Abstract
Risk behaviors such as substance use or deviance are often limited to the early stages of the life course. Whereas the onset of risk behavior is well studied, less is currently known about the decline and timing of cessation of risk behaviors of different domains during young adulthood. Prevalence and longitudinal developmental patterning of alcohol use, drinking to the point of drunkenness, smoking, cannabis use, deviance, and HIV-related sexual risk behavior were compared in a Swiss community sample (N = 2,843). Using a longitudinal cohort-sequential approach to link multiple assessments with 3 waves of data for each individual, the studied period spanned the ages of 16 to 29 years. Although smoking had a higher prevalence, both smoking and drinking up to the point of drunkenness followed an inverted U-shaped curve. Alcohol consumption was also best described by a quadratic model, though largely stable at a high level through the late 20s. Sexual risk behavior increased slowly from age 16 to age 22 and then remained largely stable. In contrast, cannabis use and deviance linearly declined from age 16 to age 29. Young men were at higher risk for all behaviors than were young women, but apart from deviance, patterning over time was similar for both sexes. Results about the timing of increase and decline as well as differences between risk behaviors may inform tailored prevention programs during the transition from late adolescence to adulthood.
Keywords: trajectory, risk behaviors, emerging adults, substance use
Risk behavior such as binge drinking, cannabis use, or deviance is common in adolescence but does not often last beyond young adulthood. Adolescence and emerging adulthood are characterized by neurobiological and physical changes, increasing impulse control, emotional and cognitive maturation, and environmental changes in the social networks and social roles that accompany transitions from youth to early adult life. These processes can result in a considerable level of distress (Allen & Sheeber, 2009; Arnett, 2004). The psychosocial stress model (Hurrelmann & Richter, 2006; Jessor & Jessor, 1977) conceptualizes adolescent risk behavior as the result of an interaction of psychosocial stressors and inadequate coping strategies. Apart from being an attempt to cope with distress, risk behaviors may be functional for the mastery of developmental tasks such as integration into a peer group or demonstrating opposition to parental or conventional norms, or may be a part of identity exploration or the desire to obtain a wide range of experience (Arnett, 2004).
With the acquisition of adult roles such as responsibilities at work, a stable partnership, or parenthood, risk behavior is thought to lose its functionality and become redundant and less socially accepted. However, for substance-related risk behaviors not only the functionality but also the addictive properties of the substance can affect the patterns of use and could lead to a more persistent course than for sexual risk behavior or deviance. Substances typically differ in the frequency of use. In the general population, smoking is mostly characterized by a dependent pattern of daily use, alcohol is used weekly, and cannabis and drinking up to the point of drunkenness are often used recreationally with a low frequency.
While onset and increase of risk behavior in adolescence is well studied, less is known about young adulthood and the age when different risk behaviors start to decline. Previous research is often cross-sectional, commonly focuses on trajectories of a single type of risk behavior or risk behaviors from the same domain, and includes mostly college students. A recent study analyzed developmental trajectories of drinking, smoking, and cannabis use in a large, nationally representative longitudinal sample of U.S. Americans 12 to 34 years of age (Chen & Jacobson, 2012). Findings demonstrate that substance use initiates during adolescence, peaks during the early and mid-20s, and then declines (see also Hammond, 2005; Lee, Mun, White, & Simon, 2010; Maggs & Schulenberg, 2004–2005; Terry-McElrath & O’Malley, 2011). Drinking showed higher and cannabis lower levels of change. Other studies have suggested that smoking is more persistent than other substance use and that even occasional smoking bears a heightened risk of later daily smoking or of nicotine dependence (e.g., Di Franza et al., 2002; Piasecki, 2006). Evidence for the course of cannabis use is inconsistent and ranges from stable high trajectories (Perkonigg et al., 2008) to a U-shaped curve (Chen & Jacobson, 2012; Lee et al., 2010) to a decrease (Jackson, Sher, & Schulenberg, 2008; Terry-McElrath & O’Malley, 2011). Sexual risk behavior was found to follow an inverted U-shaped curve for 15- to 23-year-olds (Fergus, Zimmermann, & Caldwell, 2007; Kan, Cheng, Landale, & McHale, 2010) and a stable course in 18- to 29-year-olds (Duangpatra, Bradley, & Glendon, 2009). Deviance seems to decrease from age 14 to age 18 (Mason et al., 2010). In a developmental taxonomy of antisocial behavior, Moffitt (2006) distinguished between a more frequent adolescence-limited and a less frequent life-course-persistent delinquent behavior.
To our knowledge, no previous longitudinal study has compared intraindividual trajectories of distinct risk behavior including substance use, deviance, and HIV-related sexual risk behavior in a community sample from late adolescence throughout their 20s. The aims of this study were first to compare the trajectories of different risk behaviors from age 16 to age 29, using a cohort sequential latent growth model. Assessed risk behaviors cover distinct domains and included smoking, drinking, drunkenness, cannabis use, deviance, and HIV-related sexual risk behavior with occasional and/or new partners. Second, we were interested in gender effects on the trajectories of all risk behaviors. Although several distress and personality variables were assessed in this project, the current short report is limited to describing patterns of change in risk behaviors from different domains in the whole sample.
In line with existing findings, we expected that risk behaviors would generally decline from the early to the late 20s. However we expected distinct trajectories for the assessed risk behaviors. We assumed that deviance, which may be more driven by the functionality for coping with developmental tasks, decreases more rapidly than do trajectories of substance-related risk behaviors. The latter may also be affected by the development of a physical dependence. We expected sexual risk behavior to increase at the stage of exploration and changing relationships and then to decrease in the mid to late 20s when longer term and more committed relationships have been built. We also hypothesized that socially less accepted risk behaviors like deviance, cannabis use, and drunkenness would decrease more rapidly, while habitual moderate alcohol use and smoking would decline less or remain stable, as they are easier to integrate into adult life.
Method
Participants
The sample comprised 2,844 adolescents and young adults from the Risk Behavior and Well-Being in Adolescence and Young Adulthood study (RIWA; Brodbeck, Matter, & Moggi, 2006; Brodbeck, Matter, Page, & Moggi, 2007; Brodbeck, Vilén, Bachmann, Alsaker, & Znoj, 2010), a 5-year longitudinal study based on an urban Swiss sample. At baseline (Time 1, or T1), a random sample of 16- to 24-year-old urban Swiss young men and women was selected based on the official registers of the Residents’ Administration Offices of the Swiss cities of Basel, Bern, and Zurich.
Data were collected by computer-assisted telephone interview initially in 2003 (T1), at the 2-year follow-up in 2005 (T2), and at the 5-year follow-up in 2008 (T3). Before calling the participants, a letter explaining the study aims and the procedures was sent out. At T2 and T3 letters were sent to participants who had agreed to take part in the follow-up. If necessary, an extensive search was undertaken to find their new addresses and phone numbers, including checking different telephone directories and contacting the Residents’ Administration Offices. If the new telephone number could not be found, or after 40 unsuccessful attempts to reach participants, they were allocated to the dropout group. At the 5-year follow-up, the option of completing an online questionnaire instead of a telephone interview was offered to those who lived abroad.
Sample Characteristics and Attrition
After excluding individuals with invalid telephone numbers, persons with insufficient mastery of the German language, and individuals with serious health problems that precluded participation in the interview, 2,844 of the eligible 4,031 persons were interviewed (71% response rate). In the second wave, after 2 years (T2), 2,031 persons (71% of the baseline sample) were reinterviewed. At the 5-year follow-up (T3), 1,641 persons were reinterviewed (58% of the baseline sample). Table 1 presents the sample characteristics for each wave and their effect on attrition rate. Most participants were not married. At baseline most of the participants were in comprehensive secondary school or at university (37%), in professional training (26%), or employed (20%). At T3 most participants were employed (57%) or still at university or in further education (34%). The attrition rate was higher for those who were employed or in professional training. The attrition rate at T3 was also higher for married participants and for participants who had a higher frequency of smoking, sexual risk behavior, or deviance.
Table 1. Sample Characteristics at Each Wave (Nonimputed Data) and Effects on Retention Rate.
Measures
Substance use
Respondents were asked how often in the month before the interview they had consumed each of the following substances: tobacco, alcohol, alcohol to the point of drunkenness, and cannabis (0 = never, 1 = 1–3 times a month, 2 = 1–2 times a week, 3 = 3–6 times a week, 4 = daily). Due to very low response frequencies (see Table 1), daily alcohol use was combined with alcohol use 3–6 times a week. Also, for drunkenness higher categories were collapsed to 1–2 times a week or more often. Variables were treated as binary or ordinal.
HIV-related sexual risk behavior
HIV-related sexual risk behavior was defined as (1) sexual intercourse without condom use with a casual partner, and/or (2) sexual intercourse with a new stable partner without condoms and with no HIV testing for those partners who had ever had intercourse. It was assessed for the 12-month period prior to the interviews (no/yes).
Deviance
Respondents were asked whether in the previous 12 months they had engaged in theft, violence, blackmail, or substantial damage to property of others (no/yes). These four items were combined into one measure for deviance, which was coded as yes if even one type of the above behaviors was reported.
Statistical Analysis
We adopted a cohort-sequential approach to the analysis of the multiple risk assessments. This provides a way to link adjacent segments of our three-wave longitudinal data, using responses from the different age cohorts to determine the existence of a common developmental trend or growth curve, which can be profiled over the full age range. Under this analytic approach, each cohort has a different pattern of intentional attrition, or “planned missing data.” Simultaneous analysis of each cohort with a related growth model makes it possible to build the complete curve using information from all ages simultaneously (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2011; Preacher, Wichman, MacCallum, & Briggs, 2008). Thus, our “trajectory” analysis spanned a 14-year-period from age 16 to 29, using only 5 years of longitudinal data. This approach, providing more measurement points than waves, allows for estimation of nonlinear trajectory shapes, for the population profile, even though there are only three observations per individual.
Each accelerated growth model was estimated in Mplus Version 6.1.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2011) using the diagonally weighted least squares estimator, which is appropriate for categorical data. Since this estimator yields robust results under the assumption of missing completely at random only, we performed multiple imputations with the Bayes estimation method (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2010). Variables included in the imputation models were risk behaviors at all three measurement points, age, gender, marital status, and education. We created 10 complete data sets, which were analyzed in parallel, yielding means and standard deviations for the outcomes and goodness-of-fit indices, across the 10 data sets.
We estimated models with linear and quadratic slopes. Model fit was assessed using the following indices: the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), and the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA). Although no single set of threshold values for these statistics can be relied upon in isolation, we favored models that exceeded 0.95 for TLI and CFI, although a CFI > 0.90 is sometimes considered acceptable (Bentler, 1990, 1992; Tucker & Lewis, 1973), as are models with an RMSEA below 0.05 (Steiger, 1990).
Frequent risk behaviors (i.e., alcohol use, drunkenness, smoking, and cannabis use) were treated as ordinal variables. For presenting the trajectories in one figure, we summed the estimated probabilities for all response categories. As postmodeling refinement, the mean of the slope of the cannabis trajectory in the youngest cohort was freely estimated, as it was shown to be different from the rest of the cohorts. Gender effects were analyzed as correlations between sex and intercepts, slopes, and quadratic terms.
Results
Table 2 reports the fit statistics for the linear and the quadratic models for all risk behaviors. Trajectories of alcohol use, drunkenness, smoking, and HIV-related sexual risk behavior were better described by models with quadratic slopes. The course of cannabis use and deviance were sufficiently well characterized by models with linear slopes.
Table 2. Goodness-of-Fit Indices for the Linear and the Quadratic Models (Mean Values and Standard Deviations for the 10 Imputed Data Sets).
Table 3 presents the model results for the frequency of risk behaviors (substance use per month, deviance and sexual risk behavior per year). Interindividual variance was high in the initial levels and the slopes of all risk behaviors but not for the slopes of cannabis and deviance.
Table 3. Unstandardized Parameter Estimates for the Models Measuring the Frequency of the Risk Behaviors.
Figure 1 summarizes the percentages of participants showing risk behaviors from age 16 to age 29 (i.e., the prevalence of the risk behaviors). Alcohol was the most commonly used substance. Alcohol use increased rapidly from 66% at age 16 to 79% at age 19, then increased more slowly and finally remained stable around 88% at age 25 to 29. Drinking up to the point of drunkenness was reported by almost 25% of all participants at age 16. Drunkenness peaked from ages 22 to 24 with a 30-day prevalence of 35% and then decreased again to 27% at age 29, a similar level to that at age 16. Although the prevalence rate of smoking is higher, the trajectory is similar to that for drunkenness. At age 16, 37% of the participants smoked during the month prior to the interview. Smoking prevalence increased rapidly during late adolescence, remained relatively stable from age 21 to age 24 with a prevalence rate of almost 50% and then decreased again to 39% at age 29. Cannabis use was also common, with about 25% of the 16-year-olds using it at least once in the month prior to the interview. Cannabis use then decreased steadily to 13% at age 29.
Deviance and HIV-related sexual risk behavior were assessed within 12-month time frames prior to the interviews. At age 16, 40% of all participants reported theft, violence, blackmail, or substantial damage to property of others. Deviance decreased rapidly to a 12-month prevalence of 11% at age 29. In contrast to all other risk behaviors, HIV-related sexual risk behavior increased between 16 and 29 from 6% to 12%.
Young men reported significantly higher levels of all risk behaviors. Alcohol use, drunkenness, cannabis use, and deviance showed moderate association with gender. Small but still significant gender effects were found for smoking and HIV-related sexual risk behavior. Although gender affected the intercepts, the course (slope) of most risk behaviors was independent of sex. The only exception was deviance, where females showed a slower decrease.
Discussion
The present study explored longitudinal patterning of the prevalence of distinct risk behaviors between16 and 29 years. It adds to the existing knowledge about the decline in risk behaviors tracing multiple behavioral domains in a random urban Swiss community sample over a wide age range.
Smoking and drinking up to the point of drunkenness followed an inverted U-shape trajectory, with acceleration during late adolescence, a peak around 22 to 24, and then a decrease. Alcohol consumption also followed a quadratic curve, with an acceleration from age 16 to 21, then a slower increase, and then by and large a stagnation until age 29. It is noteworthy that the legal drinking age in Switzerland is 16, similar to the case in other European countries but much lower than in the United States. Age-specific 30-day prevalence rates of drinking, smoking, and cannabis use in this sample of Swiss urban adolescents and young adults are higher than those in national samples of 10th to 12th graders in U.S. studies in the years 2001–2005 (Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2012). Interestingly, the 30-day rates of drunkenness are similar, and the rates for daily use of all substances seem to be lower in Switzerland than in the United States. In spite of these differences in the level of substance use, the growth trajectories for smoking, drinking, and drunkenness in both countries are best described by a quadratic model (Chen & Jacobson, 2012). However, while the 1-month prevalence of drinking in the United States decreases in the late 20s, it stagnates at a high level in our study.
The course of cannabis use seems to be less generalizable than the course of smoking and drinking. Previous research found inconsistent trajectories for cannabis use, including high-stable, inverted U-shaped, and decreasing trajectories. In the RIWA study cannabis declined monotonically from a prevalence rate of 26% at age 16 to 13% at age 29, in line with Lee et al. (2010) and Terry-McElrath and O’Malley (2011). Similar to the course of cannabis use and in line with previous evidence, deviance decreased linearly from a 1-year prevalence rate of 40% at age 16 to 11% at age 29.
Findings revealed gender effects for the levels of risk behaviors but not for the shape of the trends over time. As in other studies (e.g., Chen & Jacobson, 2012), young men reported higher levels of all risk behaviors. However, once risk behavior was established, both genders followed the same course. The only exception was deviance, with women showing a slower decrease than did men.
Our findings are so far in line with the notion that learning how to deal with commonly available substances such as alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis, as well as the acquisition of risk-related expertise and competence, is a developmental task during emerging adulthood (Franzkowiak, 1987). Especially the decrease of drinking up to the point of drunkenness in conjunction with the stable frequency may be interpreted as the acquisition of responsible, moderate alcohol consumption.
An exception to the general clear decrease of risk behavior during young adulthood is HIV-related sexual risk behavior, defined as unprotected intercourse with an occasional or a new partner. The late and less pronounced decline might reflect a temporal effect that adolescents and young adults engage in sexual relationships later than they become involved with substances. In a U.S. study, an inverted U-shaped trajectory was found for the number of partners, peaking around the age of 20 and then declining (Kan et al., 2010). Sexual risk behavior as the result of a dyadic interaction might also have a different functionality compared with other risk behaviors.
A higher frequency and persistence of risk behaviors correlate with negative long-term outcomes such as substance abuse or dependency or low psychosocial adjustment. However, not only persistent but also transient risk behaviors may cause physical harm such as alcohol poisoning, drug overdose, or a higher risk for accidents or infections with sexually transmitted diseases. Knowledge about developmental trajectories of distinct risk behaviors may help identifying specific periods during which universal population-based prevention programs may be optimally effective.
Limitations and Further Research
In the present article we reported only single trajectories from univariate longitudinal analyses, conducted for the different risk behaviors. However, risk behaviors tend to co-occur, which is not accounted for in this article. One further limitation is that the growth models assume that a single growth trajectory with a common starting or end point can adequately represent growth in the whole sample. Individual variation around the average trajectory, which might be important in understanding these processes for different individuals or groups, is not of interest in such modeling. Also, our primarily descriptive study does not seek to analyze predictors of the observed population trajectories. A more clinical perspective focuses on the differential vulnerability of individuals for engaging in risk behaviors and tries to identify subgroups with distinct trajectories as well as distinct explanatory variables for each subgroup. Mixture models covering different forms of risk behaviors may be more appropriate for a clinical or etiological approach, and it is our intention to explore those as a next step. Despite these shortcomings, growth models are useful for epidemiological purposes or population-based prevention, where change of behavior in a whole population is of interest and a normative development over time is expected. Due to the more parsimonious nature of the trajectories, it is also easier to compare the course of different risk behaviors (Maggs & Schulenberg, 2004–2005).
A limitation of the sample is that only adolescents and young adults who lived in three major cities at baseline were assessed. We cannot exclude that they differ from participants in rural areas. Attrition rate was influenced by a number of characteristics of the cohort: being married, being employed or in professional training, and by exhibiting a higher prevalence of smoking, deviance, and sexual risk behavior. Thus, not only individuals who showed more risk behavior but also individuals who have “settled down” more than others had a higher risk of dropping out of the study. Additionally, only self-report data were used. Deviance was assessed with a composite score of theft, violence, damaging others’ property, and blackmailing, which would likely have affected the prevalence of deviance. Although we assessed illicit drug use other than cannabis, the prevalence rate of around 2% made it difficult to model the trajectory in an accelerated cohort design. As illicit drugs include substances with highly addictive properties, an increasing trajectory may be possible.
Conclusions
Patterning of risk behavior from late adolescence to almost 30 years of age differed depending on the type of behavior. Most risk behaviors decrease from late adolescence to young adulthood, but the age when decline begins differs. While drinking up to the point of drunkenness, smoking, cannabis use, and deviance decline during young adulthood, HIV-related sexual risk behavior still increases. This might indicate a distinct functionality compared with other risk behaviors.
Acknowledgments
Tim J. Croudace is now at the Department of Health Sciences, University of York, York, North Yorkshire, United Kingdom. Anna Brown is now at the School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, United Kingdom.
This research was supported by Swiss National Science Foundation Grants 3346-65580 and 3346C0-104080 and Swiss Office of Public Health Grants 04.001846 and 07.002983. Jeannette Brodbeck was supported by a fellowship for advanced researchers from the Swiss National Science Foundation. Tim J. Croudace was supported in part by a Career Scientist Award in Public Health from the UK Department of Health/National Institute of Health Research and by Wellcome Trust Grant 088869/Z/09/Z. We would like to thank Hansjoerg Znoj, Thomas Berger, Monika Matter, and Franz Moggi for their contribution in the design and data collection for this study.
References
- Allen N. B., & Sheeber L. B. (Eds.). (2009). Adolescent emotional development and the emergence of depressive disorders. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511551963 [Google Scholar]
- Arnett J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Asparouhov T., & Muthén B. (2010). Multiple imputation with Mplus. Available at http://www.statmodel.com/download/Imputations7.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Bentler P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bentler P. M. (1992). On the fit of models to covariances and methodology to the Bulletin. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 400–404. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.3.400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodbeck J., Matter M., & Moggi F. (2006). Association of cannabis use and sexual risk behavior among young heterosexual adults. AIDS and Behavior, 10, 599–605. doi:10.1007/s10461-006-9103-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodbeck J., Matter M., Page J., & Moggi F. (2007). Motives for cannabis use as a moderator variable of distress among young adults. Addictive Behaviors, 32, 1537–1545. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.11.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodbeck J., Vilén L., Bachmann M., Alsaker F. D., & Znoj H. (2010). Predictors of sexual risk behavior in a five-year perspective: The role of psychosocial distress and hedonism. AIDS Education and Prevention, 22, 148–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen P., & Jacobson K. C. (2012). Developmental trajectories of substance use from early adolescence to young adulthood: Gender and racial/ethnic differences. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 154–163. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.05.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Franza J. R., Savageau J. A., Rigotti N. A., Fletcher K. E., Ockene J. K., McNeill A. D., et al. Wood C. (2002). Development of symptoms of tobacco dependence in youths: 30 month follow up data from the DANDY study. Tobacco Control, 11, 228–235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duangpatra K. N. K., Bradley G. L., & Glendon A. I. (2009). Variables affecting emerging adults’ self-reported risk and reckless behaviors. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 298–309. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.011 [Google Scholar]
- Fergus S., Zimmerman M. A., & Caldwell C. H. (2007). Sexual risk behavior in adolescence and young adulthood. American Journal of Public Health, 97, 1096–1101. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2005.074609 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franzkowiak P. (1987). Risk-taking and adolescent development: The functions of smoking and alcohol consumption in adolescence and its consequences for prevention. Health Promotion, 2(1), 51–61.22208515 [Google Scholar]
- Hammond D. (2005). Smoking behaviour among young adults: Beyond youth prevention. Tobacco Control, 14, 181–185. doi:10.1136/tc.2004.009621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hurrelmann K., & Richter M. (2006). Risk behaviour in adolescence: The relationship between developmental and health problems. Journal of Public Health, 14, 20–28. doi:10.1007/s10389-005-0005-5 [Google Scholar]
- Jackson K. M., Sher K. J., & Schulenberg J. E. (2008). Conjoint developmental trajectories of young adult substance use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32, 723–737. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00643.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jessor R., & Jessor S. L. (1977). Problem behavior and psychosocial development: A longitudinal study of youth. New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Johnston L. D., O’Malley P. M., Bachman J. G., & Schulenberg J. E. (2012). Monitoring the future: National results on adolescent drug use: Overview of key findings, 2011. Retrieved from http://www.monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/monographs/mtf-overview2011.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Kan M. L., Cheng Y. A., Landale N. S., & McHale S. M. (2010). Longitudinal predictors of change in number of sexual partners across adolescence and early adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 25–31. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee C., Mun E., White H. R., & Simon P. (2010).: Substance use trajectories of Black and White young men from adolescence to emerging adulthood: A two-part growth curve analysis. Journal of Ethnicity in Substance Abuse, 9, 301–319. doi:10.1080/15332640.2010.522898 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maggs J. L., & Schulenberg J. E. (2004–2005). Trajectories of alcohol use during the transition to adulthood. Alcohol Research & Health, 28, 195–201. [Google Scholar]
- Mason W. A., Hitch J. E., Kosterman R., McCarty C. A., Herrenkohl T. I., & Hawkins J. D. (2010). Growth in adolescent delinquency and alcohol use in relation to young adult crime, alcohol use disorders, and risky sex: A comparison of youth from low- versus middle-income backgrounds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1377–1385. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02292.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt T. E. (2006). Life-course-persistent versus adolescence-limited antisocial behavior. In Cicchetti D. & Cohen D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adaptation. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Muthén B., & Muthén L. (1998–2011). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Author. [Google Scholar]
- Perkonigg A., Goodwin R. D., Fiedler A., Behrendt S., Beesdo K., Lieb R., & Wittchen H.-U. (2008). The natural course of cannabis use, abuse and dependence during the first decades of life. Addiction, 103, 439–449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piasecki T. M. (2006). Relapse to smoking. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 196–215. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher K. J.,Wichman A. L.,MacCallum R. C., & Briggs N. E. (2008). Latent growth curve modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Steiger J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 173–180. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terry-McElrath Y. M., & O’Malley P. M. (2011). Substance use and exercise participation among young adults: Parallel trajectories in a national cohort-sequential study. Addiction, 106, 1855–1865. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03489.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker L. R., & Lewis C. (1973). Reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor-analysis. Psychometrika, 38, 1–10. doi:10.1007/BF02291170 [Google Scholar]