compared for effects of firing rate modulation with EEG amplitude. During EEG down states, this example neuron's firing rate was reduced by more than half. Upon acoustic stimulation with a 11.9 kHz pure tone (70 dB SPL), there was no longer a difference in total firing rates between up and down EEG states (right bars, spontaneous firing rate not subtracted). Error bars represent ±1 SEM. B, EEG amplitude modulated spiking especially when no sounds were playing in SWS (gray bar, no sound condition). Effects were generally stronger in SWS than in REM (orange) or wakefulness (black). During acoustic stimulation, however, modulation of spike rate by EEG amplitude became much weaker compared with the activity evoked by sounds (compare no sound to sound conditions; SWS: no sound = 68 ± 6% vs sound = 26 ± 2%, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 96) and slow rhythm modulation no longer differed between SWS and wakefulness (sound condition: SWS = 26 + 2% vs awake = 27 + 2%, p = 0.85, Wilcoxon rank sum, nSWS = 96, nAwake = 75). Only neurons with spontaneous rates > 3 spikes s−1 were included in this analysis so that appreciable variability occurred in trial firing rates. Error bars represent ±1 SEM. C, Locking of spontaneous activity to low-frequency EEG rhythms was enhanced in SWS compared with wakefulness (solid gray and black lines). This cannot be simply explained by the stronger power in the low-frequency EEG in SWS since the spike-field coherence measure normalizes for differences in EEG power across the different behavioral states (see Materials and Methods). In general, locking to low-frequency rhythms was poor while sounds were being played (dashed lines). Error bars represent ±1 SEM. D, High (top quartile of EEG amplitudes) and low (bottom quartile) trials could cause a 75 ± 9% swing in firing rates as seen in the overall modulation in spontaneous firing rate between up and down periods (black bar). This modulation with EEG amplitude, however, was reduced to 27 + 3% and 23 + 3% during playing of quiet (green, 0–20 dB) and loud (blue, 70–90 dB) sounds respectively (p(no sound vs quiet sound) = 1.4*10−5, p(no sound vs loud sound) = 5.5*10−6, Wilcoxon rank sum, nno sound = 51, nquiet = 25, nloud = 40). Only neurons with spontaneous rates > 3 spikes s−1 were used in this analysis. Otherwise, if firing rates were too low, little trial-by-trial variation could be observed. Error bars represent ±1 SEM. E, The SFC was strongest for low frequencies indicating that spikes were locked to slow brain rhythms. This locking was strongly diminished whether quiet (0–20 dB) or loud (70–90 dB) sounds were played (SFC at 1 Hz: no sound = 4.3*10−3 + 0.4*10−3 vs quiet sound = 2.1*10−3 + 0.3*10−3 mV2 vs loud sound = 1.8*10−3 + 0.4*10−3 mV2, p(no sound vs quiet sound) < 0.01, p(no sound vs loud sound) < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 169). Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

Acoustic stimuli.

Pure tones or narrowband noise were used to drive neurons in A1. In a minority of neurons (5%), sinusoidal amplitude modulation (2–128 Hz) was used to achieve more robust drive. The sound level of a preferred stimulus was varied between −10 and 90 dB sound pressure level (SPL) (although usually in the range of 0–70 dB SPL) to obtain the intensity-tuning curve or rate-level function. Many repeats were generally collected in awake and SWS (median awake = 30, SWS = 20 repetitions; minimum required for inclusion in analysis = 10 repetitions). Stimuli were usually 200 ms in duration and in some cases 500 ms long. To limit sleep disruption, presentation rates were generally kept low (1–2 stimuli per second). In analyses examining suppressed responses, we used our complete database of neurons (not just neurons where a rate level was collected). For these data, a larger battery of stimuli was tested as described in our previous work (Issa and Wang, 2008). Experiments were performed in a double-walled sound isolation chamber (Industrial Acoustics) lined with 3 inch acoustic absorption foam (Sonex) to create a soundproof environment. All stimuli were delivered in free-field from a speaker (B&W) located 0.9 m directly in front of the animal. The system was calibrated to produce 90 dB SPL peak output at 1 kHz with little roll-off between 0.2 and 34 kHz.

Data analysis.

For sound level tuning, a fixed analysis window starting 15 ms after stimulus onset and ending 75 ms after stimulus offset was used. Spontaneous firing rates were subtracted from total firing rates such that in all subsequent analyses the terms “rate” or “response” refer to a spontaneous rate-adjusted measure. Responses were considered driven if the mean was >2.5*SEM above 0 (where SEM was estimated individually for each stimulus). Any rate-level curves with no significant response bins in both awake and SWS were not considered further. Sound level threshold was the quietest sound level eliciting a response >4*SEM above 0. In cases where responses did not reach >4*SEM above 0, a threshold of 90 dB SPL was assigned to the nonresponsive state (Fig. 2B, denoted as NR). Response latencies were computed using a change point estimation algorithm adapted from Friedman and Priebe (1998). Briefly, two lines of differing slopes were fit to the knee preceding the peak of the response. Latency was defined as the time point at which the slope change was optimal (in the least-squares fit sense). Latencies are only reported for significantly driven responses (>2.5*SEM above 0).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Acoustic thresholds and latencies during SWS. A, Population intensity tuning data in awake (solid) and SWS (dashed). Error bars represent ± 0.5 SEM. Individual tuning curves for each neuron were first normalized by their peak value in either awake or SWS before being averaged. B, Elevated sound level thresholds in SWS compared with wakefulness (p = 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 169; NR, not responsive according to a 4*SEM criterion and assigned a threshold of 90 dB SPL). C, Response latencies in SWS (open bars) were similar to those in wakefulness (filled bars) across all sound levels tested (median latency awake = 34.5 + 1.9 vs SWS = 34.0 + 1.7 ms, p = 0.71, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 244, “all” condition).

To determine analysis windows for suppressed responses, we adapted an algorithm that we had used previously for detecting driven responses [Issa and Wang (2008); based on the study by Legéndy and Salcman (1985)]. The algorithm starts with a 100 ms seed window centered at a local minimum that grows in either the left or right direction in 50 ms segments. Window segments are added as long as their responses are below baseline (psoft < 0.02), and the response in the whole window meets a more stringent hard criterion (phard < 0.00005). We also allowed the algorithm to continue as long as the p value decreased toward the hard criterion since p values rarely started below the hard criterion when detecting suppressed events. We assumed Poisson statistics to compute p values from spike counts since low firing rates resulted in non-Gaussian distributions. Even so, responses often deviated from our Poisson assumption because of an unusual number of 0 spike trials, so we performed a post hoc permutation test for the means at the p < 0.01 level (Rice, 1995). Although the excitatory and inhibitory algorithms were run independently, they usually yielded non-overlapping events (see examples in Fig. 3A–C), which is critical to the assumption that these events represent different processes.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Detection of suppressed events and dependence on spontaneous rate. A, Well defined periods of excitation (Exc; red) and inhibition (Inh; blue) were detected among a background of high spontaneous firing by the windowing algorithm (see Materials and Methods). No false windows were detected in the 9-s-long period of spontaneous firing following the stimulus. B, The varying durations of excitatory responses were well captured by the algorithm. At higher sound levels, excitation was shorter in duration and was followed by inhibition. In some cases, an inhibitory window was detected by the algorithm but then thrown out (dark gray) because it did not pass a post hoc permutation test for the means when compared with spontaneous firing. C, An example where inhibition preceded excitation at high sound levels. Note that windows were non-overlapping even though excitatory and inhibitory algorithms were run independently. D, Suppressed rates which were already spontaneous-rate subtracted were still highly correlated with spontaneous rates (r = 0.45, p < 0.01, Pearson's correlation coefficient, n = 566), suggesting a scaling relationship between spontaneous rate and suppression. At higher spontaneous rates, higher suppression can be observed extracellularly. This was not a ceiling effect where spontaneous rates limited the amount of suppression that could be observed extracellularly since in most cases firing rates were not suppressed to 0 (i.e., suppression = spontaneous; unity line). E, For the analyses of Figures 4B and 6B in Results, suppressed rates were normalized by spontaneous rate to obtain the fraction of spontaneous rate that was suppressed. This measure removed correlation with spontaneous rate (r = 0.002, p = 0.26, Pearson's correlation coefficient, n = 566).

We computed the percentage of modulation of driven responses in sleep for each stimulus by comparing firing rates from two different behavioral states according to the following formulas:

graphic file with name zns00811-9570-m01.jpg
graphic file with name zns00811-9570-m02.jpg

where rSWS, rREM, and rAwake are the discharge rates during the SWS, rapid eye movement (REM), and awake conditions, respectively.

For suppressed responses, we computed the percentage of modulation in sleep using an additional measure:

graphic file with name zns00811-9570-m03.jpg
graphic file with name zns00811-9570-m04.jpg

where rSWS, rREM, and rAwake are the discharge rates during suppressed responses, and sSWS, sREM, and sAwake are the spontaneous discharge rates during the SWS, REM, and awake conditions, respectively. The ratios in the expressions measure the fraction of spontaneous firing that was suppressed. We used this relative measure to account for the linear dependence of suppression on the baseline firing rate (see Fig. 3D). Normalizing by baseline firing rates removed the scaling relationship between the amount of suppression observed and spontaneous rates (see Fig. 3E).

In general, we report gains for each unit by averaging gains across stimuli. For example, if the unit was responsive to more than one stimulus (e.g., responded to more than one sound level in the quiet range or was suppressed by multiple stimuli), we averaged its gains across these stimuli to obtain an overall unit gain under each condition. The main findings did not change if individual stimuli were used in the analyses instead. If anything, this often increased the statistical power of the results.

For each stimulus trial, the mean EEG amplitude was computed during the stimulus (“Sound” condition) or in the prestimulus period (“No sound” condition). Trials where EEG amplitudes were in the upper quartile were considered up trials while trials where EEG amplitudes were in the lowest quartile were considered down trials (the remaining half of trials were not considered). The modulation of spiking by up versus down states was quantified as |ratehigh − ratelow|/rateall where ratehigh (ratelow) is the mean firing rate in the upper (lower) quartiles of EEG amplitude and rateall is the mean firing rate across all trials. This trial-based analysis does not depend on the precise timing of spikes and is not based on EEG frequency but simply the mean EEG amplitude in a small window (200–500 ms) either before or during acoustic stimulation. If slow rhythms controlled all variation in spiking, then this measure would approach 1. If slow rhythms had no influence (i.e., little difference in firing rate between low and high EEG epochs), then this measure would approach 0.

The spike-field coherence (SFC) is a measure of the degree of synchrony between spikes and field potentials in different frequency bands (Fries et al., 2001). The SFC requires the spike-triggered average of the LFPs (STA), which is obtained by aligning and averaging the LFP preceding each spike. A shift predictor was computed by randomly permuting LFP trials and recomputing the STA 10 times. This randomization is important because it removes any stimulus-induced correlations in spikes and LFP activity. Often, in the first 100–200 ms following stimulation, the increase in spike rate and LFP power can artificially induce correlations. Only neurons with STAs that reached absolute values > 2.5*SD above 0 after shift predictor subtraction were considered further for SFC analysis. The raw SFC is simply the power spectrum of the STA. To distinguish a change in coherence from a change in the strength of oscillations in the LFP, the raw SFC was normalized by the mean spectrum of all LFPs used in calculating the STA. This normalization step is important since the LFP during SWS is higher in amplitude than during awake and REM.

Results

Sound level dependence of neural responses in slow-wave sleep

Sound level is an important parameter for determining when a sound becomes salient during sleep. We measured the rate-level functions of 169 neurons in A1 of three naturally sleeping marmoset monkeys during at least one episode of SWS and one episode of wakefulness. A range of sound levels was tested (usually 0–70 dB SPL) without awakening the animal. A representative neuron is shown in Figure 1A. In the awake condition, this neuron began to respond to a 780 Hz pure tone at 20 dB SPL, but its response to the same tone disappeared at 20 and 30 dB SPL during SWS (Fig. 1A, middle plot, black arrows), even though the responses at louder sound levels were preserved.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Selective loss of responses to quiet sounds during SWS. A, Example neuron whose response to quiet sounds (left, green highlight) disappeared in SWS (black arrows, middle) even though a robust response to loud sounds remained. This neuron responded to both quiet and loud sounds because of its non-monotonic tuning (right) that is often observed in A1. Stimulus was a 780 Hz pure tone. Error bars represent ±1 SEM. B, Across the population of recorded neurons, SWS modulation of responses was not different from zero for 70–90 dB sounds (median = 2%, black inverted triangle). But responses were negatively modulated during SWS at the low end (0–20 dB) of their sound level tuning curve (median = −32%, green inverted triangle; *p < 0.01). Inset, Overall mean firing rates awake (filled bars) and SWS (open bars) to quiet (left) and loud (right) sounds in A1. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

In our sample of 169 A1 neurons, 69 neurons were responsive (>2.5*SEM above baseline) to quiet sounds (0–20 dB) and 106 neurons were responsive to loud sounds (70–90 dB). Firing rates during wakefulness were similar in these sound level regimes (mean firing rates quiet = 13.5 ± 1.1 vs loud = 14.9 ± 1.3 spikes s−1, p = 0.78, Wilcoxon rank sum, nquiet = 69, nloud = 106), consistent with the non-monotonic sound level tuning commonly seen in A1 of unanesthetized animals (Brugge and Merzenich, 1973; Pfingst and O'Connor, 1981; Sadagopan and Wang, 2008). During SWS, however, the population of neurons showed a large drop in activity for quiet sounds (gain = −33 ± 6%) but not for loud sounds (gain = 9 ± 5%) (p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, nquiet = 69, nloud = 106) (Fig. 1B). The gain distribution for quiet sounds was shifted toward negative gains (p < 0.01, t test, n = 69) implying that responses during SWS were significantly weaker than those during wakefulness (Fig. 1B, green curve). For louder sound levels, however, the gain distribution was not significantly shifted from zero (p = 0.08, t test, n = 106), indicating similar responses between wakefulness and SWS (Fig. 1B, black curve). As a result, overall firing rates for quiet sounds in SWS were ∼35% weaker compared with wakefulness (0–20 dB SPL: awake = 10.7 ± 1.1 vs SWS = 7.0 ± 0.9 spikes s−1, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 69) (Fig. 1B, inset, left bars) while firing rates were not significantly different between the awake and SWS conditions for loud sounds (p = 0.47, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 106) (Fig. 1B, inset, right bars). This phenomenon is further illustrated by the population average sound level function (Fig. 2A), which shows that the average awake response is greater than the average SWS response at low sound levels. This trend was reversed at high sound levels. The decrease in sensitivity to quiet sounds resulted in elevated acoustic thresholds during SWS (mean shift = 8 ± 2 dB, p = 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 169) (Fig. 2B). One plausible mechanism for elevating acoustic thresholds in SWS is elevated spiking thresholds. However, as mentioned above, firing rates for quiet sounds were similar to those for loud sounds suggesting that these stimuli were equally above neural firing thresholds. Second, we examined response latencies as a possible correlate of spiking thresholds and found that response latencies were almost identical in wakefulness and SWS regardless of sound level (p = 0.71, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 244 stimuli) (Fig. 2C). These results suggest that a change in sound-response threshold (i.e., the lack of responsiveness to quiet sounds in SWS) may not be directly related to spiking threshold (i.e., the relationship between spiking and membrane potential).

Suppressed responses during acoustic stimulation

An important property in sharpening neural tuning (Wu et al., 2008; Sadagopan and Wang, 2010) and expanding dynamic range (Wu et al., 2006; Tan et al., 2007) is inhibition. Here, we isolated events where firing rate was suppressed below spontaneous firing rate (see Materials and Methods) as a possible window into inhibitory processes (Fig. 3A–C). In general, detecting inhibition extracellularly depended on background firing rate such that neurons with higher background firing rates exhibited stronger suppression (Fig. 3D). We normalized for this dependency by rescaling firing rates (i.e., fractional suppression = suppressed rate/spontaneous rate) (Fig. 3E). This normalized measure allowed comparison of inhibition across behavioral states independent of changes in baseline firing rate.

Figure 4A shows the time course of suppressed responses in an example neuron during the awake and SWS conditions. The neuron was more suppressed during wakefulness (∼24%) than during SWS. Over the population of neurons in A1 showing a suppressed response, the average gain of suppressed events was −23 ± 10% (negative gain implies that suppression was stronger in wakefulness compared with SWS) (Fig. 4B, white inverted triangle) and approached significance (p = 0.03, t test, n = 96). This resulted in a 40% change in population averaged firing rate during suppressed responses (median firing rate below baseline: awake = −6.5 ± 0.4 vs SWS = −3.9 ± 0.4 spikes s−1, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 283) (Fig. 4B, inset). Changes in spontaneous firing were not likely causing this effect as spontaneous firing rates did not significantly differ between the awake and SWS conditions (mean spontaneous rate: awake = 9.9 ± 0.9 vs SWS = 9.7 ± 0.9 spikes s−1, p = 0.89, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 96). If we used absolute firing rates (not normalized by spontaneous rate), we obtained similar results (gain = −22 ± 8%, p = 0.006, t test, n = 96). Also, modulation of spontaneous rates was generally smaller than the magnitude of change in suppressed rates (mean |Δsuppressed| = 7.2 ± 0.7 vs |Δspontaneous| = 4.4 ± 0.5 spikes s−1, p < 0.01, n = 96).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Suppressed responses during wakefulness and SWS. A, An example neuron whose response was suppressed during presentation of a 20 kHz pure tone (70 dB SPL) and for a short time following (vertical lines represent analysis window returned by windowing algorithm, see Materials and Methods). Suppression was weaker and shorter-lasting in SWS (dashed) than in awake (solid) (gain = −24%). Curves were generated by first subtracting spontaneous rates and then smoothing with a 50 ms moving average window. B, Percentage change of the strength of suppression in SWS. The distribution is shifted toward negative gains (mean = −23%, white inverted triangle) indicating that responses were not suppressed as strongly in SWS as in wakefulness. Inset, Population-averaged firing during suppression events did not go as far below baseline firing (0 in this figure) during SWS as during wakefulness.

From the data presented in Figures 1 and 4, SWS modulation of neural activity was in the form of a loss of driven responses above spontaneous activity and a loss of suppression below spontaneous activity. Driven and suppressed activity levels are summarized for the population as a function of sound level in Figure 5A. The decrease in driven responses (red bars) and suppressed responses (blue bars) limited the range of possible responses in SWS by ∼40%. This suggests that there is a smaller dynamic range of responses during SWS compared with wakefulness, which may place constraints on the amount of information that can be transferred across sound levels by A1 neurons. This limitation in dynamic range was present throughout the time course of the population response, especially during the sustained portion of the response (Fig. 5B).

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Population-averaged activity during wakefulness and SWS. A, Changes in population-averaged driven and suppressed responses as a function of sound level. Suppressed responses were weakened in SWS across a wide range of sound levels (negative blue bars). However, driven responses were weakened only at extremely quiet sound levels (negative red bars). Population-averaged activity was obtained by summing firing rates of all detected driven or suppressed events at each sound level without normalizing and then computing the percentage gain between awake and SWS. Error bars represent ±1 SEM. B, Population-averaged histogram of all driven (positive) and suppressed (negative) responses showing their time course. Because SWS responses (light gray) were not driven or suppressed as strongly as in awake (dark gray), the dynamic range of SWS was limited (light shaded area is encompassed by dark area). Population poststimulus time histograms were obtained by averaging all detected driven (n = 3133) and suppressed events (n = 272) during the time window in which they occurred without normalizing. Responses were smoothed by a 30 ms moving average window.

Neural responses during rapid eye movement sleep

The changes observed in A1 during SWS were not as prominent in REM sleep. As shown in Figure 6A, sleep-wakefulness gains for quiet sounds were only weakly negative (REM response slightly weaker than awake response) (mean gain0-20 = −4 + 6%) and were similar to gains for loud sounds (mean gain70–90 = −13 + 5%, p = 0.58, Wilcoxon rank sum, n0-20 = 67, n70-90 = 90). Overall, mean firing rates for quiet sounds were slightly but not significantly reduced (p = 0.35, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 67) (Fig. 6A, inset, left bars). Furthermore, neurons in REM did not show a loss of suppressed responses compared with wakefulness (mean gain = −0.3 ± 7.1%, p = 0.96, t test, n = 99) (Fig. 6B and inset).

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Driven and suppressed responses in REM. A, For A1 neurons that responded in either the quiet (0–20 dB) or loud (70–90 dB) sound level regimes, little difference was seen in REM modulation of responses (median gain quiet = −1.3% vs loud = −5.3%, p = 0.28, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 63). No clear difference was apparent in population firing rates for quiet sounds (awake = 12.0 + 1.3 vs REM = 10.7 + 1.2 spikes s−1, p = 0.35, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 67). Inset, Black, awake; orange, REM). B, Percentage change of suppressed responses during REM in A1. The distribution is near 0 (mean = −0.3%, inverted white triangle), indicating that the strength of suppression was similar in awake and REM, which can be seen in mean population suppression (inset).

Influence of slow rhythms on neural responses to sounds during sleep

Ongoing slow brain rhythms (<5 Hz) are known to modulate neural firing in awake, anesthetized, and asleep states (Steriade et al., 1993, 2001; Le Van Quyen et al., 2010), and there is evidence that this coupling extends to the theta range (4–8 Hz) (Canolty et al., 2006), but it is unclear how this modulation of neural firing in auditory cortex would be affected by the presence of acoustic stimuli during SWS. In one extreme scenario, neural activity would be dominated by EEG up and down states. At another extreme, external sounds may override slow rhythms. An intermediate possibility is that slow rhythms modulate existing responses to external sounds (e.g., multiplicative gain). To address this issue, we analyzed the difference in firing rates between EEG up states and down states (see Materials and Methods). In the example neuron shown in Figure 7A, the background firing rate (i.e., no external stimulation) increased during up states (depolarized EEG) in SWS and decreased during down states (hyperpolarized EEG) in SWS (up = 24.4 ± 10.3 vs down = 10.6 ± 3.7 spikes s−1, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 9) (Fig. 7A, left bars). This modulation with EEG amplitude, however, was absent during acoustic stimulation in this example neuron; firing rates were no longer significantly different between the two conditions (up = 60.1 ± 7.3 vs down = 63.4 ± 4.8, p = 0.53, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 9; spontaneous firing rate not subtracted) (Fig. 7A, right bars). In our population of neurons, modulation by slow rhythms during acoustic stimulation was weaker than during spontaneous activity in all behavioral states (awake: no sound = 53 ± 6% vs sound = 27 ± 2%, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 75; SWS: no sound = 68 ± 6% vs sound = 26 ± 2%, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 96; REM: no sound = 32 ± 4% vs sound = 21 ± 2%, p = 0.10, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 78) (Fig. 7B, no sound vs sound condition). Note that if the effects of slow rhythms were purely multiplicative, then fractional modulation of firing rates should have been similar between spontaneous and acoustically driven periods of activity. Rather, EEG modulation of responses in SWS was greatly reduced during acoustic stimulation and became similar to levels measured in wakefulness [p = 0.85, Wilcoxon rank sum, nSWS = 96, nAwake = 75) (Fig. 7B, sound condition; SWS (gray) vs awake (black)].

Although slow rhythms had a weak influence on spike rate during acoustic stimulation, they could influence spike timing independently of changes in spike rate. For example, the same number of spikes could become more tightly locked to low-frequency rhythms during SWS. We used the SFC to measure locking of spikes to the EEG (Fries et al., 2001). We found that locking to slow rhythms was most enhanced in SWS as compared with wakefulness (SFC at 1 Hz: awake = 3.4*10−3 ± 0.3*10−3 vs SWS = 4.4*10−3 ± 0.3*10−3 mV2, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 258) and REM (SFC at 1 Hz: REM = 3.7*10−3 ± 0.4*10−3 vs SWS = 4.4*10−3 ± 0.3*10−3 mV2, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 258) (Fig. 7C, solid lines). This strong coherence during SWS, however, declined dramatically with acoustic stimulation (SFC at 1 Hz SWS: spontaneous = 4.4*10−3 ± 0.3*10−3 vs acoustically driven = 1.1*10−3 ± 0.2*10−3 mV2, p < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 258) (Fig. 7C, compare solid to dashed gray line). It is important to note that the SFC is corrected for any locking induced by the stimulus and for differences in overall EEG power between states (see Materials and Methods).

The results in Figure 7, B and C, suggest a limited role of slow rhythms in influencing neural responses during acoustic stimulation. Of interest to this study, however, is whether the influence of slow rhythms differed between quiet and loud sound levels. A differential effect could help explain our finding that only responses to quiet sounds were weakened in SWS (Fig. 1). We found, however, that the influence of slow rhythms was similar for quiet and loud sounds whether in modulating spike rate (p = 0.76, Wilcoxon rank sum, nquiet = 25, nloud = 40) (Fig. 7D) or spike timing (p = 0.20, Wilcoxon rank sum, n = 169) (Fig. 7E). In summary, slow rhythms had a much weaker effect on externally driven activity than on spontaneous activity, and these effects were just as weak for quiet and loud sounds. Therefore, slow rhythms are unlikely to account for the differential loss of responses to quiet sounds in SWS.

Discussion

In the present study, we have demonstrated how sleep alters cortical responses to sounds compared with wakefulness. These alterations, however, were not revealed by all sounds but were specific to quiet sounds and sounds that suppressed neural responses. Previous work did not reveal these effects of SWS because sounds were typically played at moderate to high sound levels intended to drive neurons in an above threshold regime, and suppressed responses were not analyzed (Peña et al., 1999; Edeline et al., 2001). It remains to be seen what possible mechanisms may account for the decreased sensitivity of neurons during SWS. Ideally, this would require detailed intracellular recordings that are difficult to accomplish across sleep–wake transitions. Alternatively, principled manipulation of more complex stimuli and further characterization of neural receptive fields during SWS could provide insights into how neurons are modulated. Our results suggest, however, a limited role for slow rhythms across sound level. The weakened influence of up and down states during quiet levels of acoustic stimulation <30 dB SPL suggests that other mechanisms are involved in addition to slow oscillations and that sleep is a complex interplay of many factors at both the cellular and network levels.

Possible mechanisms

A simple mechanism for increasing sound-response thresholds would be to increase neural spiking thresholds (i.e., the relationship between membrane potential and spiking). However, we have found that response gains between wakefulness and SWS are similar across a range of firing rates, even near threshold (Issa and Wang, 2008). And in the present study, we report that response latencies in SWS are as short as those during wakefulness. These extracellular data cannot rule out changes in intracellular thresholds during SWS but suggest that the relationship between spiking thresholds and acoustic thresholds may not be straightforward.

Two salient properties of SWS are a reduced thalamic drive (Livingstone et al., 1981; Mariotti et al., 1989; Edeline et al., 2000) and prominent low-frequency oscillations (Steriade et al., 1993, 2001; Le Van Quyen et al., 2010). We did indeed observe a role of slow rhythms in modulating background firing. This modulation was not as strong, however, as modulation by external stimuli. This is not surprising especially since we chose stimuli intended to drive neurons well. We note here that our data only extended down to 1 Hz, whereas slow oscillations, as originally described, are in the 0.1–1 Hz range (Steriade et al., 1993), so it is still possible that EEG influences may be found in this low range. But behavior in the 1–5 Hz range can be an indicator of the influences of up and down periods as the SFC in this range was much stronger in SWS than in awake and REM (Fig. 7C), suggesting the involvement of a broadly defined low-frequency range in SWS (Stern et al., 1997; Destexhe et al., 1999; Lampl et al., 1999). Little data exist on the relative influence of slow oscillations and external stimuli on neural responses during natural sleep. One study in sleeping humans reported clear changes in somatosensory evoked potentials with the phase of slow oscillations (Massimini et al., 2003), but further studies will be needed especially at the neural level in auditory cortex to reveal the underlying dynamics.

Given that thalamic responses decrease during SWS, it seems plausible that this would lead to less bottom-up excitation and inhibition in cortex. This view, simple as it may be, explains our dual observations of weakened drive and suppression. If the change in excitatory and inhibitory processes is balanced, then there would be no net change in overall firing rates. This is consistent with the observation that overall activity is preserved from wakefulness to SWS in cortex (Issa and Wang, 2008). The properties that we observed in cortex could be directly inherited from thalamus, but a study in the auditory thalamus comparing rate-level and frequency tuning between SWS and wakefulness found uniform gains (consistently depressed thalamic responses in SWS) across sound level and frequency (Edeline et al., 2000), suggesting that our observations in cortex are unique to the thalamocortical transformation and not to earlier stages of processing.

A key prediction from our data is that inhibitory processes are weakened during SWS. This hypothesis would be easy to test either directly through intracellular recordings or indirectly by observing signatures of inhibition, such as its noted presence at high sound levels (Wu et al., 2006; Tan et al., 2007; Sadagopan and Wang, 2010) or its influence on correlated network activity (Wang, 2010). We did not test sound levels beyond 70 dB SPL except in a few cases to limit awakening the animals. But our data suggest a weak trend for SWS responses to overtake awake responses had we tested further >70 dB SPL. In this regime, inhibition is thought to play an increasing role.

Functional implications

It is difficult to speculate how neural responses in auditory cortex will translate into perception of sounds during sleep, but our data provide constraints on the processing possible during SWS and complement human behavioral and functional imaging work. The elevated acoustic thresholds that we observed at the neural level are consistent with elevated human arousal thresholds during SWS (Bonnet, 1982). Furthermore, preserved overall activity levels during SWS may still allow for residual discrimination of salient or behaviorally important acoustic events as observed in sleeping humans who will respond to their name being called (Oswald et al., 1960; Bastuji et al., 2002) or eventually awaken to the sound of an alarm clock.

Functional imaging in asleep and lightly sedated patients has suggested that auditory cortex is active but that processing in higher level areas may be compromised (Davis et al., 2007). Evoked potential studies have also found changes in SWS for higher level processing (Cote, 2002). It is possible that deficits in coding fidelity in primary auditory cortex during SWS may lead to deteriorated read-out in higher areas and limited discrimination at the behavioral level. One prediction of our data is that neurons should not be able to respond as dynamically to time-varying features of sounds since the range for analog coding with firing rate is reduced during SWS (Fig. 5). This is speculative and only highlights the importance of recording from higher areas during sleep to bridge to perception (Issa and Wang, 2008). Future work along these lines will help elucidate the mechanisms of auditory processing during sleep and provide neural insight into how most sounds escape our conscious awareness during sleep.

Footnotes

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant DC-03180 (X.W.) and a Whitaker Fellowship (E.B.I.). We thank A. Pistorio, E. Bartlett, D. Bendor, P. Crum, and Y. Zhou for help with animal care; S. Sadagopan for helpful discussion; and Y. Zhou, J. Issa, S. Sadagopan, and C. Miller for comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

References

  1. Bastuji H, Perrin F, Garcia-Larrea L. Semantic analysis of auditory input during sleep: studies with event related potentials. Int J Psychophysiol. 2002;46:243–255. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(02)00116-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bonnet MH. Performance during sleep. In: Webb WB, editor. Biological rhythms, sleep and performance. Chichester, UK: Wiley; 1982. pp. 205–237. [Google Scholar]
  3. Brugge JF, Merzenich MM. Responses of neurons in auditory cortex of the macaque monkey to monaural and binaural stimulation. J Neurophysiol. 1973;36:1138–1158. doi: 10.1152/jn.1973.36.6.1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, Kirsch HE, Berger MS, Barbaro NM, Knight RT. High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science. 2006;313:1626–1628. doi: 10.1126/science.1128115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Carskadon MA, Rechtschaffen A. Monitoring and staging human sleep. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and practices of sleep medicine. Ed 3. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders; 2000. pp. 1197–1215. [Google Scholar]
  6. Cote KA. Probing awareness during sleep with the auditory odd-ball paradigm. Int J Psychophysiol. 2002;46:227–241. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(02)00114-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Crofts HS, Wilson S, Muggleton NG, Nutt DJ, Scott EA, Pearce PC. Investigation of the sleep electrocorticogram of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) using radiotelemetry. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112:2265–2273. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00699-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Davis MH, Coleman MR, Absalom AR, Rodd JM, Johnsrude IS, Matta BF, Owen AM, Menon DK. Dissociating speech perception and comprehension at reduced levels of awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:16032–16037. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701309104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Destexhe A, Contreras D, Steriade M. Spatiotemporal analysis of local field potentials and unit discharges in cat cerebral cortex during natural wake and sleep states. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4595–4608. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04595.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Edeline JM, Manunta Y, Hennevin E. Auditory thalamus neurons during sleep: changes in frequency selectivity, threshold, and receptive field size. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:934–952. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.2.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Edeline JM, Dutrieux G, Manunta Y, Hennevin E. Diversity of receptive field changes in auditory cortex during natural sleep. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;14:1865–1880. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01821.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Friedman HS, Priebe CE. Estimating stimulus response latency. J Neurosci Methods. 1998;83:185–194. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00075-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science. 2001;291:1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.1055465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hennevin E, Huetz C, Edeline JM. Neural representations during sleep: from sensory processing to memory traces. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;87:416–440. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Issa EB, Wang X. Sensory responses during sleep in primate primary and secondary auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:14467–14480. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Lampl I, Reichova I, Ferster D. Synchronous membrane potential fluctuations in neurons of the cat visual cortex. Neuron. 1999;22:361–374. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81096-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Le Van Quyen M, Staba R, Bragin A, Dickson C, Valderrama M, Fried I, Engel J. Large-scale microelectrode recordings of high-frequency gamma oscillations in human cortex during sleep. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7770–7782. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5049-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Legéndy CR, Salcman M. Bursts and recurrences of bursts in the spike trains of spontaneously active striate cortex neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1985;53:926–939. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.4.926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Livingstone MS, Hubel DH. Effects of sleep and arousal on the processing of visual information in the cat. Nature. 1981;291:554–561. doi: 10.1038/291554a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lu T, Liang L, Wang X. Temporal and rate representations of time-varying signals in the auditory cortex of awake primates. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:1131–1138. doi: 10.1038/nn737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Mariotti M, Formenti A, Mancia M. Responses of VPL thalamic neurones to peripheral stimulation in wakefulness and sleep. Neurosci Lett. 1989;102:70–75. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(89)90309-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Massimini M, Rosanova M, Mariotti M. EEG slow (approximately 1 Hz) waves are associated with nonstationarity of thalamo-cortical sensory processing in the sleeping human. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89:1205–1213. doi: 10.1152/jn.00373.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Nielsen-Bohlman L, Knight RT, Woods DL, Woodward K. Differential auditory processing continues during sleep. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1991;79:281–290. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90124-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Oswald I, Taylor AM, Treisman M. Discriminative responses to stimulation during human sleep. Brain. 1960;83:440–453. doi: 10.1093/brain/83.3.440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Peña JL, Pérez-Perera L, Bouvier M, Velluti RA. Sleep and wakefulness modulation of the neuronal firing in the auditory cortex of the guinea pig. Brain Res. 1999;816:463–470. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)01194-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Pfingst BE, O'Connor TA. Characteristics of neurons in auditory cortex of monkeys performing a simple auditory task. J Neurophysiol. 1981;45:16–34. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.45.1.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Portas CM, Krakow K, Allen P, Josephs O, Armony JL, Frith CD. Auditory processing across the sleep-wake cycle: simultaneous EEG and fMRI monitoring in humans. Neuron. 2000;28:991–999. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00169-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Poulet JF, Petersen CC. Internal brain state regulates membrane potential synchrony in barrel cortex of behaving mice. Nature. 2008;454:881–885. doi: 10.1038/nature07150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Rice JA. Mathematical statistics and data analysis. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  30. Sadagopan S, Wang X. Level invariant representation of sounds by populations of neurons in primary auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:3415–3426. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2743-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Sadagopan S, Wang X. Contribution of inhibition to stimulus selectivity in primary auditory cortex of awake primates. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7314–7325. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5072-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Steriade M. The corticothalamic system in sleep. Front Biosci. 2003;8:d878–99. doi: 10.2741/1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Steriade M, Nuñez A, Amzica F. A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3252–3265. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-08-03252.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Steriade M, Timofeev I, Grenier F. Natural waking and sleep states: a view from inside neocortical neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85:1969–1985. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Stern EA, Kincaid AE, Wilson CJ. Spontaneous subthreshold membrane potential fluctuations and action potential variability of rat corticostriatal and striatal neurons in vivo. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77:1697–1715. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.4.1697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Tan AY, Atencio CA, Polley DB, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE. Unbalanced synaptic inhibition can create intensity-tuned auditory cortex neurons. Neuroscience. 2007;146:449–462. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Wang X, Lu T, Snider RK, Liang L. Sustained firing in auditory cortex evoked by preferred stimuli. Nature. 2005;435:341–346. doi: 10.1038/nature03565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Wang XJ. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol Rev. 2010;90:1195–1268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Wu GK, Li P, Tao HW, Zhang LI. Nonmonotonic synaptic excitation and imbalanced inhibition underlying cortical intensity tuning. Neuron. 2006;52:705–715. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Wu GK, Arbuckle R, Liu BH, Tao HW, Zhang LI. Lateral sharpening of cortical frequency tuning by approximately balanced inhibition. Neuron. 2008;58:132–143. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES