Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Sep 25.
Published in final edited form as: J Affect Disord. 2013 Jun 27;150(3):1167–1173. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.084

Altered overnight modulation of spontaneous waking EEG reflects altered sleep homeostasis in major depressive disorder: a high-density EEG investigation

DT Plante 1,*, MR Goldstein 1, EC Landsness 1, BA Riedner 1, JJ Guokas 1, T Wanger 1, G Tononi 1, RM Benca 1
PMCID: PMC3760229  NIHMSID: NIHMS501159  PMID: 23810359

Abstract

Background

Prior investigations have suggested sleep homeostasis is altered in major depressive disorder (MDD). Low frequency activity (LFA) in the electroencephalogram during waking has been correlated with sleep slow wave activity (SWA), suggesting that waking LFA reflects sleep homeostasis in healthy individuals. This study investigated whether the overnight change in waking LFA and its relationship with sleep SWA are altered in MDD.

Methods

256-channel high-density electroencephalography (hdEEG) recordings during waking (pre- and post-sleep) and during sleep were collected in 14 unmedicated, unipolar MDD subjects (9 women) and age- and sex-matched healthy controls (HC).

Results

Waking LFA (3.25–.25 Hz) declined significantly overnight in the HC group, but not in the group of MDD subjects. Overnight decline of waking LFA correlated with sleep SWA in frontal brain regions in HC, but a comparable relationship was not found in MDD.

Limitations

This study is not able to definitely segregate overnight changes in the waking EEG that may occur due to homeostatic and/or circadian factors.

Conclusions

MDD involves altered overnight modulation of waking low frequency EEG activity that may reflect altered sleep homeostasis in the disorder. Future research is required to determine the functional significance and clinical implications of these findings.

Keywords: major depressive disorder, high-density EEG, waking EEG

1. Introduction

Sleep and major depressive disorder (MDD) are intricately related. Sleep disturbance is a core feature of MDD cross-sectionally, as a diagnostic criterion for the disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), and longitudinally, as a risk factor for both incident depression (Baglioni, et al, 2011) and depressive relapse after remission (Dombrovski, et al, 2007; Karp, et al, 2004; Paykel, et al, 1995). In addition, polysomnographic abnormalities of sleep continuity, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, and slow wave sleep (SWS) are robust, but non-specific, biological markers in MDD (Benca, et al, 1992; Steiger and Kimura, 2010). Finally, experimental manipulation using sleep deprivation (total, partial, and REM/SWS-selective) transiently alleviates depressive symptoms, although the mechanisms underlying these effects remain unknown (Landsness, et al, 2011; Vogel, et al, 1975; Wirz-Justice and Van den Hoofdakker, 1999; Wu and Bunney, 1990).

Sleep and wakefulness are regulated by the interaction of a homeostatic sleep-wake dependent process and a circadian pacemaker (Borbely, 1982; Dijk, 2009). According to the two-process model, homeostatic sleep propensity (process S) builds during wakefulness, and dissipates during sleep (Borbely, 1982). Slow wave activity (SWA) during non-REM (NREM) sleep is a widely accepted marker of homeostatic process S that declines during NREM sleep independent of circadian phase, and is increased during recovery sleep following sleep deprivation (Dijk, 2009). In addition to SWA during NREM sleep, several studies have suggested that increases in low frequency EEG power during waking may also be a marker of homeostatic function. Multiple investigations in human and animal models have demonstrated waking EEG power increases in various low-frequency bands (~1–8Hz) over the course of sleep deprivation (Aeschbach, et al, 1997; Aeschbach, et al, 1999; Cajochen, et al, 1995; Cajochen, et al, 1999b; Cajochen, et al, 2002; Dumont, et al, 1999; Finelli, et al, 2000; Hung, et al, 2013; Leemburg, et al, 2010; Tinguely, et al, 2006; Vyazovskiy and Tobler, 2005). Additionally, increases in both waking low frequency activity (LFA) and SWA in NREM sleep are predominantly observed in frontal brain regions, suggesting a topographical susceptibility to homeostatic processes across the cortex (Cajochen, et al, 1999a; Cajochen, et al, 2002; Finelli, et al, 2000; Tinguely, et al, 2006; Werth, et al, 1997). Finally, prior studies have demonstrated that the overnight decline of spontaneous waking delta activity (1–4Hz) correlates with nocturnal SWA (Hulse, et al, 2011), and that the subsequent rise in SWA in response to sleep deprivation correlates with increases in theta activity (~5–8 Hz) during the preceding period of wakefulness (Finelli, et al, 2000; Vyazovskiy and Tobler, 2005),

Several hypotheses have been proposed to explain these clinical and experimental observations in MDD, including decrements (Borbely and Wirz-Justice, 1982) or increases (Beersma and van den Hoofdakker, 1992) in sleep homeostatic processes as potential mechanisms for depression. Prior investigations have examined homeostatic function in MDD using SWA during sleep (reviewed in (Armitage, 2007)) however, very few investigations have examined homeostatic function in MDD using waking EEG power. Two studies that examined theta activity over the course of sleep deprivation in seasonal affective disorder (SAD) demonstrated a progressive increase in waking low frequency power in healthy subjects, which was attenuated (Cajochen, et al, 2000) or absent (Danilenko and Putilov, 2005) in SAD, suggesting altered homeostatic mechanisms in this depressive subtype. Birchler-Pedross and colleagues recently demonstrated increased LFA in MDD women relative to controls in response to extended wakefulness, with frontal LFA correlating with depression severity (Birchler-Pedross, et al, 2011). The current study sought to further examine homeostatic function in MDD using high-density EEG (hdEEG) prior to, during, and after a night of sleep. Our primary hypothesis was that the overnight change in waking LFA would be impaired in MDD subjects, suggestive of altered homeostatic function in the disorder.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Fourteen right-handed subjects with MDD (9 women) were drawn from a larger study of sleep homeostasis in depression. Subjects were included if samples of waking EEG recordings that were obtained prior to and following their sleep recordings were available for analysis. Diagnosis of MDD was based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (SCID) (First, et al, 2002b). All subjects were experiencing a depressive episode at the time of the study, and did not meet criteria for a lifetime history of bipolar or psychotic disorders. MDD subjects were administered the 17-item Hamilton Rating scale for depression (Hamilton, 1960). Age and sex-matched healthy controls (HC) without personal history of current or past psychiatric disorders or family history of mood disorders were evaluated with the non-patient SCID (First, et al, 2002a). All subjects were free of psychotropic medications ≥ 6 months prior to enrollment.

All subjects provided informed consent and were instructed to maintain regular sleep-wake schedules, avoid napping, and to limit the use of caffeinated and alcoholic beverages for the duration of the study. Adherence was monitored using sleep-logs and actigraphy (Actiwatch, Mini-Mitter, Bend, OR). This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Wisconsin-Madison.

2.2. Study design

All participants underwent in-laboratory polysomnography (PSG) prior to their experimental night, which served to acclimate participants to the laboratory environment and to rule out evidence of primary sleep disorders. PSG included electrooculogram (EOG), submental electromyogram (EMG), electrocardiogram (ECG), bilateral tibial EMG, respiratory inductance plethysmography, pulse oximetry, and a position sensor. Subjects with evidence of clinically significant sleep disordered breathing (apnea-hypopnea index >10/hr) or sleep-related movement disorder (periodic limb movement arousal index >10/hr) during overnight PSG were excluded.

On the experimental night, participants arrived at the laboratory between 19:00 and 21:00 and were outfitted with 256-channel hdEEG net (Electrical Geodesics Inc., Eugene, OR, USA). Pre-sleep (PM) waking hdEEG recordings were obtained approximately one hour prior to lights out. Subjects then slept undisturbed in the laboratory for the remainder of the night and sleep hdEEG recordings were collected. The following morning, participants were awoken after at least 7 hours of recording time. Subsequent post-sleep (AM) waking EEG recordings were collected at least 30 minutes after awakening to minimize effects of sleep inertia (Tassi and Muzet, 2000). To quantify level of alertness, participants completed a psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) immediately prior to each waking EEG recording and mean reaction time was computed for analysis (Dinges, et al, 1997). All waking and sleep hdEEG recordings were collected with vertex referencing, using NetStation software (Electrical Geodesics Eugene, OR).

2.3. Waking EEG

During waking hdEEG data collection, subjects were comfortably seated and instructed to relax with their eyes open, fixating for 2 minutes on a target 2 meters away. EEG data were processed offline in NetStation (Electrical Geodesics) and MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA), including the MATLAB plugin EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004), as previously described (Hulse, et al, 2011). HdEEG signals were sampled at 500Hz, and data were filtered with a 0.6 Hz first order highpass filter, followed by a 59 Hz lowpass FIR Kaiser filter with stopband attenuation of 60dB and the rolloff set to 0.3 Hz. Channels were rejected using an automatic outlier detection tool (using amplitude threshold criteria) and verified by visual inspection of the data. Rejected channels were subsequently interpolated via spherical splines. Independent Component Analysis in EEGLAB was used to identify characteristic eye, muscle and cardiac artifacts for removal (Jung, et al, 2000; Makeig, et al, 1997). Any residual artifacts were excluded by visual inspection. All artifact-free segments were average-referenced and power spectra were calculated for 4-sec epochs (Welch’s averaged modified periodogram with a Hamming window), resulting in a frequency resolution of 0.25 Hz. Frequency bins below 1 Hz were excluded from data analysis because of their sensitivity to low-frequency artifacts. Similarly, spectral data were analyzed up to 25 Hz to minimize influence of high-frequency artifacts.

2.4. Sleep EEG

HdEEG signals were sampled at 500 Hz, first-order high-pass filtered in NetStation (0.1Hz), downsampled to 128 Hz, band-pass filtered (2-way least-squares FIR, 1–40 Hz) in MATLAB, and average-referenced to the average scalp voltage computed in all channels. Semi-automatic artifact rejection was conducted to remove channels with high-frequency noise or interrupted contact with the scalp. To maintain congruence between spectral analysis and traditional sleep staging based on 30 second epochs, spectral analysis of NREM sleep was performed for each channel in consecutive 6-second epochs (Welch’s averaged modified periodgram with a Hamming window), resulting in a frequency resolution of 0.17 Hz, consistent with prior studies from our laboratory ((Goldstein, et al, 2012; Landsness, et al, 2011; Plante, et al, 2012). To normalize SWA for tests of association with overnight change in waking LFA, for every channel and each artifact-free NREM epoch, the power of each frequency bin was divided by the total power for all bins of that channel, similar to prior investigations that have demonstrated an association between overnight change in waking LFA and sleep SWA (Hulse, et al, 2011). Sleep staging was performed by a registered polysomnographic technologist in 30-second epochs according to standard criteria (Iber, et al, 2007) using Alice® Sleepware (Philips Respironics, Murrysville, PA) based on 6 EEG channels at approximate 10–20 locations (F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, and O2) re-referenced to the mastoids, sub-mental EMG and EOG.

2.5. Statistics

Clinical, polysomnographic, PVT, and spectral measures between groups were compared using unpaired t-tests. Changes in overnight spectral power and PVT values from PM to AM within groups were compared using paired t-tests. To increase the signal-to-noise ratio, all hdEEG analyses were restricted to 185 channels overlying the scalp (Goncharova, et al, 2003). Global sleep and waking power was defined as the average of these 185 channels. To limit spurious findings due to multiple comparisons, overnight changes in global waking power spectra were considered significant if t-values corresponded to p<0.05 for greater than 3 contiguous 0.25Hz bins. Linear correlation was used to assess the relationship between the overnight change in waking EEG activity and normalized SWA (1–4.5Hz) during sleep. To limit spurious correlations due to outliers, correlations were restricted to data ± 2.5 standard deviations from the mean. Because prior reports have found significant correlations between waking low frequency activity and sleep SWA using all-night data (Hulse, et al, 2011), as well as specifically in the first NREM period (NREM1) (Finelli, et al, 2000), correlations with both all-night and NREM1 SWA were evaluated. Statistical non-parametric mapping with suprathreshold cluster testing was utilized to correct for multiple comparisons of topographic data (within and between group t-tests and channel-by-channel linear correlations) in MATLAB using a t-value threshold equivalent to an alpha < 0.05 for the uncorrected comparisons (Nichols and Holmes, 2002). All other comparisons and correlations were conducted in Statistica 6.0 software package (StatSoft, Tulsa, OK) and MATLAB (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA).

3. Results

3.1. Clinical and Polysomnographic (PSG) Variables

MDD and HC subjects were young adults (MDD: 22.4±2.8, range 19–27 years; HC: 22.1±2.3, range 18–28 years) (Table 1). Depression severity was mild to moderate (HRSD-17 score 16.2±5.3, range 8–25) among MDD participants. MDD subjects demonstrated a greater percentage of REM sleep relative to controls (25.8% vs. 19.4%; p=0.01) consistent with prior literature (Benca, et al, 1992). Otherwise, there were no significant differences between groups on standard PSG measures or global SWA.

Table 1.

Demographic, clinical, and polysomnographic data

HC MDD p*
(N=14) (N=14)
Sex (male/female) 5/9 5/9 -
Age (years) 22.1 (2.3) 22.4 (2.8) 0.77
HRSD-17 - 16.2 (5.3) -
TST (min) 427.3 (29.6) 411.0 (54.9) 0.34
WASO (min) 22.2 (18.2) 24.5 (22.6) 0.76
AI (#/hr) 9.2 (4.3) 11.0 (4.5) 0.30
SE (%) 92.0 (6.6) 90.5 (6.9) 0.56
SOL (min) 16.6 (18.7) 18.1 (12.6) 0.80
N1 (%) 5.9 (3.4) 6.6 (4.4) 0.65
N2 (%) 56.6 (7.3) 50.9 (9.6) 0.09
N3 (%) 18.1 (5.3) 16.7 (9.8) 0.64
SWA (µV2/Hz) 28.5 (19.1) 24.2 (12.5) 0.49
REM (%) 19.4 (7.0) 25.8 (5.2) 0.01
REML (min) 83.0 (34.5) 72.7 (39.2) 0.47

MDD, major depressive disorder; HC, healthy control; HRSD-17, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (17-item); TST, total sleep time; WASO, wake after sleep onset; AI, arousal index; SE, sleep efficiency (TST/time in bed); SOL, sleep onset latency; N1/2/3, NREM stage 1/2/3 (% of TST); REM, stage REM (% of TST); REML, REM latency (time from sleep onset to first REM sleep epoch); SWA, global slow wave activity (EEG power in 1 – 4.5 Hz range average 185 channels); Values are displayed as mean (standard deviation).

*

p-value derived using 2-tailed, independent samples t-tests. Significant items marked in bold.

3.2. Waking EEG activity

HC subjects demonstrated an overnight decline in global waking power in both a low frequency activity (LFA) range (3.25–6.25Hz) and a broad beta range (11.75–21.5Hz)(Figure 1). MDD subjects, however, did not demonstrate a significant global overnight decline in waking LFA power, and had an attenuated overnight decline in beta power (14.75–15.75Hz)(Figure 1). There were no significant differences in global waking power between MDD and HC for any frequency band at either the PM or AM time point (data not shown).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Overnight decline in EEG waking power (eyes open) from PM to AM for A) Group average of spectral power at each timepoint for HC and B) MDD subjects. Global power derived as the average of 185 channels overlaying the scalp. Lower panel denotes significant 0.25Hz bins with p<0.05 after paired t-test.

Topographic analysis of waking EEG power demonstrated that the overnight decline in LFA (3.25–6.25 Hz) among controls was most prominent in bilateral frontal and left parietooccipital channels, whereas pre-to-post sleep decline in beta activity (11.75–21.5Hz) occurred globally across the scalp (Figure 2). Conversely, there were no significant topographic changes in overnight LFA among MDD subjects (Figure 2). Moreover, the overnight topographic decline in beta activity was attenuated in MDD subjects, such that it was localized to centroparietal regions (Figure 2). Although visual inspection suggested potentially increased LFA among HC relative to MDD at the PM timepoint, between group topographic comparisons of waking power demonstrated no statistically significant differences in waking LFA or beta activity between HC and MDD subjects at either PM or AM timepoint (Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Topographic waking EEG A) low frequency activity (LFA; 3.25–6.25Hz) and B) beta activity (11.75–21.5Hz) for HC and MDD subjects (units: µV2/Hz). Horizontal columns denote topographic comparisons within group for overnight PM to AM change in HC and MDD subjects, using channel-by-channel paired t-test. Vertical columns denote channel-by-channel unpaired t-tests for between group comparisons. T-values for paired and unpaired tests plotted at each corresponding channel. White dots denote significant channels after statistical non-parametric mapping with suprathreshold cluster test.

There were no significant differences in PVT mean reaction time observed between PM and AM for either group, nor between groups at either time point ((MDD: PM 300.4±47.5 msec, AM 303.5±57.3msec; HC: PM 290.2±21.8 msec, AM 295,2±27.6 msec; p=0.44–0.75), suggesting waking EEG results were not confounded by levels of vigilance.

3.3. Relationship between overnight change in low frequency waking EEG activity and sleep slow wave activity

Among HC, the overnight change in global LF did not significantly correlate with either all-night SWA (r=−0.21, p=0.51) or SWA from NREM1 (r=−0.41, p=0.17). Topographically, however, HC demonstrated a significant negative correlation with overnight change in LFA and SWA in frontal channels, whereby greater sleep SWA (both all-night and NREM1) correlated with a greater decline in waking LFA (Figure 3). Conversely, MDD subjects did not demonstrate either significant global (all-night; r=0.44, p=0.11; NREM1: r=0.25, p=0.39) or topographic (Figure 3) correlations between overnight change in waking LFA and sleep SWA.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Correlation between overnight change in waking LFA (3.25–6.25Hz) and sleep slow wave activity (SWA) in HC and MDD subjects. A) Topographic correlations of change in waking LFA with all-night (AN) and NREM1 SWA. White dots denote significant channels after statistical non-parametric mapping with suprathreshold cluster test. Note the frontal correlation between overnight change in waking LFA and sleep SWA among HC subjects and lack of an association among MDD participants. Red circle denotes channel Fp1. B) Representative scatterplot and correlation from an individual frontal channel (Fp1) using all-night SWA data. Closed black squares denote HC; open green circles indicate MDD subjects. Note significant correlation among HC but not MDD (HC: r=−0.56; p=0.04; MDD: r=0.14; p=0.63). Similar significant correlations for HC occurred at both Fp1 and Fp2, examining both AN and NREM1 SWA, however, there were no significant correlations among MDD (data not shown).

3.4. Additional exploratory analyses

Two additional analyses were performed to substantiate the absence of a relationship between overnight change in waking LFA and sleep SWA among MDD participants. First, since prior literature has suggested patients with MDD can have a shift of SWA such that it occurs predominantly in the second NREM period (NREM2)(Kupfer, et al, 1990), the relationship between overnight change in LFA (3.25–6.25Hz) and SWA from NREM2 among MDD participants was examined; however, no significant global (r=−0.13, p=0.67) or topographic (data not shown) correlations between these variables were observed.

Moreover, because prior investigations have demonstrated correlations between sleep SWA and waking EEG activity across variable waking low frequency bands from 1–8Hz, the use of a defined LFA range (3.25–6.25 Hz) that was derived from globally significant decreases from PM to AM in the current HC group could have inadvertently missed pertinent topographic correlations among MDD subjects. Therefore, to minimize the chance of type II error, we conducted a secondary topographic analysis to explore correlations of SWA with the overnight change in waking power across the entire frequency range from 1–8 Hz (Supplementary Figure 1). In this analysis, for every channel in which there was a significant correlation (p<0.05) between SWA and ≥ one 0.25Hz bin of waking power within the range of 1–8Hz, the channel was denoted along with the direction of the correlation (a negative correlation corresponding to greater sleep SWA associated with a greater overnight decline of waking power). To further liberalize the analysis to mitigate any reasonable chance of failing to find a significant association with overnight decline in waking LFA and sleep SWA among MDD, these data were uncorrected for multiple comparisons (e.g. statistical non-parametric mapping with suprathreshold cluster testing was not applied). Using this exploratory analysis, large topographic negative correlations between the overnight decline in waking power and SWA were observed among HC subjects, most prominently in bilateral frontal and parietal regions, both utilizing all-night and NREM1 SWA (Supplemental Figure 1). However, even using this liberal post hoc analysis, there were no negative correlations between SWA and overnight change in waking EEG activity among MDD subjects observed anywhere on the scalp, and the dispersed correlations observed in frontal, parietal, and occipital regions were exclusively in the opposite (positive) direction from those observed in HC (Supplementary Figure 1).

We also examined the relationships between overnight change in waking beta activity and sleep SWA, as well as associations with depression severity and nocturnal REM sleep percentage with waking EEG power on an exploratory basis. There were no significant correlations (global or topographic) between overnight change in waking beta activity (11.75– 21.5Hz) and sleep SWA (all-night and NREM1) for either HC or MDD groups. There were also no significant correlations (global or topographic) between overnight change in LFA (3.25– 6.25Hz) or beta activity and nocturnal REM sleep percentage. Among MDD subjects, there were no significant correlations (global or topographic) for LFA or beta activity (overnight change, or PM/AM spectral power) and HRSD score.

4. Discussion

Our findings demonstrate an overnight decline in waking EEG low frequency activity (LFA) in healthy controls (HC), but not in an in age and sex-matched group of subjects with unipolar major depressive disorder (MDD). Moreover, this overnight decline in LFA in HC subjects correlated with increased frontal slow wave activity (SWA) during sleep, however, there was no such corresponding correlation among MDD subjects. In addition, HC subjects demonstrated an overnight decline in waking EEG beta power across the entire scalp, which was restricted to a smaller cortical area in MDD subjects. Strengths of this investigation include lack of confounding psychotropic medications and the use of hdEEG for spectral analysis, which allowed for topographic analyses of the data.

These results are consistent with prior investigations that have demonstrated the overnight change in waking EEG LFA after a night of sleep may reflect sleep homeostasis in healthy subjects (Hulse, et al, 2011). Moreover, these results are congruent with recent work from our laboratory that has demonstrated the pre- to post-sleep decline in waking auditory evoked potential (AEP) amplitude correlates with sleep SWA in HC, but not in MDD subjects (Goldstein, et al, 2012). Taken together, these data suggest absence of overnight changes in both waking EEG LFA and AEP amplitude may reflect abnormal sleep homeostatic mechanisms in MDD.

There are various factors that may contribute to the absence of an overnight decline in waking LFA and/or the lack of correlation of this measure with SWA during sleep among MDD subjects. MDD is a heterogenous disorder (Krishnan and Nestler, 2010), and thus it is certainly plausible that uncontrolled covariates and/or unrecognized subgroups may be responsible for the findings of this investigation. One possible factor that could mediate our findings is gender, since significant sex-related effects on sleep SWA in MDD have been described, with increases in SWA among depressed women and decreases in SWA among depressed men, relative both to each other and to healthy comparison subjects (Armitage, et al, 2000a; Armitage, et al, 2000b; Armitage, 2007; Frey, et al, 2012a; Frey, et al, 2012b; Plante, et al, 2012). In addition, women with MDD have demonstrated an enhanced homeostatic response in sleep deprivation paradigms, evidenced by increased SWA during recovery sleep and waking low frequency activity, particularly in frontal EEG derivations (Armitage, 2007; Birchler-Pedross, et al, 2011; Frey, et al, 2012a; Frey, et al, 2012b). Taken together, these data suggest that MDD women may live with or tolerate an elevated level of homeostatic sleep pressure relative to men. However, because this study is underpowered to examine the role of sex on overnight changes in waking EEG in MDD, future studies that employ adequately powered designs to explore sexrelated differences in overnight change in waking LFA in MDD are indicated.

Our experimental design was cross-sectional, and thus we are not able to determine whether absence of overnight changes in waking EEG observed in MDD are a state or trait process. The lack of a correlation between waking EEG power and depression severity may support the notion that alterations in these measures are a trait marker in MDD, however, it is also plausible that our MDD subjects did not have sufficient variability in HRSD scores (e.g. most had relatively moderate depression), to detect a correlation. In addition, correlations of waking frontal low frequency activity with depression severity have been observed in studies that have utilized extended wakefulness paradigms (Birchler-Pedross, et al, 2011), and thus the dynamics of the waking EEG, either across the day or during extended wakefulness, may be a more fruitful correlate with depressive symptoms than overnight changes in waking LFA, as evaluated in this study. In this context, it is notable that recent investigations have demonstrated local, experience-dependent changes in waking theta activity during prolonged wakefulness that correlate with similar local changes in sleep SWA in healthy subjects, suggesting sleep is locally regulated by the amount of experience-dependent plasticity during wakefulness (Hung, et al, 2013). Interventions that rapidly alleviate depressive symptoms in MDD, such as sleep deprivation and ketamine, result in changes that reflect increases in synaptic strength, including increased slow wave amplitude and slope, as well as increased brain-derived neurotropic factor (Duncan, et al, 2012; Gorgulu and Caliyurt, 2009; Kavalali and Monteggia, 2012). Given the findings of this study, further research is warranted to clarify how alterations in local, experience-dependent waking LFA may reflect specific sleep-related alterations in synaptic plasticity in MDD.

5. Limitations

A primary limitation of our study is an inability to definitively segregate overnight changes in the waking EEG that may occur due to homeostatic and/or circadian factors. Prior investigations that have utilized extended wakefulness paradigms have demonstrated that increases in waking EEG activity in the 1–8Hz range reflect both the duration of time awake (i.e., homeostatic influence) and circadian modulation (Aeschbach, et al, 1997). However, low frequency activity in frontal derivations appears to be more strongly determined by homeostatic processes than in parieto-occipital regions (Cajochen, et al, 1999b; Cajochen, et al, 2002), suggesting relative contributions of circadian and homeostatic factors on the EEG vary across the cortex. Our finding of significant correlations between overnight change in LFA and sleep SWA in frontal regions among HC supports the notion that frontal waking LFA is more strongly related to homeostatic than circadian factors. In addition, both HC and MDD participants maintained regular sleep-wake patterns prior to the study, and both groups had similar total sleep times and latency to sleep onset in the laboratory, making it less likely that one group was significantly phase advanced or delayed relative to the other. However, since both homeostatic factors and circadian rhythms have been hypothesized to be aberrant in MDD (Borbely, 1987; Germain and Kupfer, 2008), further research that utilizes sleep deprivation and/or constant routine/forced desynchrony protocols are indicated to explore further the influence of these factors on overnight changes in waking EEG in MDD.

In conclusion, we have demonstrated that healthy young adult subjects exhibit a significant overnight decline in waking low frequency EEG activity that correlates with frontal slow wave activity during sleep. A comparable relationship, however, was not found in MDD subjects of similar age and sex. These preliminary findings suggest that altered overnight modulation of waking low frequency EEG activity in MDD may reflect altered sleep homeostasis in the disorder. Further research is indicated to explore the dynamics of waking LFA in MDD, as well as the functional significance and clinical implications of these findings.

Supplementary Material

01

Supplemental Figure 1. Exploratory analysis of significant topographic correlations between overnight change in waking EEG activity across a broad low frequency range (1–8 Hz) and all night (AN) and NREM1 sleep slow wave activity (SWA) in HC and MDD subjects. Each channel at which there was a significant correlation (p<0.05) between SWA and ≥ one 0.25Hz bin of waking power within the range of 1–8Hz was denoted, along with the direction of the correlation. A negative correlation (blue) corresponds to greater sleep SWA correlating with a greater overnight decline of waking power; a positive correlation (red) corresponds to greater sleep SWA correlating with a lower overnight change of waking power. Note prominent negative correlations in frontal and parietal regions in HC, without any significant negative correlation at any channel among MDD subjects.

Acknowledgements

Role of Funding Source

The NIMH had no further role in the study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, and the decision to submit the paper for publication.

This research was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health (5P20MH077967 to GT and RB, and F30MH082601 to EL).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributors

Dr. Plante designed the study, managed literature searches and analyses, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. Mr. Goldstein performed statistical analyses, hdEEG processing and analysis, design of figures/tables, and contributed to study design and writing the manuscript. Dr. Landsness contributed to the study design, evaluation of participants, management of hdEEG processing and analyses, and writing the article. Mr. Wanger, and Mr. Guokas performed statistical analyses and hdEEG processing, and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. Dr. Riedner contributed to analysis and interpretation of the data and writing the manuscript. Dr. Tononi contributed to the study design, data analysis and interpretation, and to writing the article. Dr. Benca contributed by being the principal investigator, contributing to the study design, participating in data analysis and interpretation, and writing the article. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

Dr. Plante has owned stock in Pfizer, and has received honoraria from Oakstone Medical Publishing, the American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Kishwaukee Community Hospital, and royalties from Cambridge University Press. Dr. Tononi has consulted for Sanofi-Aventis and Takeda, and he is currently the David P. White Chair in Sleep Medicine at the University of Wisconsin–Madison, endowed by Phillips Respironics. Dr. Tononi has also received unrelated research support from Phillips Respironics. Dr. Benca has consulted for Merck and Sanofi-Aventis. All other authors declare they have no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Aeschbach D, Matthews JR, Postolache TT, Jackson MA, Giesen HA, Wehr TA. Dynamics of the human EEG during prolonged wakefulness: evidence for frequency-specific circadian and homeostatic influences. Neurosci. Lett. 1997;239:121–124. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(97)00904-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aeschbach D, Matthews JR, Postolache TT, Jackson MA, Giesen HA, Wehr TA. Two circadian rhythms in the human electroencephalogram during wakefulness. Am. J. Physiol. 1999;277:R1771–R1779. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1999.277.6.R1771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  4. Armitage R, Hoffmann R, Fitch T, Trivedi M, Rush AJ. Temporal characteristics of delta activity during NREM sleep in depressed outpatients and healthy adults: group and sex effects. Sleep. 2000a;23:607–617. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Armitage R, Hoffmann R, Trivedi M, Rush AJ. Slow-wave activity in NREM sleep: sex and age effects in depressed outpatients and healthy controls. Psychiatry Res. 2000b;95:201–213. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00178-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Armitage R. Sleep and circadian rhythms in mood disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2007;(Suppl.)(433):104–115. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.00968.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baglioni C, Battagliese G, Feige B, Spiegelhalder K, Nissen C, Voderholzer U, Lombardo C, Riemann D. Insomnia as a predictor of depression: a meta-analytic evaluation of longitudinal epidemiological studies. J. Affect Disord. 2011;135:10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Beersma DG, van den Hoofdakker RH. Can non-REM sleep be depressogenic? J. Affect. Disord. 1992;24:101–108. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(92)90024-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Benca RM, Obermeyer WH, Thisted RA, Gillin JC. Sleep and psychiatric disorders. A meta-analysis. Arch. Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49:651–668. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820080059010. discussion 669–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Birchler-Pedross A, Frey S, Chellappa SL, Gotz T, Brunner P, Knoblauch V, Wirz-Justice A, Cajochen C. Higher frontal EEG synchronization in young women with major depression: a marker for increased homeostatic sleep pressure? Sleep. 2011;34:1699–1706. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Borbely AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum. Neurobiol. 1982;1:195–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Borbely AA, Wirz-Justice A. Sleep, sleep deprivation and depression. A hypothesis derived from a model of sleep regulation. Hum. Neurobiol. 1982;1:205–210. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Borbely AA. The S-deficiency hypothesis of depression and the two-process model of sleep regulation. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1987;20:23–29. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-1017069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cajochen C, Brunner DP, Krauchi K, Graw P, Wirz-Justice A. Power density in theta/alpha frequencies of the waking EEG progressively increases during sustained wakefulness. Sleep. 1995;18:890–894. doi: 10.1093/sleep/18.10.890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cajochen C, Foy R, Dijk DJ. Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. Sleep Res Online. 1999a;2:65–69. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cajochen C, Khalsa SB, Wyatt JK, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ. EEG and ocular correlates of circadian melatonin phase and human performance decrements during sleep loss. Am. J. Physiol. 1999b;277:R640–R649. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1999.277.3.r640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cajochen C, Brunner DP, Krauchi K, Graw P, Wirz-Justice A. EEG and subjective sleepiness during extended wakefulness in seasonal affective disorder: circadian and homeostatic influences. Biol. Psychiatry. 2000;47:610–617. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00242-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cajochen C, Wyatt JK, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ. Separation of circadian and wake duration-dependent modulation of EEG activation during wakefulness. Neuroscience. 2002;114:1047–1060. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00209-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Danilenko KV, Putilov AA. Melatonin treatment of winter depression following total sleep deprivation: waking EEG and mood correlates. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1345–1352. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2004;134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dijk DJ. Regulation and functional correlates of slow wave sleep. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2009;5:S6–S15. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dinges DF, Pack F, Williams K, Gillen KA, Powell JW, Ott GE, Aptowicz C, Pack AI. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4–5 hours per night. Sleep. 1997;20:267–277. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dombrovski AY, Mulsant BH, Houck PR, Mazumdar S, Lenze EJ, Andreescu C, Cyranowski JM, Reynolds CF., 3rd Residual symptoms and recurrence during maintenance treatment of late-life depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2007;103:77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.01.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dumont M, Macchi MM, Carrier J, Lafrance C, Hebert M. Time course of narrow frequency bands in the waking EEG during sleep deprivation. Neuroreport. 1999;10:403–407. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199902050-00035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Duncan WC, Sarasso S, Ferrarelli F, Selter J, Riedner BA, Hejazi NS, Yuan P, Brutsche N, Manji HK, Tononi G, Zarate CA. Concomitant BDNF and sleep slow wave changes indicate ketamine-induced plasticity in major depressive disorder. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012:1–11. doi: 10.1017/S1461145712000545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Finelli LA, Baumann H, Borbely AA, Achermann P. Dual electroencephalogram markers of human sleep homeostasis: correlation between theta activity in waking and slow-wave activity in sleep. Neuroscience. 2000;101:523–529. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00409-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. First M, Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams J. Biometrics Research. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002a. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-Patient Edition (SCID-I/NP) [Google Scholar]
  28. First M, Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams J. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders, research version, patient edition. 2002b [Google Scholar]
  29. Frey S, Birchler-Pedross A, Hofstetter M, Brunner P, Gotz T, Munch M, Blatter K, Knoblauch V, Wirz-Justice A, Cajochen C. Challenging the sleep homeostat: Sleep in depression is not premature aging. Sleep Med. 2012a;13:933–945. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Frey S, Birchler-Pedross A, Hofstetter M, Brunner P, Gotz T, Munch M, Blatter K, Knoblauch V, Wirz-Justice A, Cajochen C. Young women with major depression live on higher homeostatic sleep pressure than healthy controls. Chronobiol. Int. 2012b;29:278–294. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.656163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Germain A, Kupfer DJ. Circadian rhythm disturbances in depression. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 2008;23:571–585. doi: 10.1002/hup.964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Goldstein MR, Plante DT, Hulse BK, Sarasso S, Landsness EC, Tononi G, Benca RM. Overnight changes in waking auditory evoked potential amplitude reflect altered sleep homeostasis in major depression. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2012;125:468–477. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01796.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Goncharova II, McFarland DJ, Vaughan TM, Wolpaw JR. EMG contamination of EEG: spectral and topographical characteristics. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114:1580–1593. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00093-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gorgulu Y, Caliyurt O. Rapid antidepressant effects of sleep deprivation therapy correlates with serum BDNF changes in major depression. Brain Res. Bull. 2009;80:158–162. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hulse BK, Landsness EC, Sarasso S, Ferrarelli F, Guokas JJ, Wanger T, Tononi G. A postsleep decline in auditory evoked potential amplitude reflects sleep homeostasis. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2011;122:1549–1555. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.01.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hung CS, Sarasso S, Ferrarelli F, Riedner B, Ghilardi MF, Cirelli C, Tononi G. Local experience-dependent changes in the wake EEG after prolonged wakefulness. Sleep. 2013;36:59–72. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson AL, Quan SF for the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology, and Technical Specifications. Westchester, Illinois: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  39. Jung TP, Makeig S, Humphries C, Lee TW, McKeown MJ, Iragui V, Sejnowski TJ. Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:163–178. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Karp JF, Buysse DJ, Houck PR, Cherry C, Kupfer DJ, Frank E. Relationship of variability in residual symptoms with recurrence of major depressive disorder during maintenance treatment. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2004;161:1877–1884. doi: 10.1176/ajp.161.10.1877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kavalali ET, Monteggia LM. Synaptic mechanisms underlying rapid antidepressant action of ketamine. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2012;169:1150–1156. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12040531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Krishnan V, Nestler EJ. Linking molecules to mood: new insight into the biology of depression. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2010;167:1305–1320. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.10030434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kupfer DJ, Frank E, McEachran AB, Grochocinski VJ. Delta sleep ratio. A biological correlate of early recurrence in unipolar affective disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1990;47:1100–1105. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810240020004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Landsness EC, Goldstein MR, Peterson MJ, Tononi G, Benca RM. Antidepressant effects of selective slow wave sleep deprivation in major depression: a high-density EEG investigation. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2011;45:1019–1026. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Leemburg S, Vyazovskiy VV, Olcese U, Bassetti CL, Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep homeostasis in the rat is preserved during chronic sleep restriction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2010;107:15939–15944. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002570107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Makeig S, Jung TP, Bell AJ, Ghahremani D, Sejnowski TJ. Blind separation of auditory event-related brain responses into independent components. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1997;94:10979–10984. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.20.10979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2002;15:1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Paykel ES, Ramana R, Cooper Z, Hayhurst H, Kerr J, Barocka A. Residual symptoms after partial remission: an important outcome in depression. Psychol. Med. 1995;25:1171–1180. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700033146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Plante DT, Landsness EC, Peterson MJ, Goldstein MR, Riedner BA, Wanger T, Guokas JJ, Tononi G, Benca RM. Sex-related differences in sleep slow wave activity in major depressive disorder: a high-density EEG investigation. BMC Psychiatry. 2012;12:146. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Steiger A, Kimura M. Wake and sleep EEG provide biomarkers in depression. J. Psychiatr Res. 2010;44:242–252. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tassi P, Muzet A. Sleep inertia. Sleep Med. Rev. 2000;4:341–353. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2000.0098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tinguely G, Finelli LA, Landolt HP, Borbely AA, Achermann P. Functional EEG topography in sleep and waking: state-dependent and state-independent features. Neuroimage. 2006;32:283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Vogel GW, Thurmond A, Gibbons P, Sloan K, Walker M. REM sleep reduction effects on depression syndromes. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1975;32:765–777. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1975.01760240093007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Vyazovskiy VV, Tobler I. Theta activity in the waking EEG is a marker of sleep propensity in the rat. Brain Res. 2005;1050:64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.05.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Werth E, Achermann P, Borbely AA. Fronto-occipital EEG power gradients in human sleep. J. Sleep Res. 1997;6:102–112. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1997.d01-36.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wirz-Justice A, Van den Hoofdakker RH. Sleep deprivation in depression: what do we know, where do we go? Biol. Psychiatry. 1999;46:445–453. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00125-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Wu JC, Bunney WE. The biological basis of an antidepressant response to sleep deprivation and relapse: review and hypothesis. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1990;147:14–21. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.1.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

Supplemental Figure 1. Exploratory analysis of significant topographic correlations between overnight change in waking EEG activity across a broad low frequency range (1–8 Hz) and all night (AN) and NREM1 sleep slow wave activity (SWA) in HC and MDD subjects. Each channel at which there was a significant correlation (p<0.05) between SWA and ≥ one 0.25Hz bin of waking power within the range of 1–8Hz was denoted, along with the direction of the correlation. A negative correlation (blue) corresponds to greater sleep SWA correlating with a greater overnight decline of waking power; a positive correlation (red) corresponds to greater sleep SWA correlating with a lower overnight change of waking power. Note prominent negative correlations in frontal and parietal regions in HC, without any significant negative correlation at any channel among MDD subjects.

RESOURCES