Abstract
Persistent neural activity, responses that outlast the stimuli that evoke them, plays an important role in neural computations and possibly in processes, such as working memory. Recent studies suggest that trace eyelid conditioning, which involves a temporal gap between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli (the trace interval), requires persistent neural activity in a region of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). This persistent activity, which could be conveyed to cerebellum via a pathway through pons, may engage the cerebellum and allow for the expression of conditioned responses. Given the substantial reciprocity observed among many brain regions, it is essential to demonstrate that persistent responses in mPFC neurons are not simply a reflection of cerebellar feedback to the forebrain, leaving open the possibility that such responses could serve as input to the cerebellum. This concern is highlighted by studies showing that hippocampal learning-related activity is abolished by cerebellar inactivation. We inactivated the cerebellum while recording single-unit activity from the mPFC of rabbits trained with a forebrain-dependent trace eyelid conditioning procedure. We report that, whereas the responses of cells that show an onset of increased spike activity during the trace interval were abolished by cerebellar inactivation, persistent responses that begin during the conditioned stimulus and persisted into the trace interval were unaffected. Therefore, conditioned stimulus-evoked persistent responses remain the strongest candidate input pattern to support the cerebellar expression of learned responses.
Introduction
It is important to determine not only the function of each brain system but also how brain systems interact. Given the ubiquitous presence of reciprocal connectivity in the brain, it is possible that some of the spike responses observed in a putatively upstream brain region could actually reflect feedback from a downstream structure. Trace eyelid conditioning provides an opportunity to study such interactions because the expression of learned responses depends on a pathway between the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and cerebellum (Kronforst-Collins and Disterhoft, 1998; Takehara et al., 2003; Kalmbach et al., 2009), via the pons (Weible et al., 2007; Siegel et al., 2012). Trace conditioning involves paired presentations of a conditioned stimulus (CS) followed by an unconditioned stimulus (US), with a stimulus-free trace interval between CS offset and US onset. It had been generally assumed that the cerebellum can support learning only when there is temporal overlap between stimuli (Ito et al., 1982; Weiss and Disterhoft, 1996; Mauk and Donegan, 1997). A recent study directly confirmed this assumption using microstimulation of cerebellar inputs in place of the CS and demonstrating that animals could not learn unless cerebellar inputs were active within 300 ms of US onset (Kalmbach et al., 2009). Importantly, during trace conditioning, neurons in the mPFC respond to the CS with spike activity that persists into the trace interval to overlap (or nearly so) with the timing of the US (so-called “persistent” responses) (Takehara-Nishiuchi and Mcnaughton, 2008; Siegel et al., 2012). Regions of mPFC that show persistent activity project to pontine regions that also show persistent activity, supporting the hypothesis that the role of the mPFC in trace conditioning may be to provide the cerebellum with an input that overlaps with the US (Siegel et al., 2012). However, a second response type was also observed in the mPFC that overlaps with the timing of the US, which displayed increased activity during the trace interval (after CS offset) and appeared to be highly correlated with the animal's learned eyelid response (i.e., “trace interval” responses; see Fig. 1A) (Siegel et al., 2012).
We used reversible inactivation to block cerebellar output during trace eyelid conditioning while recording neurons in the mPFC to determine which spike response pattern(s) are independent of cerebellar output (i.e., do not reflect feedback) and therefore remain candidate sources of the input to the cerebellum necessary for the expression of learned responses. We found that persistent responses were spared by cerebellar inactivations that abolish learned behavioral responses, whereas trace interval responses were abolished along with learned behavior during inactivations. The data suggest that trace interval responses reflect feedback originating from the motor output pathway, whereas persistent responses are a consequence neither of cerebellar output nor the expression of the learned responses. We propose that an mPFC persistent response to the CS is the strongest candidate source of input to the cerebellum necessary for trace eyelid conditioning.
Materials and Methods
Subjects and surgical procedures.
All surgical and experimental procedures were approved by the University of Texas at Austin Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health guidelines. Six New Zealand albino rabbits (males, 2.5–4 kg) were implanted with infusion guide cannula targeting the anterior interpositus and a custom microdrive housing 18 independently moveable tetrodes targeting the region of mPFC that is necessary for trace eyelid conditioning (Kronforst-Collins and Disterhoft, 1998; Kalmbach et al., 2009). Each rabbit was prepared for sterile surgery by subcutaneous injection of ketamine (45 mg/kg) and acepromazine (1.5 mg/kg) and mounted in a specialized stereotaxic apparatus (with λ1.5 mm below bregma). Surgical depth then maintained with 1–3% isoflurane. Microdrive tetrode bundles (∼1.5 mm in diameter) were positioned on the brain's surface over the right posterior mPFC (centered at 3.0 mm anterior to bregma and 1.5 mm lateral from the midline) and secured to the skull with skull screws and dental cement. In addition to recording electrodes, a single infusion guide cannula was implanted in each rabbit such that the tip was positioned 1.2 mm above the contralateral anterior interpositus nucleus (0.7 mm anterior, 5 mm lateral, and 13.3 mm ventral to λ). Rabbits were also prepared with a head bolt fixed in dental cement over the anterior skull (to hold the eyelid detector during conditioning), and two stainless steel periorbital stimulating electrodes implanted subdurally just anterior and posterior to the upper eyelid contralateral to the mPFC microdrive and ipsilateral to the interpositus infusion guide cannula. Example histology regarding placements of cannula in the cerebellum and estimated single-unit recording sites in the mPFC are shown in Figures 1B and 2. Each rabbit was given at least 1 week of recovery before training began.
Behavioral training and analysis.
Rabbits were trained with a trace eyelid conditioning protocol as previously described (Siegel et al., 2012). Briefly, rabbits were gently restrained and placed in shielded, sound-attenuating chambers. Daily training sessions were controlled by custom software and consisted of 12 blocks of 9 trials for a total of 108 trials per session, with the first trial of every block being a CS-only probe. Trials were presented at random intertrial intervals between 25 and 35 s. Each trial consisted of a 500 ms CS (a 1.3 kHz pure tone) with onset/offset rise/fall times of 5 ms to avoid audible onset and offset transients (clicks), followed by a 500 ms stimulus-free period (the trace interval) and terminated with a 50 ms train of current pulses (1 ms pulse width at 100 Hz) delivered across the periorbital electrodes (the US). The US intensity was carefully adjusted for each rabbit to just above threshold for full eyelid closure (between 1 and 3 mA). For each trial, beginning 200 ms before tone onset, the position of the external eyelid was measured for 2.5 s with an infrared emitter and collector assembly held in front of the eye and fixed to the head bolt. Eyelid responses recorded during training trials were digitized at 1 kHz and stored for subsequent offline analyses. Before each training session, the eyelid detector was calibrated by measuring the voltage response produced by a full eyelid closure and defining that voltage change as 6.0 mm (the amplitude of a full eyelid closure in rabbit). A CR was defined as an eyelid closure that exceeded 0.3 mm between CS and US onsets. Trials with eyelid responses >0.3 mm in the 200 ms before CS onset were excluded from behavioral and spike analyses (never >5 trials/session).
Cerebellar infusion procedure.
Before infusions began, each rabbit was trained for at least two sessions beyond the session during which a standard criterion of eight conditioned responses in nine consecutive trials was met (Siegel et al., 2012). To reversibly inactivate the anterior interpositus, we infused a solution of 1 mm muscimol (Tocris Bioscience) dissolved in artificial CSF (in mm as follows: 119.0 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 26.0 NaHCO3, 2.0 CaCl2, 2.0 MgCl2, 10.0 dextrose, 10.0 HEPES, pH adjusted to 7.35–7.4, and passed through a 2 μm filter to sterilize). The solution was loaded into a Hamilton syringe fitted with tygon tubing and a sterile 1.2-mm-long 33-gauge infusion cannula. The syringe was mounted in an electric pump apparatus (Bioanalytical Systems, models MD-1001 and 1020) to allow for fine control over the rate of infusion.
For infusion sessions, rabbits first received either 3 blocks (7 sessions from 2 rabbits) or 6 blocks (14 sessions from 4 rabbits) of training to collect baseline single-unit activity. The training session was then paused and the infusion cannula inserted into the deep nuclei of the cerebellum (DCN) guide cannula. Muscimol was infused at a rate of 0.2 μl/min for 10 min (2 μl total volume). The session was resumed 5 min after the completion of the infusion. An infusion was considered effective when the likelihood of CRs decreased <50% for a given block, followed by <15% CRs observed for all remaining blocks. Importantly, rabbits still showed unconditioned reflexive responses to the US (examples given in Figs. 3, 4, 5), and so the abolishment of CRs is likely the result of selective inactivation of the motor output pathway that drives the learned response, and not the result of a general disfacilitation (Pacheco-Calderón et al., 2012). Most rabbits showed an immediate effect upon resuming the session that lasted for the remainder of the session, whereas 2 rabbits (4 sessions) showed a delay in the onset of the effect (by 1–3 blocks) but showed a nearly complete effect thereafter. The behavioral results of infusion sessions were bimodal; infusion sessions that did not meet the above behavioral criteria as effective were not different from noninfusion session comparisons (see Results). Therefore, ineffective infusion sessions were included as controls (6 sessions from 4 rabbits) in addition to noninfusion sessions (taken the day before successful infusions; 14 sessions from 5 rabbits). For most rabbits in which effective inactivation was observed, at least 3 infusion sessions could be performed with successful results before the manipulation became ineffective, presumably because of the buildup of gliosis at the infusion site that prevented sufficient diffusion of the drug. However, one rabbit continued to be effective for 6 infusion sessions, until tetrodes had reached the ventral extent of the mPFC and no more data could be collected. We took advantage of this opportunity to infuse Alexa 594-conjugated dextran (1 μl at 0.2 μl/min; Invitrogen; 10,000 MW) to estimate the degree of diffusion.
Single-unit recordings.
Neural activity was acquired during control and infusion sessions with a Digital Lynx system (Neuralynx). Tetrodes were constructed from polyimide-coated nichrome wire (12 μm diameter, Kanthal Palm Coast) and gold-plated to an impedance of 0.5–1.25 MΩ at 1 kHz. Signals were passed to a multichannel, unity-gain headstage, amplified and bandpass filtered between 600 and 6000 Hz, and digitized at 32 kHz for subsequent offline analysis. Neural data were synchronized with the presentation of training stimuli by triggering the Digital Lynx I/O port with the same TTL pulses used to trigger CS and US stimuli. The first noninfusion control sessions typically began 3 sessions after rabbits achieved asymptotic performance and were followed the next day by an infusion session. After the initial set of sessions, to ensure that a given neuron was not included more than once between infusion sessions, tetrodes were advanced a minimum of 160 μm from the site of previous single-unit activity after each DCN infusion session (Siegel et al., 2012). Tetrodes were allowed to settle for 3 d to maximize recording stability and were adjusted minimally between the initial noninfusion recording session and the infusion session recorded on the following day, after which tetrodes with isolated single-unit activity were moved another 160 μm and the procedure repeated until infusions were no longer effective or until all tetrodes appeared to have reached the ventral extent of the mPFC. In short, rabbits received muscimol infusions no sooner than the fourth session of asymptotic performance and additional infusions approximately once per week until infusions were no longer effective (n = 4) or until tetrodes had reached the ventral aspect of the mPFC (n = 1; typically 3 infusion sessions were possible, and in one case 6 were effective). Tetrodes in the lateral mPFC reached the ventral extent (the corpus callosum) well before more medial tetrodes (e.g., see Fig. 2), and so single-unit activity could only be recorded from the medial half of tetrodes for some sessions.
The activity of single units was isolated offline using interactive cluster-cutting software (WinClust; J. J. Knierim and M. D. Mauk, adapted from M. A. Wilson), as previously described (Siegel et al., 2012). Briefly, isolated clusters of suprathreshold neural events with common waveform parameters were identified as single units, and the time stamps of those events (spikes) extracted and analyzed relative to the presentation of training stimuli and behavioral responses. Only single units with clustered points that showed little or no overlap (<10%) with another cluster and with unisolated background activity were included in the analysis (Siegel et al., 2008, 2012). The spike width for each spike assigned to a cluster was calculated as the time between the peak and valley of the waveform. The average spike rate for each cluster of single-unit activity was calculated as the total number of spikes observed during a session divided by the length of that session (in seconds).
Histological procedures.
At the conclusion of experiments, all rabbits were killed with an intravenous injection of Euthasol (0.3 ml/kg) and perfused intracardially with 0.9% saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde. The brains were extracted and cryoprotected in a 30% sucrose solution before sectioning the mPFC and cerebellum on a freezing microtome (50 μm sections). Sections were mounted on Microfrost Plus slides (Fisher Scientific), dried overnight, then stained with cresyl violet to visualize tetrode or cannula tracks and surrounding anatomical structures. Approximate recording locations of single-unit activity were determined and plotted on representative images as previously described (Siegel et al., 2012). The sections of cerebellum from the rabbit that received the fluorescent dextran infusion were cleared with xylenes and coverslipped with DPX mounting medium. Fluorescent images were acquired with a Zeiss Imager Z2 microscope. Images of background autofluorescence were taken to visualize anatomical structure (Zeiss filter set 38; excitation BP 470/40, dichroic FT 495, emission BP 525/50), whereas conjugated dextran was visualized using the appropriate Alexa-594 filter set (Zeiss set 71; excitation BP 594/24, dichroic FT 615, emission BP 675/100).
Analysis.
Three analyses were used to assess the effects of DCN inactivation on isolated single-unit responses to trace eyelid conditioning: a categorical analysis to determine whether cells qualified for the same response category before and after infusions, a spike response analysis to determine whether there were changes in spike rate as a result of DCN inactivation, and a final analysis to examine the time course of changes in spike responses within and across trials.
Categorical analysis.
For each isolated single unit, the spikes observed during each trial of a session were assigned to 100 ms time bins, beginning 1 s before CS onset and extending 3 s after US onset (Siegel et al., 2012). The average number of spikes observed for the 10 bins before CS onset was used as the baseline activity for that trial. The statistical reliability of changes in spike activity for each time bin across the trials in a session was determined by comparing the number of spikes observed in that bin with the average baseline activity for each trial. For each time bin (5 bins during the CS, 5 bins during the trace interval, and 30 US and post-trial bins), a significant change in spike activity was determined with a paired t test between the number of spikes observed for that bin and the baseline (with Bonferonni correction for the number of time bin comparisons, α = 0.05/40 time bins, p < 0.00125).
Single units were then categorized according to the pattern of significant changes in activity during trace conditioning trials (Siegel et al., 2012). “Persistent” responses were defined as a significant increase (or decrease) in spike activity that began before the end of the CS and persisted at least 2 bins into the trace interval (e.g., see Fig. 3A,c9.1, and Fig. 4), based on the timing parameters of inputs previously shown to engage cerebellar learning in rabbits (Kalmbach et al., 2009). “Trace interval” responses began after CS offset and before US onset (e.g., see Fig. 3, c16.1 and c10.2, and Fig. 5). A third response type was “phasic” responses to the CS that began during the CS but failed to meet persistent criteria (e.g., see Fig. 3A, c16.1, and Fig. 5A, c21.1). Initial categorization was based on the response profile observed before infusions, or for the first half of the session for noninfusion controls. Single units were then recategorized based on the response profile observed after the expression of CRs was pharmacologically blocked (or during the second half of the session for controls). Cells that failed to qualify for the same category during DCN inactivation were scored as showing a change in response pattern.
Analysis of changes in spike activity during the trace interval.
For each single-unit categorized as showing persistent or trace interval responses, differences scores were calculated by subtracting the average number of spikes during the 500 ms trace interval across preinfusion trials from that observed during the postinfusion trials of a given session (or for the first and second halves of noninfusion control sessions). Phasic tone cells were also analyzed by calculating the difference between the average number of spikes observed during the 500 ms CS epoch between preinfusion and postinfusion trials. The pre-post difference scores were compared between control and infusion sessions for each response type (two-tailed t tests with Bonferroni correction across response types). To further test whether decreased spike responses during the trace interval were abolished to baseline levels, difference scores for persistent and trace interval cells were calculated by subtracting the average number of spikes observed during the trace interval from that observed during a 500 ms baseline. (or between the CS period and baseline for phasic tone cells). We compared the difference scores across the three cell types before and after infusions (two-tailed paired t test, α = 0.05 with Bonferroni correction for family wise comparisons).
Analysis of binned and averaged raster plots.
The activity changes of mPFC persistent and trace interval cells were analyzed within and across trials by transforming the raster plots of each single-unit to a matrix. For each cell, spikes were assigned to 100 ms bins across each trial (x-axis; 10 pretrial bins, 5 CS bins, 5 trace interval bins, 10 post-trial bins) while conserving individual trials on the y-axis. The resulting 30 × 108 bin matrix of each cell (number of trial bins × number of trials) was normalized by subtracting the average number of spikes per bin observed during the pre-CS baseline from the entire matrix. The matrices of persistent and trace interval cells were averaged together according to response type and experimental condition (control or DCN inactivation), and smoothed by a single pass with a 3 × 3 median filter. Analysis that tested for changes in activity across blocks of trials within a session (9 trials/block) was used to examine the time course of changes in spike activity during the trace interval after DCN inactivation (i.e., along the y-axis; unpaired two-tailed t tests, α = 0.05 with Bonferonni correction within cell type). Analysis across the trial time (x-axis) examined changes in activity in response to presentation of trace conditioning stimuli (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, α = 0.05 with Bonferonni correction within cell type).
Results
We used cerebellar inactivation to block the motor output that drives CRs to investigate whether the persistent and/or trace interval responses observed in the mPFC may reflect a kind of feedback from the cerebellar output pathway and which pattern(s) do not, and so would remain viable candidate input patterns from the mPFC to the cerebellum (Fig. 1A, bottom). We also analyzed cells that showed “phasic” CS-evoked responses as a within manipulation control, given that such a response pattern does not overlap with the US and therefore would be unable to engage cerebellar learning. Any changes in phasic spike responses as a result of cerebellar inactivation would reflect a more global effect on mPFC cells beyond the specific response patterns hypothesized to directly support the task.
Effects of muscimol infusion in the cerebellum on trace conditioned responses
Infusions of muscimol into the cerebellum had a bimodal effect on CRs. The expression of CRs was either abolished (mean CR% after infusion = 05.5 ± 0.5, range 0–21%, 15 sessions from 5 rabbits; examples given as waterfall plots in Figs. 3A, 4A, and 5A; for specific criteria, see Materials and Methods), or little to no effect was observed (mean CR% after ineffective infusion = 76.5 ± 0.8, range 69–85%, 6 sessions from 4 rabbits). Ineffective infusions may have been the result of misplaced or obstructed cannula. Figure 1B shows the locations of each infusion site and the estimated diffusion of drug (based on the maximum volume infused at that site). Infusion sites that were ventral or posterior to the anterior interpositus were either ineffective or required greater volumes of drug to be infused to abolish learned responses. However, even at effective sites that were well targeted to the anterior interpositus, it is likely that the drug also diffused to neighboring nuclei. Therefore, we will refer to effective infusions more generally as anterior DCN inactivations. For the present study, the overriding goal was to compare mPFC single-unit responses between abolished versus intact expression of CRs. It should be noted that, although no partial effects of DCN infusions were observed, two rabbits (4 sessions) showed a delay in the onset of CR abolition by 10–30 trials (e.g., see Fig. 5A), presumably reflecting the time required for the muscimol to diffuse to the effective region. For these sessions, analysis of changes in mPFC spike responses during trace conditioning trials was adjusted according to the delayed onset (for behavioral criteria to define effective DCN inactivation, see Materials and Methods). Because the behavior between ineffective infusion and noninfusion sessions was not different (t(11.26) = 1.08, p = 0.30; Fig. 1C), neural data from both session types were pooled as control data for comparison with effective DCN inactivation sessions.
Single-unit activity recorded during trace eyelid conditioning
A total of 323 single units were recorded from the mPFC of 6 rabbits over 35 trace conditioning sessions (averaging 54 cells/rabbit and 9 cells/session). Effective DCN inactivation was observed during 15 of the 21 cerebellar infusion sessions (n = 147 cells from 5 rabbits, each contributing 44, 39, 32, 30, and 2 cells, respectively). Tetrodes with single-unit activity were moved a minimum of 160 μm between infusion sessions, and so cells recorded over more than one session from the same rabbit represent unique samples of mPFC neurons relative to other infusion sessions recorded from the same animal. Sessions from ineffective infusions served as infusion control sessions (n = 47 cells during 6 sessions from 4 rabbits). Sessions recorded one day before effective inactivation sessions served as noninfusion controls that likely included activity from many of the same single units as were recorded during effective infusion sessions because the tetrodes were not adjusted between these 2 days (n = 129 cells during 14 sessions from 5 rabbits, each contributing 50, 21, 36, 20, and 2 cells, respectively; the session before one effective inactivation was an ineffective infusion). As described above, data from both types of controls were pooled for comparison with data from effective DCN inactivation sessions.
The estimated recording sites of all cells in all conditions were located within the medial agranular cortex and caudal anterior cingulate regions of the mPFC (Fig. 2, color-coded according to response type), primarily from layers 5/6. Sixty-four percent of single units showed significantly increased or decreased spike activity in response to the CS during trace conditioning. As previously reported, some cells showed changes that persisted beyond CS offset into the trace interval (“persistent,” 22 increasing and 32 decreasing, 17% of all cells recorded), whereas others showed phasic CS responses (“phasic,” 39 increasing and 61 decreasing, 31% of all cells) or responses with onsets during the trace interval (“trace,” 33 increasing and 77 decreasing, 34% of all cells) (Siegel et al., 2012). Previous work suggests that the spike width of extracellularly recorded action potentials in conjunction with average spike rates can be used to differentiate between prefrontal pyramidal cells and interneurons in vivo (Tierney et al., 2004; Tseng et al., 2006; Dilgen et al., 2013). Scatterplots of the average spike width and firing rate for each single unit from each session suggested that only a small subset of recordings were from putative interneurons (∼5% from each condition; Fig. 2). Importantly, no response type (persistent, phasic, or trace) was overrepresented by putative interneurons. No significant difference in spike width was observed among response types showing increased activity during effective infusions (n = 8, 15, and 10 cells, respectively, F(2,30) = 1.21, p = 0.31) or among response types recorded during noninfusion control sessions (n = 14, 25, and 7 cells, respectively, F(2,32) = 0.01, p = 0.99). Furthermore, the spike widths and average firing rates of persistent, phasic, or trace cells were not significantly different between effective infusion and noninfusion controls (persistent: n = 8 and 14 cells, respectively, width: t(15.32) = 1.60, p = 0.13, rate: t(12.25) = 0.30, p = 0.77; phasic: n = 15 and 25 cells, width: t(32.07) = 0.42, p = 0.67, rate: t(31.42) = 1.56, p = 0.13; trace: n = 10 and 7 cells, width: t(4.46) = 0.37, p = 0.73, rate: t(12) = 0.87, p = 0.40). The results suggest that the large majority of single-unit recordings during DCN inactivation and control sessions were from pyramidal neurons, and that the different response types did not have different spike widths or average firing rates.
Effects of DCN inactivation on categories of mPFC spike responses
As an initial analysis, we evaluated whether each cell categorized as persistent, trace interval, or phasic tone continued to meet the same category criteria after DCN inactivation. Analysis was primarily focused on cells that showed increases in activity in response to conditioning stimuli, given that increased responses that overlap in time with the US are hypothesized to drive CR expression in downstream brain areas. It was typical during control sessions for some persistently increasing cells to alter their response pattern such that they failed to meet the same criteria during the second half of the session, whereas most maintained a significant persistent increase in spike response (n = 9 of 14 cells from 12 sessions/5 rabbits; e.g., Figs. 3B, c9.1, and 4B, c18.2). During DCN inactivation, a similar proportion of persistent cells maintained persistently increased spike responses as that observed during control sessions (n = 4 of 8 cells from 6 sessions/4 rabbits, see Fig. 6B; χ2 = 0.43, p = 0.51; e.g., Figs. 3A, c9.1, and 4A, c27.1). Of the cells that failed to meet criteria as persistent during DCN inactivation, most displayed a similar overall pattern that was simply less robust over the second half of the session during the DCN infusion (e.g., Fig. 4A, c21.1, B, c25.1). Cells that displayed a persistent decrease in activity were similarly unaffected by DCN inactivation relative to controls (DCN: n = 7 of 14 cells from 6 sessions/3 rabbits, control: n = 11 of 18 cells from 11 sessions/5 rabbits, χ2 = 0.40, p = 0.53; see Fig. 6B). Likewise, cells that displayed phasic increases and decreases were also unaffected by the abolition of CRs (increasing: n = 9 of 15 cells from 9 sessions/4 rabbits vs 13 of 24 cells from 14 sessions/6 rabbits, χ2 = 0.13, p = 0.72; decreasing: n = 8 of 19 cells from 10 sessions/5 rabbits vs 22 of 42 cells from 17 sessions/6 rabbits, χ2 = 0.55, p = 0.46; see Fig. 6B). The apparent failure of DCN inactivation to affect the response pattern of persistent and phasic tone mPFC cells suggests that these responses were not significantly affected by the abolition of CRs.
In contrast, every mPFC neuron characterized as a trace interval increasing cell failed to meet the same category criteria when CRs were abolished by DCN inactivation (n = 0 of 16 cells from 10 sessions/5 rabbits that maintained trace interval activity; Figs. 3A and 5A). In control sessions, a proportion of trace interval cells also failed to remain in the same category (n = 7 of 17 cells from 9 sessions/5 rabbits), but the proportion was significantly higher than DCN inactivation sessions (χ2 = 13.50, p < 0.001; Fig. 6B). In many cases, trace interval increasing cells also showed phasic tone and/or US associated responses that were unaffected by DCN inactivation, with the trace interval activity selectively abolished along with the behavioral responses (e.g., Figs. 3A, c16.1, and 5A, c21.1). Similarly, for decreasing trace interval cells, a higher proportion of neurons changed category during DCN inactivation relative to controls (only 5 of 24 cells from 11 sessions/4 rabbits maintained trace interval responses vs 27 of 53 cells from 18 sessions/6 rabbits for controls, χ2 = 6.17, p = 0.013; Fig. 6B). The robust effect of DCN inactivation on trace cell activity suggests that the source of input driving this mPFC response pattern is somewhere downstream from cerebellar output.
Effects of DCN inactivation on the spike activity of mPFC persistent and trace responses
As a second analysis, we tested whether mPFC response types showed changes in spike activity as a result of DCN inactivation. We focused this analysis on changes in the number of spikes observed during the trace interval after DCN inactivation for both persistent and trace interval cells, given that this is the time frame during which input to the cerebellum is necessary to support trace eyelid conditioning. For each cell, a change in spike activity was taken as the difference in the average number of spikes per trial observed during the trace interval in the preinfusion trials versus the same measure in the postinfusion trials (or first half trials vs second half trials for control sessions). For phasic tone cells, the difference in the average spike activity per trial observed during the CS period was used.
The pre-post difference scores observed for all three cell types during control sessions and DCN inactivation sessions are given in Figure 7. The difference scores during control sessions revealed that all three cell types typically show a decrease in spike activity between the first and second half of trace conditioning sessions (persistent: n = 14, t(13) = 2.48, p = 0.028; trace: n = 15, t(14) = 3.26, p < 0.005; phasic tone: n = 25, t(24) = 3.26, p < 0.003; Figure 7, gray bars). Given that all three mPFC response types show a typical decline in spike responses during the session, to evaluate the effect of DCN inactivation the decrease observed during infusions was compared with the typical decrease observed during control sessions. During DCN inactivation, persistent and phasic tone cells showed decreases in spike activity that were comparable with those observed during control sessions (persistent: n = 8 and 14 cells for DCN and controls, respectively, t(7,13) = 0.34, p = 0.74; phasic tone: n = 15 and 25 cells, t(14,24) = 0.62, p = 0.54; Figure 7A,C, left graphs, compare gray bars and red bars), even though rabbits showed a significant decrease in the expression of CRs during these sessions (persistent: n = 6 and 12 sessions for DCN and controls, respectively, t(5,11) = 15.77, p < 0.001; phasic: n = 9 and 14 sessions, t(8,13) = 14.07, p < 0.001; Figure 7A,C, right bar graphs). In contrast to persistent cells, the infusion-related decline in trace cell responses was significantly greater than that observed during control sessions (n = 16 and 16 cells for DCN and controls, respectively, t(15,15) = 2.51, p = 0.02; Fig. 7B, compare gray bars with red bars), corresponding to the decrease in CR performance during these sessions (n = 10 and 9 sessions, t(9,8) = 13.58, p < 0.001).
The results suggest that the spike activity of trace cells is significantly decreased during DCN inactivation, whereas the spike activity of persistent and phasic tone cells was not different from control sessions. To further test whether the spike activity of trace interval cells was abolished to baseline, we compared the average number of spikes observed during the trace interval during DCN inactivation with the average number of spikes observed during the pretrial baseline period. During control infusions, the spike activity of all cell types, including trace cells, was significantly greater than baseline (persistent: t(13) = 3.29, p = 0.005; trace: t(15) = 3.40, p = 0.004; phasic tone: t(24) = 4.83, p < 0.001; n = 14, 16, and 25 cells, respectively; Figure 8, left bar graph). During DCN inactivation, when CR expression was pharmacologically blocked, the spike responses of trace cells were not different from pretrial baseline activity (n = 16 cells, t(15) = 2.19, p = 0.024; familywise α for all six comparisons = 0.05, Bonferonni corrected pcrit = 0.008). Persistent and phasic tone cells continued to show significant spike responses relative to baseline (persistent: n = 8 cells, t(7) = 4.36, p = 0.002; phasic tone: n = 16 cells t(15) = 4.15, p < 0.001; Figure 8, right bar graph). The results suggest that the spike responses of mPFC trace cells are driven by feedback from the output pathway of the cerebellum that drives CRs, whereas persistent and phasic tone responses are upstream of the cerebellum.
Time course of the effects of DCN inactivation on mPFC spike activity
The previous analyses demonstrate the effects of DCN inactivation on mPFC cells based on spike activity that was averaged over trials before and after infusions. A third analysis examined changes in the spike activity of mPFC cells with greater trial-by-trial resolution to test whether the abolition of trace cell activity is abrupt, corresponding to the nearly immediate abolition of CRs. For each trace interval and persistent cell, the spike data from each trial were binned (100 ms), including the 1 s before and after each trial, creating a 30 (number of time bins per trial) by 108 (number of trials per session) matrix. Each cell's matrix was normalized to its average baseline activity, and the matrix from each cell in each response category was averaged to create a matrix of the mean increase in the number of spikes observed for that response type while conserving the individual trials across sessions (Fig. 9; the matrices of cells from sessions in which rabbits showed a delayed response were aligned accordingly).
The averaged matrix for trace cells suggests that trace interval activity decreased to baseline levels within just a few trials after DCN inactivation, in contrast to the spike activity observed during the second half of control sessions (Fig. 9A). We tested the hypothesis that trace cell activity rapidly abolished to baseline after DCN inactivation by comparing trace interval activity during the second block of trials after infusions, corresponding to the behavioral criteria used to determine complete inactivation (i.e., when CR rate <15%, see Materials and Methods), versus the same block during control conditions (Fig. 9A, right, block 8). Trace cells showed a significant decrease in activity relative to controls by the second block of trials after infusion (n = 16 and 16 cells for DCN and controls, respectively, t(16.44) = 2.64, p = 0.018; Fig. 9A). Not only was there a significant decrease relative to controls, trace cell responses were decreased to zero by the second block (single sample two-tailed test, H0: mean = 0, n = 16 cells, t(15) = 0.16, p = 0.88), indicating that trace activity was abolished during the very first block in which complete behavioral abolition was observed. There was no difference in trace cell activity between DCN inactivation and control conditions for the block just before infusions (n = 16 and 16 cells, t(29.74) = 0.03, p = 0.97; Fig. 9A, right, block 6). Consistent with previous analyses, no difference was observed between the spike responses of persistent cells during control and DCN inactivation either just before (n = 8 and 14 cells, t(19.21) = 0.21, p = 0.84) or just after infusions (t(20.86) = 0.49, p = 0.63; Fig. 9B, right, blocks 6 and 8, respectively). The data show that trace interval responses were abolished in accordance with the abolition of CRs.
The matrix of averaged spike responses for trace and persistent cells were collapsed across trials for the preinfusion and postinfusion epochs, similar to previous analyses. The same results were observed when analyzed this way, supporting the results of the previous analyses (Fig. 9C,D). For trace cells, the spike response profile during trials was significantly affected by DCN inactivation (postinfusion vs postcontrol: Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, D = 0.55, p < 0.001; preinfusion comparison: D = 0.28, p = 0.08; Fig. 9C). The spike response profile of persistent cells over the trial was not affected by DCN inactivation (post: D = 0.25, p = 0.14; pre: D = 0.23, p = 0.23; Fig. 9D). The results suggest that the spike responses of persistent cells during trace conditioning were unaffected by DCN inactivation throughout the entire trial, in addition to the lack of an effect during the trace interval as demonstrated in previous analyses. In contrast, the average spike response of trace interval cells was significantly affected by the abolition of CRs, further supporting the previous results.
Discussion
A recent study revealed two patterns of single-unit responses in the mPFC that in principle would be capable of engaging the cerebellum given that both sufficiently overlap with the US (Siegel et al., 2012). The present results suggest that one pattern, trace interval activity, depends on cerebellar output and therefore reflects feedback to the mPFC regarding behavioral output. In contrast, persistent responses were unchanged by cerebellar inactivation. Together with previous studies, the results implicate persistent responses as the best candidate input from the mPFC that can bridge the temporal gap between the CS and US, enabling the cerebellum to express trace CRs (Kalmbach et al., 2010, 2011). The results represent the first evidence of CS-evoked activity in the forebrain that overlaps with the timing of the US and that does not reflect CR-associated feedback in the trace conditioning pathway.
Trace interval (CR-associated) activity in the forebrain is feedback from the cerebellar output pathway
The trace interval activity observed in the mPFC during trace eyelid conditioning was abolished to baseline levels during cerebellar inactivation, indicating that such response patterns reflect feedback associated with learned behavioral responses. Increased neural activity that is highly correlated with behavioral CRs (i.e., here referred to as trace interval activity) is not unique to the mPFC, nor is it unique to the trace conditioning paradigm. Such activity has also been observed in the hippocampus and medial thalamic nuclei during delay eyelid conditioning (Berger et al., 1976; Halverson et al., 2010). CR-associated activity was first reported for the hippocampus and was initially interpreted as potentially driving learned responses because it appeared just before the first behavioral CRs (Berger et al., 1976). It was surprising then that animals with hippocampal lesions failed to show deficits in learning or expression during delay conditioning, and even more surprising when DCN lesions or inactivation completely abolished the CR-associated hippocampal responses along with the expression of CRs (Clark et al., 1984; Sears and Steinmetz, 1990). A similar test has not been reported for CR-associated thalamic responses or for any other forebrain region. To date, the CR-associated activity commonly observed in forebrain regions has not been shown to be definitively upstream from the cerebellum in any form of eyelid conditioning (Clark et al., 1984, 1997; Sears and Steinmetz, 1990; Halverson et al., 2010; Pacheco-Calderón et al., 2012). We suggest that any such activity is likely a reflection of cerebellar output and would also be abolished along with CRs, even for forebrain-dependent versions of eyelid conditioning.
Source(s) of feedback that drive CR-associated activity in the forebrain and potential function in the mPFC
The precise source of CR-associated feedback to the mPFC, as reflected in trace interval activity, was not addressed in this study. This pattern of spike response could derive from an input from the anterior interpositus DCN to the mPFC via the thalamus. In cats, rats, and mice, both the dentate and interpositus cerebellar nuclei project to the ventrolateral thalamus (McCrea et al., 1978; Tolbert et al., 1978; Payne, 1983; Allen Mouse Brain Connectivity Atlas). This same region of thalamus projects to the mPFC in all of these species, as well as in rabbit (Shinoda et al., 1985; Hoover and Vertes, 2007; Oswald et al., 2007; Allen Mouse Brain Connectivity Atlas), suggesting that input to PFC from the DCN is possible via the thalamus. However, the input driving CR-associated activity could also originate from anywhere along the motor output pathway, including the red nucleus (Bernays et al., 1988; Tarnecki, 1988; Ostrowska et al., 1993). Further studies are required to address the precise source(s) of CR-associated activity in the mPFC during trace eyelid conditioning.
Perhaps a more pressing question is exactly what the potential role of behavior-associated feedback to the mPFC network might be. We were unable to detect any consistent effect of DCN inactivation on the activity of persistent cells in the mPFC of trained animals. However, it is likely that these cells also receive behavior-associated feedback that is somewhat masked by the persistent response. Evidence for this possibility can be seen in Figure 9B. PFC neurons showed an apparent decrease in persistent spiking just before the US during DCN inactivation, at the time when a CR would have been observed, which was not observed when CRs were expressed before inactivation. It is possible that this feedback is not important for ongoing expression but may play an important role during other conditions, such as the acquisition of CRs. Berger et al. (1976) observed CR-associated activity in the hippocampus during delay conditioning before the expression of CRs, suggesting that DCN output can drive some downstream activity before the behavioral expression of learning. A potential role for CR-associated feedback to the mPFC during trace conditioning could be to enhance the CS-evoked persistent activity that serves as the cerebellar input to drive learning and CR expression (Halverson et al., 2010; Campolattaro et al., 2011). The increased responsiveness of persistent spike activity in the mPFC could more reliably drive the expression of CRs after the circuit has essentially learned, perhaps as a final phase of acquisition. Determining the source of the CR-associated feedback and blocking those inputs to the mPFC during learning would be an essential test of this proposed function.
CS-evoked persistent responses do not reflect CR-associated feedback
An important finding of the present study is that mPFC persistent responses were not affected by DCN inactivation and the abolition of CRs and therefore remain a possible source of input from the mPFC to enable the expression of cerebellar learning. Although we do not know whether the persistent mPFC neurons in this study projected to the pons, previous studies suggest that it is likely. First, in vitro work has shown that corticopontine projecting mPFC cells displayed persistent spike activity to phasic stimuli with activation of cholinergic or metabotropic glutamate receptors, whereas cells with commissural projections were far less likely to spike persistently under any condition (Dembrow et al., 2010; Kalmbach et al., 2013). Second, injection of anterograde tracer in the region of mPFC, where persistent responses were observed in response to conditioning stimuli, resulted in labeled axons in specific pontine regions in which cells also responded persistently (Siegel et al., 2012). It has been shown that a persistent input, via direct stimulation of pontine inputs to the cerebellum, in combination with a separate tone-like stimulation pattern is sufficient to engage cerebellar learning and the expression of CRs, which are indistinguishable from animals trained with a tone CS (Kalmbach et al., 2010, 2011).
CS-evoked persistent responses in the mPFC have been reported in diverse species during performance of different tasks that share the common feature of a stimulus-free interval between CS presentation and behavioral response (Fuster, 1973; Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Gilmartin and McEchron, 2005; Takehara-Nishiuchi and Mcnaughton, 2008; Burgos-Robles et al., 2009; Siegel et al., 2012). The mPFC shows a high degree of reciprocity with the motor output systems engaged in many of these tasks, most notably the basal ganglia and/or primary motor cortex. Because trace eyelid conditioning engages the cerebellum, we were able to pharmacologically block the motor response output at or before feedback to upstream brain regions could occur, at the DCN. Persistent activity in the mPFC during trace eyelid conditioning was not abolished with DCN inactivation and therefore cannot reflect cerebellar feedback to the forebrain. It is unlikely that such activity reflects a persistent input to the mPFC from another brain region because previous studies failed to observe persistent response patterns in the superficial input layers (Weible et al., 2003; Siegel et al., 2012). Together, the data support the hypothesis that the mPFC supports trace eyelid conditioning by generating a persistently active input to the cerebellum, via the pons, that bridges the temporal gap between the CS and US to allow the cerebellar network to express well-timed learned responses.
It is currently unknown whether persistent responses in the mPFC are acquired with training or whether such responses represent an inherent property of how some mPFC cells respond to stimuli. The hippocampus has been implicated specifically in the acquisition of trace CRs but is not necessary for the ongoing expression of learned responses (Kim et al., 1995; Takehara et al., 2003). Although the precise role of the hippocampus in trace eyelid conditioning remains unclear, at least one study briefly described a persistent-like response during trace conditioning that was observed early in training in hippocampal cells, which was no longer present as the rabbits acquired CRs (Solomon et al., 1986). It is possible that hippocampal inputs may facilitate persistent spike responses in the mPFC at the earliest stages of learning, either by providing a CS-evoked persistent input or by facilitating synapses by providing additional input that is coincident with other CS inputs (e.g., Laroche et al., 1990). Further studies will be required to explore these hypotheses and the nature of hippocampal-prefrontal interactions during trace conditioning.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Grants NIH MH74006 and MH46904 and the McKnight Foundation.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Allen Mouse Brain Connectivity Atlas. 2013. http://connectivity.brain-map.org/ [DOI] [PubMed]
- Berger TW, Alger B, Thompson RF. Neuronal substrate of classical conditioning in the hippocampus. Science. 1976;192:483–485. doi: 10.1126/science.1257783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernays RL, Heeb L, Cuenod M, Streit P. Afferents to the rat red nucleus studied by means of d-[3H]aspartate, [3H]choline and non-selective tracers. Neuroscience. 1988;26:601–619. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(88)90168-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Quirk GJ. Sustained conditioned responses in prelimbic prefrontal neurons are correlated with fear expression and extinction failure. J Neurosci. 2009;29:8474–8482. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0378-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campolattaro MM, Kashef A, Lee I, Freeman JH. Neuronal correlates of cross-modal transfer in the cerebellum and pontine nuclei. J Neurosci. 2011;31:4051–4062. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4142-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark GA, McCormick DA, Lavond DG, Thompson RF. Effects of lesions of cerebellar nuclei on conditioned behavioral and hippocampal neuronal responses. Brain Res. 1984;291:125–136. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90658-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark RE, Gohl EB, Lavond DG. The learning-related activity that develops in the pontine nuclei during classical eye-blink conditioning is dependent on the interpositus nucleus. Learn Mem. 1997;3:532–544. doi: 10.1101/lm.3.6.532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dembrow NC, Chitwood RA, Johnston D. Projection-specific neuromodulation of medial prefrontal cortex neurons. J Neurosci. 2010;30:16922–16937. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3644-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dilgen J, Tejeda HA, O'Donnell P. Amygdala inputs drive feedforward inhibition in the medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2013;110:221–229. doi: 10.1152/jn.00531.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster JM. Unit activity in prefrontal cortex during delayed-response performance: neuronal correlates of transient memory. J Neurophysiol. 1973;36:61–78. doi: 10.1152/jn.1973.36.1.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilmartin MR, McEchron MD. Single neurons in the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat exhibit tonic and phasic coding during trace fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 2005;119:1496–1510. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.6.1496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. 1995;14:477–485. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halverson HE, Lee I, Freeman JH. Associative plasticity in the medial auditory thalamus and cerebellar interpositus nucleus during eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci. 2010;30:8787–8796. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0208-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoover WB, Vertes RP. Anatomical analysis of afferent projections to the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Struct Funct. 2007;212:149–179. doi: 10.1007/s00429-007-0150-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M, Sakurai M, Tongroach P. Climbing fibre induced depression of both mossy fibre responsiveness and glutamate sensitivity of cerebellar Purkinje cells. J Physiol. 1982;324:113–134. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalmbach BE, Ohyama T, Kreider JC, Riusech F, Mauk MD. Interactions between prefrontal cortex and cerebellum revealed by trace eyelid conditioning. Learn Mem. 2009;16:86–95. doi: 10.1101/lm.1178309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalmbach BE, Ohyama T, Mauk MD. Temporal patterns of inputs to cerebellum necessary and sufficient for trace eyelid conditioning. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:627–640. doi: 10.1152/jn.00169.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalmbach BE, Voicu H, Ohyama T, Mauk MD. A subtraction mechanism of temporal coding in cerebellar cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31:2025–2034. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4212-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalmbach BE, Chitwood RA, Dembrow NC, Johnston D. Dendritic generation of mGluR-mediated slow afterdepolarization in layer 5 neurons of the prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33:13518–13532. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JJ, Clark RE, Thompson RF. Hippocampectomy impairs the memory of recently, but not remotely, acquired trace eyeblink conditioned responses. Behav Neurosci. 1995;109:195–203. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.109.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kronforst-Collins MA, Disterhoft JF. Lesions of the caudal area of rabbit medial prefrontal cortex impair trace eyeblink conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1998;69:147–162. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1997.3818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laroche S, Jay TM, Thierry AM. Long-term potentiation in the prefrontal cortex following stimulation of the hippocampal CA1/subicular region. Neurosci Lett. 1990;114:184–190. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(90)90069-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mauk MD, Donegan NH. A model of Pavlovian eyelid conditioning based on the synaptic organization of the cerebellum. Learn Mem. 1997;4:130–158. doi: 10.1101/lm.4.1.130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrea RA, Bishop GA, Kitai ST. Morphological and electrophysiological characteristics of projection neurons in the nucleus interpositus of the cat cerebellum. J Comp Neurol. 1978;181:397–419. doi: 10.1002/cne.901810210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostrowska A, Sikora E, Mierzejewska-Krzyzowska B, Zimny R. The dentatorubral projection in the rabbit with emphasis on distinction from the interpositorubral connectivity: an HRP retrograde tracer study. J Hirnforsch. 1993;34:9–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oswald BB, Knuckley B, Maddox SA, Powell DA. Ibotenic acid lesions to ventrolateral thalamic nuclei disrupt trace and delay eyeblink conditioning in rabbits. Behav Brain Res. 2007;179:111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pacheco-Calderón R, Carretero-Guillén A, Delgado-García JM, Gruart A. Red nucleus neurons actively contribute to the acquisition of classically conditioned eyelid responses in rabbits. J Neurosci. 2012;32:12129–12143. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1782-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne JN. The cerebellar nucleo-cortical projection in the rat studied by the retrograde fluorescent double-labelling method. Brain Res. 1983;271:141–144. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)91374-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sears LL, Steinmetz JE. Acquisition of classically conditioned-related activity in the hippocampus is affected by lesions of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus. Behav Neurosci. 1990;104:681–692. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.104.5.681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinoda Y, Futami T, Kano M. Synaptic organization of the cerebello-thalamo-cerebral pathway in the cat: II. Input-output organization of single thalamocortical neurons in the ventrolateral thalamus. Neurosci Res. 1985;2:157–180. doi: 10.1016/0168-0102(85)90010-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel JJ, Neunuebel JP, Knierim JJ. Dominance of the proximal coordinate frame in determining the locations of hippocampal place cell activity during navigation. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99:60–76. doi: 10.1152/jn.00731.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel JJ, Kalmbach B, Chitwood RA, Mauk MD. Persistent activity in a cortical-to-subcortical circuit: bridging the temporal gap in trace eyelid conditioning. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107:50–64. doi: 10.1152/jn.00689.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon PR, Vander Schaaf ER, Thompson RF, Weisz DJ. Hippocampus and trace conditioning of the rabbit's classically conditioned nictitating membrane response. Behav Neurosci. 1986;100:729–744. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.100.5.729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takehara K, Kawahara S, Kirino Y. Time-dependent reorganization of the brain components underlying memory retention in trace eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci. 2003;23:9897–9905. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-30-09897.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takehara-Nishiuchi K, McNaughton BL. Spontaneous changes of neocortical code for associative memory during consolidation. Science. 2008;322:960–963. doi: 10.1126/science.1161299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tarnecki R. Functional connections between neurons of interpositus nucleus of the cerebellum and the red nucleus. Behav Brain Res. 1988;28:117–125. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90087-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tierney PL, Dégenètais E, Thierry AM, Glowinski J, Gioanni Y. Influence of the hippocampus on interneurons of the rat prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20:514–524. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03501.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tolbert DL, Bantli H, Bloedel JR. Multiple branching of cerebellar efferent projections in cats. Exp Brain Res. 1978;31:305–316. doi: 10.1007/BF00237291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tseng KY, Mallet N, Toreson KL, Le Moine C, Gonon F, O'Donnell P. Excitatory response of prefrontal cortical fast-spiking interneurons to ventral tegmental area stimulation in vivo. Synapse. 2006;59:412–417. doi: 10.1002/syn.20255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weible AP, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Activity profiles of single neurons in caudal anterior cingulate cortex during trace eyeblink conditioning in the rabbit. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:599–612. doi: 10.1152/jn.01097.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weible AP, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Connections of the caudal anterior cingulate cortex in rabbit: neural circuitry participating in the acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioning. Neuroscience. 2007;145:288–302. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.11.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Eyeblink conditioning, motor control, and the analysis of limbic-cerebellar interactions. Behav Brain Sci. 1996;19:479–481. [Google Scholar]