Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Sep 18.
Published in final edited form as: Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010 Apr;67(4):328–338. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.14

Etiological and Clinical Features of Childhood Psychotic Symptoms

Results From a Birth Cohort

Guilherme Polanczyk 1, Terrie E Moffitt 1, Louise Arseneault 1, Mary Cannon 1, Antony Ambler 1, Richard S E Keefe 1, Renate Houts 1, Candice L Odgers 1, Avshalom Caspi 1
PMCID: PMC3776482  NIHMSID: NIHMS499051  PMID: 20368509

Abstract

Context

It has been reported that childhood psychotic symptoms are common in the general population and may signal neurodevelopmental processes that lead to schizophrenia. However, it is not clear whether these symptoms are associated with the same extensive risk factors established for adult schizophrenia.

Objective

To examine the construct validity of children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms by testing whether these symptoms share the risk factors and clinical features of adult schizophrenia.

Design

Prospective, longitudinal cohort study of a nationally representative birth cohort in Great Britain.

Participants

A total of 2232 twelve-year-old children followed up since age 5 years (retention, 96%).

Main Outcome Measure

Children’s self-reported hallucinations and delusions.

Results

Children’s psychotic symptoms are familial and heritable and are associated with social risk factors (eg, urbanicity); cognitive impairments at age 5; home-rearing risk factors (eg, maternal expressed emotion); behavioral, emotional, and educational problems at age 5; and comorbid conditions, including self-harm.

Conclusions

The results provide a comprehensive picture of the construct validity of children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms. For researchers, the findings indicate that children who have psychotic symptoms can be recruited for neuroscience research to determine the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. For clinicians, the findings indicate that psychotic symptoms in childhood are often a marker of an impaired developmental process and should be actively assessed.


What does it mean when a child reports experiencing hallucinations or delusions? Increasing interest in this question has been stimulated by reports that hallucinations and delusions occur among children in the community who do not have childhood schizophrenia, that preadolescent children are able to self-report these symptoms, and that groups of such children followed up to adulthood have an elevated prevalence of diagnosed psychotic illness.16 However, the clinical and theoretical significance of their symptoms is not yet clear. From a clinical perspective, it is important to know whether these children are typically characterized by particular risk contexts or clinical features that ought to be a focus of treatment. From a theoretical perspective, it is important to know how the phenomenon of childhood psychotic symptoms fits into the field’s current understanding of the origins of schizophrenia and whether children with such symptoms ought to be a focus of etiological research. Thus, we undertook a study to test whether children reporting hallucinations or delusions share the same risk correlates as adults who meet full diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia.

Two important literatures are relevant to determining the meaning of childhood hallucinations and delusions: the literature testing the neurodevelopmental theory of schizophrenia and the literature documenting the presence of psychotic symptoms in the population.

The neurodevelopmental theory of schizophrenia is relevant because it has directed scientific attention to the origins of schizophrenia in early life, many years before the illness can be diagnosed.7 Evidence from different methodological perspectives documents that individuals who later develop schizophrenia exhibited neurodevelopmental deficits as children.7 Abnormalities in behavioral, emotional, social, cognitive, and motor development and in neuroanatomy have been reported.810 These deficits are thought to signal the starting point of a risk pathway in which the endpoint is a nonaffective psychotic disorder, depending on genetic, perinatal, and environmental inputs.9,11 However, the neurodevelopmental theory has not emphasized the emergence during childhood of positive symptoms, such as hallucinations or delusions, and it is unknown whether or how such symptoms should be incorporated into the theory alongside other childhood neurodevelopmental risks.

The dimensional model of schizophrenia is relevant because it has directed scientific attention to the existence of psychotic symptoms in the general population, below the threshold for diagnosis of the illness.12 Sub-diagnostic symptoms are common in the general population of adults,1315 and evidence suggests that such symptoms are associated with the same genetic and nongenetic risk factors as the clinical disorder.1517 Subdiagnostic symptoms are thought to signal the mild end of a risk continuum for which schizophrenia or psychotic disorder is the extreme point.15,17 As in neurodevelopmental theory, progression from symptoms to clinical disorder is not necessarily inevitable and likely depends on inherited susceptibility and exposure to environmental risks during development.15 However, the dimensional model has focused on symptoms during adulthood and has not emphasized the existence of symptoms during childhood. It is unknown whether or how such childhood symptoms should be incorporated into dimensional approaches.

These 2 relevant literatures suggested to us the possibility that psychotic symptoms in childhood signal neurodevelopmental processes that are already known to lead to schizophrenia and that childhood symptoms are part of the dimension of schizophrenia risk. Our research team previously reported that members of the Dunedin (New Zealand) Longitudinal Study birth cohort who self-reported psychotic symptoms at age 11 years had an elevated risk of developing schizophreniform disorders by age 26 (odds ratio, 16.4; 95% confidence interval, 3.9–67.8),1: results which have been subsequently replicated.3 This finding was consistent with the possibility that childhood psychotic symptoms signal neurodevelopmental processes that increase the risk for schizophrenia onset in adolescence or adulthood. Corroborating this hypothesis, our research team later reported that children in the Dunedin cohort with psychotic symptoms also showed significant impairments during childhood in motor development, language, and intelligence, skills that are often impaired among individuals who develop schizophrenia.18 Other studies have evaluated individual correlates, including perinatal complications,19 paternal age,20 low IQ,21 childhood trauma,22 peer victimization,2 and behavioral problems.23

In this study, we extend this previous work by testing the hypothesis that children who report psychotic symptoms are characterized by the same extensive net-work of risk factors and correlates previously reported in the research literature on adult schizophrenia. We evaluated the occurrence of psychotic symptoms in a nationally representative British twin birth cohort of 2232 twelve-year-olds. Guided by the research literature on schizophrenia,8,2427 we tested 7 hypotheses. First, we evaluated whether, like schizophrenia, childhood psychotic symptoms are familial by testing whether children with symptoms were more likely to have mothers with psychotic-spectrum disorders and family members who had been admitted to psychiatric units or who had attempted or completed suicide.28 Second, we evaluated whether, as with schizophrenia, children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms are heritable29 by using the twin design to estimate the genetic contribution to variation in children’s symptoms. Third, we evaluated whether children with psychotic symptoms shared schizophrenia’s social risk factors by testing whether these children were more likely to live in an urban environment30 and come from disadvantaged families.31 Fourth, we evaluated whether children with psychotic symptoms shared schizophrenia’s neurodevelopmental risk factors by testing whether these children had older fathers,32 were born during winter or spring,33 had lower birth weight,34,35 suffered perinatal complications,36 or had cognitive characteristics including low IQ,37 executive functioning deficits,38,39 and impaired theory of mind.40,41 Fifth, we evaluated whether children with psychotic symptoms shared schizophrenia’s home-rearing risk factors by testing whether these children had mothers with high expressed emotion,42 lived in chaotic households,27,43 or had been victims of physical maltreatment.4446 Sixth, we evaluated schizophrenia’s early-childhood behavioral risk factors8 by testing whether 12-year-olds with psychotic symptoms had, as 5-year-olds, shown externalizing and internalizing problems, social isolation,10 or educational problems.10 Seventh, we evaluated schizophrenia’s comorbid conditions by testing whether children with psychotic symptoms self-reported more concurrent antisocial behavior,24 depression and anxiety symptoms,24 and tobacco use47 and cannabis use48 and whether they were more likely to harm themselves.49

METHODS

PARTICIPANTS

Participants were members of the Environmental Risk (E-Risk) Longitudinal Twin Study, which tracks the development of a nationally representative birth cohort of 2232 British children. The sample was drawn from a larger birth registry of twins born in England and Wales from 1994 through 1995.50 Details about the sample have been reported previously,51 including in this journal.52 Briefly, the E-Risk sample was constructed from 1999 through 2000, when 1116 families with same-sex 5-year-old twins (93% of those eligible) participated in home-visit assessments. Families were recruited to represent the United Kingdom population of families with newborns in the 1990s, based on residential location throughout England and Wales and mother’s age (ie, older mothers having twins via assisted reproduction were underselected and teen-aged mothers with twins were overselected). Follow-up home visits were conducted when the children were aged 7 years (98% participation), 10 years (96% participation), and, most recently, 12 years (96% participation). The sample includes 55% monozygotic and 45% dizygotic twin pairs. Sex is evenly distributed within zygosity (49% were boys). Parents gave informed consent and children gave assent. Confidentiality was preserved, and the child’s general practitioner was notified only when a mother reported her child was a risk to self or others. The Maudsley Hospital Ethics Committee approved each phase of the study.

EVALUATING CHILDREN’S PSYCHOTIC SYMPTOMS

At age 12, children were assessed for psychotic symptoms in a private interview conducted by mental health trainees or professionals. Interviewers had no prior knowledge about the child. Different staff members interviewed the child’s parents. We investigated 7 psychotic symptoms (Table 1). Our item choice was guided by the Dunedin Study age-11 interview protocol1 and a subsequent instrument prepared for the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children.2 Our protocol took a conservative approach to designating a child’s report as a symptom. First, when a child endorsed any symptom, the interviewer probed using standard prompts designed to discriminate between experiences that were plausibly real (eg, “I was followed by a man after school”) vs potential symptoms (eg, “I was followed by an angel who guards my spirit”) and wrote down the child’s narrative description of the experience. Interviewers coded each experience as “not a symptom” (0), “probable symptom” (1), or “definite symptom” (2). Second, a psychiatrist expert in schizophrenia (M.C.), a psychologist expert in interviewing children (L.A.), and a child and adolescent psychiatrist (G.P.) reviewed all the written narratives to confirm the interviewers’ codes (but without consulting other data sources about the child or family). Third, because ours was a sample of twins, experiences limited to the twin relationship (eg, “My twin and I often know what each other are thinking”) were coded as “not a symptom.”

Table 1.

Frequency of Children’s Self-reported Psychotic Symptomsa

No. (%)
of 2127 Children
Psychotic Symptom Probable
Symptom
Definite
Symptom
Hallucinations
  Have you heard voices that other people cannot hear? 169 (7.9) 90 (4.2)
  Have you ever seen something or someone that other people could not see? 168 (7.9) 42 (2.0)
Delusions
  Have you ever thought you were being followed or spied on? 54 (2.5) 15 (0.7)
  Have you ever felt like you were under the control of some special power? 41 (1.9) 16 (0.8)
  Have you ever known what another person was thinking, even though that person wasn’t speaking, like read their mind? 14 (0.7) 5 (0.2)
  Have you ever believed that you were sent special messages through television or radio? 26 (1.2) 3 (0.1)
  Have other people ever read your thoughts? 9 (0.4) 0
a

Symptoms are not mutually exclusive.

DISTRIBUTION OF CHILDREN’S PSYCHOTIC SYMPTOMS

Table 1 shows the frequency of children’s psychotic symptoms, coded as probable or definite. Auditory and visual hallucinations were the most common symptoms, and mind reading was the least common symptom. Psychotic symptoms were reported by 416 children (19.6%): 291 (13.7%) reported only probable symptoms and 125 (5.9%) reported at least 1 definite symptom. Among children with at least 1 definite psychotic symptom, multiple symptoms were typical: 36 (28.8%) reported multiple definite symptoms and 76 (60.8%) also reported probable symptoms.

DEFINING TARGET GROUPS OF CHILDREN FOR ANALYSIS

To test the heritability of psychotic symptoms, we summed the response codes (0, 1, and 2) across the 7 symptoms for each child. Scores in the sample ranged from 0 to 13 (mean [SD],0.4 [1.0]). To test the hypotheses about the risk factors and correlates of children’s psychotic symptoms, we compared 2 groups of children: those who did not present any definite psychotic symptom (hereafter called the “symptom-absent group”; n = 2002) vs children who presented at least 1 definite psychotic symptom according to the clinical review of narratives (hereafter called the “symptom-present group”; n= 125). One boy in this group had, by age 12 years, received a formal diagnosis of schizophrenia by the family physician and was being treated with antipsychotic medication, according to his mother. Analyses were performed with and without the inclusion of this child, and results did not differ; therefore, we decided to keep him in the sample.

SCHIZOPHRENIA-RELATED RISK FACTORS AND CORRELATES

Table 2 lists the measures used to test each hypothesis about the risk factors and correlates of children’s psychotic symptoms. The table provides information about each measure, its source, the age at which it was obtained, and descriptive statistics.

Table 2.

Description of the Investigated Risk Factors and Correlates of Children’s Psychotic Symptoms

Measure Respondent Description of the Measure Age at
Evaluation, y
Sample
Distributiona
Familiality
  Maternal psychosis-spectrum disorder Mother Diagnostic Interview Schedule53 diagnosis according to DSM-IV54 evidence of social, occupational, or self-care dysfunction; diagnosis reviewed by a clinician 10 5.5
  Admission to psychiatric units Parents First- or second-degree relatives who have ever been admitted to a psychiatric unit 12 26.3
  Family history of suicide Parents First- or second-degree relatives with a positive history of attempted or completed suicide 12 16.4
Social risk factors
  Urban residence Neighbors Classification of children’s neighborhood as a city or other type of urban setting based on a community-level survey of more than 5600 residents living in the same postcode (ie, street or apartment building) as each E-Risk family 12 50.9
  Socioeconomic disadvantage Parents Lowest tertile of socioeconomic index, a composite of parental income, education, and occupation55 5 33.2
Neurodevelopmental risk factors
    Paternal age, y Parents Father’s age at child’s birth Birth 31.8 (6.3)
    Birth in winter or spring Parents Child’s date of birth Birth 49.5
    Birth weightb Parents Absolute values for weight were standardized with reference to birth weight in relation to gestational age of 19 000 twins born in England from 1988 through 199256 Birth 0 (1)
    Multiple perinatal complications Parents Two or more of the following: high blood pressure, diabetes, preeclampsia, vaginal bleeding, water breaking >11 h before labor, slow infant growth, or rubella during pregnancy Birth 21.9
    IQc Child WPPSI Revised57; children were administered 2 subtests: Vocabulary and Block Design, and IQ scores were prorated following procedures described by Sattler.58 5 100 (15)
    Executive functioningc Child Children were administered 3 executive functions tests: Mazes,59 a WPPSI subtest; Day-Night,60 a nonverbal analog of the Stroop task; and Sentence Working Memory, based on the Baddeley model of working memory,61,62 which requires the child to hold 1 (or more) item in active working memory while processing necessary information for the generation of the second (and so forth) item; children’s scores on the 3 tests were averaged and standardized. 5 100 (15)
    Theory of mindc Child Battery of Theory of Mind tasks,63 administered in a set order of increasing difficulty. The test questions tapped children’s ability to attribute a first-order false belief to a story character, to make inferences from an attributed false belief, and to attribute a second-order false belief to a story character; children’s responses were summed and standardized. 5 100 (15)
Home-rearing risk factors
  Maternal expressed emotionb Mother, coded by independent raters Assessed using a 5-min speech sample eliciting expressed emotion from the mother; speech samples were audiotaped and coded by 2 independent raters. Maternal negativity (coded on a 6-point scale) is a global measure of the whole speech sample, indexing negativism expressed in the interview about the child. Maternal warmth (6-point scale) is a global measure of the whole speech sample, indexing warmth expressed in the interview about the child.64 10 Negativity, 0 (1); warmth, 0 (1)
  Household chaosb Mother and child Items indicating extent of routine, privacy, predictability, and organization in the home43 12 0 (1)
  Physical maltreatment Mother, coded by clinicians Interview using the reliable standardized clinical protocol from the Multi-Site Child Development Project, a protocol designed to enhance mothers’ comfort with reporting valid child maltreatment information while also meeting researchers’ legal and ethical responsibilities for reporting; examples included: victim of adjudicated assault by a teenaged sibling, punished by being burned with matches, injured (eg, fractures or dislocations) from neglectful or abusive parental care, and/or formally registered with a child protection team for physical abuse.65,66 5,7,10, and 12 5.6
Behavioral, emotional, and educational risk factors at age 5 y
    Antisocial behaviorb Mother, teacher, and child CBCL/TRF Aggression and Delinquency subscales67; Berkeley Puppet Interview68,69 for children 5 0 (1)
    ADHD symptomsb Mother and teacher DSM-IV Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder items54,70 5 0 (1)
    Internalizing problemsb Mother and teacher CBCL/TRF Anxiety, Withdrawn, and Somatic subscales67 5 0 (1)
    Social isolationd Mother and teacher Positive endorsement of CBCL/TRF67 items (eg, would rather be alone than with others; not liked by other children) 5 Mothers, 5.6; teachers, 2.9
    Educational problems Teacher 1 or More of the following: referred to special educational service, works less hard than other students, and is learning less than other students67 5 38
Comorbid behavioral and emotional problems at age 12 y
    Antisocial behaviorb Child DSM-IV Conduct Disorder items54 12 0 (1)
    Depressive symptomsb Child Children’s Depression Inventory71 12 0 (1)
    Anxiety symptomsb Child Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children72 12 0 (1)
    Tobacco use or experimentation Child “Have you ever tried smoking a cigarette?” 12 12
    Cannabis use or experimentation Child “Have you ever tried any hash or cannabis?” 12 1
    Self-harm/suicidal behavior Mother Deliberately harms self or attempts suicide 12 3

Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; E-Risk, Environmental Risk Longitudinal Twin Study; TRF, Teacher Report Form; WPPSI, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence.

a

Data are given as mean (SD) or the percentage of participants.

b

Values are standardized: mean (SD), 0 (1).

c

Values are standardized: mean (SD), 100 (15).

d

The social isolation items in the withdrawn subscale of the CBCL/TRF were analyzed separately

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

To test the relative magnitude of genetic and environmental influences on psychotic symptoms, we first examined the phenotypic correlation within pairs of monozygotic and dizygotic twins. We then used Mplus statistical software73 to decompose variance in children’s psychotic symptoms into latent genetic (ie, the sum of the average effects of individual alleles at all loci), latent family-wide environmental, and latent child-specific environmental factors.74

To test the hypotheses about the risk factors and correlates of children’s psychotic symptoms, we conducted logistic and linear regression analyses comparing symptom-absent with symptom-present groups of children. Because each study family contains 2 children, statistical analyses were corrected conservatively for the nonindependence of the twin observations by using tests based on the sandwich or Huber/White variance estimator75 in Stata statistical software, version 10.76

RESULTS

CHARACTERISTICS OF CHILDHOOD PSYCHOTIC SYMPTOMS

Heritable

The within-pair correlation for the scale measuring psychotic symptoms was 0.41 among monozygotic twins and 0.22 among dizygotic twins. The fact that the correlation was greater among monozygotic vs dizygotic twins suggests a genetic influence for this phenotype. Specifically, genetic effects accounted for 43% of the variance (95% confidence interval, 34%–52%), and child-specific environmental factors and error accounted for 57% (51%–64%).

Familial

Table 3 presents the risk factors and correlates for symptom-absent vs symptom-present children. Children with psychotic symptoms were more likely than children without psychotic symptoms to have mothers with psychosis-spectrum disorders as well as family members who had been admitted to psychiatric units and who had attempted or completed suicide (Table 3).

Table 3.

Schizophrenia-Related Risk Factors and Correlates Observed Among Symptom-Present vs Symptom-Absent Children

Psychotic Symptoms at Age 12 ya
Symptom-Absent Group
(n=2002)
Symptom-Present Group
(n=125)
OR (95% CI)
or B (SE)
P Value
Demographic characteristics
  Boys 963 (48.1) 71 (56.8) 1.4 (0.9–2.1) .09
  Ethnic minority 201 (10.0) 8 (6.4) 0.6 (0.3–1.5) .28
Familiality
  Maternal psychosis-spectrum disorder 99 (5.1) 14 (11.7) 2.5 (1.2–4.9) .01
  Family members admitted to psychiatric units 509 (25.5) 47 (37.9) 1.8 (1.2–2.7) .007
  Family members with suicide attempts 312 (15.7) 36 (29.0) 2.2 (1.4–3.5) .001
Social risk factors
  Urban residence 987 (51.1) 79 (64.8) 1.8 (1.2–2.7) .009
  Socioeconomic disadvantage 658 (32.9) 53 (42.4) 1.5 (1.0–2.3) .049
Neurodevelopmental risk factors
  Paternal age at birth, mean (SD), y 31.9 (6.3) 31.8 (6.9) 0.03 (0.80) .97
  Birth in winter or spring 933 (49.4) 66 (55.0) 1.3(0.8–1.9) .28
  Birth weight, mean (SD) z score 0.02 (1.0) −0.19 (0.98) −0.21 (0.10) .04
  Multiple perinatal complications 353 (21.5) 31 (29.5) 1.5 (0.9–2.5) .09
  IQ, mean (SD) 100.5 (14.9) 93.0 (14.6) −7.51 (1.43) <.001
  Executive functioning score, mean (SD) 100.2 (14.8) 97.5 (15.8) −2.65 (1.46) .07
  Theory of mind score, mean (SD) 100.3 (15.0) 95.6 (13.8) −4.74 (1.32) <.001
Home-rearing risk factors
  Maternal expressed emotion score
    Negativity, mean (SD) −0.01 (1.0) 0.24 (1.0) 0.24 (0.10) .02
    Warmth, mean (SD) 0 (0.9) −0.02 (1.1) −0.02 (0.10) .84
  Household chaos score, mean (SD)
    Maternal report −0.02 (1.0) 0.26 (1.1) 0.27 (0.11) .02
    Child report −0.03 (1.0) 0.53 (1.1) 0.56 (0.10) <.001
  Physical maltreatment 101 (5.0) 20 (16.0) 3.6 (2.0–6.4) <.001
Behavioral, emotional, and educational risk factors at age 5 y
  Antisocial behavior, mean (SD) z score
    Maternal report −0.01 (0.99) 0.22 (1.12) 0.24 (0.10) .02
    Teacher report −0.01 (0.99) 0.31 (1.19) 0.33 (0.11) .005
    Child report −0.02 (0.99) 0.29 (1.10) 0.31 (0.12) .01
  ADHD Symptoms, mean (SD) z score
    Maternal report −0.02 (0.99) 0.28 (1.03) 0.31 (0.09) .001
    Teacher report −0.02 (0.99) 0.32 (1.15) 0.33 (0.12) .006
  Internalizing problems, mean (SD) z score
    Maternal report −0.01 (0.98) 0.29 (1.17) 0.31 (0.11) .006
    Teacher report −0.01 (0.99) 0.16 (1.14) 0.17 (0.11) .12
  Social isolation
    Maternal report 106 (5.3) 12 (9.6) 1.9 (1.0–3.5) .04
    Teacher report 52 (2.8) 7 (6.0) 2.3 (0.9–5.5) .07
  Educational problems 683 (36.2) 70 (59.8) 2.6 (1.8–3.9) <.001
Comorbid behavioral and emotional problems at age 12 y
  Antisocial behavior, mean (SD) z score −0.03 (0.97) 0.52 (1.26) 0.55 (0.12) <.001
  Depressive symptoms, mean (SD) z score −0.06 (0.90) 0.85 (1.75) 0.90 (0.16) <.001
  Anxiety symptoms, mean (SD) z score −0.04 (0.99) 0.58 (1.0) 0.62 (0.10) <.001
  Tobacco use or experimentation 219 (11.1) 29 (25.0) 2.7 (1.7–4.2) <.001
  Cannabis use or experimentation 19 (1.0) 2 (1.7) 1.8 (0.4–7.7) .45
  Self-harm/suicidal behavior 51 (2.6) 11 (8.8) 3.7 (1.8–7.5) <.001

Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; β, regression coefficient; CI, confidence interval; OR, odds ratio.

a

Data are given as number (percentage) of participants unless otherwise indicated.

Schizophrenia-Related Risk Factors

With regard to social risk factors, children with psychotic symptoms were more likely than children without psychotic symptoms to live in an urban environment and to come from disadvantaged families. With regard to neurodevelopmental risk factors, children with psychotic symptoms had lower birth weights for their gestational age and were slightly more likely to have multiple perinatal complications (P=.09). They exhibited significantly lower IQs, slightly greater executive deficits (P=.07), and impaired theory of mind. With regard to home-rearing risk factors, children with psychotic symptoms were reared by mothers who had more negative expressed emotion toward them (but not less warmth), lived in more chaotic households (according to both parent and child reports), and were more likely to have been physically maltreated (Table 3).

Early Childhood Behavioral Risk Factors

Twelve-year-olds with psychotic symptoms had more externalizing behavior problems (antisocial, inattentive, and hyperactive behaviors) as 5-year-olds, according to all informants. According to maternal reports, 12-year-olds with psychotic symptoms also had more internalizing problems at age 5, but this finding was only a trend according to teacher reports. Finally, 12-year-olds with psychotic symptoms were more likely to have been socially isolated at age 5, according to mothers and, marginally so, teachers, and to have had educational problems at age 5 (Table 3).

Concurrent Behavioral and Emotional Problems

Children with psychotic symptoms reported that they engaged in more antisocial behavior and experienced more symptoms of depression and anxiety than children without psychotic symptoms. Children with psychotic symptoms were also more likely to have used tobacco but not cannabis. Finally, according to their mothers, children with psychotic symptoms were more likely to have engaged in self-harm (Table 3). According to maternal reports, the self-harm behaviors included cutting with razors, beating head against the wall, and attempted hanging. Two children made suicide attempts resulting in hospitalization, in which the children followed voices of command. (Although not included in our measure of self-harm behaviors, one child attempted to cut his mother with a knife following a voice of command.) There were no completed suicides.

ADDITIONAL ANALYSES

We conducted 3 sets of additional analyses. First, because we included children with only probable symptoms in the symptom-absent group, we investigated how the distribution of risk factors and correlates in these children compares with the distribution of risk factors and correlates in children with definite symptoms and no symptoms. Children with only probable symptoms generally had intermediate scores, between those of children with no symptoms and children with definite symptoms. Because the nature of their self-reported symptoms was more ambiguous and clinically uncertain, including them in the symptom-absent group is a conservative approach.

Second, because it is conceivable that the risk factors and correlates investigated are not specific to schizophrenia, we investigated their association with childhood psychotic symptoms controlling for concurrent depressive symptoms. Only 4 of 25 correlates were no longer independently associated with psychotic symptoms at a P value of ≤.05: socioeconomic disadvantage (P=.09), mother’s report of antisocial behavior at age 5 (P=.06), and mother’s (P = .14) and teacher’s (P=.15) report of social isolation at age 5. The effect of all 21 other risk factors and correlates remained statistically significant, including self-harm (odds ratio, 2.0; 95% confidence interval, 1.0–3.9; P=.04).

Third, because it is conceivable that psychotic symptoms are associated with neurological disorders, and in some cases may be caused by them, we investigated whether migraine, epilepsy, seizures, or other neurological disorders were more common in the symptom-present group. The occurrence of childhood psychotic symptoms was not associated with these neurological disorders in our sample (odds ratio, 1.8; 95% confidence interval, 0.7–4.5; P= .23). Five children in the symptom-present group (4.0%) and 46 in the symptom-absent group (2.3%) presented these conditions.

COMMENT

We examined psychotic symptoms and their risk factors in a birth cohort of 12-year-old twins ascertained from the general population. Results confirmed that a significant minority of 12-year-olds in the community self-report hallucinations and delusions. In addition, these symptoms are associated with many of the same risk factors and correlates as adult schizophrenia, including genetic, social, neurodevelopmental, home-rearing, and behavioral risks.

Our findings suggest that the continuum model of psychosis15 may apply to preadolescents, as well as to the adults for which it was developed. The prevalence of psychotic symptoms in our birth cohort of 12-year-olds was 5.9%, which is similar to the reported prevalence of childhood psychotic symptoms in other contemporary community samples of adolescents between ages 11 and 17 years46,21,77 (although prevalence estimates vary as the measures and numbers of questions used to assess psychotic symptoms vary). Furthermore, hallucinations were the most frequent psychotic symptom reported, a pattern previously reported in community21,22 and clinical samples.78,79 This estimated prevalence of psychotic symptoms in child populations markedly exceeds the estimated prevalence of early-onset schizophrenia,80 just as the prevalence of psychotic symptoms in adult populations markedly exceeds the prevalence of adult psychotic disorders.15,24 The presence of psychotic symptoms in preadolescence adds support to the hypothesis that symptoms may signal a longstanding trait diathesis, which in some but not all individuals converts to clinical disorder during adolescence or adulthood.

Our findings also suggest that the neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia9,11 is useful for understanding the pathogenesis of childhood psychotic symptoms. As with adult schizophrenia, childhood psychotic symptoms were familial and heritable, associated with early impairments in cognitive functioning, and linked to premorbid behavioral, emotional, and educational problems. (Childhood psychotic symptoms were not associated with ethnicity.) Three findings, in particular, warrant comment.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to evaluate the familiality and heritability of psychotic symptoms in a representative sample of children from the general population. Consistent with studies of schizophrenia,81 we found that childhood hallucinations and delusions are influenced by genetic and environmental factors that are unique in the life of each sibling (and measurement error). Our twin data yielded a moderate coefficient of heritability (43%) that is similar to heritability estimates for adult psychotic symptoms and schizotypy82,83 and somewhat lower than heritability estimates for schizophrenia,29 suggesting that individuals with symptoms that progress to clinical disorder are influenced by a stronger genetic load. Alternatively, it is possible that the heritability of psychotic symptoms, assessed here at age 12, may increase with age as does the heritability of some other psychiatric phenotypes.84 The nonsignificant quantitative estimate of environmental factors shared by siblings in a family is also consistent with previous studies of genetic and environmental influences on schizotypy85 and schizophrenia.86 Our study revealed that, like schizophrenia itself, childhood psychotic symptoms are associated with measured social and home-rearing risk factors that might be expected to have similar consequences for siblings growing up together (eg, urban residence, socioeconomic disadvantage, household chaos). How is it possible that measured family-environment risks predicted psychotic symptoms, whereas the twin model estimated no family-environment effect on psychotic symptoms? Whether such risk factors lead to childhood psychotic symptoms may depend on each child’s genetic vulnerabilities.87 When influences of siblings’ shared environments interact with genetic vulnerability, these effects are included in the genetic component of twin models.

Second, we observed several cognitive deficits among children as young as 5 years who later developed psychotic symptoms. Impaired theory of mind at age 5 characterized children with psychotic symptoms, corroborating the hypothesis that a deficit in the capacity to infer and represent others’ mental states is a trait of individuals with psychotic disorders.40 Moreover, the IQ deficit that we found among children with psychotic symptoms at age 5 is the same size as the IQ deficit observed among individuals with schizophrenia before illness onset: approximately one-half of a standard deviation.37 Of interest, our composite measure of executive functions at age 5 (planning ability, inhibitory control, and working memory) was only weakly, and not significantly, associated with psychotic symptoms at age 12. It is possible that age 5 years may be too young to detect executive functioning deficits in relation to psychosis; recent research suggests that deficits in executive functions may emerge in later childhood or early adolescence.88 Although the neurodevelopmental theory of schizophrenia has not emphasized childhood hallucinations and delusions, our findings suggest that such symptoms should be incorporated into the theory. Neurodevelopmental risk factors such as low IQ are common in the population and are associated with other mental disorders in adulthood.89 The key question is why only a few children with neurodevelopmental deficits and lower cognitive reserve eventually progress to adult schizophrenia, whereas most do not. Neurodevelopmental risks in early childhood affect many children; a smaller subset of children with neurodevelopmental risk go on to experience psychotic symptoms in preadolescence, and a smaller subset of these preadolescents with psychotic symptoms go on to develop schizophrenia. Thus, the psychotic symptoms we have studied may help to identify a key turning point in the canalization from neurodevelopmental risk into clinical disorder and to elucidate the nature of the association between low childhood IQ and schizophrenia.

Third, we found that 12-year-olds with psychotic symptoms had significantly more behavioral, emotional, and educational problems by age 5. Moreover, when children manifested psychotic symptoms as 12-year-olds, they also had significantly more antisocial behavior, depression symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and self-harm. These findings are consistent with reports of higher rates of psychiatric comorbidity in clinical samples of children with psychosis.78,79 These findings are also consistent with prospective3,10 and retrospective90,91 studies showing that adults with schizophrenia had elevated rates of aggression, anxiety, depression, and social and educational problems as children. Strikingly, like young people with clinical psychosis79 and adults with schizophrenia,49 children with psychotic symptoms were more likely to engage in self-harm or suicidal behavior (independent of depression). Given that children’s self-harm was reported by their mothers in our study, not the children themselves, and that children can conceal self-harm from parents, the association between psychotic symptoms and self-harm may be underestimated here.

Our study has limitations. First, we were unable to evaluate the role of all important schizophrenia risk factors, such as delayed motor development. Second, we did not examine how the various schizophrenia risk factors were themselves correlated and whether these risk factors had independent or overlapping associations with children’s psychotic symptoms. Rather, the goal of this study was to assess the construct validity of children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms by evaluating the no-mological network surrounding these symptoms. Such an evaluation of construct validity requires testing hypothesized relations between children’s symptoms and risk factors and correlates for which there are theoretical grounds to expect significant associations.92 To our knowledge, the results of this study provide the most comprehensive picture to date of the clinical and theoretical significance of children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms. Third, we studied a cohort of twins, who may not represent singletons. However, prior comparisons have found no twin to singleton differences in behavior problems, IQ, or personality traits.9398 Nevertheless, replication of findings in studies of singletons is important. Fourth, we evaluated only a set of 7 positive symptoms. A more extensive assessment, including negative symptoms, may be desirable to identify risk factors and correlates that may be specific to particular symptom dimensions.

Our study also has strengths. We studied a nationally representative sample, followed up to age 12 years with 96% retention. Psychotic symptoms were assessed by well-trained mental health interviewers in a private interview with each child, and reports were subsequently reviewed by expert clinicians. Risk factors were assessed prospectively through multiple informants or formal testing of the child using measures with documented validity, independent of the assessment of psychotic symptoms. Incidentally, only 2% of the children with psychotic symptoms had used cannabis and only 4% had neurological disorders, indicating that the psychotic symptoms reported were not the result of these causes.

Our results have implications for research and clinical practice. For researchers, the findings indicate that children in the community who have psychotic symptoms can be recruited for neuroscience research into the pathogenesis of schizophrenia,99 years before the prodrome and long before the neurobiological picture is muddied by medications, substance abuse, and the rapid changes in brain development that characterize adolescence.100 For example, a neuroimaging paradigm recently used with ultra–high-risk and first-episode research cases, aged 14 to 30 years, could be extended to children assessed here.101 Because the risk factors and correlates investigated are not specific to schizophrenia, it is possible that childhood psychotic symptoms are developmental precursors not only of schizophrenia but also of other disorders. Longitudinal evaluation of individuals throughout adulthood can address this issue. Because, as our findings show, psychotic symptoms generally occurred in the context of other disorders, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, antisocial conduct, depression, and anxiety, it is feasible to screen pediatric psychiatric patients to identify children with hallucinations and delusions. It is our impression, based on experience with the E-Risk and Dunedin cohorts, that ages 11 to 12 years are an ideal window for obtaining self-reports of covert experiences such as hallucinations and delusions in a clinical interview. On the one hand, pre-adolescents are cognitively mature enough to understand what the questions are about. Therefore, they can provide valid self-reports of hallucinations and delusions. On the other hand, they have not yet learned by sad experience that they must conceal their psychotic symptoms to avoid stigma, ridicule, and rejection by others. Thus, preadolescents are willing to share frank self-reports with a sensitive interviewer.

For clinicians, the findings indicate that psychotic symptoms in childhood are often a marker of an impaired developmental process and should be actively assessed. Psychotic symptoms generally occurred in the context of other childhood psychiatric problems, indicating that it is worthwhile to ask all preadolescent psychiatric patients about hallucinations and delusions. Even if the psychotic symptoms are not themselves impairing, they are associated with important risk factors, such as chaotic household, maternal negativity, and physical maltreatment, and with behavioral problems, such as early tobacco use and self-harm, that should be a focus of attention. Whether interventions focused on childhood psychotic symptoms will prove necessary, feasible, or cost-effective is an important and unanswered question.

Acknowledgments

Funding/Support: This research was supported by grants G9806489, G0100527, and G0601483 from the United Kingdom Medical Research Council; grant MH077874 from the National Institute of Mental Health; a 2008 National Alliance of Research on Schizophrenia and Depression Young Investigator Award (Dr Polanczyk); a Clinician Scientist Award from the Health Research Board, Ireland (Dr Cannon); and a Royal Society-Wolfson Merit Award (Dr Caspi). Dr Odgers is a William T. Grant Scholar.

Additional Contributions: We thank the families and staff of the E-Risk Longitudinal Twin Study.

Footnotes

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Poulton R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, Murray R, Harrington H. Children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(11):1053–1058. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Schreier A, Wolke D, Thomas K, Horwood J, Hollis C, Gunnell D, Lewis G, Thompson A, Zammit S, Duffy L, Salvi G, Harrison G. Prospective study of peer victimization in childhood and psychotic symptoms in a nonclinical population at age 12 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(5):527–536. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Welham J, Scott J, Williams G, Najman J, Bor W, O’Callaghan M, McGrath J. Emotional and behavioural antecedents of young adults who screen positive for non-affective psychosis: a 21-year birth cohort study. Psychol Med. 2009;39(4):625–634. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Scott J, Martin G, Bor W, Sawyer M, Clark J, McGrath J. The prevalence and correlates of hallucinations in Australian adolescents: results from a national survey. Schizophr Res. 2009;107(2–3):179–185. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Dhossche D, Ferdinand R, Van der Ende J, Hofstra MB, Verhulst F. Diagnostic outcome of self-reported hallucinations in a community sample of adolescents. Psychol Med. 2002;32(4):619–627. doi: 10.1017/s003329170200555x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Yoshizumi T, Murase S, Honjo S, Kaneko H, Murakami T. Hallucinatory experiences in a community sample of Japanese children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43(8):1030–1036. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000126937.44875.6b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Rapoport JL, Addington AM, Frangou S, Psych MR. The neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: update 2005. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(5):434–449. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Welham J, Isohanni M, Jones P, McGrath J. The antecedents of schizophrenia: a review of birth cohort studies. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(3):603–623. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Marenco S, Weinberger DR. The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia: following a trail of evidence from cradle to grave. Dev Psychopathoi. 2000;12(3):501–527. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmot M. Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet. 1994;344(8934):1398–1402. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(94)90569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Murray RM, Lappin J, Di Forti M. Schizophrenia: from developmental deviance to dopamine dysregulation. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;18(suppl 3):S129–S134. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.van Os J, Hanssen M, Bijl RV, Ravelli A. Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophr Res. 2000;45(1–2):11–20. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00224-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Johns LC, Cannon M, Singleton N, Murray RM, Farrell M, Brugha T, Bebbington P, Jenkins R, Meltzer H. Prevalence and correlates of self-reported psychotic symptoms in the British population. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;185:298–305. doi: 10.1192/bjp.185.4.298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wiles NJ, Zammit S, Bebbington P, Singleton N, Meltzer H, Lewis G. Self-reported psychotic symptoms in the general population: results from the longitudinal study of the British National Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;188:519–526. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.012179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med. 2009;39(2):179–195. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lataster T, Myin-Germeys I, Derom C, Thiery E, van Os J. Evidence that self-reported psychotic experiences represent the transitory developmental expression of genetic liability to psychosis in the general population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B(8):1078–1084. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Dutta R, Greene T, Addington J, McKenzie K, Phillips M, Murray RM. Biological, life course, and cross-cultural studies all point toward the value of dimensional and developmental ratings in the classification of psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33(4):868–876. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cannon M, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Taylor A, Murray RM, Poulton R. Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(5):449–456. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.5.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Zammit S, Odd D, Horwood J, Thompson A, Thomas K, Menezes P, Gunnell D, Hollis C, Wolke D, Lewis G, Harrison G. Investigating whether adverse prenatal and perinatal events are associated with non-clinical psychotic symptoms at age 12 years in the ALSPAC birth cohort [published online February 12,2009] Psychol Med. 2009;39(9):1457–1467. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708005126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Zammit S, Horwood J, Thompson A, Thomas K, Menezes P, Gunnell D, Hollis C, Wolke D, Lewis G, Harrison G. Investigating if psychosis-like symptoms (PLIKS) are associated with family history of schizophrenia or paternal age in the ALSPAC birth cohort. Schizophr Res. 2008;104(1–3):279–286. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Horwood J, Salvi G, Thomas K, Duffy L, Gunnell D, Hollis C, Lewis G, Menezes P, Thompson A, Wolke D, Zammit S, Harrison G. IQ and non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;193(3):185–191. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kelleher I, Harley M, Lynch F, Arseneault L, Fitzpatrick C, Cannon M. Associations between childhood trauma, bullying and psychotic symptoms among a school-based adolescent sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;193(5):378–382. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.049536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Laurens KR, Hodgins S, Maughan B, Murray RM, Rutter ML, Taylor EA. Community screening for psychotic-like experiences and other putative antecedents of schizophrenia in children aged 9–12 years. Schizophr Res. 2007;90(1–3):130–146. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Murray RM, Jones PB, Susser ES, van Os J, Cannon M. The Epidemiology of Schizophrenia. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Messias EL, Chen CY, Eaton WW. Epidemiology of schizophrenia: review of findings and myths. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2007;30(3):323–338. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2007.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.McGrath JJ. The surprisingly rich contours of schizophrenia epidemiology. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(1):14–16. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.1.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cannon M, Clarke MC. Risk for schizophrenia: broadening the concepts, pushing back the boundaries. Schizophr Res. 2005;79(1):5–13. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Byrne M, Agerbo E, Mortensen PB. Family history of psychiatric disorders and age at first contact in schizophrenia: an epidemiological study. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 2002;43:s19–s25. doi: 10.1192/bjp.181.43.s19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sullivan PF, Kendler KS, Neale MC. Schizophrenia as a complex trait: evidence from a meta-analysis of twin studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(12):1187–1192. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lewis G, David A, Andreasson S, Allebeck P. Schizophrenia and city life. Lancet. 1992;340(8812):137–140. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(92)93213-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Byrne M, Agerbo E, Eaton WW, Mortensen PB. Parental socio-economic status and risk of first admission with schizophrenia: a Danish National Register-based study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2004;39(2):87–96. doi: 10.1007/s00127-004-0715-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Malaspina D, Harlap S, Fennig S, Heiman D, Nahon D, Feldman D, Susser ES. Advancing paternal age and the risk of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(4):361–367. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Mortensen PB, Pedersen CB, Westergaard T, Wohlfahrt J, Ewald H, Mors O, Andersen PK, Melbye M. Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia. N Engl J Med. 1999;340(8):603–608. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199902253400803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Jones PB, Rantakallio P, Hartikainen AL, Isohanni M, Sipila P. Schizophrenia as a long-term outcome of pregnancy, delivery, and perinatal complications: a 28-year follow-up of the 1966 north Finland general population birth cohort. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(3):355–364. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rifkin L, Lewis S, Jones P, Toone B, Murray R. Low birth weight and schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 1994;165(3):357–362. doi: 10.1192/bjp.165.3.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Cannon M, Jones PB, Murray RM. Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: historical and meta-analytic review. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(7):1080–1092. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Woodberry KA, Giuliano AJ, Seidman LJ. Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(5):579–587. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kerns JG, Nuechterlein KH, Braver TS, Barch DM. Executive functioning component mechanisms and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(1):26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Keshavan MS, Diwadkar VA, Montrose DM, Rajarethinam R, Sweeney JA. Premorbid indicators and risk for schizophrenia: a selective review and update. Schizophr Res. 2005;79(1):45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Brüne M. “Theory of mind” in schizophrenia: a review of the literature. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31(1):21–42. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sprang M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, van Engeland H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191:5–13. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.035899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kuipers L, Bebbington P. Expressed emotion research in schizophrenia: theoretical and clinical implications. Psychol Med. 1988;18(4):893–909. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700009831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Evans GW, Gonnella C, Marcynyszyn LA, Gentile L, Salpekar N. The role of chaos in poverty and children’s socioemotional adjustment. Psychol Sci. 2005;16(7):560–565. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01575.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Morgan C, Fisher H. Environment and schizophrenia: environmental factors in schizophrenia: childhood trauma—a critical review [published online November 14, 2006] Schizophr Bull. 2007;33(1):3–10. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Read J, van Os J, Morrison AP, Ross CA. Childhood trauma, psychosis and schizophrenia: a literature review with theoretical and clinical implications. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112(5):330–350. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00634.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bendall S, Jackson HJ, Hulbert CA, McGorry PD. Childhood trauma and psychotic disorders: a systematic, critical review of the evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(3):568–579. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.de Leon J, Diaz FJ. A meta-analysis of worldwide studies demonstrates an association between schizophrenia and tobacco smoking behaviors. Schizophr Res. 2005;76(2–3):135–157. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Moore TH, Zammit S, Lingford-Hughes A, Barnes TR, Jones PB, Burke M, Lewis G. Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective mental health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet. 2007;370(9584):319–328. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61162-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Palmer BA, Pankratz VS, Bostwick JM. The lifetime risk of suicide in schizophrenia: a reexamination. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(3):247–253. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Trouton A, Spinath FM, Plomin R. Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): a multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems in childhood. Twin Res. 2002;5(5):444–448. doi: 10.1375/136905202320906255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Moffitt TE E-Risk Study Team. Teen-aged mothers in contemporary Britain. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43(6):727–742. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kim-Cohen J, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Pawlby SJ, Caspi A. Maternal depression and children’s antisocial behavior: nature and nurture effects. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(2):173–181. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Robins LN, Cottier L, Bucholz KK, Compton W. Diagnostic Interview Schedule for DSM-IV. St Louis, MO: Washington University School of Medicine; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Trzesniewski KH, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Maughan B. Revisiting the association between reading achievement and antisocial behavior: new evidence of an environmental explanation from a twin study. Child Dev. 2006;77(1):72–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00857.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Buckler JM, Green M. Birth weight and head circumference standards for English twins. Arch Dis Child. 1994;71(6):516–521. doi: 10.1136/adc.71.6.516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Revised. London, England: The Psychological Corporation; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Sattler J. Assessment of Children: WISC-III and WPPSI-R Supplement. San Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler, Publisher, Inc; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Grodzinsky GM, Diamond R. Frontal lobe functioning in boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Dev Neuropsychol. 1992;8:427–445. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Gerstadt CL, Hong YJ, Diamond A. The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3.5–7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition. 1994;53(2):129–153. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Baddeley A. Exploring the central executive. Q J Exp Psychol. 1996;49A:5–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Baddeley AD. Working Memory. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Hughes C, Adlam A, Happe F, Jackson J, Taylor A, Caspi A. Good test-retest reliability for standard and advanced false-belief tasks across a wide range of abilities. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41(4):483–490. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Morgan J, Rutter M, Taylor A, Arseneault L, Tully L, Jacobs C, Kim-Cohen J, Polo-Tomas M. Maternal expressed emotion predicts children’s antisocial behavior problems: using monozygotic-twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral development. Dev Psychol. 2004;40(2):449–161. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Valente E. Social information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later conduct problems. J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104(4):632–643. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.4.632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Rutter M, Tomas MP, Moffitt TE. The caregiving environments provided to children by depressed mothers with or without an antisocial history. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(6):1009–1018. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.6.1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Measelle JR, Ablow JC, Cowan PA, Cowan CP. Assessing young children’s views of their academic, social, and emotional lives: an evaluation of the self-perception scales of the Berkeley Puppet Interview. Child Dev. 1998;69(6):1556–1576. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Arseneault L, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Rijsdijk FV, Jaffee SR, Ablow JC, Measelle JR. Strong genetic effects on cross-situational antisocial behaviour among 5-year-old children according to mothers, teachers, examiner-observers, and twins’ self-reports. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44(6):832–848. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Kuntsi J, Eley TC, Taylor A, Hughes C, Asherson P, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Co-occurrence of ADHD and low IQ has genetic origins. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004;124B(1):41–47. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Kovacs M. Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) Manual. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.March JS, Parker JD, Sullivan K, Stallings P, Conners CK. The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Child ren(MASC): factor structure, reliability, and validity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(4):554–565. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Mplus, version 5 [computer program] Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Prescott CA. Using the Mplus computer program to estimate models for continuous and categorical data from twins. Behav Genet. 2004;34(1):17–40. doi: 10.1023/B:BEGE.0000009474.97649.2f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Williams RL. A note on robust variance estimaton for cluster-correlated data. Biometrics. 2000;56(2):645–646. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341x.2000.00645.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Stata, version 9.0 [computer program] College Station, TX: StataCorp; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Yung AR, Nelson B, Baker K, Buckby JA, Baksheev G, Cosgrave EM. Psychotic-like experiences in a community sample of adolescents: implications for the continuum model of psychosis and prediction of schizophrenia. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2009;43(2):118–128. doi: 10.1080/00048670802607188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Biederman J, Petty C, Faraone SV, Seidman L. Phenomenology of childhood psychosis: findings from a large sample of psychiatrically referred youth. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004;192(9):607–614. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000138228.59938.c3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Ulloa RE, Birmaher B, Axelson D, Williamson DE, Brent DA, Ryan ND, Bridge J, Baugher M. Psychosis in a pediatric mood and anxiety disorders clinic: phenomenology and correlates. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(3):337–345. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200003000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Nicolson R, Rapoport JL. Childhood-onset schizophrenia: what can it teach us? In: Rapoport JL, editor. Childhood Onset of “Adult” Psychopathology: Clinical and Research Advances. Washington, DC: American Psychopathological Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Tsuang M. Schizophrenia: genes and environment. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47(3):210–220. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00289-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.MacDonald AW, III, Pogue-Geile MF, Debski TT, Manuck S. Genetic and environmental influences on schizotypy: a community-based twin study. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27(1):47–58. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Hay DA, Martin NG, Foley D, Treloar SA, Kirk KM, Heath AC. Phenotypic and genetic analyses of a short measure of psychosis-proneness in a large-scale Australian twin study. Twin Res. 2001;4(1):30–40. doi: 10.1375/1369052012128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Bergen SE, Gardner CO, Kendler KS. Age-related changes in heritability of behavioral phenotypes over adolescence and young adulthood: a meta-analysis. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007;10(3):423–433. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.3.423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Linney YM, Murray RM, Peters ER, MacDonald AM, Rijsdijk F, Sham PC. A quantitative genetic analysis of schizotypal personality traits. Psychol Med. 2003;33(5):803–816. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703007906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Cardno AG, Marshall EJ, Coid B, Macdonald AM, Ribchester TR, Davies NJ, Venturi P, Jones LA, Lewis SW, Sham PC, Gottesman II, Farmer AE, McGuffin P, Reveley AM, Murray RM. Heritability estimates for psychotic disorders: the Maudsley twin psychosis series. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(2):162–168. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.2.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.van Os J, Rutten BP, Poulton R. Gene-environment interactions in schizophrenia: review of epidemiological findings and future directions. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(6):1066–1082. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Reichenberg A, Caspi A, Harrington H, Houts R, Keefe RS, Murray RM, Poulton R, Moffitt TE. Static and dynamic cognitive deficits in childhood preceding adult schizophrenia: a 30-year study [published online ahead of print January 4, 2010] Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(2):160–169. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, Martin LT, Kubzansky L, Harrington H, Poulton R, Caspi A. Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: a test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166(1):50–57. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Hollis C. Developmental precursors of child-and adolescent-onset schizophrenia and affective psychoses: diagnostic specificity and continuity with symptom dimensions. Br J Psychiatry. 2003;182:37–44. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Reichenberg A, Weiser M, Rabinowitz J, Caspi A, Schmeicller J, Mark M, Kaplan Z, Davidson M. A population-based cohort study of premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral functioning in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and nonpsychotic bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(12):2027–2035. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Wiggins JS. Personality and Prediction: Principles of Personality Assessment. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Co; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Gjone H, Novik TS. Parental ratings of behavior problems: a twin and general-population comparison. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995;36(7):1213–1224. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01366.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Levy F, Hay D, McLaughlin M, Wood C, Waldman I. Twin-sibling differences in parental reports of ADHD, speech, reading, and behavioural problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996;37(5):569–578. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01443.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Moilanen I, Linna SL, Ebeling H, Kumpulainen K, Tamminen T, Piha J, Almqvist F. Are twins’ behavioural/emotional problems different from singletons’? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;8(suppl 4):62–67. doi: 10.1007/pl00010702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.van den Oord EJ, Koot HM, Boomsma Dl, Verhulst FC, Orlebeke JF. A twin-singleton comparison of problem behavior in 2- to 3-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995;36(3):449–458. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01302.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Kendler KS, Martin NG, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. Self-report psychiatric symptoms in twins and their nontwin relatives: are twins different? Am J Med Genet. 1995;60(6):588–591. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320600622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Johnson W, Krueger RF, Bouchard TJ, Jr, McGue M. The personalities of twins just ordinary folks. Twin Res. 2002;5(2):125–131. doi: 10.1375/1369052022992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Bressan RA, Shih MC, Hoexter MQ, Lacerda AL. Can molecular imaging techniques identify biomarkers for neuropsychiatric disorders? Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2007;29(2):102–104. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462007000200003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, Gogtay N, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Evans A, Rapoport JL, Giedd JN, Wise SP. Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28(14):3586–3594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Takahashi T, Wood SJ, Yung AR, Soulsby B, McGorry PD, Suzuki M, Kawasaki Y, Phillips LJ, Velakoulis D, Pantelis C. Progressive gray matter reduction of the superior temporal gyrus during transition to psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(4):366–376. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES