Abstract
Recent insights suggest that predators should include (mildly) toxic prey when non-toxic food is scarce. However, the assumption that toxic prey is energetically as profitable as non-toxic prey misses the possibility that non-toxic prey have other ways to avoid being eaten, such as the formation of an indigestible armature. In that case, predators face a trade-off between avoiding toxins and minimizing indigestible ballast intake. Here, we report on the trophic interactions between a shorebird (red knot, Calidris canutus canutus) and its two main bivalve prey, one being mildly toxic but easily digestible, and the other being non-toxic but harder to digest. A novel toxin-based optimal diet model is developed and tested against an existing one that ignores toxin constraints on the basis of data on prey abundance, diet choice, local survival and numbers of red knots at Banc d'Arguin (Mauritania) over 8 years. Observed diet and annual survival rates closely fit the predictions of the toxin-based model, with survival and population size being highest in years when the non-toxic prey is abundant. In the 6 of 8 years when the non-toxic prey is not abundant enough to satisfy the energy requirements, red knots must rely on the toxic alternative.
Keywords: diet choice, hydrogen sulphide, optimal foraging theory, predator–prey interactions, survival rate, toxins
1. Introduction
Toxic food is better avoided, and there is a large literature on how predators learn to avoid toxic prey [1–6]. Nevertheless, an emerging alternative view is that predators should not entirely neglect toxic prey as long as this could increase their opportunity to gain energy [7–13]. Mildly toxic prey species that are not directly lethal upon ingestion could be valuable during times when non-toxic food is in short supply [14,15]. There are a number of cases where predators have been reported to consume toxic but not-immediately-lethal prey [16–21], but the dietary choices [7,17,19–21] and subsequent demographic consequences [18] remain unexplained in mechanistic and functional terms.
Optimization models may help us to understand how predators should strategically trade off the minimization of toxin ingestion with the maximization of energy gain. Recent state-dependent models predict that the hungrier a predator is, the more likely it is to accept toxic prey [9,10], a prediction that was upheld that empirically [8,13]. Furthermore, through a predator's hunger state, the willingness to include mildly toxic prey should depend on the abundance and availability of non-toxic food, which is a prediction that allows field testing. However, when it comes to field testing, in both the models and the experiments, the only difference between prey types was their degree of toxicity, and this may be quite unrealistic.
In nature, prey species differ in many more defence traits than degrees of toxicity. By making it difficult for a predator to detect, capture, ingest or digest prey [22,23], non-toxic and nutritious prey species may escape predation. Predators therefore need to deal with multiple constraints, and may face much steeper trade-offs between energy gain and toxin avoidance than hitherto assumed. Here, we will focus on such a system in which a predator faces the choice between an easy to digest toxic prey and a much harder to digest non-toxic prey. Building upon the existing digestive rate model (DRM) developed by Hirakawa [24], which includes a digestive constraint but not a toxin constraint, we have developed a novel toxin-digestive rate model (TDRM) to generate food-density-dependent predictions on optimal diet and maximum energy intake rates for systems where prey differ in toxicity. The predictions of both the DRM and the TDRM are then put to the test in an 8-year field study on food abundance, diet choice, survival rate and population size in a molluscivore vertebrate predator, the red knot (Calidris canutus canutus, hereafter knot), in its non-breeding area at Banc d'Arguin (Mauritania), characterized by a highly sulfidic environment in which the most abundant mollusc prey is toxic, while other prey types are not.
(a). Study system
The intertidal flats at Banc d'Arguin are densely covered by seagrass (mainly Zostera noltii Hornem.) [25]. Detritus is produced at a high rate, which is degraded anaerobically by sulphate-reducing bacteria [26], causing a build-up of high concentrations of hydrogen sulphide in sediment pore water [27,28]. Sulphide is toxic to many organisms as its lipid solubility enables it to freely penetrate biological membranes, eventually slowing down the functioning of mitochondria and the production of ATP [29]. A specialized group of organisms that can profit from high sulphide concentrations in seagrass beds are Lucinidae [30], heterodont bivalves that live in symbiosis with chemoautotrophic bacteria inside their gill structures [31]. These bacteria oxidize sulphide that is provided by the lucinid host to synthesize sugars which fuel both the growth of the lucinid host and its endosymbiotic bacteria [32]. The lucinid Loripes lucinalis (hereafter Loripes) is the dominant bivalve in Banc d'Arguin, with densities of up to 4000 individuals per m2 [33,34], and hence Banc d'Arguin can be considered as a chemosynthesis-based ecosystem [35].
Banc d'Arguin is an important non-breeding area for Arctic-breeding shorebirds, hosting more than two million individuals in winter, with knots being the most abundant molluscivore [36]. Knots face a trade-off between feeding on the superabundant but toxic Loripes [37] and a much less abundant but non-toxic prey, Dosinia isocardia (hereafter Dosinia); numerically, Loripes and Dosinia together make up 75 per cent of all molluscs that are ingestible by knots [38,39] and dominate the diet of knots [40]. Knots face an additional trade-off: Loripes has a very thin shell, whereas Dosinia has a thicker armature. As knots ingest their prey whole [41], they often face a digestive processing constraint [42], which can be alleviated by selecting bivalves that have high flesh-to-shell mass ratios [43]. The toxicity of Loripes for knots has recently been investigated experimentally [37]. Captive knots that were given a Loripes-only diet quickly developed diarrhoea, thereby losing significant amounts of water. Their compensatory water consumption could not prevent a decrease in food intake. When given a diet of non-toxic Dosinia, birds recovered within an hour. Intake rates on Loripes available ad libitum were three times lower than expected on the basis of maximal shell mass processing rates, whereas intake rates on Dosinia available ad libitum matched the prediction of a model that predicted intake as constrained by the processing of shells. When given the choice between Dosinia and Loripes, the captive birds included both prey types in their diet, which maximized their energy intake rate as predicted by a model developed for ad libitum situations.
(b). Toxin-digestive rate model
The TDRM is developed for non-ad libitum circumstances, where foragers need to search for their prey. In its most simple form, it assumes that there are just two prey types i = 1,2, which can each be characterized by energy contents ei, indigestible ballast mass ki, toxin contents si, handling time hi, searching efficiency ai and density Di. The problem is finding the acceptance probabilities P = (p1, p2) for both prey types that maximize the forager's long-term energy intake rate Y. The latter is given by the multi-species version of Holling's disc equation [44]:
1.1 |
In the ‘classical prey model’ [45], which ignores possible digestive and toxin constraints, finding the optimal solution is straightforward. First, rank prey types such that . Always accept type 1 (p1 = 1), and accept type 2 (p2 = 1) whenever otherwise reject (p2 = 0). This model, called the ‘contingency model’ (CM) [46], has been upheld in many diet studies on a variety of foragers [47], but was refuted in the case of knots [43,48]. As knots face a digestive constraint, they should and do take a prey's ballast mass into account when selecting their diet [43].
If ballast intake rate X for the optimal solution in the CM exceeds digestive constraint c, then the forager faces a digestive bottleneck, in which case the CM yields a suboptimal solution [24]. Then, the rate-maximizing diet choice can be found using the DRM [24]. This model can be solved graphically by plotting energy intake rate Y against ballast intake rate X for all possible combinations of P, including partial preferences for either type (figure 1a). Then, by drawing digestive constraint c (vertical bar in figure 1a), one can work out which diet choice P yields the maximum sustainable energy intake rate Y under constraint c (asterisk in figure 1a). For details, we refer to the original paper by Hirakawa [24] and its first applications in knots [43], for which such an ‘all-or-nothing constraint’ has explained intake rate [42], prey choice [43,48], patch choice [49], selection of stopover sites [50] and even digestive organ sizes [42,51,52]. As already mentioned by Hirakawa [24], the same graphical procedure can be followed when the forager faces a toxin rather than a digestive constraint (replacing ballast intake rate X by toxin intake rate Z and ballast contents ki by toxin contents si).
However, a forager's energy intake rate may be bottlenecked by both a digestive and toxin constraint. This occurs when, accounting for digestive constraint c in the DRM (i.e. when X > c in the optimal CM solution), toxin intake rate Z in the optimal DRM solution exceeds q. This can occur only when the highest-digestive-quality prey (i.e. the one with the highest ei/ki) is most toxic (i.e. the one with the highest ei/si; in our Loripes–Dosinia case, this condition was always upheld; electronic supplementary material, table S1). Graphically, the optimal solution under both constraints can be found by adding a third axis to Hirakawa's state space (figure 1b; note that we have added the third axis to the existing two-dimensional plane, making reading the details easier; we could have also plotted X, Y and Z three-dimensionally). Solving the model analytically is equally straightforward and we will refer to it as TDRM (note that TDRM equals a DRM when only one of the two constraints operates, which in turn equals a CM when none of the constraints is present). First, maximal sustainable ballast intake rate X is set by digestive constraint c,
1.2a |
which can be written as
1.2b |
Similarly, maximally tolerable toxin intake rate Z is set by toxin constraint q,
1.3a |
which can be written as
1.3b |
Solving equations (1.2b) and (1.3b) for the two unknown variables p1 and p2 yields the optimal acceptance probabilities
1.4 |
and
1.5 |
2. Material and methods
(a). Benthos
Our study period spans from 2003 to 2010, in which we collected 1024 benthos samples in 13 consecutive expeditions: Dec. 2003 (n = 84), Dec. 2004 (n = 26), Apr. 2005 (n = 39), Dec. 2005 (n = 8), Nov. 2006 (n = 6), Apr. 2007 (n = 229), Aug. 2007 (n = 8), Oct. 2007 (n = 12), Feb. 2008 (n = 142), Apr. 2008 (n = 78), Nov. 2008 (n = 56), Oct. 2009 (n = 224) and Oct. 2010 (n = 112). Following procedures described elsewhere [28,43,49], a benthos sample represented a sediment core (diameter: 15 cm) taken to a depth of 20 cm and sieved over a 1 mm sieve. Top (0–4 cm) and bottom (4–16 cm) parts of the sample were sieved separately in order to distinguish between prey that are accessible and inaccessible to knots [53]. In the laboratory, each mollusc was identified to species level, and shell length was determined (± 0.1 mm). The latter allowed us to distinguish between ingestible and non-ingestible prey (knots can ingest all size classes of Loripes and Dosinia < 13.2 mm). By drying (3 days at 60°C), weighing (±0.1 mg) and incinerating (5 h at 550°C) flesh and shell separately, we determined individual flesh ash-free dry mass AFDMflesh and shell dry mass DMshell from subsamples. The relationships of AFDMflesh and DMshell with shell length were used to predict missing values for those prey items that were not weighed. Next, numerical density (D in equations (1.1)–(1.5)), AFDMflesh (e in equation (1.1)) and DMshell (k in equations (1.2b), (1.4), (1.5)) were averaged per year per species (available items only, i.e. those accessible and ingestible), diet models and were used to calculate available biomass densities and as input variables in the two diet models (see the electronic supplementary material, table S1; toxin contents s was equated to flesh contents e in case of Loripes because toxin constraint q is expressed in terms of Loripes flesh intake). Further parameter values used were searching efficiency a = 4 cm2 s−1 [28,54], handling time h = 1 s [28], toxin constraint q = 0.1 mg AFDMflesh s−1 [37] (Loripes only) and gizzard mass = 10 g [50], resulting in digestive constraint c = 5 mg DMshell s−1 [42].
All samples were taken in the vicinity (less than 5 km) of Iwik, Banc d'Arguin (19°53′ N, 16°18′ W). Samples collected in 2003, 2004 and 2006 were taken closer to Iwik (0–3 km) than in other years (1–5 km). Spatial differences at this scale might have had little influence. Yet smaller-scale spatial parameters such as distance to gullies, affecting the presence of seagrass [55], might have had a larger effect. Loripes is mostly found in seagrass, whereas Dosinia is almost as abundant in bare as in seagrass habitat [38], and differences in prey densities between years may thus in part be due to differences in spatial design (on average, seagrass covers 80% of the intertidal surface at Banc d'Arguin [25]). We tested potential biases for both spatial scales by comparing our 2004 data (0–3 km to Iwik) with those of an independent study also from 2004 by Honkoop et al. [38], who sampled mudflats 1–5 km away from Iwik and took an equal number of samples in bare and in seagrass habitat. 2004 was a notable year in which Dosinia was more abundant than Loripes (1142.7 versus 23.9 m−2 in our study and 216.6 versus 198.2 m−2 in the study by Honkoop et al. [38]; after correcting their stratified data for the 80% seagrass coverage of the intertidal flats and for the species-specific availability fractions, 0.73 for Dosinia and 0.70 for Loripes [28]). We repeated all analyses by replacing our 2004 benthos data by those of Honkoop et al. [38], which revealed that neither the outcome of the survival analyses nor the outcome of the diet comparisons was sensitive to our spatially inconsistent sampling programme (see the section on sensitivity analysis with respect to benthos sampling in the electronic supplementary material).
(b). Diet composition
During six of the 13 expeditions, we collected 77 faecal samples (2003, n = 21; 2004, n = 6; Apr. 2007 n = 8; Oct. 2007, n = 14; Feb. 2008, n = 11; 2009, n = 17), samples usually containing 40–60 droppings. Samples were sorted using standard methodology [56], which has recently been calibrated for knots feeding on Dosinia and Loripes [40]. In short, after drying (3 days at 60°C), shell fragments that were retained on a 300 μm sieve were sorted out and weighed per species, yielding species-specific estimates of ingested DMshell (after correcting for 35% of DMshell not being retained on the sieve [40]). Next, hinges were assorted to species and their heights were determined in order to reconstruct ingested size distributions. The latter was needed to express a species's relative diet contribution in terms of total AFDMflesh consumed, because AFDMflesh/DMshell ratios are size-dependent [56]. Relative diet compositions were logit-transformed before calculating the annual averages [57].
(c). Annual survival rates
Survival estimates were based on capture/resighting data of a total of 1595 individually marked knots. The birds were captured and resighted during annual three-week expeditions in November/December 2002–2010 [58], yielding annual survival estimates for seven consecutive years (2003–2009; because survival rate cannot be separated from resighting probability for 2010 when modelled with time dependence). The birds were aged upon capture [59], distinguishing hatch-year birds (juveniles) from older birds (adults). Apparent (or local) survival (Φ) and recapture probabilities (p) were estimated from live encounter data using Cormack–Jolly–Seber models [60]. As benthos and diet data were collected throughout the entire study area, we pooled the data of the two sites in our study area, Abelgh Eiznaya and Baie d'Aouatif [58,61]. Based on knowledge gained from earlier analyses, we made some a priori assumptions to reduce the number of parameters in order to increase the precision of the survival estimates: it has been shown that a time-since-marking (tsm) effect explained most of the variation in annual survival [58], and we thus considered tsm effects to account for transients or handling effects on survival in the first year after capture (Φ1) versus subsequent years (Φ2+). It has further been shown that age at capture (adult versus juveniles) explained a significant part of the variation in survival [58], and we thus included age at capture in our models. Note that knots were treated as adults after their first year (more than 12 months of age), and consequently no age differences existed within the Φ2+ category. As we were interested in which of the two diet models best explained the annual variation in survival rate, we included intake rates predicted by the TDRM and DRM, respectively, as continuous variables in the models. Additionally, to test for survival differences among years, we included time as a factor (time), but also tested whether there was a linear trend in survival rate over time (Time), because an earlier analysis indicated a decline in knot survival over time [62]. In all models, resighting probability p was modelled as a function of time (again as a factor) and site, as observation effort differed between the two sites, and logistic improvements suggested resighting efforts differed between years [58]. Both adults and juveniles forage on open mudflats during low tide and assemble at roosts during high tide, and we had no reason to expect p to differ between age classes.
The global model was Φage × tsm + time psite + time and we tested the goodness of fit using the median-ĉ (c-hat) test implemented in the mark software v. 6.0 [63]. The level of overdispersion was estimated at ĉ = 1.05 ± 0. Models were constructed and run in R (v. 2.15.0) using the RMark v. 2.1.4 package [64] as an interface for program mark [63]. We used model averaging to calculate survival and resighting probability, and present parameter estimates as Model selection was based on Akaike's information criterion corrected for small sample size and overdispersion (ĉ; QAICc). Based on the earlier-mentioned assumptions, the candidate model set consisted of all biologically and ecologically plausible combinations of parametrizations for Φ and p (see the electronic supplementary material, table S2).
(d). Estimating and predicting population dynamics
Each year between 2002 and 2010, we carried out a single count of all knots roosting in the Iwik study region. This took place during a daytime spring high tide in November/December. Birds were counted using telescopes by four or five teams of two observers, each counting a subsection of our study area.
We modelled the population trend for 2002–2010 using adult and juvenile survival rates estimated by the most parsimonious model (i.e. survival model 1 in electronic supplementary material, table S2). In this statistical model, TDRM energy intake rates Y served as input, which were predicted on the basis of equation (1.1) using (i) the observed densities of both Loripes and Dosinia, (ii) the observed densities of Loripes only, and (iii) the observed densities of Dosinia only. The last two hypothetical scenarios allow us to hypothesize how much knot population dynamics depend on the presence of either Loripes or Dosinia. As applied before when modelling knot population dynamics [65], we used a two-dimensional matrix population model, in which fecundity (f; equal to 0 for juveniles and 0.14 yr−1 for adults [66]), juvenile survival (Φjuv) and adult survival (Φad) determine how the number of juveniles (Njuv) and adults (Nad) in year t affect the number of juveniles and adults in year t + 1:
2.1 |
The 2002 count was used as the initial population size in the model.
3. Results
(a). Annual survival rate
TDRM models were substantially better supported than models including DRM intake rates (cumulative QAICc weight: 0.38 for models including TDRM intake rates, and 0.00 for models including DRM intake rates; electronic supplementary material, table S2; figure 2a,b). Although models including annual variation as explanatory factor (i.e. factor time) scored high in the model selection process (cumulative QAICc weight: 0.48; electronic supplementary material, table S2), they added extra parameters (complexity) to the models and should thus be less favoured. There was no evidence for a time trend in survival (i.e. models including Time; cumulative QAICc weight: 0.14; electronic supplementary material, table S2). Furthermore, there was no support for adult survival being different in the first year after marking, compared with subsequent years (model 2 versus model 3, ΔQAICc = 0.34). Model-averaged survival estimates can be found in the electronic supplementary material, table S3.
(b). Diet composition
The observed contribution of Loripes to the diet was less than predicted by the DRM (figure 2c; t = −3.44, d.f. = 4, p = 0.03). For 3 of 5 years for which we had diet data available, the DRM predicted that knots should fully ignore Dosinia (figure 2c). In those three years (2007, 2008, 2009), the abundance of Loripes was so high that, even if knots would feed on Loripes only—the prey with the highest flesh-to-shell mass ratio—their gizzard would not be able to achieve the required shell mass processing rate (i.e. knots would face a digestive constraint). Hence, only a proportion of encountered Loripes should have been accepted (see the electronic supplementary material, table S4; note that this is different from conceptual figure 1a where, for reasons of visual clarity, we assumed that even maximum ballast intake rates on prey type 1, i.e. k1/h1, are below digestive constraint c).
By contrast, diet compositions predicted by TDRM matched the observed diets (figure 2d; t = −1.26, d.f. = 4, p = 0.28). In 3 of 5 years, the intake rate on Loripes would have exceeded the toxin constraint if all encountered Loripes were accepted. Hence, only a proportion of the encountered Loripes should have been accepted for this reason (see the electronic supplementary material, table S4). In those years, knots following the TDRM could accept all encountered (ingestible) Dosinia as the occurrence of the toxin constraint kept shell mass processing rates low, and thereby prevented a digestive constraint. Only in the year that Loripes was less abundant than Dosinia (2004) does the TDRM predict a digestive rather than a toxin constraint. In 2004, knots should thus have accepted all encountered Loripes and only a fraction of the encountered (ingestible) Dosinia (see the electronic supplementary material, table S4).
(c). Predicted and observed population dynamics
Predicted knot population size declined over time, with the decline being steepest if Dosinia would have been removed from the system (−79% from 2002 to 2010), followed by the scenario when Loripes would have been removed (−74%). However, even with both prey included in the diet, knot numbers were predicted to decrease over time (−39%; figure 3). This last model agreed best with the observed decline in knot numbers from 22 859 in 2002 to 12 465 in 2010 (−45%; figure 3).
4. Discussion
Knot annual survival rates correlated strongly with annual variations in Dosinia abundance (figure 4a; Pearson's r = 0.91), but showed no trend with Loripes abundance (figure 4b; Pearson's r = −0.72). This strongly suggests that knots need non-toxic Dosinia to survive and cannot rely on Loripes only, even though Loripes is much more abundant and has a much higher flesh-to-shell ratio. The reasoning for this dependency is rather simple: in order to prevent lethal intoxication, knots can ingest Loripes up to a rate that is only half of their required intake rate [37], and they need prey such as Dosinia to meet their energy demands. On the other hand, Dosinia was not abundant enough for knots to fully rely on them as an energy source.
The TDRM, which seems to capture the essence of the knots' dietary problem, assumes the following strategy: accept toxic but energy-rich Loripes until toxin constraint is met, then add bulky Dosinia until the digestive constraint is met. According to our calculations, knots faced both constraints only in 2006, when both prey species occurred in high densities (figure 4c; for detailed calculations, see the electronic supplementary material, ‘figure 4c explained’). In most years (six of eight; figure 4c), however, Dosinia was not abundant enough for the birds to become digestively constrained, whereas the presence of Loripes was usually high enough to meet the toxin constraint (figure 4c). This explains the negative correlation between the relative amount of Loripes in the diet and the available density of Dosinia (figure 5a): although the absolute rate at which Loripes was eaten was likely to be constant each year (equal to toxin constraint q), the absolute rate at which Dosinia was eaten increased with the available Dosinia density as long as birds were not digestively constrained (this would occur at a Dosinia density of 0.6–0.7 g AFDMflesh m−2). A recent study showing year-round changes in Dosinia and Loripes densities also suggests that the relative contribution of Loripes to the diet of knots increased as Dosinia stocks became depleted throughout winter [39].
In Banc d'Arguin, knots need an average energy intake rate of approximately 0.2 mg AFDMflesh s−1 in order to maintain body mass [67]. In most years, knots would only achieve half of this rate only if they would fully neglect Loripes and only accept Dosinia as their prey. By adding Loripes to their diet, knots would just meet their required energy demand. A plot of the predicted intake rate with (grey band in figure 5b) and without (dashed line in figure 5b) Loripes against the available Dosinia densities shows that energy intake rate without accepting Loripes would be insufficient for subsistence in 6 of 8 years (also see electronic supplementary material, table S4). Only in 2004 and 2006 would knots have been able to achieve their minimum energetic requirements on Dosinia alone (see the electronic supplementary material, table S4; although we modelled knots as ‘intake rate maximizers’, they could just as well have featured as ‘sulphide minimizers’ in these 2 years by fully ignoring Loripes; however, the diet data available for 2004 suggest they did not; figure 5a).
Note that rate maximization while feeding allows for the minimization of daily feeding time if a fixed amount of daily energy is required [68]. Minimizing daily feeding time can be beneficial if foraging comes at a cost, such as for example enhanced predation risk [69]. This justifies our approach to analyse survival as a continuous function of intake rate rather than as a simple step function of whether metabolic demands are met. Note further that in poor Dosinia years, notably in 2009 (see figure 5b; electronic supplementary material, table S4), knots would not even have been able to survive on the combination of Loripes and Dosinia alone, and would have needed to include other prey types in their diet (which knots indeed did, especially in 2009 [40]).
With Loripes and Dosinia being by far the most abundant available bivalves at Banc d'Arguin [38], there are not many alternative mollusc prey types to include in the diet. This notion, and the fact that the last years of our study period have not shown high densities of Dosinia (figure 4c), may explain why the local knot population has declined during especially the second half of our study period (figure 3). However, TDRM energy intake rate showed no trend over time (r = 0.51, F1,6 = 2.07, p = 0.20). In addition, also in 1980s, when knot numbers were 40–50 per cent times higher than nowadays [70], Dosinia and other non-toxic alternatives were never very abundant [71]. Being a migratory species, it may thus very well be that the carrying capacity of the population is set elsewhere outside Banc d'Arguin [67]; for example, in the Wadden Sea southward staging area, where commercial fisheries led to impaired (re)fuelling opportunities [72].
It is yet unclear what determines the probability of high densities of Dosinia, but the negative correlation between annual averages of Dosinia and Loripes densities is remarkable (figure 4c; r = −0.76, F1,6 = 8.30, p = 0.03). As has been suggested elsewhere [28], this indicates some form of competition between the two species. Alternatively, there may be differences in environmental conditions among years that steer the negative correlation. For example, observed dynamics in seagrass abundance may underlie this correlation [55,58], with Loripes more strongly linked to seagrass habitat than Dosinia [38].
It is exciting to hypothesize about how defence strategies in one prey may have been selected for given the defence strategy in another prey. For example, is the bulkiness of Dosinia an evolutionary response to the toxicity of Loripes? The comparison between the DRM and the TDRM allows us to hypothesize along these lines: it suggests that toxicity of Loripes might have increased predation pressure on Dosinia, inducing, on an evolutionary time scale, extra armature in Dosinia. The reason behind this is that intake rates on Dosinia are much higher in the TDRM than in the DRM, especially in years of high Loripes abundance (see the electronic supplementary material, table S4). Under the DRM, which treats Loripes as if it was non-toxic, knots can reach their digestive constraint on Loripes only, leaving no room to add bulky Dosinia. By contrast, under the TDRM, many Dosinia can be added to the diet because intake rates on Loripes are reduced because of the toxicity constraint.
At the same time, the evolution of thick-shelled armature in Dosinia may have led to increased predation pressure on Loripes, which in turn may have increased Loripes's toxicity. Namely, if Dosinia had been relatively thinner shelled than Loripes (i.e. when eD/kD > eL/kL), then knots would prefer Dosinia over Loripes and would fully neglect Loripes in Dosinia-rich years. Note that the mechanism of enhanced predation pressure on one prey type as a consequence of induced anti-predator defence in the other prey type proposed here is a classic example of ‘trait-mediated indirect interactions’, which have received renewed attention in the ecological literature [73–75].
With the chemoautotrophically fuelled Loripes being the top most abundant bivalve in the system, Banc d'Arguin can be classified as a chemosynthesis-based ecosystem [35]. In contrast to Banc d'Arguin, most chemosynthesis-based ecosystems, such as deep-sea vents and seep systems, are renowned for their lack of predators [19,76,77]. Possibly, such systems lack predators because of the overwhelming densities of toxic prey, whereas non-toxic alternatives are not at hand [78]. The presence of a suitable non-toxic prey may explain why predators are able to thrive at Banc d'Arguin. Hydrothermal vents and deep-sea cold seeps are geographically more isolated than seagrass beds, and also more hostile because of the limited availability of dissolved oxygen in the deep sea. Their isolated positions make it costly for predators to switch between ‘phototrophic’ and ‘chemotrophic’ prey, which could be the reason that such systems are frequented little by predators originating from photosynthetic communities [77,79,80]. By contrast, in seagrass beds, the difference between the anaerobic sulphidic and the aerobic non-toxic environment is just a matter of metres in a horizontal direction (bare versus seagrass mosaics [38,81]), or even centimetres when considered vertically (sulphide concentrations strongly increase in the first 12 cm of the sediment layer [28]). This allows predators to ‘make the best of both worlds’ by adding toxic prey to their non-toxic diet as long as toxin levels do not exceed a given threshold. This mimics the problems recognized long ago for terrestrial herbivores, in which diet selection [82,83], habitat use [84], and fitness and population processes [85] are governed by the occurrence of toxins in the form of secondary plant metabolites or as products from endosymbiotic relationships [86,87]. Our work seems to be the first to make similar problems apparent in a system with predators and prey rather than herbivores and plants.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Parc National du Banc d'Arguin (PNBA) for their permission to work in the park and for using their facilities. The local crew at Iwik, most notably Amadou Abderahmane Sall, Sidi Ely, Mohammed Camara and Lemhaba Ould Yarba, helped in many different ways, while Brecht De Meulenaer, Joop van Eerbeek, Eelke Folmer, Erik Jansen, Han Olff, Erik Rosendaal and Hanneke Gillis assisted in collecting and processing benthos samples. The last two named also analysed dropping samples. About 40 volunteers from 10 different countries helped with catching and resighting knots. Carlos Martínez del Rio and two anonymous referees commented on a draft, and Dick Visser polished the figures. This work was supported by operating funds from NIOZ, and by grants from Schure-Beijerinck-Popping Fonds to J.L., the Prins Bernhard Cultuurfondsprijs voor Natuurbehoud to T.P., a MAVA-grant (Switzerland) to T.P., an NWO-WOTRO Integrated Programme (grant no. W.01.65.221.00) to T.P. and an NWO-VIDI grant (no. 864.09.002) to J.A.v.G.
References
- 1.Alatalo RV, Mappes J. 1996. Tracking the evolution of warning signals. Nature 382, 708–710. 10.1038/382708a0 (doi:10.1038/382708a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Endler JA, Mappes J. 2004. Predator mixes and the conspicuousness of aposematic signals. Am. Nat. 163, 532–547. 10.1086/382662 (doi:10.1086/382662) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gittleman JL, Harvey PH. 1980. Why are distasteful prey not cryptic? Nature 286, 149–150. 10.1038/286149a0 (doi:10.1038/286149a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Greenlees MJ, Phillips BL, Shine R. 2010. Adjusting to a toxic invader: native Australian frogs learn not to prey on cane toads. Behav. Ecol. 21, 966–971. 10.1093/beheco/arq095 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arq095) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Halpin CG, Rowe C. 2010. Taste-rejection behaviour by predators can promote variability in prey defences. Biol. Lett. 6, 617–619. 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0153 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2010.0153) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Speed MP. 2000. Warning signals, receiver psychology and predator memory. Anim. Behav. 60, 269–278. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1430 (doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1430) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Skelhorn J, Rowe C. 2010. Birds learn to use distastefulness as a signal of toxicity. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 1729–1734. 10.1098/rspb.2009.2092 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.2092) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Barnett CA, Bateson M, Rowe C. 2007. State-dependent decision making: educated predators strategically trade off the costs and benefits of consuming aposematic prey. Behav. Ecol. 18, 645–651. 10.1093/beheco/arm027 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arm027) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sherratt TN. 2003. State-dependent risk-taking by predators in systems with defended prey. Oikos 103, 93–100. 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.12576.x (doi:10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.12576.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sherratt TN, Speed MP, Ruxton GD. 2004. Natural selection on unpalatable species imposed by state-dependent foraging behaviour. J. Theor. Biol. 228, 217–226. 10.1016/j.jtbi.2003.12.009 (doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2003.12.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Skelhorn J, Rowe C. 2007. Predators’ toxin burdens influence their strategic decisions to eat toxic prey. Curr. Biol. 17, 1479–1483. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.064 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.064) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Speed MP. 1993. When is mimicry good for predators? Anim. Behav. 46, 1246–1248. 10.1006/anbe.1993.1321 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1993.1321) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Barnett CA, Skelhorn J, Bateson M, Rowe C. 2012. Educated predators make strategic decisions to eat defended prey according to their toxin content. Behav. Ecol. 23, 418–424. 10.1093/beheco/arr206 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arr206) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kokko H, Mappes J, Lindström L. 2003. Alternative prey can change model-mimic dynamics between parasitism and mutualism. Ecol. Lett. 6, 1068–1076. 10.1046/j.1461-0248.2003.00532.x (doi:10.1046/j.1461-0248.2003.00532.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Lindström L, Alatalo RV, Lyytinen A, Mappes J. 2001. Strong antiapostatic selection against novel rare aposematic prey. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 9181–9184. 10.1073/pnas.161071598 (doi:10.1073/pnas.161071598) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Williams B, Lovenburg V, Huffard C, Caldwell R. 2011. Chemical defense in pelagic octopus paralarvae: tetrodotoxin alone does not protect individual paralarvae of the greater blue-ringed octopus (Hapalochlaena lunulata) from common reef predators. Chemoecology 21, 131–141. 10.1007/s00049-011-0075-5 (doi:10.1007/s00049-011-0075-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Beckmann C, Shine R. 2011. Toad's tongue for breakfast: exploitation of a novel prey type, the invasive cane toad, by scavenging raptors in tropical Australia. Biol. Invasions 13, 1447–1455. 10.1007/s10530-010-9903-8 (doi:10.1007/s10530-010-9903-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Garcia R, Sulkin S, Lopez M. 2011. Effects on larval crabs of exposure to algal toxin via ingestion of heterotrophic prey. Mar. Biol. 158, 451–460. 10.1007/s00227-010-1572-8 (doi:10.1007/s00227-010-1572-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kicklighter CE, Fisher CR, Hay ME. 2004. Chemical defense of hydrothermal vent and hydrocarbon seep organisms: a preliminary assessment using shallow-water consumers. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 275, 11–19. 10.3354/meps275011 (doi:10.3354/meps275011) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lincango MP, Causton CE, Alvarez CC, Jimenez-Uzcategui G. 2011. Evaluating the safety of Rodolia cardinalis to two species of Galapagos finch; Camarhynchus parvulus and Geospiza fuliginosa. Biol. Control 56, 145–149. 10.1016/j.biocontrol.2010.10.006 (doi:10.1016/j.biocontrol.2010.10.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gray HM, Kaiser H, Green DM. 2010. Does alkaloid sequestration protect the green poison frog, Dendrobates auratus, from predator attacks? Salamandra 46, 235–238. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Caro T. 2005. Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Jeschke JM, Tollrian R. 2000. Density-dependent effects of prey defences. Oecologia 123, 391–396. 10.1007/s004420051026 (doi:10.1007/s004420051026) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hirakawa H. 1995. Diet optimization with a nutrient or toxin constraint. Theor. Popul. Biol. 47, 331–346. 10.1006/tpbi.1995.1015 (doi:10.1006/tpbi.1995.1015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wolff WJ, Smit C. 1990. The Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania, as an environment for coastal birds. Ardea 78, 17–38. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Jørgensen BB. 1982. Mineralization of organic matter in the sea bed: the role of sulphate reduction. Nature 296, 643–645. 10.1038/296643a0 (doi:10.1038/296643a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Calleja ML, Marbà N, Duarte CM. 2007. The relationship between seagrass (Posidonia oceanica) decline and sulfide porewater concentration in carbonate sediments. Estuar. Coast. Shelf Sci. 73, 583–588. 10.1016/j.ecss.2007.02.016 (doi:10.1016/j.ecss.2007.02.016) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.van Gils JA, van der Geest M, Jansen EJ, Govers LL, de Fouw J, Piersma T. 2012. Trophic cascade induced by molluscivore predator alters pore-water biogeochemistry via competitive release of prey. Ecology 93, 1143–1152. 10.1890/11-1282.1 (doi:10.1890/11-1282.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Bagarinao T. 1992. Sulfide as an environmental factor and toxicant: tolerance and adaptations in aquatic organisms. Aquat. Toxicol. 24, 21–62. 10.1016/0166-445X(92)90015-F (doi:10.1016/0166-445X(92)90015-F) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Taylor JD, Glover EA, Smith L, Dyal P, Williams ST. 2011. Molecular phylogeny and classification of the chemosymbiotic bivalve family Lucinidae (Mollusca: Bivalvia). Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 163, 15–49. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Taylor JD, Glover EA. 2006. Lucinidae (Bivalvia): the most diverse group of chemosymbiotic molluscs. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 148, 421–438. 10.1111/j.1096-3642.2006.00261.x (doi:10.1111/j.1096-3642.2006.00261.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Johnson M, Diouris M, Le Pennec M. 1994. Endosymbiotic bacterial contribution in the carbon nutrition of Loripes lucinalis (Mollusca: Bivalvia). Symbiosis 17, 1–13. [Google Scholar]
- 33.van der Heide T, et al. 2012. A three-stage symbiosis forms the foundation of seagrass ecosystems. Science 336, 1432–1434. 10.1126/science.1219973 (doi:10.1126/science.1219973) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.van der Geest M, van Gils JA, van der Meer J, Olff H, Piersma T. 2011. Suitability of calcein as an in situ growth marker in burrowing bivalves. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 399, 1–7. 10.1016/j.jembe.2011.01.003 (doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2011.01.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dubilier N, Bergin C, Lott C. 2008. Symbiotic diversity in marine animals: the art of harnessing chemosynthesis. Nat. Rev. Microbiol. 6, 725–740. 10.1038/nrmicro1992 (doi:10.1038/nrmicro1992) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Altenburg W, Engelmoer M, Mes R, Piersma T. 1982. Wintering Waders on the Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania. Report of the Netherlands Ornithological Expedition 1980. Leiden, The Netherlands: Stichting Veth tot steun aan Waddenonderzoek. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Oudman T, Onrust J, de Fouw J, Spaans B, Piersma T, van Gils JA. In preparation. Toxin and digestive constraint explain partial preferences of an energy maximizing predator.
- 38.Honkoop PJC, Berghuis EM, Holthuijsen S, Lavaleye MSS, Piersma T. 2008. Molluscan assemblages of seagrass-covered and bare intertidal flats on the Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania, in relation to characteristics of sediment and organic matter. J. Sea Res. 60, 255–263. 10.1016/j.seares.2008.07.005 (doi:10.1016/j.seares.2008.07.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ahmedou Salem MV, van der Geest M, Piersma T, Saoud Y, van Gils JA. Submitted Seasonal changes in mollusc abundance in a tropical intertidal ecosystem, Banc d'Arguin (Mauritania): a test of the ‘shorebird depletion’ hypothesis.
- 40.Onrust J, de Fouw J, Oudman T, van der Geest M, Piersma T, van Gils JA. In press. Red Knot diet reconstruction revisited: context dependence revealed by experiments at Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania Bird Study.
- 41.Piersma T, Koolhaas A, Dekinga A. 1993. Interactions between stomach structure and diet choice in shorebirds. Auk 110, 552–564. 10.2307/4088419 (doi:10.2307/4088419) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.van Gils JA, Piersma T, Dekinga A, Dietz MW. 2003. Cost-benefit analysis of mollusc-eating in a shorebird. II. Optimizing gizzard size in the face of seasonal demands . J. Exp. Biol. 206, 3369–3380. 10.1242/jeb.00546 (doi:10.1242/jeb.00546) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.van Gils JA, de Rooij SR, van Belle J, van der Meer J, Dekinga A, Piersma T, Drent R. 2005. Digestive bottleneck affects foraging decisions in red knots Calidris canutus. I. Prey choice. J. Anim. Ecol. 74, 105–119. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2004.00903.x (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2004.00903.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Holling CS. 1959. Some characteristics of simple types of predation and parasitism. Can. Entomol. 91, 385–398. 10.4039/Ent91385-7 (doi:10.4039/Ent91385-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Stephens DW, Krebs JR. 1986. Foraging theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Belovsky GE. 1984. Herbivore optimal foraging: a comparative test of 3 models. Am. Nat. 124, 97–115. 10.1086/284254 (doi:10.1086/284254) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Sih A, Christensen B. 2001. Optimal diet theory: when does it work, and when and why does it fail? Anim. Behav. 61, 379–390. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1592 (doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1592) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Quaintenne G, van Gils JA, Bocher P, Dekinga A, Piersma T. 2010. Diet selection in a molluscivore shorebird across Western Europe: does it show short- or long-term intake rate-maximization? J. Anim. Ecol. 79, 53–62. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01608.x (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01608.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.van Gils JA, Dekinga A, Spaans B, Vahl WK, Piersma T. 2005. Digestive bottleneck affects foraging decisions in red knots Calidris canutus. II. Patch choice and length of working day. J. Anim. Ecol. 74, 120–130. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2004.00904.x (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2004.00904.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.van Gils JA, Battley PF, Piersma T, Drent R. 2005. Reinterpretation of gizzard sizes of red knots world-wide emphasises overriding importance of prey quality at migratory stopover sites. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 2609–2618. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3245 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3245) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.van Gils JA, Piersma T, Dekinga A, Battley PF. 2006. Modelling phenotypic flexibility: an optimality analysis of gizzard size in red knots (Calidris canutus). Ardea 94, 409–420. [Google Scholar]
- 52.van Gils JA, Dekinga A, van den Hout PJ, Spaans B, Piersma T. 2007. Digestive organ size and behavior of red knots (Calidris canutus) indicate the quality of their benthic food stocks. Isr. J. Ecol. Evol. 53, 329–346. 10.1560/IJEE.53.3.329 (doi:10.1560/IJEE.53.3.329) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Zwarts L, Blomert A-M, Wanink JH. 1992. Annual and seasonal variation in the food supply harvestable by knot Calidris canutus staging in the Wadden Sea in late summer. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 83, 129–139. 10.3354/meps083129 (doi:10.3354/meps083129) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Piersma T, van Gils J, de Goeij P, van der Meer J. 1995. Holling's functional response model as a tool to link the food-finding mechanism of a probing shorebird with its spatial distribution. J. Anim. Ecol. 64, 493–504. 10.2307/5652 (doi:10.2307/5652) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Folmer EO, van der Geest M, Jansen E, Olff H, Anderson TM, Piersma T, van Gils JA. 2012. Seagrass-sediment feedback: an exploration using a non-recursive structural equation model. Ecosystems 15, 1380–1393. 10.1007/s10021-012-9591-6 (doi:10.1007/s10021-012-9591-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Dekinga A, Piersma T. 1993. Reconstructing diet composition on the basis of faeces in a mollusc-eating wader, the knot Calidris canutus. Bird Study 40, 144–156. 10.1080/00063659309477140 (doi:10.1080/00063659309477140) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Warton DI, Hui FKC. 2011. The arcsine is asinine: the analysis of proportions in ecology. Ecology 92, 3–10. 10.1890/10-0340.1 (doi:10.1890/10-0340.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Leyrer J, Lok T, Brugge M, Dekinga A, Spaans B, van Gils JA, Sandercock BK, Piersma T. 2012. Small-scale demographic structure suggests preemptive behavior in a flocking shorebird. Behav. Ecol. 23, 1226–1233. 10.1093/beheco/ars106 (doi:10.1093/beheco/ars106) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Prater AJ, Merchant JH, Vuorinen J. 1977. Guide to the identification and ageing of holarctic shorebirds. Tring, UK: BTO. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Lebreton J-D, Burnham KP, Clobert J, Anderson DR. 1992. Modeling survival and testing biological hypotheses using marked animals: a unified approach with case studies. Ecol. Monogr. 62, 67–118. 10.2307/2937171 (doi:10.2307/2937171) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Leyrer J, Spaans B, Camara M, Piersma T. 2006. Small home ranges and high site fidelity in red knots (Calidris c. canutus) wintering on the Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania . J. Ornithol. 147, 376–384. 10.1007/s10336-005-0030-8 (doi:10.1007/s10336-005-0030-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Leyrer J, Lok T, Brugge M, Spaans B, Sandercock BK, Piersma T. In press Mortality within the annual cycle: seasonal survival patterns in Afro-Siberian red knots. J. Ornithol. [Google Scholar]
- 63.White GC, Burnham KP. 1999. Program MARK: survival estimation from populations of marked animals. Bird Study 46, S120–S139. 10.1080/00063659909477239 (doi:10.1080/00063659909477239) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Laake J, with contributions from E. Rakhimberdiev, B. Augustine. 2012 Package ‘RMark’: R code for MARK analysis, v. 2.1.4. See http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/RMark/RMark.pdf.
- 65.Baker AJ, et al. 2004. Rapid population decline in red knots: fitness consequences of decreased refuelling rates and late arrival in Delaware Bay. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 875–882. 10.1098/rspb.2003.2663 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2663) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.van den Hout PJ, van Gils JA, Robin F, van der Geest M, Dekinga A, Piersma T. In preparation. Cryptic interference forces juvenile red knots to forage longer in a dangerous environment. [Google Scholar]
- 67.van Gils JA, van der Geest M, Kraan C, Folmer EO, Jansen EJ, Piersma T. 2009. How the carrying capacity of the Wadden Sea regulates the number of wintering waders at Banc d'Arguin. Limosa 82, 134–140. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Schoener TW. 1971. Theory of feeding strategies. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. S 2, 369–404. 10.1146/annurev.es.02.110171.002101 (doi:10.1146/annurev.es.02.110171.002101) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.McNamara JM, Houston AI. 1994. The effect of a change in foraging options on intake rate and predation rate. Am. Nat. 144, 978–1000. 10.1086/285721 (doi:10.1086/285721) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Hagemeijer W, Smit C, de Boer P, van Dijk A, Ravenscroft N, van Roomen M, Wright M. 2004. Wader and waterbird at the Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania, January 2000. In WIWO Report 81. Beek-Ubbergen, The Netherlands: Foundation Working Group International Waterbird and Wetland Research (WIWO). [Google Scholar]
- 71.Wolff WJ, Duiven AG, Duiven P, Esselink P, Gueye A, Meijboom A, Moerland G, Zegers J. 1993. Biomass of macrobenthic tidal flat fauna of the Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania. Hydrobiologia 258, 151–163. 10.1007/BF00006193 (doi:10.1007/BF00006193) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kraan C, van Gils JA, Spaans B, Dekinga A, Piersma T. 2010. Why Afro-Siberian red knots Calidris canutus canutus have stopped staging in the western Dutch Wadden Sea during southward migration. Ardea 98, 155–160. 10.5253/078.098.0204 (doi:10.5253/078.098.0204) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Bolker B, Holyoak M, Křivan V, Rowe L, Schmitz O. 2003. Connecting theoretical and empirical studies of trait-mediated interactions. Ecology 84, 1101–1114. 10.1890/0012-9658(2003)084[1101:CTAESO]2.0.CO;2 (doi:10.1890/0012-9658(2003)084[1101:CTAESO]2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Werner EE, Peacor SD. 2003. A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology 84, 1083–1100. 10.1890/0012-9658(2003)084[1083:AROTII]2.0.CO;2 (doi:10.1890/0012-9658(2003)084[1083:AROTII]2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Yamauchi A, Yamamura N. 2005. Effects of defense evolution and diet choice on population dynamics in a one-predator-two-prey system. Ecology 86, 2513–2524. 10.1890/04-1524 (doi:10.1890/04-1524) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Bergquist DC, Ward T, Cordes EE, McNelis T, Howlett S, Kosoff R, Hourdez S, Carney R, Fisher CR. 2003. Community structure of vestimentiferan-generated habitat islands from Gulf of Mexico cold seeps. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 289, 197–222. 10.1016/S0022-0981(03)00046-7 (doi:10.1016/S0022-0981(03)00046-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Carney RS. 1994. Consideration of the oasis analogy for chemosynthetic communities at Gulf of Mexico hydrocarbon vents. Geo-Mar. Lett. 14, 149–159. 10.1007/BF01203726 (doi:10.1007/BF01203726) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Tunnicliffe V. 1991. The biology of hydrothermal vents: ecology and evolution. Oceanogr. Mar. Biol. 29, 319–407. [Google Scholar]
- 79.MacAvoy SE, Morgan E, Carney RS, Macko SA. 2008. Chemoautotrophic production incorporated by heterotrophs in Gulf of Mexico hydrocarbon seeps: an examination of mobile benthic predators and seep residents. J. Shellfish Res. 27, 153–161. 10.2983/0730-8000(2008)27[153:CPIBHI]2.0.CO;2 (doi:10.2983/0730-8000(2008)27[153:CPIBHI]2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Cordes EE, Becker EL, Fisher CR. 2010. Temporal shift in nutrient input to cold-seep food webs revealed by stable-isotope signatures of associated communities. Limnol. Oceanogr. 55, 2537–2548. 10.4319/lo.2010.55.6.2537 (doi:10.4319/lo.2010.55.6.2537) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 81.van der Heide T, Bouma TJ, van Nes EH, van de Koppel J, Scheffer M, Roelofs JGM, van Katwijk MM, Smolders AJP. 2010. Spatial self-organized patterning in seagrasses along a depth gradient of an intertidal ecosystem. Ecology 91, 362–369. 10.1890/08-1567.1 (doi:10.1890/08-1567.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Dearing MD, Foley WJ, McLean S. 2005. The influence of plant secondary metabolites on the nutritional ecology of herbivorous terrestrial vertebrates. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. S 36, 169–189. 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.36.102003.152617 (doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.36.102003.152617) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Schmidt KA. 2000. Interactions between food chemistry and predation risk in fox squirrels. Ecology 81, 2077–2085. 10.1890/0012-9658(2000)081[2077:IBFCAP]2.0.CO;2 (doi:10.1890/0012-9658(2000)081[2077:IBFCAP]2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Moore BD, Foley WJ. 2005. Tree use by koalas in a chemically complex landscape. Nature 435, 488–490. 10.1038/nature03551 (doi:10.1038/nature03551) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.DeGabriel JL, Moore BD, Foley WJ, Johnson CN. 2009. The effects of plant defensive chemistry on nutrient availability predict reproductive success in a mammal. Ecology 90, 711–719. 10.1890/08-0940.1 (doi:10.1890/08-0940.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Saikkonen K, Faeth SH, Helander M, Sullivan TJ. 1998. Fungal endophytes: a continuum of interactions with host plants. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. S 29, 319–343. 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.29.1.319 (doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.29.1.319) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Verstraete B, Van Elst D, Steyn H, Van Wyk B, Lemaire B, Smets E, Dessein S. 2011. Endophytic bacteria in toxic South African plants: identification, phylogeny and possible involvement in gousiekte. PLoS ONE 6, e19265. 10.1371/journal.pone.0019265 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019265) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]