Abstract
Objectives and Methods. The aim of the present study was to evaluate, by means of receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves, whether cognitive vulnerabilities (CV), as measured by three well-known instruments (the Beck Hopelessness Scale, BHS; the Life Orientation Test-Revised, LOT-R; and the Attitudes Toward Self-Revised, ATS-R), independently discriminate between subjects with different severities of depression. Participants were 467 young adults (336 females and 131 males), recruited from the general population. The subjects were also administered the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). Results. Four first-order (BHS Optimism/Low Standard; BHS Pessimism; Generalized Self-Criticism; and LOT Optimism) and two higher-order factors (Pessimism/Negative Attitudes Toward Self, Optimism) were extracted using Principal Axis Factoring analysis. Although all first-order and second-order factors were able to discriminate individuals with different depression severities, the Pessimism factor had the best performance in discriminating individuals with moderate to severe depression from those with lower depression severity. Conclusion. In the screening of young adults at risk of depression, clinicians have to pay particular attention to the expression of pessimism about the future.
1. Introduction
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is one of the most widespread psychiatric disorder [1–4] and the leading cause of disability as measured by years lived with disability (YLDs) [5–9]. Currently, MDD is estimated to be the fourth leading cause of global disease burden [10, 11], and, by the year 2020, it is projected to reach the second place in the ranking of the major causes of Disability Adjusted Life Years (DALYs). Today, depression is already the second cause of DALYs in the age category 15–44 years [11]. The estimated 12-month prevalence of MDD was 6.6% in the USA [12] and 3.0% in Italy [13].
Many approaches have been taken in attempts to explain the origins of depression [14]. Whereas some of these theories involved genetics and biological functioning, other approaches focused on the study of personal characteristics of individuals who are believed to be vulnerable to experiencing depressive episodes. A large body of research examining cognitive models of vulnerability for depression hypothesized that the way the individual interprets his/her experiences represents a protective or risk factor for the development of depressive disorders when negative stressful life events occur [15]. Depressed people engage in prolonged and repetitive thinking about the self, the world, and the future in a negative way with detrimental effects on mood [16–18]. Cognitive vulnerability (CV) for depression may be defined as a trait-like tendency to interpret information in a negative and distorted way when facing a subjectively perceived stressful event [19]. The literature indicated that CV may play a crucial role in the development and maintenance of depressive disorders not only in adulthood but also in childhood and adolescence (for a review see [20]).
Despite similarities, currently there are several different theories which hypothesize the cognitive processes to be a diathesis for the development of depressive disorders. Two of the most renowned theories are the hopelessness theory [21] and Beck's theory [22, 23]. According to the hopelessness theory, three kinds of maladaptive inferences that people may make when confronted with negative events contribute to the development of hopelessness and, in turn, depressive symptoms: causal attributions, inferred consequences, and inferred characteristics about the self. Hopelessness and depressive symptoms are likely to occur when negative life events are (1) attributed to stable (i.e., likely to persist over time) and global (i.e., likely to affect many areas of life) causes; (2) viewed as likely to lead to further negative consequences; and (3) construed as implying that the person is unworthy or deficient. Similarly, Beck [22, 23] and Beck et al. [24] hypothesized depressogenic self-schemata, activated by the occurrence of negative life events, that take the form of overly pessimistic views of the self, the world, and the future (the negative cognitive triad). Also, Carver and Ganellen [25] considered self-punitiveness as a salient feature of depression, associated with the holding of overly high standards, the tendency to be too critical with the self for failing to attain a standard, and the tendency to generalize from a single failure to the broader sense of self-worth. To measure these three potential self-regulatory vulnerabilities to depression, Carver and colleagues developed the Attitudes Toward Self (ATS).
Several researches have provided support for the critical role of CV in the origin and course of depressive disorders [26–30]. For example, Beckham et al. [26] reported that all the cognitive triad components are significantly associated with the severity of depressive symptoms in depressed patients. Evans et al. [28] indicated that holding a negative self-schema is an independent risk factor for the onset of depression in women. Furthermore, pessimistic views about the future have been found to be highly predictive of suicide behaviors [31, 32] and worse health and social functioning [33] in psychiatric patients. Again, in a longitudinal study, it was reported that undergraduate students with a more generalized negative cognitive style were 3.5–6.8 times more likely to report more depressive episodes compared to students with a less generalized negative cognitive style [29]. Some studies also indicated a partial overlap between the hopelessness and Beck's theories [34, 35].
Our literature review suggests that investigating the role of CV in the development of depression may be beneficial for a better understanding and treatment of depressive disorders. What is more is that research has failed to investigate the role of CV for depression in discriminating different levels of severity of depression. Thus, the aim of the present study was to evaluate, by means of ROC curves, whether some dimensions of CV for depression, as measured by three well-known instruments, that are the Beck Hopelessness Scale (BHS) [24], the Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R) [36], and the Attitudes Toward Self-Revised (ATS-R) [37], discriminate independently between subjects with different severities of depression.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
Participants were 467 young adults (131 males, 28.1%; 336 females, 71.9%). Mean age of the sample was 23.58 ± 5.03 years (range 18–39). Subjects were included if they had an age between 18 and 40 years old. Exclusion criteria were the presence of any condition affecting the ability to complete the assessment, including illiteracy and denial of informed consent.
All participants were nonrandomly recruited in Central Italy between February 2012 and June 2012. They accepted to participate voluntarily and gave their informed consent.
2.2. Measures
The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [38], the BHS, the LOT-R, and the ATS-R were administered to all subjects.
The BDI-II is a well-known self-report inventory composed of 21 items designed to assess the presence and severity of depressive symptoms, according to DSM-IV [39] criteria. Respondents endorse specific statements reflecting their feelings over the last two weeks, including today. Each statement is rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 to 3, based on the severity of depressive symptoms. Importantly, extensive literature has supported the psychometric properties of the scale in clinical and nonclinical samples [40, 41]. In the current sample, Cronbach alpha was 0.86.
The BHS [24] is a 20-item scale for measuring the cognitive component of the depression. The scale assesses three major aspects of hopelessness: feelings about the future, loss of motivation, and expectations. Responding to the 20 true or false items, individuals have to either endorse a pessimistic statement or deny an optimistic statement. Research consistently have supported a positive relationship between BHS scores and measures of depression, suicidal intent, and current suicidal ideation [31, 42–44]. The Italian version of the BHS has reported good psychometric properties [45, 46].
The LOT-R is a 6-item scale measuring optimism: three items are positively worded (e.g., “I'm always optimistic about my future”), and the other three items are negatively worded (e.g., “If something can go wrong for me, it will”). Each item is rated on a five-point Likert-type scale (from 1—“I agree a lot”—to 5—“I disagree a lot”). Sum scores range between 6 and 30 with higher scores indicating higher optimism.
The ATS-R is a self-report measure designed to measure three potential self-regulatory vulnerabilities to depression. One of them is the holding of overly high standards, the second is the tendency to be self-critical at any failure to perform well, and the third is the tendency to generalize from a single failure to the broader sense of self-worth. The negative generalization scale has been found to be the dimension more strongly associated with depression [37, 47, 48]. Items are rated on a Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (“I agree a lot”) to 5 (“I disagree a lot”). A study assessing the psychometric properties of the Italian version of the ATS-R has been recently published, indicating that the measure is a valid instrument for the study of the role of cognitive tendencies as potential diathesis in the development of depression [49].
2.3. Statistical Analysis
The BHS, LOT-R, and ATS-R items were subjected to Principal Axis Factoring in order to obtain common factors of vulnerability for depression. The number of latent factors to retain was selected using the scree test [50] and confirmed by means of Velicer's Minimum Average Partial (MAP) test [51]. Promax rotation with Kaiser Normalization was used in order to produce correlated factors. According to Kline [52], items were retained if they had a factor loading of 0.40 and higher and if they were loaded on a single factor. Then, factor scores for all subjects were calculated and used to assess the presence of possible higher-order factors.
A generalized linear model with robust estimator was used to assess independent associations between depression severity and factors of vulnerability for depression. Associations were reported as odds ratio and their 95% confidence intervals (95% CI).
In order to assess the performance of the dimensions of vulnerability for depression in categorizing individuals on the basis of depression severity, a series of ROC test procedures were performed [53].
All analyses were performed with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS 17.0 for Windows).
3. Results
Descriptive statistics are listed in Table 1. The mean BDI-II score was 10.40 ± 8.36 (quartiles [25°/50°/75°]: 4/9/15), and 12.5% of the sample had scores of 20 and higher indicating moderate to severe depressive symptoms, consistent with the nonclinical nature of the sample.
Table 1.
% | |
---|---|
Men | 28.2 |
Age—M ± SD | 23.59 ± 5.03 |
Job | |
Employed | 32.9 |
Students | 63.7 |
Unemployed | 3.4 |
Marital status | |
Not married | 87.8 |
Married | 10.8 |
Divorced | 1.4 |
Education | |
5 years | 0.6 |
8 years | 12.3 |
13 years | 72.0 |
16+ years | 15.1 |
BDI-II—M ± SD | 10.40 ± 8.36 |
BDI ≥ 20 | 12.5 |
The scree test and Velicer's MAP test indicated a factorial solution with four factors, explaining 47% of the variability of the data (Table 2). Nine items were loaded on the first factor (BHS Optimism), which explained 19.2% of the variance (eigenvalue = 6.92), with loadings ranging from 0.53 [BHS item no. 5] to 0.88 [BHS item no. 19]: 8 items from the BHS endorsing an optimistic statement and 1 item from the ATS-R denying higher standard for the self. On the second factor (BHS Pessimism), which explained 15.7% of the total variance (eigenvalue = 5.65), 9 items presented loadings ranging from 0.43 [BHS item no. 14] to 0.75 [BHS item no. 17]: all items were from the BHS and endorsed a pessimistic statement. On the third factor (Generalized Self-Criticism), which explained 7.3% of the variance (eigenvalue = 2.61), 4 items had loadings ranging from 0.62 [ATS-R item no. 9] to 0.75 [ATS-R item no. 10]: all items were extracted from the original Generalization and Self-Criticism factors of the ATS-R. On the fourth factor (LOT-R Optimism), which explained 4.8% of the variance (eigenvalue = 1.74), 3 items had loadings ranging from −0.51 [LOT-R item no. 10] to −0.69 [LOT-R item no. 4]: the items were from the LOT-R, and the individual had to endorse an optimistic statement. ATS-R items no. 3 and no. 6 loaded on two factors (factors no. 1 and no. 3) and were excluded from the solution. All factors were rated so that higher scores measured the presence of pessimism or negative attitudes toward the self or denied the presence of optimism. Correlation between factors ranged between −0.18, for the association between BHS Optimism and Generalized Self-Criticism, and 0.45, for the association between BHS Optimism and LOT-R Optimism.
Table 2.
Factor | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
BHS Optimism | BHS Pessimism | Generalized Self-Criticism | LOT Optimism | |
BHS no. 1 | 0.67 | — | — | — |
BHS no. 2 | — | 0.53 | — | — |
BHS no. 3 | 0.75 | — | — | — |
BHS no. 4 | — | — | — | — |
BHS no. 5 | 0.53 | — | — | — |
BHS no. 6 | 0.82 | — | — | — |
BHS no. 7 | — | 0.69 | — | — |
BHS no. 8 | — | — | — | — |
BHS no. 9 | — | 0.61 | — | — |
BHS no. 10 | 0.66 | — | — | — |
BHS no. 11 | — | 0.68 | — | — |
BHS no. 12 | — | 0.49 | — | — |
BHS no. 13 | 0.70 | — | — | — |
BHS no. 14 | — | 0.43 | — | — |
BHS no. 15 | 0.55 | — | — | |
BHS no. 16 | — | 0.74 | — | — |
BHS no. 17 | — | 0.75 | — | — |
BHS no. 18 | — | — | — | — |
BHS no. 19 | 0.88 | — | — | |
BHS no. 20 | — | 0.70 | — | — |
ATS no. 1 | −0.74 | — | — | — |
ATS no. 2 | — | — | 0.69 | — |
ATS no. 3 | −0.55 | — | 0.47 | — |
ATS no. 4 | — | — | — | — |
ATS no. 5 | — | — | — | — |
ATS no. 6 | −0.50 | — | 0.53 | — |
ATS no. 7 | — | — | — | — |
ATS no. 8 | — | — | 0.66 | — |
ATS no. 9 | — | — | 0.62 | — |
ATS no. 10 | — | — | 0.75 | — |
LOT no. 1 | — | — | — | −0.62 |
LOT no. 3 | — | — | — | — |
LOT no. 4 | — | — | — | −0.69 |
LOT no. 7 | — | — | — | — |
LOT no. 9 | — | — | — | — |
LOT no. 10 | — | — | — | −0.51 |
| ||||
Cronbach alpha | 0.84 | 0.84 | 0.78 | 0.70 |
Extraction method: Principal Axis Factoring. Rotation method: Promax with Kaiser Normalization.
Factor scores were entered in a second-order factor analysis to extract higher-order common factors. The analysis resulted in two factors with eigenvalues >1. On the first factor (Optimism), which explained 43.3% of the variance (eigenvalue = 1.73), first-order factors LOT-R Optimism (0.80) and BHS Optimism (0.72) loaded. On the second factor (Pessimism/Negative attitudes toward self), which explained 34.0% of the variance (eigenvalue = 1.36), first-order factors BHS Pessimism (0.62) and Generalized Self-Criticism (0.69) loaded. The factors were weakly correlated with each other (r = 0.20).
A generalized linear model with robust estimator was performed to assess whether the factors measuring CV for depression were independently associated with the BDI-II (Table 3). The model fitted the data well (Likelihood Ratio χ 4 2 = 200.03; P < 0.001). The four factors were inserted as independent variables in the analysis and the BDI-II as criterion. Only the BHS Pessimism and the LOT-R Optimism were independently associated with BDI-II scores, indicating that (1) subjects with higher scores on the BHS Pessimism were 52.4 times more likely to have higher BDI-II scores (95% CI: 17.3/158.4; P < 0.001) compared with those reported having lower scores and (2) people with higher scores on the LOT-R Optimism were 9.1 times more likely to have higher BDI-II scores (95% CI: 3.9/21.6; P < 0.001) compared with people having higher scores. A second generalized linear model was performed with higher order factors as independent variables (not reported in the tables). The model fitted the data well (Likelihood Ratio χ 2 2 = 170.31; P < 0.001). Both the higher-order factors were independently associated with the BDI-II: (1) subjects with higher scores on Optimism were 10.1 times more likely to have higher BDI-II scores (95% CI: 4.7/21.5; beta = 2.31 [std. error = 0.39]; P < 0.001) compared with those having lower scores; and (2) people with higher scores on Pessimism/Negative Attitudes Toward Self were 91.1 times more likely to have higher BDI-II scores (95% CI: 32.7/254.3; beta = 4.51 [std. error = 0.52]; P < 0.001) than people reported having lower scores.
Table 3.
Parameter | Beta | Std. error | OR | 95% Wald confidence interval for OR | P< | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | |||||
BHS Optimism | 0.12 | 0.36 | 1.13 | 0.55 | 2.31 | 0.74 |
BHS Pessimism | 3.96 | 0.56 | 52.35 | 17.31 | 158.37 | <0.001 |
Generalized Self-Criticism | 0.33 | 0.47 | 1.39 | 0.56 | 3.47 | 0.48 |
LOT Optimism | 2.21 | 0.44 | 9.13 | 3.87 | 21.56 | <0.001 |
Likelihood Ratio χ 2 4 = 200.03; P < 0.001; AIC = 3013.74; AICC = 3013.93; Pearson χ 2 448 = 20017.73; value/DF = 44.68.
A series of ROC curves indicated that first-order and second-order factors of vulnerability for depression were able to discriminate individuals with different depression severities (Table 4). The performance of the BHS Optimism was quite stable at any level of depression. A randomly chosen individual had 57%–60% of probability to have higher scores on the BHS Optimism compared with a randomly chosen individual with lower levels of depression. Generalized Self-Criticism performance was the best when discriminating individuals with moderate to severe depression from other individuals: at these levels of depression, a randomly chosen individual with moderate to severe depression (BDI-II ≥ 20) had 67% of probability to have higher scores on Generalized Self-Criticism than a randomly chosen individual with lower levels of depression. The performance of the BHS Pessimism increased linearly from lower levels (BDI-II cutoffs of 5 and 9: AUC [area under the curve] = 0.72–0.74) to higher levels of depression (AUC of 0.81 and 0.87, resp., for BDI-II cutoffs of 15 and 19). The performance of the LOT-R Optimism was similar for lower cutoffs (AUC of 0.69 and 0.70, resp., for BDI-II cutoffs of 5 and 9) and higher cutoffs (AUC of 0.74 for BDI-II cutoffs of 15 and 19). When analyzing the performance of the second-order factors, a randomly chosen individual had 66%–72% of probability to have higher scores on Optimism than a randomly chosen individual with lower levels of depression, while a randomly chosen individual had 68%–81% of probability to have higher scores on Pessimism/Negative Attitudes Toward Self than a randomly chosen individual with lower levels of depression. Thus, the presence of Pessimism and Negative Attitudes Toward Self was more useful in discriminating individuals with moderate to severe depression than denying an optimistic one. However, the BHS Pessimism appeared to have the best performance in discriminating individuals at any level of depression severity.
Table 4.
Variables | Area under the curve | Std. error | Asymptotic sig. |
---|---|---|---|
BDI-II > 5 | |||
BHS Optimism | 0.57 | 0.03 | 0.05 |
BHS Pessimism | 0.72 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
Generalized Self-Criticism | 0.58 | 0.03 | 0.01 |
LOT Optimism | 0.69 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
Optimism | 0.66 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
Pessimism/Negative Attitudes Toward Self | 0.68 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
BDI-II > 9 | |||
BHS Optimism | 0.58 | 0.03 | 0.01 |
BHS Pessimism | 0.74 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Generalized Self-Criticism | 0.59 | 0.03 | 0.001 |
LOT Optimism | 0.70 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Optimism | 0.67 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
Pessimism/Negative Attitudes Toward Self | 0.70 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
BDI-II > 15 | |||
BHS Optimism | 0.60 | 0.03 | 0.01 |
BHS Pessimism | 0.81 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Generalized Self-Criticism | 0.60 | 0.04 | 0.01 |
LOT Optimism | 0.74 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
Optimism | 0.72 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
Pessimism/Negative Attitudes Toward Self | 0.75 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
BDI-II > 19 | |||
BHS Optimism | 0.59 | 0.03 | 0.05 |
BHS Pessimism | 0.87 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Generalized Self-Criticism | 0.67 | 0.04 | <0.001 |
LOT Optimism | 0.74 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
Optimism | 0.71 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
Pessimism/Negative Attitudes Toward Self | 0.81 | 0.03 | <0.001 |
4. Discussion
The aim of the present study was to evaluate, by means of ROC curves, whether CV for depression, as measured by three well-known instruments, may independently discriminate between subjects with different depression severities.
Our results indicated that the instruments we administered measure four common vulnerabilities for depression: (1) denying optimism/endorsing high standards (BHS/ATS-R); (2) endorsing pessimism (BHS); (3) generalizing self-criticism (ATS-R); and (4) denying optimism (LOT-R). These dimensions are loaded on two second-order factors: (1) denying optimism and (2) endorsing pessimism and generalizing self-criticism.
In our sample of young adults, the assessment of pessimism with the BHS, compared with other factors, had the best performance in discriminating individuals with different levels of depression severity, and its performance improved from lower cutoffs to higher cutoffs of depression. A randomly chosen individual with moderate to severe depression had 87% of probability to have higher BHS Pessimism compared with a randomly chosen individual with lower levels of depression.
Pessimism was also independently associated with depression severity while controlling for other dimensions of vulnerability for depression. People having more severe pessimism were above 52 times more at risk to have higher depression compared with people with milder pessimism. This finding is consistent with the studies indicating a strong link between negative beliefs about the future and depression [54–58]. For example, Strunk et al. [56], investigating the relationship between depressive symptoms and bias in future event prediction, documented that individuals with elevated depressive symptoms showed a pessimistic bias in making predictions about the outcomes of future life events by overpredicting that undesirable events would happen to them and underpredicting that desirable events would happen to them. Alford et al. [59], in a sample of university students, found that in males hopelessness may predict specifically future depression severity but not anxiety.
From a psychometric point of view, our results may be related to the differences in the methods employed in the three scales we administered to measure CV for depression: (1) the BHS uses dichotomous format of response, while for the LOT-R and the ATS-R the respondents are asked to rate each item on a Likert-type scale; (2) both the BHS and LOT-R ask the individuals to rate positive items which are directly associated with the construct they measure and negative statements which are negatively associated with these constructs. Furthermore, while the BHS is considered a measure of pessimism, the LOT-R is generally considered a measure of optimism [60].
Scheier and Carver [60], investigating construct validity of the LOT-R, reported the fit of a two-factor model: one factor contained positively worded items and the other included negatively worded items. The authors considered that the two-factor solution was probably due to item wording rather than content item [60]. Indeed, responses, particularly to the positively and negatively worded items, reflected a complicated combination of substantively meaningful trait effects and apparently idiosyncratic method effects associated with the wording of particular items [61–63].
Nevertheless, in the last decades several authors reported results supporting the hypothesis that Optimism and Pessimism are two distinct constructs with different patterns of correlations with other psychological constructs [64–70]. Recently, similar results have been found for the BHS: in a sample of medical patients, a bifactor model was the best-fitting solution and the most parsimonious among models evaluated [71]. Results from the higher-order factor analysis we performed support the hypothesis that the two factors often extracted from factorizing LOT-R and BHS items are not an artifact due to item wording and may have different roles as factors of vulnerability for depression.
Finally, our study has some limitations. First, above 60% of our sample were composed of university students and may not be representative of the Italian population, so that generalizability of the results to older people and to individuals with low school attainment is not suggested. Second, our sample is nonclinical and only 12.5% of the subjects reported having moderate to severe depression at the BDI-II, so that our results may not be replicable in clinical samples, even though cognitive theories of depression did not predict differences in the associations of cognitive variables with depressive symptoms between clinical and nonclinical samples [21, 72, 73]. Third, the measures used in the present study were limited to only paper-and-pencil self-report instruments and we did not administered clinician-rated scales to measure depression, as recommended elsewhere [74]. Thus, our results may be biased by social desirability [75, 76]. Fourth, we did not assess participants for family history of mood disorders, which may have predisposed individuals to develop future depression and negative cognitive biases. Nevertheless, there are several noteworthy strengths, including the large size of sample and the use of three different measures often used to assess cognitive vulnerability for depression.
In conclusion, when screening young adults at risk for depression, clinicians have to pay particular attention to expressions of pessimism about the future, which may discriminate well individuals with moderate to severe depression from individuals reported having lower depression severity. Furthermore, at these levels of depression severity, Generalized Self-Criticism is less effective in discriminating individuals at risk for depression compared not only with statements of pessimism but also with statements denying optimism.
Conflict of Interests
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests for this research.
Authors' Contribution
All authors have made substantial contributions to conception and design or acquisition of data or analysis and interpretation of data, have been involved in drafting the paper or revising it critically for important intellectual content, and have given final approval of the version to be published.
References
- 1.Blanco C, Okuda M, Markowitz JC, Liu S-M, Grant BF, Hasin DS. The epidemiology of chronic major depressive disorder and dysthymic disorder: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2010;71(12):1645–1656. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05663gry. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hasin DS, Goodwin RD, Stinson FS, Grant BF. Epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcoholism and Related Conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(10):1097–1106. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.10.1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lehtinen V, Joukamaa M. Epidemiology of depression: prevalence, risk factors and treatment situation. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Supplement. 1994;89(377):7–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1994.tb05794.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ohayon MM. Epidemiology of depression and its treatment in the general population. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2007;41(3-4):207–213. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Haslam C, Atkinson S, Brown SS, Haslam RA. Anxiety and depression in the workplace: effects on the individual and organisation (a focus group investigation) Journal of Affective Disorders. 2005;88(2):209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Shippee ND, Shah ND, Williams MD, Moriarty JP, Frye MA, Ziegenfuss JY. Differences in demographic composition and in work, social, and functional limitations among the populations with unipolar depression and bipolar disorder: results from a nationally representative sample. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2011;9(article 90) doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-9-90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wang PS, Beck AL, Berglund P, et al. Effects of major depression on moment-in-time work performance. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161(10):1885–1891. doi: 10.1176/ajp.161.10.1885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Whitton SW, Whisman MA. Relationship satisfaction instability and depression. Journal of Family Psychology. 2010;24(6):791–794. doi: 10.1037/a0021734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Woo J-M, Kim W, Hwang T-Y, et al. Impact of depression on work productivity and its improvement after outpatient treatment with antidepressants. Value in Health. 2011;14(4):475–482. doi: 10.1016/j.jval.2010.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ustun TB, Ayuso-Mateos JL, Chatterji S, Mathers C, Murray CJL. Global burden of depressive disorders in the year 2000. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;184:386–392. doi: 10.1192/bjp.184.5.386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.World Health Organisation. Depression. What is depression? 2011, http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/depression/en/
- 12.Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, et al. The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;289(23):3095–3105. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.23.3095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.de Girolamo G, Polidori G, Morosini P, et al. La prevalenza dei disturbi mentali in italia. Il progetto ESEMED-WMH. Una sintesi. Centro Nazionale di Epidemiologia, Sorveglianza e Promozione della Salute, Roma, Italia, 2005.
- 14.Gotlib IH, Hammen CL. Handbook of Depressioned. 2nd edition. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Whitehouse WG, et al. Depressogenic cognitive styles: predictive validity, information processing and personality characteristics, and developmental origins. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1999;37(6):503–531. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00157-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Watkins ER. Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134(2):163–206. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Balsamo M. Anger and depression: evidence of a possible mediating role for rumination. Psychological Reports. 2010;106(1):3–12. doi: 10.2466/PR0.106.1.3-12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Koster EHW, De Lissnyder E, Derakshan N, De Raedt R. Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2011;31(1):138–145. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Zhong M, Wang X, Xiao J, et al. Amygdala hyperactivation and prefrontal hypoactivation in subjects with cognitive vulnerability to depression. Biological Psychology. 2011;88(2-3):233–242. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jacobs RH, Reinecke MA, Gollan JK, Kane P. Empirical evidence of cognitive vulnerability for depression among children and adolescents: a cognitive science and developmental perspective. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28(5):759–782. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Abramson LY, Metalsky GI, Alloy LB. Hopelessness depression: a theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review. 1989;96(2):358–372. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Beck AT. Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York, NY, USA: Harper & Row; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Beck AT. Cognitive models of depression. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy. 1987;1:5–37. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Beck AT, Weissman A, Lester D, Trexler L. The measurement of pessimism: the hopelessness scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1974;42(6):861–865. doi: 10.1037/h0037562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Carver CS, Ganellen RJ. Depression and components of self-punitiveness: high standards, self-criticism, and overgeneralization. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1983;92(3):330–337. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.92.3.330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Beckham EE, Leber WR, Watkins JT, Boyer JL, Cook JB. Development of an instrument to measure Beck's cognitive triad: the Cognitive Triad Inventory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1986;54(4):566–567. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.54.4.566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Abramson LY, Alloy LB, Hankin BL, Haeffel GJ, MacCoon DG, Gibb BE. Cognitive vulnerability-stress models of depression in a self-regulatory and psychobiological context. In: Gotlib IH, Hammen CL, editors. Handbook of Depression. New York, NY, USA: The Guilford Press; 2002. pp. 268–294. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Evans J, Heron J, Lewis G, Araya R, Wolke D. Negative self-schemas and the onset of depression in women: longitudinal study. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;186:302–307. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.4.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Whitehouse WG, Hogan ME, Panzarella C, Rose DT. Prospective incidence of first onsets and recurrences of depression in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115(1):145–156. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Abela JRZ, Aydin C, Auerbach RP. Operationalizing the “vulnerability” and “stress” components of the hopelessness theory of depression: a multi-wave longitudinal study. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006;44(11):1565–1583. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Beck AT, Steer RA, Kovacs M, Garrison B. Hopelessness and eventual suicide: a 10-year prospective study of patients hospitalized with suicidal ideation. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1985;142(5):559–563. doi: 10.1176/ajp.142.5.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Beck AT, Brown G, Berchick RJ, Stewart BL, Steer RA. Relationship between hopelessness and ultimate suicide: a replication with psychiatric outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1990;147(2):190–195. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Pompili M, Innamorati M, Gonda X, et al. Affective temperaments and hopelessness as predictors of health and social general functioning in mood disorders patients: a prospective follow-up study. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Metalsky GI, Joiner TE., Jr. Vulnerability to depressive symptomatology: a prospective test of the diathesis-stress and causal mediation components of the hopelessness theory of depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;63(4):667–675. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.63.4.667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Pössel P, Thomas SD. Cognitive triad as mediator in the hopelessness model? a three-wave longitudinal study. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2011;67(3):224–240. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW. Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67(6):1063–1078. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.67.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Carver CS, La Voie L, Kuhl J, Ganellen RJ. Cognitive concomitants of depression: a further examination of the roles of generalization, high standards, and self-criticism. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 1988;7:350–365. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, Tex, USA: Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 39.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edition. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Arbisi PA. Review of the Beck depression inventory-II. In: Plake BS, Impara JC, editors. The Fourteenth Mental Measurements Yearbook. Lincoln, Neb, USA: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Balsamo M, Saggino A. Test per l’assessment della depressione nel contesto italiano: un’analisi critica. Psicoterapia Cognitiva e Comportamentale. 2007;13:167–199. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Beck AT, Steer RA, Beck JS, Newman CF. Hopelessness, depression, suicidal ideation, and clinical diagnosis of depression. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 1993;23(2):139–145. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Klonsky ED, Kotov R, Bakst S, Rabinowitz J, Bromet EJ. Hopelessness as a predictor of attempted suicide among first admission patients with psychosis: a 10-year cohort study. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2012;42(1):1–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.2011.00066.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Innamorati M, Pompili M, Gonda X, et al. Psychometric properties of the Gotland Scale for Depression in Italian psychiatric inpatients and its utility in the prediction of suicide risk. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011;132(1-2):99–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Pompili M, Iliceto P, Lester D, et al. BHS Beck Hopelessness Scale. Florence, Italy: O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pompili M, Tatarelli R, Rogers JR, Lester D. The hopelessness scale: a factor analysis. Psychological Reports. 2007;100(2):375–378. doi: 10.2466/pr0.100.2.375-378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Carver CS. Generalization, adverse events, and development of depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality. 1998;66(4):606–619. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Eisner LR, Johnson SL, Carver CS. Cognitive responses to failure and success relate uniquely to bipolar depression versus mania. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117(1):154–163. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Innamorati M, Tamburello S, Contardi A, et al. Psychometric properties of the attitudes toward self-revised in Italian young adults. Depression Research and Treatment. 2013;2013:6 pages. doi: 10.1155/2013/209216.209216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Cattell RB. The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1966;1:245–276. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr0102_10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Velicer WF. Determining the number of components from the matrix of partial correlations. Psychometrika. 1976;41(3):321–327. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kline P. A Handbook of Test Construction: Introduction to Psychometric Design. London, UK: Methuen; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Gleitman H. Psychologyed. 2nd edition. New York, NY, USA: W.W. Norton; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Alloy LB, Ahrens AH. Depression and pessimism for the future: biased use of statistically relevant information in predictions for self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;52(2):366–378. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.2.366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lavender A, Watkins E. Rumination and future thinking in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2004;43(2):129–142. doi: 10.1348/014466504323088015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Strunk DR, Lopez H, DeRubeis RJ. Depressive symptoms are associated with unrealistic negative predictions of future life events. Behaviour research and therapy. 2006;44(6):861–882. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Beck AT, Riskind JH, Brown G, Steer RA. Levels of hopelessness in DSM-III disorders: a partial test of content specificity in depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1988;12(5):459–469. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Strunk DR, Adler AD. Cognitive biases in three prediction tasks: a test of the cognitive model of depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2009;47(1):34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Alford BA, Lester JM, Patel RJ, Buchanan JP, Giunta LC. Hopelessness predicts future depressive symptoms: a prospective analysis of cognitive vulnerability and cognitive content specificity. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1995;51(3):331–339. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199505)51:3<331::aid-jclp2270510303>3.0.co;2-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Scheier MF, Carver CS. Optimism, coping, and health: assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology. 1985;4(3):219–247. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.4.3.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Balsamo M, Romanelli R, Innamorati M, Ciccarese G, Carlucci L, Saggino A. The state-trait anxiety inventory: shadows and lights on its construct validity. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2013 [Google Scholar]
- 62.Cronbach LJ. Response sets and test validity. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 1946;6:475–494. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Millham J, Kellogg RW. Need for social approval: impression management or self-deception? Journal of Research in Personality. 1980;14(4):445–457. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Marshall GN, Wortman CB, Kusulas JW, Hervig LK, Vickers RR., Jr. Distinguishing optimism from pessimism: relations to fundamental dimensions of mood and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;62(6):1067–1074. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Mroczek DK, Spiro A, III, Aldwin CM, Ozer DJ, Bossé R. Construct validation of optimism and pessimism in older men: findings from the normative aging study. Health Psychology. 1993;12(5):406–409. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.12.5.406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Lai JC. Differential predictive power of the positively versus the negatively worded items of the Life Orientation Test. Psychological Reports. 1994;75(3):1507–1515. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1994.75.3f.1507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Robinson-Whelen S, Kim C, MacCallum RC, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Distinguishing optimism from pessimism in older adults: is it more important to be optimistic or not to be pessimistic? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;73(6):1345–1353. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.73.6.1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Herzberg PY, Glaesmer H, Hoyer J. Separating optimism and pessimism: a robust psychometric analysis of the revised Life Orientation Test (LOT-R) Psychological Assessment. 2006;18(4):433–438. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.4.433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Kivimäki M, Elovainio M, Singh-Manoux A, Vahtera J, Helenius H, Pentti J. Optimism and pessimism as predictors of change in health after death or onset of severe illness in family. Health Psychology. 2005;24(4):413–421. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.4.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Räikkönen K, Matthews KA, Flory JD, Owens JF, Gump BB. Effects of optimism, pessimism, and trait anxiety on ambulatory blood pressure and mood during everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999;76(1):104–113. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.1.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Innamorati M, Lester D, Balsamo M, et al. Factor validity of the Beck Hopelessness Scale in Italian medical patients. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. In press. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Beck AT. Beyond belief: a theory of modes, personality, and psychopathology. In: Salkovskis PM, editor. Frontiers of Cognitive Therapy. New York, NY, USA: Guildford Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Beck AT. Cognitive Therapy of Depression. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 74.Campbell DT, Fiske DW. Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin. 1959;56(2):81–105. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Arnold HJ, Feldman DC. Social desirability response bias in self-report choice situations. Academy of Management Journal. 1981;24:377–385. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Maccoby EE, Maccoby NA. The interview: a tool of social science. In: Lindzey G, editor. Handbook of Social Psychology. Cambridge, Mass, USA: Addison-Wesley; 1954. pp. 449–487. [Google Scholar]