Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Sep 16.
Published in final edited form as: Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Jan 8;13(2):55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.005

Looking for cognition in the structure within the noise

Adam Johnson *, André A Fenton 1, Cliff Kentros 2, A David Redish 3
PMCID: PMC3774297  NIHMSID: NIHMS363474  PMID: 19135406

Abstract

Neural activity in the mammalian CNS is determined by both observable processes, such as sensory stimuli or motor output and covert, internal cognitive processes that cannot be directly observed. We propose methods to identify these cognitive processes by examining the covert structure within the apparent “noise” in spike trains. Contemporary analyses of neural codes include encoding (tuning curves derived from spike trains and behavioral, sensory, or motor variables), decoding (reconstructing behavioral, sensory, or motor variables from spike trains and hypothesized tuning curves), and generative models (predicting the spikes trains from hypothesized encoding models and decoded variables). We review examples of each of these processes in hippocampal activity, and propose a general methodology to examine cognitive processes via the identification of dynamic changes in covert variables.

Keywords: Mouse splenic macrophages, dendritic cells, B cells possess functional circadian molecular clocks

Introduction

The standard neurophysiological approach to understanding how neuronal activity encodes information is to examine neural activity while a subject is repeatedly presented with the same stimulus, or performs multiple trials of the same behavior. Typically, the relationship of a single neuron’s activity to an overt variable is described by the cell’s tuning curve, which is constructed under the assumption that noise sources are independent of the overt variable of interest and will thus average to zero in the tuning curve. In addition to this noise assumption, the standard approach also assumes that neurons independently contribute to the representation of the overt variable so that a neural representation can be understood by computing an average of the independent tuning curves.

Although the standard “encoding” approach has been used to successfully characterize neural representations of overt variables in several areas of neuroscience, it has been more difficult to apply the encoding approach to identify representations of covert cognitive variables like attention, decisions and planning. One difficulty is found at the level of experimental design because such covert variables may not be expressed repeatedly and reliably, even given an identical set of external conditions. However, even beyond this experimental difficulty, the noise and independence assumptions that underlie the definition of tuning curves are at odds with the theoretical framework of ensemble representations e.g., cell assemblies1. Within an ensemble framework, information is represented by conjoint activity distributed across a functionally-defined neuronal subgroup or cell assembly composed of many neurons. Furthermore, each neuron in a cell assembly can participate in other cell assemblies that represent different overt and covert variables. According to this view, understanding a neural representation requires characterizing conjoint neural activity rather than just an average from individual neurons. It is also unlikely that a neuron will be tuned to a single variable. Both of these considerations are difficult to reconcile with the standard encoding approach based on tuning curves.

A growing body of evidence supports the cell assembly framework, especially in recent studies of the representation of location in hippocampal ensemble activity. In this review we consider evidence that what may appear to be noise in tuning curves actually results from operation of an experimentally unobserved (covert) variable or process. We focus on recent studies of hippocampal place cell representations of locations, which have gone beyond the encoding approach to provide compelling evidence of covert cognitive variables in hippocampal discharge. Based on these studies, we outline a general generative approach for finding covert cognitive variables in the apparent noise of tuning curves.

An early example: Decoding mental rotation in cortical discharge

An important study by Georgopoulos et al. showed that coherent, cell assembly-like dynamics within a neuronal population were related to cognitive function2. Monkeys were trained to reach toward a target selected on each trial from one of eight potential directions around a circle . However, on a subset of probe trials, a bright target indicated that the rewarded direction would be rotated ninety degrees from the signaled direction. Georgopoulos et al.2 characterized the correspondence between observed spiking activity in the motor cortex immediately before reaching and the direction of the subsequent reach by computing tuning curves for a population of cells during the unrotated trials. Prior to reaching on the rotated probe-trials, spiking within this population changed dynamically, such that the orientation represented by this population rotated through 90 degrees. This study showed that apparent noise activity in the motor cortex was actually organized as a coherent, dynamic representation of movements prior to reaching and demonstrated that a covert cognitive process could be observed by characterizing the dynamics of spiking activity in a population of neurons.

Georgopoulos et al.2 is an early demonstration that explanations of spiking activity in the neocortex by the overt stimulus and response properties of static tuning curves could be substantially improved by the addition of experimentally covert, dynamic cognitive variables. Although several more recent studies have shown spiking activity that is most readily explained as cell assembly dynamics in the neocortex35, these investigations into cognition have been hampered by the long training times required for primate experiments (which tend to lead to the use of non-cognitive processes6). In contrast, many behavioral tasks that model cognition are quite amenable to unit recordings in rodents; often allowing for recordings throughout both acquisition and performance of the task7, 8. The combination of these tasks, the highly selective spiking activity of hippocampal pyramidal cells and the well-established use of chronic recording technologies make the hippocampus a model system for studying the representation of cognitive variables in neural discharge.

Dynamic spatial information in hippocampal ensembles

The location of the animal relative to its environment is the clearest signal in the spiking activity of hippocampal pyramidal cells recorded in behaving rodents, leading to the adoption of the term “place cell” 79. Each pyramidal neuron primarily spikes when the animal is in a particular part of its environment (the cell’s place field), which suggests that the activity across the place cell population creates a map-like representation of the animal’s current position in an environment 79. This idea led to the convention of constructing a spatial tuning curve for individual place cells as a time-averaged firing rate map as a function of position 10, 11. This time-independent firing rate map can be interpreted as a tuning curve of firing to spatial position. The strength of the spatial signal, the remarkable stability of these firing rate maps in constant environmental conditions and their response to well-defined changes of the spatial environment 8, 1013 led to the prevailing view that a given hippocampal pyramidal neuron is either a place cell or not active in a given environment 14. Momentary spiking activity that deviated from a cell’s tuning curve was taken as noise and ignored by averaging across several minutes of recording.

Changes in place field firing (under, for example, environmental changes12, 13) were identified as changes between multiple maps1518. This hypothesis led to the suggestion that these transitory events in which spiking activity deviated from a cell’s tuning curve might reflect self-consistent information about other locations or other maps, presumably as the animal shifted its attention from its current position to other kinds of information. If this were true, then one should be able to detect these covert cognitive shifts between input streams by examining the “noise” for statistical structure even if the underlying parameters of that structure are unknown. Identifying cognitive function then entails finding the cognitive bases for the covert parameters: tying these often-fleeting sets of active neurons that coalesce and disappear to the psychological processes that they presumably underlie, such as memory, decision-making, and attention. We review current progress towards these admittedly ambitious goals in the hippocampus and describe a general computational approach towards finding covert cognitive processes in the firing patterns of neural ensembles.

Dynamics at long timescales

While we are mainly concerned with the dynamics of hippocampal neurons at the millisecond timescale, there is abundant evidence for the dynamic influence of cognitive processes on the firing of hippocampal neurons at longer timescales, notably with regard to the property of place cells called “remapping”8, 12,19. While a place cell can indeed have the same place field for months in a constant environment 20, unlike sensory neurons, place cells can also completely change their spatial tuning curves between experiences within an environment 13, 19, 21-23. While this remapping most commonly happens in response to changes in environmental cues (e.g. putting the animal in a novel environment), remapping is not a straightforward sensory transformation of the changed cues 8, 13, 2330. Moreover, place cells can remap without any change whatsoever in the available spatial cues, for instance when an animal changes its behavioral strategy31, uses different coordinate systems 3235 or as information about a context is acquired 36, 37. Often, the remapping can best be thought of as multiple stable states, with different environments or conditions associated with particular sets of place cells that are stably retrieved upon subsequent reintroductions to the appropriate environment, or indeed as the animal switches between tasks or coordinate systems.

What might govern these shifts in the information reflected by hippocampal neurons, and how might they be tied to cognition? Kentros et al.22 found that place fields in mice spontaneously remapped far more often than those of rats under the same simple behavioral conditions (simply chasing after randomly dropped food pellets in a familiar environment). However, mice recorded while accurately performing a spatial task in the same familiar environment had place fields that were as stable as those of rats. Kentros et al. suggested that the key difference was that the animals doing the spatial task had reason to pay attention to the available spatial cues, so they remembered them, which was reflected in the stability of their place fields. The studies described above support the hypothesis that covert cognitive processes such as what the animal is paying attention to influence the long-term dynamics of place fields. However, attention typically operates at much shorter timescales, on the order of seconds to milliseconds. This suggests the possibility of finding task-related shifts in network activity in hippocampal neurons at these timescales as well, as we discuss in the following section.

Dynamics at short timescales

Might the covert variables that control map retrieval upon entering an environment also operate on short timescales to repeatedly switch between multiple maps in constant conditions? If the tuning curve maps were sufficiently different, almost any repetitive map switching would appear as noise in place cell tuning curves38. Indeed, in the simple foraging tasks in which stable place fields are typically observed, a place cell has unexpectedly high levels of spiking variability inside its place field 39. In these tasks, a robust place cell may emit 20 or more action potentials on a single pass through a place field, but fail to emit any action potentials seconds later on a pass that is behaviorally indistinguishable. The statistics of these deviations are incompatible with the hypothesis that place cell activity simply varies randomly about a mean described by a single spatial tuning curve 38, 39 and instead support a hypothesis that place cell activity reflects a small number of spatial tuning curves that differ mainly in firing rate and are alternatively switched on and off with a period of about one second 38, 40, 41.

This proposal is analogous to the suggestion that the hippocampus maintains multiple spatial maps of the environment and somehow switches between those maps very quickly. Support for this proposal comes from studies by Harris et al.42 and Jackson and Redish41. Harris et al. initially showed that predicting the spiking activity of hippocampal place cells using both position and the spiking activities of a set of simultaneously recorded place cells was significantly better than predicting hippocampal place cell activity using position information alone. Harris43 argued that the covariation of place cell spiking activity was evidence for use of multiple cell assemblies within the hippocampus. Jackson and Redish41 showed that coherent fast switching between multiple hippocampal cell assemblies could explain the excess variability observed within place cell spiking activity observed by Fenton and Muller39. Furthermore, Jackson and Redish41 showed that fast switching between cell assemblies was clearly aligned to specific behavioral phases in certain tasks and produced multiple and distinct tuning curve maps. The cell assemblies observed on the linear track, for instance, were generally aligned with the animal’s running direction and their projection onto spatial position was apparent as directional place fields. Although directional place fields have been previously explained as indicative of multiple reference frames (maps) in linear track tasks10, 17, 18, 32, 41, 44, Jackson and Redish41 showed that reference frames are not specific to linear track tasks and can explain the excess variability first identified by Fenton and Muller39.

The high levels of place cell spiking variability is unlikely to be noise, because it has an across cell organization that can be explained as coordinated activity4143. These observations further suggest that internal, unobservable or covert processes mediate the cell’s active tuning curve and, consequently, determines the discharge of the cell at that moment. Observations that these cell assembly dynamics are modulated by cognitive demands 22, 38 and aligned to specific task components 41 suggest that cell assembly dynamics are better described as a reflection of covert cognitive processes than of noise.

Extra-field spikes during sleep, rest, and directed behavior

While place fields described pyramidal cell firing during awake behavior, these same pyramidal cells fired during specific sleeps states (e.g. during sharp-waves occurring within slow-wave sleep and during REM sleep7, 45). This sleep-related firing was difficult to explain from a traditional tuning-curve perspective. Similarly, place fields fire extra spikes outside their place fields during rest, grooming, eating, and other, non-attentive pausing behaviors 7. Subsequent studies showed that these extra-field spikes entail a reactivation of place cell firing sequences during sleep states that is both reliable and coherent. Cell pairs, ensembles, and the temporal order therein that were active during awake behavior are reactivated during subsequent sleep states 4652 - hippocampal pyramidal cell spiking activity continues to be organized with respect to the representation of space during sleep. The firing of hippocampal cells during sleep is better described as replay than as noise. Decoding algorithms applied to neural ensembles found that the decoded location during these rest states deviated from the rat’s observed location53, but Jensen and Lisman were not able to identify any structure in these processes and concluded that they were noise. Recent studies, however, have found that these extra-field spikes occurring during rest can be understood as reactivation of recently experienced behaviors 5459. These results suggest that the noise identified by Jensen and Lisman53 actually contains structure and reflects a covert (cognitive) event.

Johnson and Redish60 examined place cell firing at a decision point. At difficult decision-points rats pause, but remain attentive to their surroundings: they turn back and forth, orienting down potential choices in a process termed vicarious trial-and-error (VTE61). During these behaviors, the hippocampus remains in an active theta state (similar to running behavior7, 60, 62). Johnson and Redish found that during these behaviors, place cells fired spikes even if the animal was outside of the cell’s place field, at a location where the tuning curve predicted zero spikes60. Decoding activity during these behaviors revealed a sequential sweep of positions from the animal’s current position to potential future positions on each arm. The non-local forward representations contained sequential structure, were predominantly ahead of the animal, and were related to the orientation of the animal during the VTE behavior. These data suggest that place cell activity that occurs outside of a cell’s place field signals a covert process related to the prediction of potential spatial positions available to the animal rather that simple noise.

Discussion: Encoding, decoding, and generative approaches

The studies reviewed above suggest a correspondence between covert, cognitive variables and deviation of neural activity from a cell’s tuning curve. The conclusions from these studies result from at least one of three distinct approaches to analysis, encoding, decoding, and generative. Each approach is a distinct way to evaluate the validity of a hypothesized neural code and determine how well a variable can be related to neural activity59 (see Box 1.)

Box 1.

Current technology allows the simultaneous recording of large neural ensembles from awake, behaving animals. The simultaneity of the large neural ensembles allows the identification of both decoded behavioral variables (reconstruction) and the prediction of spike trains (generative models) from hypothesized processes.

Encoding

Tuning curves are generated from the correlations of behavioral variables and simultaneously observed spike trains. Behavioral variables can include sensory inputs, motor outputs, or task-related behavioral variables (as shown here).

Decoding

By definition, a tuning curve encodes a description of an observed variable, such as a behavior as a function of spike trains. Through standard methods, it is possible to invert this description to predict the behavioral variable from an observation of spike trains.

Generative models

Because the tuning curve is a description of the spike train as a function of the behavioral variable, it is possible to predict the spike trains from the observed behavior and/or the value of a hypothesized or decoded covert variable.

Comparisons

The decoded behavioral variable can be compared with the actual behavioral variable. These differences (and similarities) can be examined for structure which provides evidence for cognitive processes. Similarly, the predicted spike trains can be compared with the actual spike trains for differences and similarities which can be examined for underlying structure which provides evidence for cognitive processes.

Figure B.1. Example of the encoding/decoding/generative models cycle.

Figure B.1

Encoding– Tuning curves are generated from behavior variables (here the location of the rat during foraging within a 1m cylinder) and from ensemble spike trains (here showing diagrammatic action potentials from nine of 93 cells; spikes from each cell are indicated by a different color). Decoding–Combining tuning curves and observed spike trains produces decoded variables. This example shows a Bayesian probability distribution of the location of the rat given the tuning curves and spike trains at a given time. White star indicates the observed location of the animal. Generative Models– Combining decoded behavioral variables with tuning curves produces predicted spike trains.

Encoding approaches

If a cell reliably changes its firing as a function of different stimulus conditions or differences in a behavioral parameter, then one can say that the cell is tuned to the parameter in question63. The tuning curve thus describes how activity represents information about the parameter64. Within the encoding approach, validity of a neural code is based on how much information the neural code provides about the variable in question6365. The amount of information can be measured using either Shannon information64 or Fisher information63. However, information measures depend on the concept of a signal separated from noise, and they all effectively measure the extent to which the signal can be differentiated from an unstructured, unexplainable noise component. Identifying covert, cognitive processes using encoding approaches depends on making covert processes overt by aligning cognitive events to behavioral tasks so that they always occur at the same time on each trial or by creating tasks that differ only in the hypothesized covert processes. The encoding approach has enjoyed some success within tightly controlled behavioral experiments on attention22, categorization66, and various other aspects of decision-making67, including complex higher-order transformations of complex variables68, 69. However, encoding approaches offer only limited and indirect methods for assessing the dynamic organization of neural populations that, according to cell assembly concepts, support cognitive function by coordinated changes across many cells within a single trial.

Decoding approaches

Decoding compares a sample of neural activity to an established tuning curve in an attempt to predict the value of the encoded behavioral, environmental or cognitive parameter53, 64, 70, 71. Decoded results that deviate from the observation are interpreted as errors 53, 7072. Average errors are used to distinguish between multiple decoding algorithms7072, but individual decoding errors are typically thought to indicate noise or the inadequacy of the neural code used for decoding.

Analysis of covert, cognitive processes present a distinct problem for the decoding approach to neural codes because the cognitive variables implied by these processes are not generally experimentally observable. In decoding approaches, these cognitive processes will likely appear as decoding errors. To avoid the problem of calling these deviations ‘errors’, decoding approaches to covert, cognitive processes have either highlighted unexpected structure within the distribution of decoding errors or compared the time series of predicted (decoded) cognitive variables with a hypothetical time series that is derived from subjective expectations about the cognitive processing. The application of the decoding approach to cognitive processing has been successful in a number of studies on population vector rotation in motor cortex2, 4 and route replay in hippocampus 49, 53, 54, 5759. However, the decoding approach to cognitive processing provides a weak form of statistical testing because it depends entirely on how the hypothesized cognitive time series has been defined. As a result, decoding approaches to covert cognitive processes are usually reducible to an extended cross correlation analysis that is informed by each cell’s tuning curve, and this requires the potentially dubious assumption that the covert cognitive variable is constant across many trials2, 4, 48, 49, 51, 55, 73.

Generative approaches

The studies of within session multi-stability and extra-field spiking used what we call the generative approach (see Box 1). The generative approach exploits the fact that a tuning curve is not merely descriptive; it can also be used as a basis for generating a prediction of neural activity from a given set of behavioral, environmental or cognitive parameters39, 41, 42, 59, 64. The generative approach enables direct comparisons between the predictions that a spike should occur and actual observations. The generative approach can be used in flexible tasks that are more likely to involve cognition. As a result, the generative approach provides a framework for detecting and quantifying the multi-neuron cell assembly dynamics that Hebb (1949) originally proposed.

Support for the validity of a proposed neural code within the generative approach is determined by how well an observed set of neural activity can be predicted from behavioral, environmental or cognitive variables41, 42, 59, 64. At a superficial level, the application of the generative approach to covert, cognitive processes faces the same problem encountered within decoding approaches (that a hypothetical time series of a cognitive variable must be proposed for evaluating validity). However, several applications of the generative approach have circumvented this problem by inferring the time series of a covert variable’s value on the basis of neural ensemble activity 41, 42, 59. Instead of relying on assumed psychological processes, these variants of the generative approach either implicitly42 or explicitly41, 59 used decoding algorithms to infer the time series of the covert variable. These studies showed that apparent noise at the single unit level was actually self-consistent within the set of simultaneously recorded neurons. The ensemble activity was coordinated even when it deviated from what the experimenter expected given the animal’s position in the maze and presumed spatial map; these experimentally unexpected deviations were consistent with switching between spatial maps41, route replay59 and other non-local spatial representations60. The success of the generative approach in identification of covert processes illustrates the power of basing validity tests of the neural code on a biological signal (ensemble activity) instead of relying on more indirect expectations that arbitrarily delimit cognition.

Conclusion

Cognitive processes fundamentally and often subtly change how information is represented in the brain. As a result, understanding the neural bases of cognition requires one to distinguish between noise and covert, internally generated changes in neuronal firing patterns. The generative approach provides a distinct advantage over approaches that stop at encoding or decoding because generative approaches can quantitatively differentiate between random noise and structured variability that is by definition not noise, but cannot be accommodated by the tuning curve of the neuron. As techniques for recording large neural ensembles become a more standard tool for investigating the neural basis of cognition across the brain, the theoretical problem of how to analyze neural data under conditions where constancy of the cognitive variable cannot be safely assumed becomes critically important. Analysis of this ”excess” variability provides a point of departure for understanding how attention, memory consolidation, and decision-making contribute to and refine traditional ideas of spatial representations present in hippocampal activity. Further technological advances in the quality and density of recording neural ensembles will offer unprecedented opportunities to understand the dynamics of neural population activity with the promise of defining many of the largely covert, subjective processes we call cognition in terms of objective biological mechanisms.

Fig. 1. Decoded direction from a population of motor cortex neurons shows mental rotation.

Fig. 1

Motor cortex neurons were recorded during direct reaches to a target or during reaches to a location offset by 90 degrees. In standard trials, decoding yielded a constant direction. In contrast, decoding during reaches 90 degrees counterclockwise from the target produced a decoded direction of reaching that rotated systematically from the trained direction to the rotated target. This covert, mental rotation of the planned movement could only be observed through the decoding process. The population in this study were recorded sequentially, not simultaneously, which makes the assumption of a constant cognitive representational dynamic from trial to trial. Modern technology enabling the simultaneous recording of large neural ensembles has alleviated this constraint.

Fig. 2. Remapping depends on covert, cognitive variables.

Fig. 2

(A) Observations of place field distributions between two experiences on a task show remapping under some conditions but not others. (B) These effects are significantly modulated by cognitive variables, such as attention to the presence of a spatial task. (C) Diagram of the identification of structure in the place cell remapping. Tuning curves (green) are generated from observations of spiking data and behavioral variables (orange). The likelihood of remapping is dependent on covert processes of attention, task, and behavioral relevance.

Fig. 3. During simple tasks, there is noise in place fields that can be explained as changes in covert, cognitive variables.

Fig. 3

(A) The noise in place field firing differs between attentive and goal-related conditions. In general foraging tasks, there is a high variability. In tasks with a goal, the variability decreases. Taking only firing during goal-approach produces variability consistent with the predicted tuning curves. (B) Using generative models, it is possible to split place cell firing rate maps apart using hypothesized covert variables. [S = split by covert variable; R = random shuffled control]. (C) Diagram of the identification of structure as covert variables. Cell assembly dynamics suggest the presence of covert variables (purple). Tuning curves (green) are generated by combining observations of spiking data and behavioral variables (orange) with those hypothesized covert variables (purple). The hypothesized covert variables explain large portions of the residual “noise” (blue).

Fig 4. Replay after behavior is an example of a covert, cognitive event identified by clear structure within extraneous spiking.

Fig 4

(A) Direct examination of cell firing shows that cells fire in the same sequence during post-behavior sleep [bottom] as the place fields during behavior [top]. (B) Direct observation of firing during sharp-waves during awake states shows sequential replay akin to that seen during sleep. (C) Diagram of the identification of structure in the noise. Tuning curves (green) are generated from observations of spiking data and behavioral variables (orange, top). When compared to observations of spiking data from sleep states (orange, bottom), these tuning curves can be used to decode represented position (blue). Structure in the decoding leads to the conclusion that there are covert variables (replay). (D) Diagram of the identification of structure in the noise during awake states. Tuning curves (green) are generated from observations of spiking data and behavioral variables (orange, top). During sharp-waves, there is a mismatch between the spiking predicted by the tuning curves and the observed spiking. When decoding represented position (blue), structure in the decoding leads to the conclusion that there are covert variables (replay).

Fig 5. Extraneous spiking during awake behaviors can be explained by covert, cognitive “planning-like” events.

Fig 5

(A) Decoding extra-field spikes at the choice point shows non-local sweeps of locations ahead of the animal down potential paths. (B) These decoded variables are significantly different at the choice point relative to a matched-time approach to that choice point. (C) Diagram of the identification of structure in the noise during awake states. Tuning curves (green) are generated from observations of spiking data and behavioral variables (orange, top). At the choice-point, there is a mismatch between the spiking predicted by the tuning curves and the observed spiking. When decoding represented position (blue), structure in the decoding leads to the conclusion that there are covert variables (forward sweeps).

Acknowledgments

We thank Matthijs van der Meer, Anoopum Gupta, Jadin Jackson, David Crowe, Matt Chafee, Dave Rowland, Aldis Weible, and the Cognitive Sciences Center at the University of Minnesota for helpful discussions. This work was supported by NIH grant MH080318 (ADR), NSF grant IOS0725001 (AAF), NIH training grant (T32 HD007151) (AJ), and a Dissertation Fellowship from the Graduate School at the University of Minnesota (AJ).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributor Information

André A. Fenton, Email: afenton@downstate.edu.

Cliff Kentros, Email: cliff@uoneuro.uoregon.edu.

A. David Redish, Email: redish@umn.edu.

References

  • 1.Hebb DO. The organization of behavior, a neuropsychological theory. Wiley; 1949. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Georgopoulos AP, et al. Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. Science. 1989;243:234–236. doi: 10.1126/science.2911737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Averbeck BB, et al. Parallel processing of serial movements in prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:13172–13177. doi: 10.1073/pnas.162485599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Crowe DA, et al. Dynamics of parietal neural activity during spatial cognitive processing. Neuron. 2005;47:885–891. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Vaadia E, et al. Dynamics of neuronal interactions' cannot be explained by 'neuronal transients. Proc Biol Sci. 1995;261:407–410. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hikosaka O, et al. Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures. Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:464–471. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01439-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.O'Keefe J, Nadel L. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Clarendon Press; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Redish AD. Beyond the cognitive map : from place cells to episodic memory. MIT Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Muller R. A quarter of a century of place cells. Neuron. 1996;17:813–822. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80214-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.McNaughton BL, et al. The contributions of position, direction, and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats. Exp Brain Res. 1983;52:41–49. doi: 10.1007/BF00237147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Muller RU, et al. Spatial firing patterns of hippocampal complex-spike cells in a fixed environment. J Neurosci. 1987;7:1935–1950. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-07-01935.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Muller RU, Kubie JL. The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells. J Neurosci. 1987;7:1951–1968. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-07-01951.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bostock E, et al. Experience-dependent modifications of hippocampal place cell firing. Hippocampus. 1991;1:193–205. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450010207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Thompson LT, Best PJ. Place cells and silent cells in the hippocampus of freely-behaving rats. J Neurosci. 1989;9:2382–2390. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-07-02382.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.McNaughton BL, et al. Deciphering the hippocampal polyglot: the hippocampus as a path integration system. J Exp Biol. 1996;199:173–185. doi: 10.1242/jeb.199.1.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Samsonovich A, McNaughton BL. Path integration and cognitive mapping in a continuous attractor neural network model. J Neurosci. 1997;17:5900–5920. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-15-05900.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Touretzky DS, Redish AD. Theory of rodent navigation based on interacting representations of space. Hippocampus. 1996;6:247–270. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:3<247::AID-HIPO4>3.0.CO;2-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Redish AD, Touretzky DS. Cognitive maps beyond the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 1997;7:15–35. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1997)7:1<15::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Colgin LL, et al. Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:469–477. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Thompson LT, Best PJ. Long-term stability of the place-field activity of single units recorded from the dorsal hippocampus of freely behaving rats. Brain Res. 1990;509:299–308. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90555-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Barnes CA, et al. Multistability of cognitive maps in the hippocampus of old rats. Nature. 1997;388:272–275. doi: 10.1038/40859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kentros CG, et al. Increased attention to spatial context increases both place field stability and spatial memory. Neuron. 2004;42:283–295. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00192-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Leutgeb S, et al. Independent codes for spatial and episodic memory in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Science. 2005;309:619–623. doi: 10.1126/science.1114037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tanila H, et al. Discordance of spatial representation in ensembles of hippocampal place cells. Hippocampus. 1997;7:613–623. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1997)7:6<613::AID-HIPO4>3.0.CO;2-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Knierim JJ, et al. Interactions between idiothetic cues and external landmarks in the control of place cells and head direction cells. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:425–446. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Guzowski JF, et al. Environment-specific expression of the immediate-early gene Arc in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:1120–1124. doi: 10.1038/16046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Redish AD, et al. Independence of firing correlates of anatomically proximate hippocampal pyramidal cells. J Neurosci. 2001;21:RC134. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-05-j0004.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hayman RM, et al. Context-specific acquisition of location discrimination by hippocampal place cells. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18:2825–2834. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.03035.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Leutgeb JK, et al. Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science. 2007;315:961–966. doi: 10.1126/science.1135801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wills TJ, et al. Attractor dynamics in the hippocampal representation of the local environment. Science. 2005;308:873–876. doi: 10.1126/science.1108905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Markus EJ, et al. Interactions between location and task affect the spatial and directional firing of hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci. 1995;15:7079–7094. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-11-07079.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gothard KM, et al. Dynamics of mismatch correction in the hippocampal ensemble code for space: interaction between path integration and environmental cues. J Neurosci. 1996;16:8027–8040. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-24-08027.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gothard KM, et al. Binding of hippocampal CA1 neural activity to multiple reference frames in a landmark-based navigation task. J Neurosci. 1996;16:823–835. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-02-00823.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Redish AD, et al. Dynamics of hippocampal ensemble activity realignment: time versus space. J Neurosci. 2000;20:9298–9309. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-24-09298.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rosenzweig ES, et al. Hippocampal map realignment and spatial learning. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:609–615. doi: 10.1038/nn1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Moita MA, et al. Putting fear in its place: remapping of hippocampal place cells during fear conditioning. J Neurosci. 2004;24:7015–7023. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5492-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Sharp PE, et al. Influences of vestibular and visual motion information on the spatial firing patterns of hippocampal place cells. J Neurosci. 1995;15:173–189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-01-00173.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Olypher AV, et al. Properties of the extra-positional signal in hippocampal place cell discharge derived from the overdispersion in location-specific firing. Neuroscience. 2002;111:553–566. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00586-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Fenton AA, Muller RU. Place cell discharge is extremely variable during individual passes of the rat through the firing field. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:3182–3187. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lansky P, et al. The overdispersion in activity of place cells. Neurocomputing. 2001;38–40:1393–1399. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Jackson J, Redish AD. Network dynamics of hippocampal cell-assemblies resemble multiple spatial maps within single tasks. Hippocampus. 2007;17:1209–1229. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Harris KD, et al. Organization of cell assemblies in the hippocampus. Nature. 2003;424:552–556. doi: 10.1038/nature01834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Harris KD. Neural signatures of cell assembly organization. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:399–407. doi: 10.1038/nrn1669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.O'Keefe J, Recce ML. Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus. 1993;3:317–330. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450030307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pavlides C, Winson J. Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodes. J Neurosci. 1989;9:2907–2918. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-08-02907.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Louie K, Wilson MA. Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron. 2001;29:145–156. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00186-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science. 1994;265:676–679. doi: 10.1126/science.8036517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL. Replay of neuronal firing sequences in rat hippocampus during sleep following spatial experience. Science. 1996;271:1870–1873. doi: 10.1126/science.271.5257.1870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lee AK, Wilson MA. Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep. Neuron. 2002;36:1183–1194. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01096-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.O'Neill J, et al. Reactivation of experience-dependent cell assembly patterns in the hippocampus. Nat Neurosci. 2008 doi: 10.1038/nn2037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Nadasdy Z, et al. Replay and time compression of recurring spike sequences in the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 1999;19:9497–9507. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-21-09497.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kudrimoti HS, et al. Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4090–4101. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04090.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Jensen O, Lisman JE. Position reconstruction from an ensemble of hippocampal place cells: contribution of theta phase coding. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:2602–2609. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.5.2602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Foster DJ, Wilson MA. Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature. 2006;440:680–683. doi: 10.1038/nature04587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Jackson JC, et al. Hippocampal sharp waves and reactivation during awake states depend on repeated sequential experience. J Neurosci. 2006;26:12415–12426. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4118-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.O'Neill J, et al. Place-selective firing of CA1 pyramidal cells during sharp wave/ripple network patterns in exploratory behavior. Neuron. 2006;49:143–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Diba K, Buzsaki G. Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1241–1242. doi: 10.1038/nn1961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Csicsvari J, et al. Place-selective firing contributes to the reverse-order reactivation of CA1 pyramidal cells during sharp waves in open-field exploration. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;26:704–716. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05684.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Johnson A, Redish AD. Measuring distributed properties of neural representations beyond the decoding of local variables — implications for cognition. In: Holscher C, Munk MHJ, editors. Mechanisms of information processing in the Brain: Encoding of information in neural populations and networks. Cambridge University Press; 2008. pp. 95–119. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Johnson A, Redish AD. Neural ensembles in CA3 transiently encode paths forward of the animal at a decision point. J Neurosci. 2007;27:12176–12189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3761-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Tolman EC. Prediction of vicarious trial and error by means of the schematic sowbug. Psychological ReviewPsychological Review. 1939;46:318–336. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Vanderwolf CH. Limbic-diencephalic mechanisms of voluntary movement. Psychol Rev. 1971;78:83–113. doi: 10.1037/h0030672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Series P, et al. Tuning curve sharpening for orientation selectivity: coding efficiency and the impact of correlations. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:1129–1135. doi: 10.1038/nn1321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Rieke F, et al. Spikes. MIT Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Averbeck BB, et al. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:358–366. doi: 10.1038/nrn1888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Wallis JD, et al. Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature. 2001;411:953–956. doi: 10.1038/35082081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Platt ML, Glimcher PW. Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature. 1999;400:233–238. doi: 10.1038/22268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Fortes AF, et al. Comparative and categorical spatial judgments in the monkey: "high" and "low". Anim Cogn. 2004;7:101–108. doi: 10.1007/s10071-003-0195-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Padoa-Schioppa C, Assad JA. Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature. 2006;441:223–226. doi: 10.1038/nature04676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Zhang K, et al. Interpreting neuronal population activity by reconstruction: unified framework with application to hippocampal place cells. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:1017–1044. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Brown EN, et al. A statistical paradigm for neural spike train decoding applied to position prediction from ensemble firing patterns of rat hippocampal place cells. J Neurosci. 1998;18:7411–7425. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-18-07411.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Salinas E, Abbott LF. Vector reconstruction from firing rates. J Comput Neurosci. 1994;1:89–107. doi: 10.1007/BF00962720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Euston DR, et al. Fast-forward playback of recent memory sequences in prefrontal cortex during sleep. Science. 2007;318:1147–1150. doi: 10.1126/science.1148979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES