Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2013 Aug 5;110(35):E3255. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1309838110

Reply to Mahowald and Gibson and to Heggarty: No problems with short words, and no evidence provided

Mark Pagel a,b,1, Quentin D Atkinson c, Andreea S Calude d, Andrew Meade a
PMCID: PMC3761576  PMID: 24137736

Mahowald and Gibson (1) suggest that the shorter word length of frequently used words, and not their stability, could mean that chance sound correspondences account for the pattern of results we report for cognate relationships among proto-words in our study of seven Eurasian language families (2). However, their −0.24 correlation between the phonological length of contemporary English words and our measure of cognate class size is not relevant to the question of chance sound correspondences among the proto-words from different language families. What must be demonstrated is that shorter proto-words were more likely to be judged cognate simply on the basis of their length.

In this regard, we find no empirical evidence that phonological word length influences judgments of cognacy among the proto-words in our sample. For example, we find no relationship between cognate class size and the phonological length of Indo-European proto-words, controlling for the words’ frequency of use and their rates of lexical replacement in the Indo-European languages (P = 0.56). On the other hand, and in agreement with results we report in our paper, rates of lexical replacement do predict cognate class size, after controlling for phonological word length and frequency of use (P = 0.0003). We found similar patterns for the six other language families we studied.

Heggarty (3) makes a number of assertions about the Languages of the World Etymological Database (LWED) and the impossibility of identifying deep ancestry, but does not provide empirical evidence to support his claims. This repeats the major obstacle to progress in the field—a reliance on assertions rather than clear statistical evidence. Providing such evidence was precisely the starting point of our paper: our previous research (4) empirically demonstrated the possibility of a small subset of words retaining traces of their ancestry long enough to connect languages or language families separated by more than 10,000 y. We then incorporated that work into a statistical model that makes predictions about the words most likely to show evidence of ancestry among the seven Eurasian language families, and these predictions were supported.

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1. Mahowald K, Gibson E (2013) Short, frequent words are more likely to appear genetically related by chance. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:E3253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 2.Pagel M, Atkinson QD, S Calude A, Meade A. Ultraconserved words point to deep language ancestry across Eurasia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(21):8471–8476. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1218726110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Heggarty P (2013) Ultraconserved words and Eurasiatic? The “faces in the fire” of language prehistory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:E3254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 4.Pagel M, Atkinson QD, Meade A. Frequency of word-use predicts rates of lexical evolution throughout Indo-European history. Nature. 2007;449(7163):717–720. doi: 10.1038/nature06176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES