Skip to main content
NeuroImage : Clinical logoLink to NeuroImage : Clinical
. 2012 Nov 7;2:17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.008

The impact of serotonin transporter genotype on default network connectivity in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders

Jillian Lee Wiggins a,, Scott J Peltier b, Jirair K Bedoyan c, Melisa Carrasco d, Robert C Welsh e,f, Donna M Martin c,d,g, Catherine Lord h, Christopher S Monk a,d,f,i
PMCID: PMC3777679  PMID: 24179754

Abstract

Compared to healthy controls, individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have weaker posterior–anterior connectivity that strengthens less with age within the default network, a set of brain structures connected in the absence of a task and likely involved in social function. The serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) genotypes that result in lowered serotonin transporter expression are associated with social impairment in ASD. Additionally, in healthy controls, low expressing 5-HTTLPR genotypes are associated with weaker default network connectivity. However, in ASD, the effect of 5-HTTLPR on the default network is unknown. We hypothesized that 5-HTTLPR's influence on posterior–anterior default network connectivity strength as well as on age-related changes in connectivity differs in the ASD group versus controls. Youth with ASD and healthy controls, ages 8–19, underwent a resting fMRI acquisition. Connectivity was calculated by correlating the posterior hub of the default network with all voxels. Triallelic genotype was assessed via PCR and Sanger sequencing. A genotype-by-diagnosis interaction significantly predicted posterior–anterior connectivity, such that low expressing genotypes (S/S, S/LG, LG/LG) were associated with stronger connectivity than high expressing genotypes (LA/LA, S/LA, LA/LG) in the ASD group, but the converse was true for controls. Also, youth with ASD and low expressing genotypes had greater age-related increases in connectivity values compared to those with high expressing genotypes and controls in either genotype group. Our findings suggest that the cascade of events from genetic variation to brain function differs in ASD. Also, low expressing genotypes may represent a subtype within ASD.

Keywords: Functional MRI, Resting connectivity, Development, Serotonin transporter gene, Autism, Default network

Highlights

► Youth with autism and controls genotyped for 5-HTTLPR ► Posterior–anterior default network connectivity assessed with MRI during rest ► Stronger connectivity in low versus high expressing genotypes in autism ► Weaker connectivity in low versus high expressing genotypes in controls ► Largest age-related increases in connectivity in ASD with low expressing genotypes

1. Introduction

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by social and communicative impairments and rigid repetitive behaviors. The prevalence of ASD has sharply increased in recent years and is currently 1 in 88 (CDC, 2012). Deciphering the complex etiology of ASD is thus a priority, and progress will likely involve examining the condition using multiple methodologies, including neuroimaging and molecular genetics.

As alterations in brain connectivity have been repeatedly implicated in ASD (Hughes, 2007), attention has been focused on identifying perturbations in fundamental, large-scale networks, such as the default network, that may contribute to ASD symptoms. In healthy adults, the default network (including the posterior cingulate, angular gyri, superior frontal gyri/Brodmann's area (BA) 10, and anterior cingulate/BA 10) is active and functionally connected in the absence of a demanding task (Raichle and Snyder, 2007). Functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity of the default network in healthy adults (Greicius et al., 2009). The default network contains posterior and anterior hubs (Buckner et al., 2008) that typically display strong long-range connectivity but are distinct from one another (Horovitz et al., 2009).

The primary purpose of the default network is a subject of debate. The default network may relate to basic central nervous system functions such as maintaining the balance of excitatory and inhibitory inputs or interpreting information from the environment (Raichle and Snyder, 2007). Alternatively, the primary purpose of the default network may be related to social cognition, including self-referential processes (Gusnard et al., 2001) and mentally projecting oneself into hypothetical situations (Buckner and Carroll, 2007).

Studies on adults with ASD (Cherkassky et al., 2006; Kennedy and Courchesne, 2008; Monk et al., 2009) as well as adolescents (Weng et al., 2010; Anderson et al., 2011; Wiggins et al., 2011) found weaker connectivity between the posterior and anterior default network compared to controls. Moreover, the weaker the posterior–anterior default network connectivity, the worse the social impairment in individuals with ASD (Monk et al., 2009; Weng et al., 2010).

A few studies have investigated the development of the default network. For healthy individuals, posterior–anterior connectivity is weaker during childhood and adolescence than during adulthood both functionally (Fair et al., 2008; Stevens et al., 2009; Wiggins et al., 2011) and structurally (Supekar et al., 2010). These studies indicate that connectivity of this network increases in strength over childhood and adolescence in healthy individuals. In contrast, youth with ASD have attenuated increases in posterior–anterior connectivity with age compared to controls (Wiggins et al., 2011).

Identifying the genetic factors that influence the default network in ASD is important to further elucidate the complex etiology of ASD. The serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region variant (5-HTTLPR; Lesch et al., 1996) in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) is relevant to the default network in ASD. The S and LG alleles of 5-HTTLPR are associated with decreased serotonin transporter expression relative to the LA allele (A to G SNP in the L allele, rs25531; Hu et al., 2006). The low expressing alleles of 5-HTTLPR have been associated with worse social symptoms in ASD (Tordjman et al., 2001; Brune et al., 2006). In healthy adolescents, 5-HTTLPR is known to influence the default network: those with low expressing genotypes exhibit weaker posterior–anterior connectivity than adolescents with high expressing genotypes (Wiggins et al., 2012). Moreover, in healthy children and adolescents, 5-HTTLPR also impacts the development of default network connectivity such that youth with high expressing genotypes have greater age-related increases in posterior–anterior connectivity than those with low expressing genotypes (Wiggins et al., 2012). A previous study found that serotonin transporter binding in the anterior default network is decreased in individuals with autism (Nakamura et al., 2010). However, no study has yet examined how 5-HTTLPR affects default network connectivity or its development in individuals with ASD.

The present study addresses these two gaps in the literature on ASD: the role of 5-HTTLPR in default network connectivity and in the development of default network connectivity. This is accomplished by directly examining the influence of 5-HTTLPR variants on posterior–anterior default network connectivity as well on as age-related changes in connectivity in a sample of children and adolescents with ASD and controls. We hypothesized that the relationship between 5-HTTLPR genotype and posterior–anterior default network connectivity strength differs in the ASD group versus controls. Additionally, we hypothesized that the relationship between 5-HTTLPR and changes in connectivity across childhood and adolescence differs in the ASD group compared to controls.

2. Material and methods

2.1. Participants

Fifty-four children and adolescents with ASD and 66 healthy controls, aged 8.3 to 19.6 years, were included in this study (see Table 1 for participant characteristics). From a total of 105 participants with ASD and 82 controls recruited, 51 participants with ASD and 16 controls were excluded because of head movement exceeding 2.5 mm translation or 2.5° rotation, declining to complete the MRI scan due to discomfort, failure to return a saliva sample for genotyping, or technical problems with the MRI.

Controls were recruited through flyers posted at community organizations in the Ann Arbor, Michigan area. The University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center (UMACC) referred potential participants to our study and diagnosed participants with an ASD (autistic disorder, Asperger's syndrome, or pervasive developmental disorder — not otherwise specified) using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000), the Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised (ADI-R; Lord et al., 1994), and clinical consensus (Lord et al., 2006). The University of Michigan Institutional Review Board approved the procedures. Participants over age 18 gave written informed consent; participants under age 18 gave written assent and their parents gave written informed consent. Cognitive functioning was evaluated for controls with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT; Dunn and Dunn, 1997) and the Ravens Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1960); participants with ASD were given these measures or the Differential Ability Scales II — School Age (Elliott, 2005), the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales (Roid, 2003), the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children IV (Wechsler, 2003), or the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999). Participants with orthodontic braces, medical conditions contraindicated for MRI, or history of seizures or neurological disorders were excluded. Control participants were screened for psychological disorders with the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach and Edelbrock, 1981), Social Responsiveness Scale (Constantino et al., 2003), Social Communication Questionnaire (Rutter et al., 2003), Obsessive Compulsive Inventory — Revised (Foa et al., 2010), Child Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1992), and Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (March et al., 1997). All control participants scored below clinical cutoffs for affected status. Individuals with the low and high expressing genotypes did not differ in any of the symptom measures or cognitive functioning in either the ASD or control group (Inline Supplementary Table S1). Prior studies utilized portions of this dataset (Weng et al., 2010, 2011; Wiggins et al., 2011, 2012).

Inline Supplementary Table S1.

Table S1.

Autism Spectrum Disorders Group


Control Group



Low Expressing Genotypes
Higher Expressing Genotypes


Low Expressing Genotypes
Higher Expressing Genotypes


S/S S/LG LG/LG LA/LA S/LA LA/LG S/S S/LG LG/LG LA/LA S/LA LA/LG
Number of participants 10 5 1 12 25 1 18 3 1 21 22 1
Total N 16 38 22 44
χ2 p
Gender (F:M) 1:15 7:31 6:16 11:33 3.76 (df = 3) 0.29
Handedness (L:R:ambidextrous) 4:11:00 5:28:01 4:18:00 3:38:00 6.17 (df = 6) 0.41
Caucasian 94% 90% 64% 82%
F(1,116)* p
Age 13.5 (2.78) 13.9 (2.72) 14.8 (2.35) 14.1 (3.26) 1.11 0.295
Verbal CF 115 (25.6) 111 (18.8) 113 (13.1) 115 (13.7) 0.81 0.370
Nonverbal CF 113 (17.6) 101 (21.9) 103 (11.6) 101 (13.0) 2.756 (df = 1,112) 0.100
t(52)** p t(64)*** p
SRS 74.5 (12.2) 78.0 (11.1) 1.01 (df = 51) 0.318 43.6 (7.68) 42.5 (6.37) .590 (df = 63) 0.557
SCQ 19.4 (7.67) 21.73 (6.85) 1.05 (df = 46) 0.299 2.95 (2.84) 3.36 (4.23) .400 (df = 59) 0.691
MASC 42.3 (21.5) 45.2 (16.8) .509 (df = 47) 0.613 34.8 (14.1) 31.4 (15.2) .847 (df = 60) 0.400
CDI 7.13 (4.26) 8.47 (6.38) .748 (df = 51) 0.458 5.86 (4.00) 4.47 (5.27) 1.094 (df = 63) 0.278
CBCL Internal 60.0 (18.0) 56.4 (21.3) 0.537 0.593 45.0 (9.43) 45.2 (10.7) 0.09 0.926
CBCL External 49.0 (15.6) 51.7 (20.9) 0.457 0.650 44.4 (7.33) 41.7 (10.4) 1.09 0.279
CBCL Total 57.4 (17.1) 58.1 (22.0) 0.110 0.913 44.2 (8.81) 42.4 (10.48) 0.67 0.504
OCI-R 18.0 (13.8) 18.9 (13.3) .225 (df = 47) 0.823 10.1 (6.81) 10.1 (9.10) 0.004 (df = 59) 0.997

Note: Some participants were missing data. This is noted with altered df.

*df = 1,116 unless otherwise specified.

**df = 52 unless otherwise specified.

***df = 64 unless otherwise specified.

CF = cognitive functioning.

SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale.

SCQ = Social Communication Questionnaire – Lifetime.

MASC = Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children.

CDI = Children’s Depression Inventory.

CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist.

OCI-R = Obsessive Compulsive Inventory – Revised;

Likelihood ratio test used for chi-square analyses.

Inline Supplementary Table S1
mmc1.xlsx (46.8KB, xlsx)

Controls were recruited through flyers posted at community organizations in the Ann Arbor, Michigan area. The University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center (UMACC) referred potential participants to our study and diagnosed participants with an ASD (autistic disorder, Asperger's syndrome, or pervasive developmental disorder — not otherwise specified) using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000), the Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised (ADI-R; Lord et al., 1994), and clinical consensus (Lord et al., 2006). The University of Michigan Institutional Review Board approved the procedures. Participants over age 18 gave written informed consent; participants under age 18 gave written assent and their parents gave written informed consent. Cognitive functioning was evaluated for controls with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT; Dunn and Dunn, 1997) and the Ravens Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1960); participants with ASD were given these measures or the Differential Ability Scales II — School Age (Elliott, 2005), the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales (Roid, 2003), the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children IV (Wechsler, 2003), or the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999). Participants with orthodontic braces, medical conditions contraindicated for MRI, or history of seizures or neurological disorders were excluded. Control participants were screened for psychological disorders with the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach and Edelbrock, 1981), Social Responsiveness Scale (Constantino et al., 2003), Social Communication Questionnaire (Rutter et al., 2003), Obsessive Compulsive Inventory — Revised (Foa et al., 2010), Child Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1992), and Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (March et al., 1997). All control participants scored below clinical cutoffs for affected status. Individuals with the low and high expressing genotypes did not differ in any of the symptom measures or cognitive functioning in either the ASD or control group (Inline Supplementary Table S1). Prior studies utilized portions of this dataset (Weng et al., 2010, 2011; Wiggins et al., 2011, 2012).

Inline Supplementary Table S1 can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.008.

2.2. Genetic analyses

5-HTTLPR genotype was ascertained using previously published procedures (Wiggins et al., 2012). Participants donated saliva samples using the Oragene DNA kit (DNA Genotek; Kanata, Canada). PCR and agarose genotyping were used to determine S versus L allele. Sanger sequencing was utilized to determine the A to G single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the L allele (rs25531; Hu et al., 2006) and to confirm PCR genotyping.

In autism, individuals with the low expressing genotype (S/S) have been shown to differ in neurochemical metabolism compared to L allele carriers in the anterior portion of the default network (Endo et al., 2010). As such, participants were put into two genotype groups: low expressing genotypes (S/S, S/LG, LG/LG) versus medium and high expressing genotypes (LA/LA, S/LA, LA/LG, hereafter referred to as "high expressing" genotypes). (The LG allele is equivalent to the S allele in serotonin transporter expression level (Hu et al., 2006), so for the purposes of the analyses, the two alleles were grouped together.) This genotype grouping is consistent with a number of non-ASD studies that found recessive effects of the low expressing 5-HTTLPR alleles, often in adolescent populations (e.g., Cicchetti et al., 2007; Surguladze et al., 2008; Benjet et al., 2010). Nevertheless, we conducted additional analyses to examine whether our results still stood when the alleles were grouped differently (see Section 3.1.4 Alternative genotype groupings).

Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium was tested based on the insertion/deletion polymorphism. Genotype frequencies were in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium for the ASD group (χ2 = 0.742, df = 1, p = 0.389), but there was a trend toward disequilibrium for the control group (χ2 = 3.74, df = 1, p = 0.053). When including only Caucasians for the control group, the trend disappeared (χ2 = 0.654, df = 1, p = 0.419). Because of this, additional analyses were performed to address the potential effects of multiple ancestries within the sample.

2.3. fMRI data acquisition

T2*-weighted blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) images were acquired during a resting state scan, in which participants were instructed not to think of anything in particular and to let their minds wander while looking at a fixation cross. Over the 10-minute resting state scan, 300 images were acquired (Glover and Law, 2001; TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 22 cm, 64 × 64 matrix, 40 contiguous axial 3 mm slices). Slices were acquired parallel to the intercommissural (AC–PC) line. High-resolution 3D T1 axial overlay (TR = 8.9, TE = 1.8, flip angle = 15°, FOV = 26 cm, slice thickness = 1.4 mm, 124 slices; matrix = 256 × 160) and spoiled gradient (SPGR; flip angle = 15°, FOV = 26 cm, 1.4 mm slice thickness, 110 slices) anatomical images were also collected. Participants wore a pulse oximeter and abdominal pressure belt to record cardiac and respiratory rhythms, synchronized to the fMRI data, for subsequent physiological artifact correction. Further details on the acquisition parameters have been previously published (Weng et al., 2011; Wiggins et al., 2012). Prior to the MRI scan, participants practiced in a mock scanner to acclimate to the scanning environment.

2.4. fMRI data analysis

2.4.1. Data preprocessing

The standard pre-processing procedure from the University of Michigan Functional MRI Center was applied to the fMRI data. This procedure includes removing outliers from the raw k-space data, reconstructing the k-space data to image space, applying a field map correction to reduce artifacts from susceptibility regions, and correcting for slice timing. RETROICOR was utilized to remove noise associated with cardiac and respiratory rhythms (Glover et al., 2000). To address potential effects of head motion, functional images were realigned to the 10th image. Details on these steps are available in multiple papers utilizing this pre-processing stream (e.g., Weng et al., 2011; Wiggins et al., 2012). The high-resolution T1 anatomical images were then co-registered to the functional images using the SPM5 Matlab toolbox (Wellcome Department of Neurology, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk). After removing variance associated with head motion (see Section 2.4.2 Addressing head motion), functional images were smoothed with an isotropic 8 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel using SPM5. A low-pass filter of .08 Hz was applied as well to isolate the frequency band where default network activation has been found.

2.4.2. Addressing head motion

Recent papers have emphasized the importance of addressing head motion, which can introduce spurious correlations in connectivity analyses (Power et al., 2012; Satterthwaite et al., 2012; Van Dijk et al., 2012). In addition to the standard realignment of images to correct for head motion, we took several steps to address the potential effects of head motion on our results. First, we excluded participants whose head motion exceeded 2.5 mm in the x, y, or z direction or 2.5° in the roll, pitch, or yaw directions.

Second, we removed variance associated with head motion by creating nuisance regressors from motion estimated in the x, y, z, roll, pitch, and yaw directions and retaining the residuals for processing.

Third, we calculated the mean motion for each person (i.e., mean absolute displacement of each volume as compared to the previous volume, calculated as square root ((xi + 1 − xi)2 + (yi + 1 − yi)2 + (zi + 1 − zi)2) for i = 1, …, 300 images), as in Van Dijk et al (2012). We then conducted a 2-way ANOVA with post-hoc contrasts to examine whether mean motion differed between the ASD and control groups and by genotype. We also performed a 3-way interaction analysis to examine whether the interaction of genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age significantly predicted mean motion.

Fourth, even though groups may not differ on overall mean motion, it is possible that the different motion distributions within groups could influence findings. Because of this, we repeated the analyses for our main hypotheses with a subsample of our participants matched on mean motion to examine whether the results persisted when the groups' motion distributions were equivalent (see Section 3.1.1 Matched head motion distributions). Several other studies have utilized matching to reduce the likelihood that the connectivity patterns they observed were an artifact of head motion (Fair et al., 2007, 2008; Dosenbach et al., 2010). Our matching procedure was as follows: first, we split participants into four groups: individuals with the low expressing genotypes (ASD and control) and individuals with the high expressing genotypes (ASD and control). Within each group, we binned participants by mean motion into .001 mm bins. Participants were removed randomly until the number of participants in each corresponding bin for the ASD and control groups were the same within both the low and high expressing genotypes.

2.4.3. Connectivity images

A self-organizing map algorithm was applied to the images to derive a data-driven reference from the posterior hub of the default network to calculate connectivity for each individual, as described in previous publications (Peltier et al., 2003; Wiggins et al., 2011, 2012). An example of a posterior hub identified for an individual using the self-organizing map algorithm is shown in Inline Supplementary Fig. S1. The advantage of using this data-driven method is that, unlike traditional a priori seed analyses, seed placement is not based on data from adult control brains. When using the self-organizing map algorithm, the default network reference, which is correlated with every other voxel in the brain to calculate connectivity, is not biased toward the control group but rather tailored for each individual.

Inline Supplementary Fig. S1.
Fig. S1.

Fig. S1

An example of the default network posterior hub identified through the self-organizing map algorithm. Following procedures described in Wiggins et al. (2011), the self-organizing map algorithm, a data-driven method, was applied to the data to organize voxels into networks. An experienced investigator blind to condition identified the network that contained the posterior hub (posterior cingulate and angular gyri/inferior parietal lobules) of the default network for each individual. The posterior hub was then used as an individualized reference to calculate default network connectivity for each participant. An example of the posterior hub from one individual is shown here. Data are from a single 64 × 64 slice in the transverse plane. White indicates that the voxel is a member of the posterior hub; gray indicates that the voxel does not belong in the posterior hub. The brain is masked for illustration purposes to highlight the posterior hub. At this point in the data-processing stream, brains are not yet normalized.

A self-organizing map algorithm was applied to the images to derive a data-driven reference from the posterior hub of the default network to calculate connectivity for each individual, as described in previous publications (Peltier et al., 2003; Wiggins et al., 2011, 2012). An example of a posterior hub identified for an individual using the self-organizing map algorithm is shown in Inline Supplementary Fig. S1. The advantage of using this data-driven method is that, unlike traditional a priori seed analyses, seed placement is not based on data from adult control brains. When using the self-organizing map algorithm, the default network reference, which is correlated with every other voxel in the brain to calculate connectivity, is not biased toward the control group but rather tailored for each individual.

Inline Supplementary Fig. S1 can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.008.

The connectivity images generated by this method were normalized to Montreal Neurological Image (MNI) space by estimating the transformation matrix for the SPGR image to SPM's template MNI image, and applying that transformation to the connectivity images. The end product is a normalized image for each subject that indicates, with a Z value at each voxel, how highly functionally connected (correlated) that voxel is to the posterior hub of the default network identified by the self-organizing map algorithm.

2.4.4. Group-level analyses

The connectivity images were then entered into second-level analyses in SPM8 to test hypotheses at a group level. As a preliminary step, we first examined whether the ASD group and the control group exhibited default network connectivity by applying small volume corrections using masks covering the default network (posterior cingulate, precuneus, angular gyri, inferior parietal lobules, parahippocampal gyri, superior frontal gyri, anterior cingulate, BA 32, BA 10), from the Wake Forest Pickatlas (Maldjian et al., 2002). Also as a preliminary step, we compared the ASD and control groups on long-range default network connectivity, using the anterior masks in small volume corrections (Inline Supplementary Table S2).

Inline Supplementary Table S2.
Table S2.

Default network connectivity for ASD and control groups. Functional connectivity in the (A) Control group, (B) ASD group, (C) ASD > Control group, (D) Controls > ASD group. The threshold was set at p < 0.05 uncorrected with the number of contiguous voxels set at k ≥ 10. L = left, R = right. A full list of the default network structures used can be found in the Methods.

A). Control group
Region Brodmann's Area Cluster size t
df = 65
MNI Coordinates
x y z
L posterior cingulate 23 808 14.75 − 6 − 52 24
R posterior cingulate 10 853 15.22 4 − 52 24
L precuneus 31 2062 15.55 − 6 − 50 30
39 229 10.89 − 44 − 74 38
R precuneus 31 2291 16.42 4 − 52 32
39 206 11.11 42 − 68 34
L angular gyrus 39 390 12.20 − 50 − 64 34
L inferior parietal lobule 39 735 11.88 − 46 − 68 38
40 74 6.42 − 48 − 50 24
R inferior parietal lobule 40 587 12.66 50 − 62 38
13 82 8.15 46 − 50 24
L parahippocampal gyrus 901 5.03 − 28 − 34 − 10
R parahippocampal gyrus 30 776 4.26 10 − 46 2
L superior frontal gyrus 10 3432 9.70 − 8 58 − 8
R superior frontal gyrus 8 3159 10.82 18 30 48
L anterior cingulate 10 810 9.09 − 2 58 2
R anterior cingulate 10 981 9.75 4 58 2
L prefrontal cortex 10 1006 10.07 − 4 56 − 8
32 311 7.80 − 2 50 0
R prefrontal cortex 10 905 10.80 8 66 8
32 307 8.11 6 40 − 10



(B). ASD group
Region Brodmann's Area Cluster size t
df = 53
MNI Coordinates
x y z
L posterior cingulate 23 817 11.14 − 2 − 44 24
R posterior cingulate 23 818 12.10 4 − 44 24
L precuneus 31 1978 11.34 − 2 − 50 30
19 203 7.89 − 44 − 74 40
R precuneus 31 2335 13.74 12 − 48 30
39 233 7.00 46 − 76 34
L angular gyrus 39 381 9.13 − 46 − 68 30
R angular gyrus 39 399 9.15 48 − 74 34
L inferior parietal lobule 39 1090 8.62 − 42 − 64 38
R inferior parietal lobule 39 649 7.61 44 − 72 38
39 294 6.43 46 − 50 22
L parahippocampal gyrus 39 54 3.27 − 10 − 48 2
12 2.44 − 24 − 12 − 14
30 34 2.02 − 14 − 34 − 6
R parahippocampal gyrus 35 379 3.73 22 − 28 − 14
L superior frontal gyrus 10 3307 7.50 − 12 66 18
R superior frontal gyrus 10 2576 6.90 8 64 24
L anterior cingulate 11 682 6.07 − 2 42 − 10
R anterior cingulate 11 719 6.44 2 42 − 10
L prefrontal cortex 10 1044 7.56 − 10 66 18
32 260 5.79 − 2 40 − 10
R prefrontal cortex 10 849 6.90 8 64 24
32 248 6.08 2 46 − 4



(C). Controls > ASD group
Region Brodmann's Area Cluster size t
df = 118
MNI Coordinates
x y z
L posterior cingulate 31 331 2.94 − 2 − 62 24
R posterior cingulate 31 408 3.77 8 − 58 24
L precuneus 31 903 3.75 − 2 − 70 28
19 42 2.09 − 44 − 74 38
R precuneus 31 1009 4.02 12 − 56 30
39 33 3.11 42 − 68 34
L angular gyrus 39 72 2.29 − 50 − 72 34
R angular gyrus 39 261 3.95 44 − 66 30
L inferior parietal lobule 39 51 2.17 − 46 − 72 38
R inferior parietal lobule 39 186 3.04 46 − 70 42
L parahippocampal gyrus 35 715 3.75 − 24 − 22 − 20
R parahippocampal gyrus 20 287 2.94 34 − 22 − 28
L superior frontal gyrus 11 334 3.75 − 6 58 − 24
6 72 2.63 − 16 24 56
10 30 2.46 − 2 62 2
9 92 2.43 − 4 52 28
R superior frontal gyrus 11 505 4.21 6 58 − 24
8 1040 3.36 22 36 50
L anterior cingulate 25 63 2.88 − 2 12 − 10
R anterior cingulate 25 76 2.98 4 8 − 12
10 97 2.60 4 58 2
L prefrontal cortex 10 132 2.56 − 8 60 − 8
R prefrontal cortex 10 375 3.66 12 66 − 4
32 13 1.98 14 46 − 4
32 16 1.97 8 46 4



(C). ASD group>Controls
Region Brodmann's Area Cluster size t
df = 118
MNI Coordinates
x y z
L posterior cingulate 30 44 2.73 − 24 − 70 6
L precuneus 7 11 1.98 − 28 − 56 54
R precuneus 7 331 3.50 14 − 58 60
L inferior parietal lobule 40 1258 3.41 − 60 − 38 28
R inferior parietal lobule 40 1412 3.81 64 − 46 22
L superior frontal gyrus 6 520 3.58 − 4 10 54
9 94 2.74 − 38 44 36
10 32 2.64 − 38 58 18
R superior frontal gyrus 6 217 3.15 2 10 56
6 164 2.62 20 − 4 74
10 19 2.33 38 58 18
6 12 2.06 24 4 58
L anterior cingulate 32 15 1.91 − 10 26 28
L prefrontal cortex 10 136 3.48 − 46 50 14
10 43 3.36 − 44 50 8
32 264 3.61 − 12 10 40
R prefrontal cortex 10 53 2.96 48 50 4
10 26 2.51 38 58 16
32 199 2.90 12 6 42
Inline Supplementary Table S2
mmc2.pdf (91.6KB, pdf)

The connectivity images were then entered into second-level analyses in SPM8 to test hypotheses at a group level. As a preliminary step, we first examined whether the ASD group and the control group exhibited default network connectivity by applying small volume corrections using masks covering the default network (posterior cingulate, precuneus, angular gyri, inferior parietal lobules, parahippocampal gyri, superior frontal gyri, anterior cingulate, BA 32, BA 10), from the Wake Forest Pickatlas (Maldjian et al., 2002). Also as a preliminary step, we compared the ASD and control groups on long-range default network connectivity, using the anterior masks in small volume corrections (Inline Supplementary Table S2).

Inline Supplementary Table S2 can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.008.

To address our first hypothesis, a voxel-wise multiple regression was created to examine the interaction of genotype (low expressing (S/S, S/LG, LG/LG) versus high expressing (LA/LA, S/LA, LA/LG)) by diagnosis (ASD versus control group). For this model, three regressors were entered – genotype, diagnosis, and the interaction of genotype-by-diagnosis – predicting connectivity with the posterior hub. To determine whether the beta for the interaction was significant in the anterior default network, a small volume correction was performed on the image mapping the betas of the interaction using a mask of the bilateral BA 10, where alterations in long-range default network connectivity have consistently been found in ASD samples (e.g., Monk et al., 2009; Weng et al., 2010; Wiggins et al., 2011), as well as where effects of 5-HTTLPR have been found (Wiggins et al., 2012). The small volume correction takes into account the geometric qualities of the mask when doing a correction for multiple comparisons based on the number of resels (a measure related to the number of independent observations) within the mask (Worsley et al., 1996). Significance thresholds within BA 10 were corrected for multiple comparisons using family wise error (FWE) correction (Worsley et al., 1996). Post-hoc comparisons were also performed in SPSS, comparing each subgroup pair on connectivity values extracted and averaged from a 4 mm sphere around the peak of the interaction, with a Bonferroni-corrected α level of 0.05/6 = 0.0083.

To address our second hypothesis, we created a model to examine the three-way interaction among genotype (low versus high expressing), diagnosis (ASD versus control group), and age. In this model, the three-way interaction term was entered, as well as all lower order terms. A small volume correction applied the same mask as in the first hypothesis, bilateral BA 10, to the image mapping the betas for the 3-way interaction to examine whether the three-way interaction significantly predicted connectivity with the posterior hub in the anterior default network. Post-hoc analyses were also performed to further characterize the interaction. Connectivity values from a 4 mm sphere around the peak of the 3-way interaction were extracted and averaged, then exported to SPSS. In SPSS, the simple slopes (changes in connectivity for every unit increase in age) for four subgroups (controls with low expressing genotypes, controls with high expressing genotypes, individuals with ASD and low expressing genotypes, individuals with ASD and high expressing genotypes) were tested against zero.

3. Results

Individuals with the low and high expressing genotypes within the control and the ASD groups did not differ on any of the symptom measures (Inline Supplementary Table S1). Both the ASD group and the control group exhibited default network connectivity, and previous findings of weaker posterior–anterior default network connectivity in the ASD group were replicated (Inline Supplementary Table S2; Cherkassky et al., 2006; Kennedy and Courchesne, 2008; Monk et al., 2009; Weng et al., 2010; Wiggins et al., 2011).

Individuals with the low and high expressing genotypes within the control and the ASD groups did not differ on any of the symptom measures (Inline Supplementary Table S1). Both the ASD group and the control group exhibited default network connectivity, and previous findings of weaker posterior–anterior default network connectivity in the ASD group were replicated ( Inline Supplementary Table S2; Cherkassky et al., 2006; Kennedy and Courchesne, 2008; Monk et al., 2009; Weng et al., 2010; Wiggins et al., 2011).

The four groups (individuals with ASD and low expressing genotypes, individuals with ASD and high expressing genotypes, controls with low expressing genotypes, controls with high expressing genotypes) did not differ in mean head motion (genotype-by-diagnosis: F1,116 = 0.040, p = .841). Additionally, age did not relate to head motion differently across the four groups (genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age: β = .058, t112 = 0.256, p = 0.799).

The first hypothesis, that the relationship between 5-HTTLPR genotype and posterior–anterior default network connectivity strength differs in the ASD group versus controls, was confirmed. There was a significant genotype-by-diagnosis interaction predicting degree of connectivity between the posterior hub and the anterior default network in the left hemisphere (xyz = − 34, 62, 0, t116 = 4.24, p = 0.021, corrected for multiple comparisons within bilateral BA 10; Fig. 1). Specifically, 5-HTTLPR genotype influences posterior–anterior connectivity strength differently for individuals with ASD versus controls. Two pair-wise comparisons survived a Bonferroni correction, indicating that individuals with low expressing genotypes within the ASD group had significantly stronger connectivity than individuals with ASD and high expressing genotypes (p = 0.001) as well as controls with low expressing genotypes (p = 0.003). The genotype-by-diagnosis interaction was also significant in the right anterior default network (xyz = 44, 56, − 6, t116 = 4.17, p = 0.027, corrected for multiple comparisons within bilateral BA 10; Inline Supplementary Fig. S2).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Impact of 5-HTTLPR genotypes on posterior–anterior default network connectivity is different in youth with ASD compared to controls. Voxels in color indicate places where connectivity between that area and the posterior default network is differentially influenced by 5-HTTLPR in the ASD group versus controls. A significant genotype-by-diagnosis interaction in the anterior default network (xyz = − 34, 62, 0, t116 = 4.24, p = 0.021, corrected for multiple comparisons within bilateral BA 10) is depicted in the transverse section of the brain (upper). For this and the subsequent brain image, the threshold was set at p < 0.01 for illustration purposes. To show the interaction, contrast values from a 4 mm sphere around the peak voxel (xyz = − 34, 62, 0) were extracted and plotted (lower). In the bar graph, controls show the pattern found in previous research (Wiggins et al., 2012), but youth with ASD show a different pattern. Brackets with asterisks indicate significant differences at a Bonferroni-corrected α-level of 0.05/6 = 0.0083.

Inline Supplementary Fig. S2.

Fig. S2.

Fig. S2

Right hemisphere — Impact of 5-HTTLPR on posterior–anterior default network connectivity differs in youth with ASD compared to controls. Voxels in color indicate places where genotype influenced connectivity between that area and the posterior hub differently for the ASD group and the control group. A significant genotype-by-diagnosis interaction in the anterior default network (xyz = 44, 56, − 6, t116 = 4.17, p = 0.027, corrected for multiple comparisons within bilateral BA 10) is depicted in the sagittal section of the brain (upper), with the threshold set at p < 0.01 for illustration purposes. To depict the interaction, contrast values from a 4 mm sphere around the peak voxel (xyz = 44, 56, − 6) were extracted and plotted (lower).

The first hypothesis, that the relationship between 5-HTTLPR genotype and posterior–anterior default network connectivity strength differs in the ASD group versus controls, was confirmed. There was a significant genotype-by-diagnosis interaction predicting degree of connectivity between the posterior hub and the anterior default network in the left hemisphere (xyz = − 34, 62, 0, t116 = 4.24, p = 0.021, corrected for multiple comparisons within bilateral BA 10; Fig. 1). Specifically, 5-HTTLPR genotype influences posterior–anterior connectivity strength differently for individuals with ASD versus controls. Two pair-wise comparisons survived a Bonferroni correction, indicating that individuals with low expressing genotypes within the ASD group had significantly stronger connectivity than individuals with ASD and high expressing genotypes (p = 0.001) as well as controls with low expressing genotypes (p = 0.003). The genotype-by-diagnosis interaction was also significant in the right anterior default network (xyz = 44, 56, − 6, t116 = 4.17, p = 0.027, corrected for multiple comparisons within bilateral BA 10; Inline Supplementary Fig. S2).

Inline Supplementary Fig. S2 can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.008.

Inline Supplementary Fig. S2 can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.008.

Our second hypothesis, that the relationship between 5-HTTLPR and changes in posterior–anterior connectivity across childhood and adolescence differs in ASD compared to controls, was also confirmed. We found a significant genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age interaction predicting degree of connectivity between the posterior and anterior default network (xyz = − 6, 40, − 6, t112 = 4.09, p = 0.037, corrected for multiple comparisons within bilateral BA 10; Fig. 2). 5-HTTLPR genotype differentially influences age-related changes in posterior–anterior connectivity strength in individuals with ASD compared to controls. Post-hoc analyses to further characterize the interaction indicated that only individuals with ASD with low expressing genotypes had significant increases in connectivity values with age (simple slope = 0.708, p = 0.002), whereas the other subgroups' relationships between connectivity and age did not significantly differ from zero: ASD group, high expressing genotype, simple slope = − .268, p = 0.104; controls, low expressing, simple slope = 0.216, p = 0.335; controls, high expressing, simple slope = − .185, p = 0.229.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

5-HTTLPR influences age-related changes in posterior–anterior default network connectivity differently in youth with ASD compared to controls. Voxels in color indicate places where connectivity between that area and the posterior hub changes across age differently for the ASD group and the control group. A significant genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age interaction in the anterior default network (xyz = − 6, 40, − 6, t112 = 4.09, p = 0.037, corrected for multiple comparisons within bilateral BA 10) is depicted in the transverse section of the brain (upper). To illustrate connectivity levels in each individual, contrast values from a 4 mm sphere around the peak voxel (xyz = − 6, 40, − 6) were extracted and plotted (lower).

3.1. Additional analyses

In imaging and genetic studies with disordered populations, head motion, population stratification, psychotropic medication status, allele grouping, and degree of smoothing are potential factors influencing associations. Because of this, additional analyses were performed to determine whether these factors account for our results. As these additional analyses required a reduced sample size and/or more complex models, thereby diminishing the power to detect effects, we utilized a threshold of p < 0.05 without family-wise error correction. To summarize, the hypotheses were still confirmed even when taking into account each of these factors.

3.1.1. Matched head motion distributions

Our matching procedure is described in Section 2.4.2 Addressing head motion. In total, 24 participants (20%) were removed in order to match the groups' motion distributions. (See Inline Supplementary Fig. S3 for a visual representation of the participants removed from each bin.) After removing participants until the group distributions were matched on mean head motion, our first hypothesis was still confirmed. The interaction of genotype-by-diagnosis significantly predicted connectivity in bilateral BA 10 (left: xyz = − 34, 62, 0, t92 = 4.22, p = 0.000029; right: xyz = 38, 60, 2, t92 = 3.84, p = 0.00011) with a subsample of participants matched on head motion. Our second hypothesis was confirmed as well with the matched subsample, as a genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age interaction significantly predicted connectivity in BA 10 (xyz = − 10, 36, − 6, t88 = 4.22, p = 0.000029).

Inline Supplementary Fig. S3.
Fig. S3.

Fig. S3

Matching participants on mean motion. Participants were randomly removed from .001 mm bins until the same number of participants remained in corresponding bins across the ASD and control groups for both (A) high expressing and (B) low expressing genotypes. Bar graph shows proportion removed in each bin.

Our matching procedure is described in Section 2.4.2 Addressing head motion. In total, 24 participants (20%) were removed in order to match the groups' motion distributions. (See Inline Supplementary Fig. S3 for a visual representation of the participants removed from each bin.) After removing participants until the group distributions were matched on mean head motion, our first hypothesis was still confirmed. The interaction of genotype-by-diagnosis significantly predicted connectivity in bilateral BA 10 (left: xyz = − 34, 62, 0, t92 = 4.22, p = 0.000029; right: xyz = 38, 60, 2, t92 = 3.84, p = 0.00011) with a subsample of participants matched on head motion. Our second hypothesis was confirmed as well with the matched subsample, as a genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age interaction significantly predicted connectivity in BA 10 (xyz = − 10, 36, − 6, t88 = 4.22, p = 0.000029).

Inline Supplementary Fig. S3 can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.008.

Inline Supplementary Fig. S3 can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.008.

3.1.2. Population stratification

To determine whether the findings were primarily driven by differing ancestries within our sample, 5 non-Caucasian individuals with ASD and sixteen non-Caucasian controls were excluded and the group-level analyses addressing our hypotheses were repeated. In line with our first hypothesis, including Caucasian participants only, the genotype-by-diagnosis interaction predicting connectivity strength was significant in both left (xyz = − 34, 62, − 2, t95 = 3.95, p = 0.000075) and right (xyz = 38, 58, 4, t95 = 3.14, p = 0.0011) BA 10. Supporting our second hypothesis, the genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age interaction predicting connectivity was significant in BA 10 when including only Caucasian participants (xyz = − 20, 50, 2, t91 = 3.51, p = 0.00035).

3.1.3. Medication effects

Next, twenty-four individuals with ASD taking psychotropic medication and 1 control taking levothyroxine (a thyroid medication) were excluded before repeating the analyses. Like the analyses only including Caucasians, findings including only non-medicated participants also mirrored the original findings with the entire dataset. Supporting the first hypothesis, with only non-medicated participants, the genotype-by-diagnosis interaction was significant (left BA 10: xyz = − 34, 62, 0, t91 = 3.71, p = 0.00018; right BA 10: xyz = 32, 60, 6, t91 = 4.55, p = 0.0000084). The second hypothesis, genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age interaction predicting connectivity, also held with non-medicated participants only in BA 10 (xyz = − 6, 40, − 6, t87 = 2.97, p = 0.0019).

3.1.4. Alternative genotype groupings

Moreover, additional analyses were conducted with alternate genotype groupings to examine whether the findings persisted when participants split into different genotype groups. First, we ran the analyses with the genotype in three groups based on expressing level: low expressing (S/S, S/LG, LG/LG) versus medium expressing (S/LA, LA/LG) vs high expressing (LA/LA) genotypes. Consistent with the first hypothesis, there was a significant genotype-by-diagnosis interaction in both the left (xyz = − 34, 62, 0, F2,114 = 9.22, p = 0.00019) and right (xyz = 44, 56, − 6, F2,114 = 9.19, p = 0.00020) anterior default network. The three-way genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age interaction was also significant with this genotype grouping in the anterior default network (xyz = − 6, 40, − 8, F2,108 = 11.23, p = 0.000037), consistent with the second hypothesis.

Second, we examined the hypotheses with the genotype grouping S/S versus heterozygotes (S/LA and S/LG) versus LA/LA. With this alternative genotype grouping, there was a significant genotype-by-diagnosis interaction in the left (xyz = − 34, 62, − 2, F2,114 = 7.68, p = 0.00074) and right (xyz = 44, 56, − 6, F2,114 = 10.45, p = 0.000068) anterior default network. Moreover, consistent with the second hypothesis, the three-way interaction was significant in the anterior default network (xyz = − 6, 40, − 8, F2,108 = 8.11, p = 0.00052) with this alternative grouping. To summarize, the original result pattern persisted even when genotypes were grouped two alternate ways in the statistical analyses.

3.1.5. 5 mm smoothing kernel

The degree of smoothing can also affect results. We re-did the analyses with a 5 mm (instead of 8 mm) FWHM Gaussian kernel for spatial smoothing of the functional images. Consistent with the first hypothesis, the genotype-by-diagnosis interaction was significant in the left (xyz = − 48, 54, 6, t116 = 2.25, p = 0.013) and right (xyz = 42, 58, 10, t116 = 3.96, p = 0.00065) BA 10. Moreover, consistent with the second hypothesis, the three-way interaction was significant in BA 10 (xyz = 42, 48, 24, t112 = 3.03, p = 0.002) with the 5 mm smoothing kernel, albeit in a more lateral location within BA 10.

4. Discussion

In results confirming both the first hypothesis (genotype-by-diagnosis) and the second hypothesis (genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age), individuals with ASD and low expressing genotypes stood out among the other subgroups, exhibiting the greatest connectivity as well as the sharpest increase in connectivity values with age. This overall pattern suggests that individuals with low expressing genotypes may represent a subtype of ASD, which is consistent with previous research linking 5-HTTLPR to symptom subtypes rather than a global ASD diagnosis in a larger sample (Tordjman et al., 2001; Brune et al., 2006). Linking genotype to brain phenotypes may be a more sensitive way to identify subtypes of ASD than linking genotype to behavior, as individuals with ASD and the low and high expressing genotypes did not differ on any of the symptom measures (Inline Supplementary Table S2). Future research could examine other aspects of this potential subtype of ASD, including responsiveness to specific interventions and long-term prognosis.

In results confirming both the first hypothesis (genotype-by-diagnosis) and the second hypothesis (genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age), individuals with ASD and low expressing genotypes stood out among the other subgroups, exhibiting the greatest connectivity as well as the sharpest increase in connectivity values with age. This overall pattern suggests that individuals with low expressing genotypes may represent a subtype of ASD, which is consistent with previous research linking 5-HTTLPR to symptom subtypes rather than a global ASD diagnosis in a larger sample (Tordjman et al., 2001; Brune et al., 2006). Linking genotype to brain phenotypes may be a more sensitive way to identify subtypes of ASD than linking genotype to behavior, as individuals with ASD and the low and high expressing genotypes did not differ on any of the symptom measures (Inline Supplementary Table S2). Future research could examine other aspects of this potential subtype of ASD, including responsiveness to specific interventions and long-term prognosis.

There are two main possibilities to explain why 5-HTTLPR influences the ASD group and control group differently. First, a gene-by-gene interaction may account for our results. The specifics of the complex genetic etiology of ASD are a subject of intense inquiry. Nonetheless, as ASD is highly heritable (Miles, 2011), individuals with ASD may carry a systematically different overall genetic profile than controls. Causative gene products may interact with 5-HTTLPR, leading to alterations in expression levels that then produce a different brain phenotype. Future research probing this possibility may include examining other autism genes and their involvement in serotonin metabolism.

Alternatively, a gene-by-environment interaction may explain our findings. As 5-HTTLPR is sensitive to environmental input (Belsky et al., 2009), it may be that 5-HTTLPR affects brain function in individuals with ASD differently than controls because individuals with ASD experience an altered social environment brought about by the reactions of others to their symptoms. Particularly during adolescence, an important social development period in which relationships with peers become more important (Youniss and Haynie, 1992), individuals with ASD may miss out on social opportunities with peers and thus find themselves in an environment with reduced social stimuli. This environment could affect epigenetically-sensitive serotonin transporter expression and subsequently, brain function. Future studies incorporating comprehensive environmental measures and focusing on molecular mechanisms of altered serotonin transporter expression, such as methylation, will be necessary to probe this possibility.

This study has several limitations, as some confounds make imaging and genetics research, particularly with pediatric clinical populations, more challenging. First, in our study, mean head motion did not differ among the groups we compared: individuals with ASD and low expressing genotypes, individuals with ASD and high expressing genotypes, controls with low expressing genotypes, and controls with high expressing genotypes. Also, age did not relate to head motion differently across the four groups. Nevertheless, motion remains a concern in all functional connectivity studies, so we took several steps to address motion: first, only participants with movement under 2.5 mm and 2.5° in all translation and rotation directions were included; second, we realigned the functional images; third, we removed variance associated with movement in the x, y, z, roll, pitch, and yaw directions; fourth, we repeated the analyses with a subsample matched on head motion and found that our hypotheses were still confirmed even when motion distributions were the same across individuals with ASD and controls in the low and high expressing genotype groups.

Another limitation is that the cross-sectional design utilized in this study precludes inferences about developmental trajectories within individuals. Future studies may use a longitudinal design to rule out birth cohort effects. Additionally, longitudinal studies will be useful to examine whether brain differences earlier in development predict later symptom presentation and responsiveness to particular treatments.

Third, we did not exclude any racial or ethnic group when recruiting participants, which can contribute to spurious associations in genetic studies. Although it should be acknowledged that Caucasians in our sample may not all be of the same ancestry, we repeated the analyses with non-Caucasian participants removed to determine whether results were primarily due to several different ancestries within the sample. The genotype-by-diagnosis and genotype-by-diagnosis-by-age interactions predicting posterior–anterior connectivity were found even with non-Caucasians excluded from the analyses, suggesting that the results were not primarily driven by population stratification. Nevertheless, the lack of understanding of genetic effects in different racial/ethnic groups is a widespread problem in the field that must be addressed in future work.

4.1. Conclusions

This is the first study, to our knowledge, to examine the influence of 5-HTTLPR genotype on the default network in individuals with ASD. We found that the relationship between 5-HTTLPR genotype and posterior–anterior default network connectivity is different in individuals with ASD compared to controls. Specifically, consistent with previous research in controls (Wiggins et al., 2012), high expressing genotypes were associated with stronger connectivity than low expressing genotypes. However, the pattern was reversed for the ASD group: individuals with ASD and low expressing genotypes had stronger connectivity than individuals with ASD and high expressing genotypes. Also, we found that youth with ASD and low expressing genotypes had greater age-related increases in connectivity values compared to others in the ASD group with high expressing genotypes and to controls with either low or high expressing genotypes. The present findings provide evidence that the cascade of events from genetic variation to brain function is markedly different in ASD versus typically developing, healthy individuals. Moreover, the findings suggest that the impact of genotype on brain function is not static but rather develops and changes with age. Thus, understanding how 5-HTTLPR affects brain function in ASD is dependent on the developmental timeframe.

Although replication of our findings with a larger sample is necessary, the present study lays the groundwork to better understand the genetic and brain mechanisms that are involved in ASD. The present study documented a different impact of 5-HTTLPR on both default network connectivity and the development of default network connectivity in ASD compared to controls. Future studies may expand on these findings by examining the structural connections within the default network in vivo using diffusion tensor imaging. Moreover, the resting connectivity approach used in this study will be useful to examine the brain activation patterns of lower functioning individuals with ASD or very young children. These individuals are underrepresented in functional MRI studies because they are often unable to comply with the demands of a task requiring responses in the scanner. The relatively low demand of a resting fMRI acquisition, on the other hand, may allow lower functioning and younger participants to be successfully scanned. Obtaining brain data from individuals with a greater range of cognitive abilities and ages will allow researchers to gain a broader, more representative picture of ASD and the developmental trajectory of ASD earlier than mid-childhood. To conclude, the findings from our study open a path for a research program to better understand genetic influences on brain function in ASD.

Conflict of interest

C.L. receives royalties from the publisher of diagnostic instruments used on participants in this paper, Western Psychological Services. She gives all profits to charity.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by an Autism Speaks Pre-doctoral Fellowship (4773) and a Michigan Institute for Clinical and Health Research (MICHR) Pre-doctoral Fellowship (UL1RR024986) to J.L.W.; an Autism Speaks grant (2573) to C.S.M.; National Institutes of Health grants to D.M.M. (R01 NS54784, R01 DC009410), J.K.B. (K12 HD028820), S.J.P. (MH079871), C.L. (U19 HD35482, MH066496); a MICHR pilot award to D.M.M. (U024600); and an Elizabeth E. Kennedy (Children's Research) Fund Award from the Department of Pediatrics, University of Michigan, to J.K.B.

We thank Dr. Douglas Noll for methodological advice and the staff of the University of Michigan Functional MRI Center and DNA Sequencing Core for technical support. We thank Jeffrey M. Rosen for programming assistance, as well as Nicole Cook and Samantha Ashinoff for participant recruitment and data collection assistance. We are grateful to the families who participated.

Footnotes

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

References

  1. Achenbach T.M., Edelbrock C.S. Behavioral problems and competencies reported by parents of normal and disturbed children aged four through sixteen. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1981;46:1–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson J.S., Nielsen J.A., Froehlich A.L., DuBray M.B., Druzgal T.J., Cariello A.N., Cooperrider J.R., Zielinski B.A., Ravichandran C., Fletcher P.T., Alexander A.L., Bigler E.D., Lange N., Lainhart J.E. Functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging classification of autism. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2011;134:3742–3754. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Belsky J., Jonassaint C., Pluess M., Stanton M., Brummett B., Williams R. Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Molecular Psychiatry. 2009;14:746–754. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Benjet C., Thompson R.J., Gotlib I.H. 5-HTTLPR moderates the effect of relational peer victimization on depressive symptoms in adolescent girls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2010;51:173–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02149.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brune C.W., Kim S.J., Salt J., Leventhal B.L., Lord C., Cook E.H., Jr. 5-HTTLPR genotype-specific phenotype in children and adolescents with autism. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:2148–2156. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.12.2148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Buckner R.L., Carroll D.C. Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2007;11:49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buckner R.L., Andrews-Hanna J.R., Schacter D.L. The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. CDC . Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Vol. 61. 2012. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 14 sites, United States, 2008; pp. 1–19. (Surveillance Summaries). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cherkassky V.L., Kana R.K., Keller T.A., Just M.A. Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism. Neuroreport. 2006;17:1687–1690. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000239956.45448.4c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cicchetti D., Rogosch F.A., Sturge-Apple M.L. Interactions of child maltreatment and serotonin transporter and monoamine oxidase A polymorphisms: depressive symptomatology among adolescents from low socioeconomic status backgrounds. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:1161–1180. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Constantino J.N., Davis S.A., Todd R.D., Schindler M.K., Gross M.M., Brophy S.L., Metzger L.M., Shoushtari C.S., Splinter R., Reich W. Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: comparison of the Social Responsiveness Scale with the Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2003;33:427–433. doi: 10.1023/a:1025014929212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dosenbach N.U., Nardos B., Cohen A.L., Fair D.A., Power J.D., Church J.A., Nelson S.M., Wig G.S., Vogel A.C., Lessov-Schlaggar C.N., Barnes K.A., Dubis J.W., Feczko E., Coalson R.S., Pruett J.R., Jr., Barch D.M., Petersen S.E., Schlaggar B.L. Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science. 2010;329:1358–1361. doi: 10.1126/science.1194144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dunn L.M., Dunn L.M. 3rd ed. American Guidance Services; Circle Pines, MN: 1997. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. [Google Scholar]
  14. Elliott C.D. The differential ability scales. In: Flanagan D.P., Harrison P.L., editors. Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues. Guilford Press; New York: 2005. pp. 402–424. [Google Scholar]
  15. Endo T., Kitamura H., Tamura R., Egawa J., Sugai T., Fukui N., Suzuki Y., Someya T. 5-HTTLPR polymorphism influences prefrontal neurochemical metabolites in autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Research. 2010;183:170–173. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fair D.A., Dosenbach N.U., Church J.A., Cohen A.L., Brahmbhatt S., Miezin F.M., Barch D.M., Raichle M.E., Petersen S.E., Schlaggar B.L. Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104:13507–13512. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705843104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fair D.A., Cohen A.L., Dosenbach N.U., Church J.A., Miezin F.M., Barch D.M., Raichle M.E., Petersen S.E., Schlaggar B.L. The maturing architecture of the brain's default network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105:4028–4032. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800376105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Foa E.B., Coles M., Huppert J.D., Pasupuleti R.V., Franklin M.E., March J. Development and validation of a child version of the obsessive compulsive inventory. Behavior Therapy. 2010;41:121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2009.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Glover G.H., Law C.S. Spiral-in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2001;46:515–522. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Glover G.H., Li T.Q., Ress D. Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2000;44:162–167. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200007)44:1<162::aid-mrm23>3.0.co;2-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Greicius M.D., Supekar K., Menon V., Dougherty R.F. Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19:72–78. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gusnard D.A., Akbudak E., Shulman G.L., Raichle M.E. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001;98:4259–4264. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071043098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Horovitz S.G., Braun A.R., Carr W.S., Picchioni D., Balkin T.J., Fukunaga M., Duyn J.H. Decoupling of the brain's default mode network during deep sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:11376–11381. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901435106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hu X.Z., Lipsky R.H., Zhu G., Akhtar L.A., Taubman J., Greenberg B.D., Xu K., Arnold P.D., Richter M.A., Kennedy J.L., Murphy D.L., Goldman D. Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive–compulsive disorder. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2006;78:815–826. doi: 10.1086/503850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hughes J.R. Autism: the first firm finding = underconnectivity? Epilepsy & Behavior. 2007;11:20–24. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2007.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kennedy D.P., Courchesne E. The intrinsic functional organization of the brain is altered in autism. NeuroImage. 2008;39:1877–1885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kovacs M. Multi-Health Systems; North Tonawanda, NY: 1992. Children's Depression Inventory (CDI) Manual. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lesch K.P., Bengel D., Heils A., Sabol S.Z., Greenberg B.D., Petri S., Benjamin J., Muller C.R., Hamer D.H., Murphy D.L. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–1531. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lord C., Rutter M., Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1994;24:659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lord C., Risi S., Lambrecht L., Cook E.H., Jr., Leventhal B.L., DiLavore P.C., Pickles A., Rutter M. The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2000;30:205–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lord C., Risi S., DiLavore P.S., Shulman C., Thurm A., Pickles A. Autism from 2 to 9 years of age. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:694–701. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.6.694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Maldjian J., Laurienti P., Burdette J., Kraft R. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage. 2002;19:1233–1239. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00169-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. March J.S., Parker J.D., Sullivan K., Stallings P., Conners C.K. The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): factor structure, reliability, and validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:554–565. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Miles J.H. Autism spectrum disorders—a genetics review. Genetics in Medicine: Official Journal of the American College of Medical Genetics. 2011;13:278–294. doi: 10.1097/GIM.0b013e3181ff67ba. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Monk C.S., Peltier S.J., Wiggins J.L., Weng S.J., Carrasco M., Risi S., Lord C. Abnormalities of intrinsic functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage. 2009;47:764–772. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nakamura K., Sekine Y., Ouchi Y., Tsujii M., Yoshikawa E., Futatsubashi M., Tsuchiya K.J., Sugihara G., Iwata Y., Suzuki K., Matsuzaki H., Suda S., Sugiyama T., Takei N., Mori N. Brain serotonin and dopamine transporter bindings in adults with high-functioning autism. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:59–68. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Peltier S.J., Polk T.A., Noll D.C. Detecting low-frequency functional connectivity in fMRI using a self-organizing map (SOM) algorithm. Human Brain Mapping. 2003;20:220–226. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Power J.D., Barnes K.A., Snyder A.Z., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Raichle M.E., Snyder A.Z. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. NeuroImage. 2007;37:1083–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041. (discussion 1097-1089) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Raven J.C. Lewis; London, UK: 1960. Guide to Using the Standard Progressive Matrices. [Google Scholar]
  41. Roid G.H. Riverside Publishing; Itasca, IL: 2003. Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales, 5th Edition: Examiner's Manual. [Google Scholar]
  42. Rutter M., Bailey A., Berument S., Le Couteur A., Lord C., Pickles A. Western Psychological Services; Los Angeles, Calif: 2003. Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) [Google Scholar]
  43. Satterthwaite T.D., Wolf D.H., Loughead J., Ruparel K., Elliott M.A., Hakonarson H., Gur R.C., Gur R.E. Impact of in-scanner head motion on multiple measures of functional connectivity: relevance for studies of neurodevelopment in youth. NeuroImage. 2012;60:623–632. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Stevens M.C., Pearlson G.D., Calhoun V.D. Changes in the interaction of resting-state neural networks from adolescence to adulthood. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:2356–2366. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Supekar K., Uddin L.Q., Prater K., Amin H., Greicius M.D., Menon V. Development of functional and structural connectivity within the default mode network in young children. NeuroImage. 2010;52(1):290–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Surguladze S.A., Elkin A., Ecker C., Kalidindi S., Corsico A., Giampietro V., Lawrence N., Deeley Q., Murphy D.G., Kucharska-Pietura K., Russell T.A., McGuffin P., Murray R., Phillips M.L. Genetic variation in the serotonin transporter modulates neural system-wide response to fearful faces. Genes, Brain, and Behavior. 2008;7:543–551. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00390.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tordjman S., Gutknecht L., Carlier M., Spitz E., Antoine C., Slama F., Carsalade V., Cohen D.J., Ferrari P., Roubertoux P.L., Anderson G.M. Role of the serotonin transporter gene in the behavioral expression of autism. Molecular Psychiatry. 2001;6:434–439. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Van Dijk K.R., Sabuncu M.R., Buckner R.L. The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage. 2012;59:431–438. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wechsler D. Harcourt Assessment; San Antonio, TX: 1999. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) [Google Scholar]
  50. Wechsler D. Harcourt Assessment; San Antonio, TX: 2003. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children — 4th Edition (WISC-IV) [Google Scholar]
  51. Weng S.J., Wiggins J.L., Peltier S.J., Carrasco M., Risi S., Lord C., Monk C.S. Alterations of resting state functional connectivity in the default network in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research. 2010;1313:202–214. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Weng S.J., Carrasco M., Swartz J.R., Wiggins J.L., Kurapati N., Liberzon I., Risi S., Lord C., Monk C.S. Neural activation to emotional faces in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:296–305. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02317.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wiggins J.L., Peltier S.J., Ashinoff S., Weng S.J., Carrasco M., Welsh R.C., Lord C., Monk C.S. Using a self-organizing map algorithm to detect age-related changes in functional connectivity during rest in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research. 2011;1380:187–197. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wiggins J.L., Bedoyan J.K., Peltier S.J., Ashinoff S., Carrasco M., Weng S.J., Welsh R.C., Martin D.M., Monk C.S. The impact of serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype on the development of resting-state functional connectivity in children and adolescents: a preliminary report. NeuroImage. 2012;59(3):2760–2770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Worsley K.J., Marrett S., Neelin P., Vandal A.C., Friston K.J., Evans A.C. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Human Brain Mapping. 1996;4:58–73. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Youniss J., Haynie D.L. Friendship in adolescence. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP. 1992;13:59–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Inline Supplementary Table S1
mmc1.xlsx (46.8KB, xlsx)
Inline Supplementary Table S2
mmc2.pdf (91.6KB, pdf)

Articles from NeuroImage : Clinical are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES