Abstract
The southern coastline of Australia forms part of the worlds' only northern boundary current system. The Bonney Upwelling occurs every austral summer along the south-eastern South Australian coastline, a region that hosts over 80% of the worlds population of an endangered endemic otariid, the Australian sea lion. We present the first data on the movement characteristics and foraging behaviour of adult male Australian sea lions across their South Australian range. Synthesizing telemetric, oceanographic and isotopic datasets collected from seven individuals enabled us to characterise individual foraging behaviour over an approximate two year time period. Data suggested seasonal variability in stable carbon and nitrogen isotopes that could not be otherwise explained by changes in animal movement patterns. Similarly, animals did not change their foraging patterns despite fine-scale spatial and temporal variability of the upwelling event. Individual males tended to return to the same colony at which they were tagged and utilized the same at-sea regions for foraging irrespective of oceanographic conditions or time of year. Our study contrasts current general assumptions that male otariid life history strategies should result in greater dispersal, with adult male Australian sea lions displaying central place foraging behaviour similar to males of other otariid species in the region.
Introduction
The distribution and foraging patterns of marine mammal top predators are influenced by bottom-up processes affecting lower trophic levels, such as oceanic currents and coastal upwelling events. These predators use three broad foraging strategies in response to the challenges of locating sufficient food to survive and reproduce in changing environments: epipelagic foragers such as sub-Antarctic fur seals Arctocephalus tropicalis [1] focus foraging efforts in the upper water column to depths of 200 m, while mesopelagic depths (200–2000 m) are the realm of predators such as the northern and southern elephant seals Miroungia angustirostris and M. leonina [2], [3] and benthic foragers such as Weddell seals [4], southern sea lions Otaria flavescens [5] and New Zealand sea lions Phocarctos hookeri feed on the sea floor [6]. Typically, epipelagic and mesopelagic foragers rely on predictable dynamic oceanographic features such as frontal zones and upwellings where enhanced productivity supports higher densities of prey [7], [8]. This imparts a degree of seasonality to foraging behaviour and this has been observed in a wide variety of predators including king penguins Aptenodytes paptagonicus [9], southern elephant seals [10], New Zealand fur seals A. forsteri [11] and sub-Antarctic fur seals [12]. As postulated by the ‘meeting point’ hypothesis, benthic or epibenthic prey tend to congregate around features such as shelf-breaks and reef systems [13], [14], [15], making such features important to benthic predators such as yellow-eyed penguins Megadyptes antipodes [16] and Australian fur seals A. pusillus doriferus [17].
Investigating the foraging behaviour of air-breathing marine predators typically involves the use of expensive geospatial tracking devices to describe movements of large, wild, elusive predators during foraging trips to sea. Consequently, prohibitive costs and logistical difficulties often limits answering questions of seasonal and intersexual differences in foraging to those that may be answered using data from a small subsample of animals [18], [19].Researchers face a further challenge when working with marine mammal predators such as otariid seals that undergo seasonal molts, as this restricts when tracking devices can be attached thereby defining the onset and duration of the temporal window over which tracking can occur. This limits the conclusions to be drawn on foraging behaviour to the specific temporal window over which tracking occurred. Otariids are a polygynous, sexually dimorphic order with adult males weighing two to four times the mass of adult females [20]. Female otariids have relatively long lactation periods, ranging from four to 36 months which constrain foraging to within reach of their dependent offspring [21]. Adult males, free from the constraints of providing parental care, should be able to forage across a wider range to ensure they can meet their greater size-related energetic costs [22].
Our understanding of the foraging behaviour of adult male otariids comes from limited studies of only seven of the 16 extant species. California sea lion adult males display both benthic and pelagic foraging behaviour, leaving the Gulf of California at the end of the breeding season to migrate north as far as Oregon into more productive waters before returning late spring with the onset of upwelling [22], [23]. Male Southern sea lions Otaria flavescens in South America also exhibit benthic and pelagic foraging behaviour, travelling twice the distance covered by conspecific adult females [24]. Pelagically-foraging male Antarctic fur seals A. gazella display post-breeding seasonal migration into (presumably) richer foraging grounds ∼900 km from their breeding colonies on South Georgia [25] whereas male northern fur seals Callorhinus ursinus move from shelf to oceanic waters outside the breeding season but do not travel as far as conspecific adult females [26]. In Australia, limited dive data suggests adult male Australian fur seals forage benthically [27] and show considerable individual variability in post-breeding foraging behaviour, with some individuals remaining in the same foraging grounds utilized by adult females and others moving considerable distances [28]. Further west, adult male New Zealand fur seals A. forsteri in South Australia feed along a shelf-break and their dives are typically pelagic, but approximately 20% of their dives are benthic [29], [30]. In all of these studies, conclusions on the temporal variability in foraging behaviour have been limited by the battery longevity of telemetry devices and how long they can remain attached.
The use of cost-effective stable isotope biogeochemistry has provided greater insights into foraging ecology across a range of marine mammals [31]. There is strong support for the upward-cascade of stable nitrogen isotopes (δ15N) from primary producers to the top of the food web. In marine ecosystems a decreasing stable carbon ratio (δ13C) gradient has been identified with distance from shore [32], [33]. Isotope ratios provide ecological geotrophic data that can be used to describe temporal and individual variation in foraging location and diet outside the scope of traditional tracking methods, using δ13C and δ15N from metabolically active (blood) and inert (whiskers, feathers etc) tissues in several seal and marine bird species [34], [35], [36], [37], [38]. Multi-year, individual variation in foraging behaviour has been described using isotope values from serially-subsampled whiskers of adult male Antarctic fur seals [39] and adult female Australian sea lions Neophoca cinerea [40].
The biogeography and unique reproductive ecology of the Australian sea lion has made characterising its foraging ecology challenging. This endangered species [41] has a census estimate of ∼15,000 animals making it one of the rarest otariids in the world. Australian sea lions are distributed from the Pages islands in South Australia to the Abrolhos Islands in Western Australia with 85% of pup production occurring within South Australian waters [42] (Figure 1). A protracted, aseasonal breeding pattern (∼17.5 mo) that is temporally asynchronous across its range is unique amongst pinnipeds where annual synchronous breeding is the norm [43]. The lack of synchrony to breeding is intriguing given the highly seasonal breeding patterns of Australia's other two resident otariids, the Australian and New Zealand fur seals. The fact that adult females display long-term fine-scale foraging site fidelity further suggests that there are adequate resources available year-round [44].
The hypothesis that Australian sea lion life history and foraging strategies reflect a species adapted to an oligotrophic, aseasonal marine environment has been suggested [44]. However, a seasonal cold water upwelling system along the South Australian coastline attracts large aggregations of cetaceans annually during the austral summer [45]. Furthermore, the region is occupied by the largest and fastest-growing breeding colonies of New Zealand fur seals in Australia [46] and supports some of the largest volume commercial fisheries in the country. Rather than being an aseasonal environment, the region appears to be replete with mesoscale areas of primary productivity and seasonality. Although upwellings and associated frontal systems tend to attract pelagic predators, the presence of the largest Australian sea lion breeding colonies of the species in the nearby vicinity suggests, indirectly at least, upwellings also create conditions suitable for benthic foragers. The advent of animal-borne sensors of the same quality as those used by oceanographers provides previously unavailable high-resolution oceanographic datasets along individual animal trajectories [47]. In light of the unique reproductive ecology of Australian sea lions and complete absence of information on adult male Australian sea lion at-sea movement, we employed a combination of telemetric, oceanographic and biogeochemical techniques similar to those successfully used on conspecific adult females [40]. Specifically we wished to determine 1) the temporal and spatial extent of the regions' seasonal upwelling system, 2) whether individuals altered their at-sea behaviour in relation to changing environmental conditions and 3) the degree of individual variability and temporal consistency of foraging behaviours.
Materials and Methods
Study sites and sampling
Movement and environmental data were collected using oceanographic sensors deployed on a single adult male Australian sea lion from each of seven breeding colonies spanning the South Australian range between October 2009 and June 2010 (Figure 1). Males were initially sedated using zolazepam-tilamine (300–350 mg Zoletil® Virbac Ltd) delivered remotely by a NO2-powered tranquilizer gun (Taipan, Tranquil Arms Ltd). Once immobilized, animals were anaesthetized using isoflurane delivered through a portable gas anesthesia machine (5% induction, 1–3% maintenance; Veterinary Companies of Australia). A Conductivity-Temperature-Depth Satellite-Relay Data Logger (CTD-SRDL; Sea Mammal Research Unit Ltd, Scotland) weighing ∼540 g was attached to the pelage distal to the midpoint of the back of each individual using two-part epoxy glue (Araldite® K-268). Prior to recovery from anesthesia procedure a whisker was clipped at the base of the muzzle from each individual for isotopic analysis.
CTD-SRDL sampling protocols
The sensor platform within the CTD-SRDL comprised a Keller PA-7 pressure transducer (accuracy 2±0.3DBar), a custom-made temperature sensor (resolution 0.001°C, accuracy ±0.005°C) and an inductive coil to measure conductivity (resolution 0.002 mS/cm, accuracy ±0.01 mS/cm). Instruments were calibrated after assembly [47] and CTD profiles collected during dives were transmitted via the ARGOS satellite network. Due to bandwidth constraints of the ARGOS network, not all the collected profiles could be transmitted. For a detailed overview of the instruments, onboard data handling, compression, transmission, location and dive depth estimation see [47].
Vibrissae preparation and isotopic analysis
All vibrissae were cleaned as described in [38]. Available data on otariid whisker growth rates suggest linear growth at a rate of approximately 3 mm/month [39], [48]. We use this growth rate as a general guide to sample monthly isotopic values indicative of geotrophic behaviour, similar to that performed by [40], acknowledging that the growth rate is estimated in the absence of species-specific verification. Vibrissae were cut into 3 mm segments starting at the base and placed in labelled 7 ml glass scintillation vials. All samples for isotope analysis were analysed at the Australian National University Environmental Biology Stable Isotope Facility. Analysis was performed using a Microass isoChrom CFIR mass spectrometer coupled to a Carlo Erba EA-1100 CHN-O analyser. The standard control ratio for 13C and 15N were Pee Dee Belemnite and atmospheric nitrogen, respectively. Observed measurement error rates were recorded as <±0.1‰ (δ13C) and <±0.3‰ (δ15N).
Analysis
Location data processing
Raw ARGOS data were pre-processed to remove extreme outliers by removing locations with unclassified error estimates (LC- Z) and by using a swim-speed filter with a moving average of 2 ms−1 [49]. Further filtering involved estimating locations using a Kalman filter under a state-space framework in the R package ‘crawl’ [50]. A foraging trip was defined as continuous periods at-sea greater than 6 h duration [51] and its initial state (location) identified when the instrument entered or exited haulout mode (triggered by an onboard saltwater switch). Using these definitions, location records for each animal were partitioned into foraging trips for which duration (d) and distance travelled (total horizontal Great Circle distance, km) were calculated.
Habitat utilization
To quantify space-use and the environment the animal experienced while foraging, two parameters were derived for each foraging trip. First Passage Time (FPT) was used to identify the scale at which Area Restricted Search (ARS; i.e. inferring active foraging behaviour) was conducted [52], [53]. To perform FPT, each track was redescretized into 1 km step lengths and a new time index interpolated for each new position. The FPT of circle radii ranging from 0.5 km to twice the maximum daily step length [54] were calculated and the radii responsible for varlogmaxFPT identified for each animal (herein referred to as ARS patch size). FPT values for this radii were then plotted against time for each track, with ARS behaviour being inferred at segments of tracks that took the longest time to traverse (i.e. expressed maximum FPT values) [55], [56], [57]. The second set of metrics calculated were the utilization distributions (95% trip range and 50% core range, km2) for each trip of each individual, using a Brownian Bridge movement model (BBMM) described in [58] to account for serial autocorrelation in location data implemented in the R package ‘adehabitatHR’ [59]. Static parameters (depth of the sea floor (m) and bathymetric slope (°)) were derived from a 9 arc-second (250 m) resolution bathymetric digital elevation model of the region [60] and values for each metric were extracted for each FPT location using R.
Spatio-temporal variability in oceanography
Surface mixed layer depth (MLD) can be defined in terms of a critical density gradient or by a finite change in density from surface values [61]. Given the coarse nature of depth sampling provided by compressing Temperature-Salinity (T-S) profiles we employed the latter [62], calculating seawater potential density anomalies (σ ⊖, kg m−3) at one metre intervals for each CTD profile using the Thermodynamic Equation of State of seawater [63]. To avoid aliasing issues with the diurnal heating-cooling cycle and to acknowledge the effect of surface turbulence, we use a conservative value of 0.125 kg m−3 and define the surface σ ⊖ as the value calculated for the bottom of the first depth bin (4 m) to determine MLD at the location of each CTD profile [61], [64], [65]. Monthly means of temperature and salinity at 1 m depth and at the sea floor (determined as being represented by the maximum depth bin of each CTD profile – see below), thermocline intensity (maximum change in temperature per metre, °C m−1) and MLD (m) were calculated and interpolated across the study area on a 1 km resolution grid using ODV4.5.1 [66] and R. Parameter values were then extracted along each track at locations with FPT values.
Spatio-temporal variation in foraging behaviour
Prior to modelling, a coarse characterisation of dive behaviour was made by regressing maximum dive depth on extracted mean water depth for each individual (regression F>91.2, R2 >0.83, p<0.001 in all cases). This exploratory process indicated adult male Australian sea lions exhibited a benthic diving strategy, suggesting environmental conditions at the surface may have been unimportant to foraging. Accordingly, in conjunction with static features (bathymetric slope and depth), we considered only temperature and salinity at the sea floor, MLD, transition layer thickness (the depth between the bottom of the MLD and the sea floor) and thermocline intensity as biologically-relevant, dynamic variables. The significance of temporal variability in core (UD-50) and home (UD-95) ranges, both between individuals and among trips for each individual, were characterised using Kruskal Wallis Χ2. Subsequently, for those individuals that expressed significant variability, we examine the influence of environmental conditions on individual core, home and ARS patch sizes using generalised additive mixed models (GAMM in R package ‘mgcv’). Accounting for the expected high degree of correlation between most oceanographic variables (Pearsons Product Moment Correlation t>36.5, p<0.001 for all significant cases), models ranged in complexity from null models to the inclusion of all uncorrelated covariates (and their interactions). However all models included month-since-deployment as a fixed factor and individual foraging trip as a random factor (Table S1). Penalised regression splines for each parameter were employed in all GAMM's and optimised by restricted maximum likelihood (REML) methods [67]. ANOVA's were used to check for correlation of residuals against fitted values and confirm the appropriateness of candidate models (those which contained significant explanatory terms). The optimal model for each individual was then identified by a likelihood ratio test and confirmed using Bayesian Information Criteria.
We tested the effect of environmental variability on FPT using a series of individual Cox Proportional Hazard models (CPH) of the form: h(t) = exp(β1 X 1 +β2 X 2 +β3 X 3 +...+βp X p)h 0(t), where h(t) is the risk (hazard) of an individual leaving an area (defined by the FPT across a predetermined radius) at time t, X represents a physical variable (bathymetry, slope, rugosity), β denotes the regression coefficient fitted to each variable during the modelling process and h0(t) the baseline hazard function (the risk of leaving an area when all explanatory variables equal zero). In this context, a hazard ratio provides a quantitative means to assess how an animal responds to changes in environmental parameters [68], [69]. For a more comprehensive review of the technique and the manner in which results are interpreted, see [70]. Similar to the GAMM process, all possible combinations of (uncorrelated) environmental parameters and their interactions including null models were fitted during model selection. Optimal model selection (from those models which had significant explanatory terms) was performed using Akaike Information Criteria corrected for effective sample size (AICc) [70]. All CPH models were fitted in R using the ‘survival’ package.
Variation in whisker isotopes
Temporal variation in foraging behaviour was quantified using univariate ARIMA (p,d,q) (AutoRegressive Integrated Moving Average) models to stable carbon (δ13C) and nitrogen (δ15N) isotope values implemented by a three-step Box Jenkins method [71]. ARIMA (p,d,q) models require the estimation of the number of autoregressive parameters (p), the differencing order (d) and the variance of the error term (‘moving average’) parameters (q). When data exploration suggested seasonality, additional parameters in the form (p,d,q)s required estimation with ‘s’ representing the periodicity of seasonal influence. Thus, the minimum-parameter first order Seasonal ARIMA(1,0,0)(0,0,0)0 is essentially an aseasonal first-order autoregression that can be characterised as a temporally-correlated random walk, with the current value being influenced only by the value immediately preceding it. Conditional sum of squares (to assess initial parameter values) followed by exact maximum likelihood methods were employed to estimate each parameter [72]. Finally, models were validated by checking for autocorrelation of residuals using autocorrelograms and portmanteau tests (Ljung-Box Q statistic) [73]. Optimal ARIMA models were then selected using Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) as this method penalises overfitting and is widely used in time series analyses [74].
Results
Movement behaviour
Between November 2009 and May 2010 CTD-SRDL datasets were collected from seven adult male Australian sea lions along the South Australian range of the species (Figure 1). Transmitters remained attached for an average 161d (±13) (range 99–213 d), recording 1486 (±528) locations per individual. Post-processing and filtering left approximately 1182 (±248) locations each. Adult males made a mean 28 (±3) foraging trips (range 19–42) described by approximately 49.5 (±9.46) location fixes per trip. Foraging trips were an average 3.6d (±0.49) in duration (range 0.5–7.1 d) with animals covering a mean 167.9 (±33.26) km (range 7.2–368 km) per trip (Table 1). Individuals dived to a mean depth of 62.2 m (±9.46 m) with mean and maximum dive depth ranges of 15–86 m and 50–137 m, respectively (Table 1). The conductivity sensor failed on one transmitter (Seal Slide), which was then excluded from all subsequent oceanographic and habitat preference analyses. The remaining six individuals collected an average 897 (±68) CTD profiles each (range 594–1125) throughout the tracking period, at a mean rate of 38.2 (±12.6) profiles per foraging trip (range 26–51). Animals that travelled the furthest (West Island and Seal Bay) had the largest ARS patch size, core and home ranges while the individual at Seal Slide had a mean home range less than 200 km2 (Table S1).
Table 1. Deployment and foraging trip data from seven adult male Australian sea lions instrumented between November and December 2009 with satellite-linked Conductivity, Temperature and Depth (CTD) loggers.
Animal ID | Instrumentation date | Duration (d) | Trips | Dive depth (m) | ||||||||
Total | At-sea | N | Distance (km) | Duration (d) | Speed (km h−1) | |||||||
mean (sd) | range | mean (sd) | range | mean (sd) | range | mean (sd) | max | |||||
West Is. | 11-Dec-09 | 160.5 | 106.3 | 19 | 350.9 (44.5) | 254.2–446.6 | 5.6 (0.8) | 4.2–7.7 | 2.6 (0.4) | 2–3.2 | 86.3 (19.4) | 137.5 |
Nicholas Baudin | 08-Dec-09 | 178.6 | 120.3 | 29 | 216.3 (73.9) | 127.2–417.2 | 4.1 (1.2) | 1.6–7.3 | 2.2 (0.5) | 1.5–3.3 | 65.7 (16.5) | 101.5 |
West Waldegrave | 10-Dec-09 | 205.7 | 110 | 42 | 113.5 (52.1) | 10.7–309.3 | 2.6 (1.3) | 1.1–9 | 1.9 (0.7) | 0.2–4.2 | 55.7 (15.7) | 88.5 |
Price Is. | 28-Nov-09 | 213.4 | 51.8 | 33 | 43.4 (32.9) | 15–107.4 | 1.6 (1) | 0.5–3.9 | 1.2 (1.1) | 0.01–6.6 | 15.2 (7.13) | 50.5 |
Liguanea | 29-Nov-09 | 99.1 | 52.8 | 25 | 82.1 (62.1) | 13.1–198.9 | 2.1(1.6) | 0.3–5.4 | 1.7 (0.8) | 0.6–3.6 | 83.9 (32.1) | 129.5 |
Seal Bay | 03-Dec-09 | 175.1 | 123.8 | 23 | 299.8 (33.9) | 237.3–368.1 | 5.4 (0.6) | 4.3–7.1 | 2.3 (0.3) | 1.8–2.7 | 79.2 (18.6) | 105.3 |
Seal Slide | 07-Nov-09 | 179.9 | 96.8 | 27 | 69.7 (26.31) | 7.2–134.4 | 3.6 (0.8) | 1.5–4.6 | 1 (0.3) | 0.2–1.8 | 49.7 (14.5) | 68.5 |
Deployment duration was typically in excess of five months with the exception of the individual at Liguanea which ceased transmitting after little over three months. Foraging trips ranged from 0.3–7.7 d and 7.2–446.6 km in duration and distance, respectively, conducting between 19 and 42 foraging trips each. Most animals spent between 50–70% of their respective tracking periods at sea. The male from West Island achieved the greatest maximum dive depth, while the individual from Price Island reached a mean depth of 15.2 m and spent less than 25% of its tracking period at sea.
Spatio-temporal variability in the oceanography of the eastern Great Australian Bight
Using the threshold criteria of [75] for defining upwelled water (temperature:<17°C, σ ⊖>26 kg m−3, salinity:<35.6 kg m−3), upwelling occurred between December and March along the western Eyre Peninsula reaching as far as Cape Bauer near Streaky Bay (Figure 2). Upwelled water also reached the surface consistently between Coffin Bay and Elliston over the same period, being most evident during March (Figure 2). Cross-sectional visualization of the water structure also revealed substantial variation over the region (Figure 3). Although the upwelling plume did not reach the surface at the western extent of the study region, it was detectable at West Island as shallow as 30 m in March. In all cases there was diminished thermal stratification of the water column towards the start of the Austral winter (May) signifying the end of the upwelling season (Figure 3). Between January and May, individuals experienced significantly different monthly mean benthic temperatures and salinities (between-individuals in the same month; Kruskal-Wallis X2>13.7, p<0.001). Mean benthic conditions also varied significantly for each individual throughout their respective tracking period with the exception of West Island (between-months for the same individual; Kruskal-Wallis X2>14.1, p<0.001 in all significant cases; Figures 3). There were positive relationships between mixed layer depth and transition layer depth with ocean depth (mixed layer depth: regression F1,185 = 88, p<0.001, R2 = 0.33; transition layer depth: regression F1,185 = 641, p<0.001, R2 = 0.78). Thus, when upwelling was present, individuals that foraged in the deepest water experienced the thickest mixed layer and thickest transition layer (ANOVA F>4.6, p<0.05 in all cases).
Spatio-temporal variability of habitat use by individuals
Generally, individual movement patterns did not change throughout the study period (Figure 1). However, the male at Nicholas Baudin did move between its tagging site and nearby Olive Island on several occasions though at-sea movements tended to be in the same general areas. Similarly the Liguanea male also moved between colonies, visiting Lewis Island in the Thorny Passage and Dangerous Reef in the southern Spencer Gulf on several occasions (Figure 1). Only males from West Is, Nicholas Baudin and Seal Bay displayed significant relationships between areal metrics and any environmental variables (Table S1). The male at West Island decreased both core and home ranges in response to rising benthic salinity (GAMM Fsmooth(benthic salinity) = 7.6 and 5.1, respectively, P<0.05 in both cases). Conversely, despite exploiting at-sea regions close to those used by the West Island male, the individual at Nicholas Baudin did not appear to respond to changes in dynamic features, instead decreasing its core range at shallower depths (GAMM Fsmooth(bathymetry) = 44.6, P<0.001) and steeper slopes (GAMM Fsmooth(bathymetry) = 3.5, P<0.05). At Seal Bay the male started to increase its core range when bottom temperatures started to rise above 13°C (GAMM Fsmooth(benthic temperature) = 6.9, P<0.001; Table S2).
In terms of residency times along individual tracks, optimal CPH models failed to include any dynamic variable for any individual except for the West Island male, which showed an aversion to warmer bottom temperatures (Table S3). The male at West Waldegrave expressed a general preference for habitat in shallower water, decreasing residency times by ∼10% with each metre of depth (Table S3). The Liguanea individual displayed a preference for flat terrain, displaying a strong aversion to bathymetric slope. For both males these patterns were consistent over time, with a low spread of per-trip variability in the risk of leaving (∼17% in both cases). Variability in FPT of the males at Nicholas Baudin, Price Is. and Seal Bay were parsimoniously characterised by a null model (Table S3).
Isotopic variability
Individual values for δ13C and δ15N ranged from -14.7 to -18.2‰ and 14.8 to 17.7‰, respectively. Mean carbon and nitrogen isotope ratios generally increased along a longitudinal cline (Kruskal Χ2>161.8, p<0.001 in both cases; Table 2). The adult male at Seal Bay displayed the lowest mean carbon ratios of all colonies (−17.6±0.16‰), followed by Liguanea (-16.8±0.1‰) (Students t-test t>15.6, p<0.001 in all cases; Table 2). The notable exception in mean nitrogen values was the individual from Liguanea, which had the lowest ratios detected (15±0.04‰), more than 2‰ below the highest recorded mean value (West Island, 17.1±0.06‰,) (Students t-test, t = 25.7, p<0.001). Individual δ13C and δ15N variability (where present) were best characterised by first-order ARIMA models with a seasonal periodicity ranging from 3–12 months (Table S4, Figure 4). Significant seasonal patterns (in terms of variance of carbon isotope ratios) were detected for all animals but the magnitude of seasonality differed significantly between individuals (Bartletts test, K = 174, p<0.001; Figure 4). Variance around mean carbon ratios for individuals at Seal Bay and Seal Slide (0.6‰ and 0.26‰, respectively) were a minimum of an order of magnitude greater than all other individuals (Table 2).
Table 2. Stable carbon (δ13C) and nitrogen (δ15N) isotope data from whiskers of seven adult male Australian sea lions sampled along the South Australian coastline.
Animal ID | Instrumentation date | δ13C (‰) | δ15N (‰) | ||||
mean | variance | range | mean | variance | range | ||
West Is. | 11-Dec-09 | −15.5 | 0.07 | −16–15 | 17.1 | 0.10 | 16.5–17.7 |
Nicholas Baudin | 08-Dec-09 | −15.5 | 0.01 | −15.7–15.4 | 16.2 | 0.06 | 15.7–16.6 |
West Waldegrave | 10-Dec-09 | −15.2 | 0.01 | −15.4–15 | 16.7 | 0.08 | 16.3–17.1 |
Price Is. | 28-Nov-09 | −15.1 | 0.01 | −15.4–14.9 | 16.2 | 0.08 | 15.6–16.6 |
Liguanea | 29-Nov-09 | −15.2 | 0.08 | −15.4–14.7 | 15.9 | 0.08 | 15.4–16.4 |
Seal Slide | 03-Dec-09 | −17.6 | 0.60 | −18.7–16.2 | 15.5 | 0.04 | 15.2–16 |
Seal Bay | 07-Nov-09 | −16.8 | 0.26 | −17.9–16 | 15.0 | 0.08 | 14.8–15.7 |
Mean, variance and ranges (‰) shown for each animal. δ15N generally showed a distinction between animals east and west of Spencer Gulf/137°longitude. Individuals at Seal Slide and Seal Bay displayed variance in δ13C (in bold) approaching an order of magnitude greater than any other individual.
Discussion
We present the first comprehensive study of adult male Australian sea lion foraging ecology, identifying long-term individual fidelity to geographical areas and substantial inter-individual variability in foraging habitat selectivity throughout the formation and decline of a seasonal upwelling system. Integrated biogeochemical, telemetric and oceanographic data provides a powerful means of exploring the interactions between a wide-ranging marine predator and its environment in the context of oceanographic variability. Given the strong positive relationship between maximum dive depth and bathymetry, we conclude that adult male Australian sea lions exhibit a benthic foraging strategy. Whether they are obligate benthic foragers is unknown. Adult male Australian sea lions in South Australia display minimal reliance on dynamic environmental variation typified by seasonal upwelling. The study spanned one of the most intense upwelling events in South Australian history and was measured in situ by the animals themselves, thus we are confident our data appropriately reflect the response of adult males to dynamic oceanography. Some individuals conducted cross-shelf foraging trips perpendicular to the dominant oceanographic currents in the region but others occupied foraging grounds much closer to the colonies at which they were instrumented. The existence of summer upwelling along the southern coastline of Australia has been known for over 40 years [76]. Previous studies of upwelling dynamics throughout the region have been based on limited in-situ data, conceptual models or on studies conducted over a short time period (4–6 weeks) [75], [77]. Fitting adult male Australian sea lions with oceanographic sensors has enabled us to collect data describing the four-dimensional spatial extent of a seasonal upwelling event in southern Australia over a six-month period. We show that adult male Australian sea lions may be a tractable group for future study of biological and physical oceanographic processes in this unique northern boundary current system.
Foraging behaviour unaffected by dynamic oceanographic processes
Trophic structure and function in marine ecosystems is a result of the interaction between the dynamic features of physical ocean processes and the structure of the benthos [78]. A lag between the onset of physical ocean processes and primary and secondary productivity often results in temporal and spatial variability in structure and composition of the foodweb throughout the water column [79]. It has been suggested that spatial de-coupling between physical oceanography and primary and secondary productivity may have been responsible for the lack of fine-scale relationships between Antarctic fur seal foraging behaviour and chlorophyll a [80]. The interactions between oceanic processes and the continental shelf can also influence community structures. Off the coast of Oregon, differences in current strength and upwelling intensity along an asymmetrical shelf result in greater food availability that persists outside the upwelling season at inshore communities along the widest sections of the shelf [79]. In California, larval euphausids spawned during winter upwelling reach adulthood approximately four months later, creating a bloom in zooplankton [81]. Adult euphausiids then track a shoreward collapse in oceanic productivity into the Monterey Bay at the end of the upwelling season, coinciding with the seasonal aggregation of blue whales [81], [82].
The eastern Great Australian Bight can also be characterised by an asymmetrical shelf structure increasing in width westwards and punctuated along its length by submarine canyons with the eastern end often described as the centre of seasonal upwelling [75], [77], [83]. Nutrient rich, carbon-depleted waters from the subtropical front (STF) pulse onto the shelf during the Austral summer in a series of upwelling events [77] forming what is referred to as the ‘Kangaroo Island pool’ [75]. Available literature on the upwelling process suggests that this pool represents the source of upwelled water along the eastern Great Australian Bight [75]. Subsequent wind-shear pulls water from this pool and drives it along the coastline of the western Eyre Peninsula throughout the Austral summer in a series of wind-trapped coastal upwelling events. Progressive mixing of upwelled water with more 13C-enriched coastal and shelf waters coupled with prolonged onshore winds may result in the pooling of nutrients along the wider western margins of the shelf in a process similar to that outlined by [78]. Further alteration of the carbon signal would occur with the introduction of warm, carbon-depleted Leeuwin Current water at the start of winter [84]. Our study supports this hypothesis, with seasonal variation in 13C appearing to decrease at sites west of Kangaroo Island reflecting the mixing of 13C-depleted waters from the upwelling site with more 13C rich coastal waters farther west. Interestingly, adult male Australian sea lions who displayed the lowest δ13C also fed consistently at the lowest trophic levels in the study. Interaction between topographical breaks in the shelf, longitudinal variation in the distribution of upwelled water, the intrusion of a tropical water mass into the region at the end of summer and the spatial decorrelation between the availability of nutrients and secondary productivity are likely to lead to variability in prey guild composition, age structure or a combination of both. These complex, fine-scale processes likely drive the patterns of isotopic markers we describe and explain why most adult male foraging behaviour did not track with dynamic changes in the physical environment.
Spatial and temporal variation in isotopic signatures suggest foraging behaviour is temporally persistent
There are several assumptions implicit to the interpretation of our isotopic data. Foremost, we had no species-specific whisker growth rates thus our assumption of 3mm representing an isotopic timeline of one month may be incorrect. Furthermore, the possibility that individuals undergo periodic whisker replacement may also have confounded our interpretation. Empirical estimates of whisker growth rates in otariids are restricted to Steller sea lions and Antarctic fur seals [39], [48] with both studies suggesting whiskers grow consistently at similar rates (0.1–0.16 mm d−1) and are retained year-round, in contrast to phocid seals such as the gray seal and harbor seal [48], [85]. Thus we argue the context in which we interpret our isotopic data is likely to be robust to small errors in timeline estimation. Furthermore, the seasonal carbon cycles we detect at the eastern end of our study reflect the seasonal cycle of upwelling in the region. Importantly, the biogeochemical markers used in our study highlighted what appeared to be broad differences in the trophic ecology of individuals foraging towards the epicentre of the upwelling compared to those at its western extremity. Individuals at Seal Bay and Seal Slide displayed a discrete shift in δ13C, a pattern commonly interpreted in other otariid and seabird species as representing seasonal migration [34], [39], [86], [87]. Taken in isolation, the geotrophic data we present for these individuals may have been mistakenly interpreted as reflecting broad-scale differences in the foraging ecology of individual adult males, with eastern males undergoing seasonal changes in foraging location.
Our study is a cautionary tale about relying solely on biogeochemical markers to infer geotrophic behaviour. The incorporation of prolonged time-series telemetry data and information about oceanography enables us to interpret our isotopic data in a more appropriate manner. There were no consistent trends in adult male at-sea movement that related to dynamic oceanographic variability and the manner in which individuals explored their environment. Adult males in the region that experienced the greatest environmental variation (Kangaroo Island) did not alter their movement patterns throughout the entire upwelling season (Dec – May). Indeed, the individual who foraged on the shelf break consistently occupied similar habitat to those recorded for adult male New Zealand fur seals [30]. The lack of seasonal foraging patterns in adult male Australian sea lions and New Zealand fur seals suggests there is adequate, suitable prey available all year at or near the benthos though the strong seasonality of the marine environment in the region may result in a more diverse, seasonally-variable suite of prey items. The strong seasonal signal in 13C and 15N detected in these eastern individuals probably reflects seasonal changes in the structure of prey guilds rather than changes in where individuals foraged. Thus we strongly recommend employing biotelemetry devices to put isotopic data into context, particularly in temperate latitudes or coastal marine environments where isoscapes are poorly defined or highly variable. Assuming our interpretation is correct, we also caution against generalising adult male Australian sea lion diet based on limited temporal or spatial sampling. The apparent long-term fidelity of adult male Australian sea lions to individual colonies we show does provide an opportunity to generate species-specific whisker growth rates. Biological markers such as isotopically-labelled glycine could be administered to individuals during the initial deployment of transmitters, creating a highly-visible marker that would be evident in whiskers removed at the end of a deployment [48].
Individual variation in foraging movement
If suitable prey exists year-round along the South Australian coastline then why do males not forage more broadly, particularly given they are not restricted by the need to provide parental care? Even though some individuals in our study could conceivably reach the same foraging grounds as males from neighboring colonies, they did not do so. This suggests that the parameters we measured were not sufficient to explain strong individual preferences for particular foraging habitat. However, tracking one male at each colony hampers our ability to generalise any behaviour. For example we do not know whether the strategies adopted by individuals we followed are representative of all males at a colony, or are one of several behaviours within a colony [40]. The former hypothesis points towards sufficiently strong local ecological and environmental pressure to influence all the males resident at each colony, whereas the latter may indicate strong individual experiences of known features associated with consistent prey [13]. Coastal upwelling was most persistent around the lower western Eyre Peninsula, where individuals at Price Island and West Waldegrave remained foraging relatively close to shore. These explanatory factors are not likely to be mutually exclusive; males may be able to access a preferred foraging location from several nearby colonies, unlike those at other more isolated colonies. Thus we recommend additional tracking to test these hypotheses and elucidate further on the influence of oceanographic variability at a colony-level. An inability to link environmental parameters with foraging activity point towards information missing from our analyses. A notable gap in our data is the availability and diversity of prey, which should be filled by direct sampling throughout the region. Furthermore, additional telemetric studies on adult male Australian sea lions should incorporate measures of individual internal condition such as age or body mass that may influence the inter-individual variability in strategies we describe.
Our interpretations may be confounded by assuming that the location at which a male was initially captured represents its home colony. On more than one occasion the Nicholas Baudin male hauled out at nearby Olive Island, the fifth largest breeding colony of the species that at the time of instrumentation was over two months past a breeding episode. Conversely, the Liguanea male made several trips up the Thorny Passage into the southern Spencer Gulf, stopping at two colonies of which one was entering a breeding cycle during the tracking period (Lewis Island, Figure 1). This individual was observed at the breeding colony on two separate occasions and was in a physical condition normally associated with breeding, but was not seen mate-guarding or competing with other males (Heidi Ahonen, pers. com). With the exception of males at Liguanea and Nicholas Baudin, repeated haulouts at the same colony generally support our assumption but further tracking (particularly during breeding episodes) is required to confirm.
Both [29] and [28] propose that the lack of post-breeding dispersal of male Australian and New Zealand fur seals may relate to increased selective benefits incurred by a) acquiring knowledge of breeding areas throughout the year and b) establishing themselves within the adult male hierarchy at each colony. Additionally, if there is plasticity in the timing of breeding events, there may also be physiological costs to misjudging its onset. Adult male otariids must gain considerable mass prior to breeding in order to compete successfully for territories and females, though accumulating fat is likely to have associated storage and transport costs [88]. Australian sea lion males are confronted with prolonged, aseasonal breeding events that occur asynchronously between colonies. Accordingly they may choose to forage within an area that provides regular access to colonies with breeding schedules that are familiar and can be continually monitored, rather than risk moving to colonies whose breeding chronology is unknown. Physiological costs could also be minimized by accurately predicting the onset of breeding at a colony and delaying fat deposition as long as possible. We propose that the general lack of seasonal dispersal by adult male Australian sea lions is driven by the unique reproductive cycle of adult females and the need to predict the onset of breeding at colonies.
Conclusions
We present evidence that the foraging behaviour of adult male Australian sea lions is not affected by seasonal changes in oceanographic conditions. Rather, unlike pelagic predators that rely on dynamic environmental features to locate prey, benthically-foraging adult male Australian sea lions may focus on static features that act as predictable prey aggregation sites. Our study contrasts current general assumptions that male otariid life history strategies should result in greater dispersal [23], with adult male Australian sea lions displaying central place foraging behaviour similar to Australian and New Zealand fur seal males [28], [88] and a high degree of fidelity to foraging locations.
In light of extremely low levels of adult female migration and what appears to be a general lack of adult male movement between colonies, the presumption that sufficient male-mediated geneflow occurs to obviate the risks of inbreeding may also be inappropriate. Future work should focus along two mutually non-exclusive lines 1) fully characterising the extent of individual male variability in foraging behaviour both within and between colonies, particularly in relation to breeding cycles of colonies, and 2) determining whether aseasonal foraging behaviour and an apparent lack of dispersal translates into reduced male-mediated geneflow.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
Data was sourced from the Integrated Marine Observing System (IMOS). We thank the Sea Mammal Research Institute (SMRU) for assistance with instrument configuration and calibration (Phil Lovell, Simon Halliwell and Mike Fedak), and Barossa Helicopters, Commercial Helicopters, Ceduna Boat Charter, and Department of Environment, Water and Natural Resources (DEWNR) staff from Ceduna, Streaky Bay, Port Lincoln and Kangaroo Island for assistance with the deployment and recovery of instruments. Animal handling procedures were approved by the Primary Industries and Regions South Australia (PIRSA) Animal Ethics Committee and by DEWNR (Permit to undertake scientific research A24684). Research was undertaken with support from the South Australian Research and Development Institute (SARDI) and Marine Innovation Southern Australia (MISA).
Funding Statement
Funding was received from the Integrated Marine Observing System (IMOS) – IMOS is supported by the Australian Government through the National Collaborative Research Infrastructure Strategy and the Super Science Initiative. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Georges J-Y, Bonadonna F, Guinet C (2000) Foraging habitat and diving activity of lactating Subantarctic fur seals in relation to sea-surface temperatures at Amsterdam Island. Marine Ecology Progress Series 196: 291–304. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Campagna C, Le Boeuf BJ, Blackwell SB, Crocker DE, Quintana F (1995) Diving behaviour and foraging location of female southern elephant seals from Patagonia. Journal of Zoology (London): 236(231).
- 3.LeBoeuf BJ (1994) Variation in the diving pattern of northern elephant seals with age, mass sex, and reproductive condition. In: LeBoeuf BJ, Laws RM, editors. Elephant seals: population ecology, behaviour, and physiology. Berkeley: University of California Press. pp. 237–252.
- 4.Hindell MA, Harcourt R, Waas JR, Thompson D (2002) Fine-scale three-dimensional spatial use by diving, lactating female Weddell seals Leptonychotes weddellii. Marine Ecology-Progress Series: 275–284.
- 5. Thompson D, Duck CD, McConnell BJ, Garret J (1998) Foraging behaviour and diet of lactating female southern sea lions (Otaria flavescens) in the Falkland Islands. Journal of Zoology, London 246: 135–146. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Crocker DE, Gales NJ, Costa DP (2001) Swimming speed and foraging strategies of New Zealand sea lions (Phocarctos hookeri). Journal of Zoology 254: 267–277. [Google Scholar]
- 7. Olson DB, Backus RH (1985) The concentrating of organisms at fronts: a cold-water fish and a warm-core Gulf Stream ring. Journal of Marine Research 43: 113–137. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Schneider DC (1990) Seabirds and fronts: a brief overview. Polar Record 8: 17–21. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Guinet C, Koudil M, Bost CA, Durbec JP, Georges JY, et al. (1997) Foraging behaviour of satellite-tracked king penguins in relation to sea-surface temperatures obtained by satellite telemetry at Crozet Archipelago, a study during three austral summers. Marine Ecology Progress Series 150: 11–20. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Field I, Hindell MA, Slip DJ, Michael KJ (2002) Foraging strategies of southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina) in relation to frontal zones and water masses. Antarctic Science 13: in press.
- 11. Harcourt RG, Bradshaw CJA, Dickson K, Davis LS (2002) Foraging ecology of a generalist predator, the female New Zealand fur seal. Marine Ecology-Progress Series 227: 11–24. [Google Scholar]
- 12. De Bruyn PJN, Tosh CA, Oosthuizen WC, Bester MN, Arnould JPY (2009) Bathymetry and frontal system interactions influence seasonal foraging movements of lactating Subantarctic fur seals from Marion Island. Marine Ecology Progress Series 394: 263–276. [Google Scholar]
- 13. Fréon P, Dagorn L (2000) Review of fish associative behaviour: toward a generalisation of the meeting point hypothesis. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 10: 183–207. [Google Scholar]
- 14. Klimley AP, Holloway C (1999) School fidelity and homing synchronicity of yellowfin tuna, Thunnus albacares. Marine Biology 133: 307–317. [Google Scholar]
- 15. Langton RW, Auster PJ, Schneider DC (1995) A spatial and temporal perspective on research and management of groundfish in the northwest Atlantic. Reviews in Fisheries Science 3: 201–229. [Google Scholar]
- 16. Mattern T, Ellenberg U, Houston DM, Davis LS (2007) Consistent foraging routes and benthic foraging behaviour in yellow-eyed penguins. Marine Ecology Progress Series 343: 295–306. [Google Scholar]
- 17. Arnould JPY, Kirkwood R (2007) Habitat selection by female Australian fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus). Aquatic Conservation: Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems 17: S53–S67. [Google Scholar]
- 18. Cronin MA, McConnell BJ (2008) SMS seal: A new technique to measure haul out behaviour in marine vertebrates. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 362: 43–48. [Google Scholar]
- 19. Hays GC, Broderick AC, Godley BJ, Luschi P, Nichois WJ (2003) Satellite telemetry suggests high levels of fishing-induced mortality in marine turtles. Marine Ecology-Progress Series 262: 305–309. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gentry RL, Kooyman GL (1986) Fur Seals: Maternal Strategies on Land at Sea. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
- 21.Gentry RL, Costa DP, Croxall JP, David JHM, Davis RW, et al.. (1986) Synthesis and conclusions. In: Gentry RL, Kooyman GL, editors. Fur seals: maternal strategies on land and at sea. Princeton: Princeton University Press. pp. 220–278.
- 22. Weise MJ, Harvey JT, Costa DP (2010) The role of body size in individual-based foraging strategies of a top marine predator. Ecology 91: 1004–1015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Weise MJ, Costa DP, Kudela RM (2006) Movement and diving behavior of male California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) during anomalous oceanographic conditions of 2005 compared to those of 2004. Geophysical Research Letters 33: L22S10. [Google Scholar]
- 24. Campagna C, Werner R, Karesh W, Marín MR, Koontz F, et al. (2001) Movements and location at sea of South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens). Journal of Zoology 255: 205–220. [Google Scholar]
- 25. Boyd IL, McCafferty DJ, Reid K, Taylor R, Walker TR (1998) Dispersal of male and female Antarctic fur seals (Arctocephalus gazella). Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 55: 845–852. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Loughlin T, Ingraham Jr., W, Baba N, Robson B (1999) Use of a surface-current model and satellite telemetry to assess marine mammal movements in the Bering Sea. Dynamics of the Bering Sea Edited by TR Loughlin and K Ohtani University of Alaska Sea Grant, Fairbanks, Alaska: 615–630.
- 27. Hindell MA, Pemberton D (1997) Successful use of a translocation program to investigate diving behavior in a male Australian fur seal, Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus. Marine Mammal Science 13: 219–228. [Google Scholar]
- 28. Kirkwood R, Lynch M, Gales NJ, Dann P, Sumner MD (2006) At-sea movements and habitat use of adult male Australian fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus). Canadian Journal of Zoology 84: 1781–1788. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Page B, McKenzie J, Goldsworthy SD (2005) Inter-sexual differences in New Zealand fur seal diving behaviour. Marine ecology Progress series 304: 249–264. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Page B, McKenzie J, Sumner MD, Coyne M, Goldsworthy SD (2006) Spatial separation of foraging habitats amongst New Zealand fur seals. Marine Ecology Progress Series 323: 263–279. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Crawford K, McDonald RA, Bearhop S (2008) Applications of stable isotope techniques to the ecology of mammals. Mammal Review 38: 87–107. [Google Scholar]
- 32. Hobson KA, Schell D, Renouf D, Noseworthy E (1996) Stable-carbon and nitrogen isotopic fractionation between diet and tissues of captive seals: implications for dietary reconstructions involving marine mammals. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Acquatic Science 53: 528–533. [Google Scholar]
- 33. Post DM (2002) Using stable isotopes to estimate trophic position: models, methods, and assumptions. Ecology 83: 703–718. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Aurioles D, Koch PL, Le Boeuf BJ (2006) Differences in foraging location of Mexican and California elephant seals: Evidence from stable isotopes in pups. Marine Mammal Science 22: 326–338. [Google Scholar]
- 35. Awkerman JA, Hobson KA, Anderson DJ (2007) Isotopic (delta N-15 and delta C-13) evidence for intersexual foraging differences and temporal variation in habitat use in waved albatrosses. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 85: 273–279. [Google Scholar]
- 36. Bearhop S, Phillips RA, McGill R, Cherel Y, Dawson DA, et al. (2006) Stable isotopes indicate sex-specific and long-term individual foraging specialisation in diving seabirds. Marine Ecology-Progress Series 311: 157–164. [Google Scholar]
- 37. Forero MG, Hobson KA (2003) Using stable isotopes of nitrogen and carbon to study seabird ecology: applications in the Mediterranean seabird community. Scientia Marina 67: 23–32. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lowther AD, Goldsworthy SD (2010) Detecing alternate foraging ecotypes in Australian sea lion (Neophoca cinerea) breeding colonies using stable isotope analysis. Marine Mammal Science 27.
- 39. Cherel Y, Kernaleguen L, Richard P, Guinet C (2009) Whisker isotopic signature depicts migration patterns and multi-year intra- and inter-individual foraging strategies in fur seals. Biology Letters 5: 830–832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Lowther AD, Hamer D, Harcourt RG, Goldsworthy SD (2011) Creatures of habit: foraging habitat fildelity of adult female Australian sea lions. Marine Ecology Progress Series 443: 249–263. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Goldsworthy S, Gales N (2008) Neophoca cinerea. IUCN red list of threatened species.
- 42. Myers N, Mittermeier RA, Mittermeier CG, Da Fonseca GAB, Kent J (2000) Biodiversity hotspots for conservation priorities. Nature 403: 853–858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Atkinson S (1997) Reproductive biology of seals. Reviews of Reproduction 2: 175–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gales NJ, Costa DP (1997) The Australian sea lion: a review of an unusual life history. In: Hindell MA, Kemper C, editors. Marine Mammal Research in the Southern Hemisphere. Sydney: Beatty and Sons. pp. 78–87.
- 45. Urbano F, Cagnacci F, Calenge C, Dettki H, Cameron A, et al. (2010) Wildlife tracking data management: a new vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 365: 2177–2185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Baylis AMM, Page B, Goldsworthy SD (2008) Effect of seasonal changes in upwelling activity on the foraging locations of a wide-ranging central-place forager, the New Zealand fur seal. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 86: 774–789. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Boehme L, Lovell P, Biuw M, Roquet F, Nicholson J, et al.. (2009) Technical Note: Animal-borne CTD-Satellite Relay Data Loggers for real-time oceanographic data collection. Ocean Science.
- 48. Hirons AC, Schell DM, St Aubin DJ (2001) Growth rates of vibrissae of harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) and Steller sea lions (Eumetopias jubatus) Canadian Journal of Zoology. 79: 1053–1061. [Google Scholar]
- 49. McConnell B, Chambers C, Fedak M (1992) Foraging ecology of southern elephant seals in relation to the bathymetry and productivity of the Southern Ocean. Antarctic Science 4: 393–393. [Google Scholar]
- 50. Johnson DS, London JM, Lea MA, Durban JW (2008) Continuous-time correlated random walk model for animal telemetry data. Ecology 89: 1208–1215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Arnould JPY, Hindell MA (2001) Dive behaviour, foraging locations, and maternal-attendance patterns of Australian fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus doriferus). Canadian Journal of Zoology 79: 35–48. [Google Scholar]
- 52. Bailey H, Mate BR, Palacios DM, Irvine L, Bograd SJ, et al. (2009) Behavioural estimation of blue whale movements in the Northeast Pacific from state-space model analysis of satellite tracks. Endang Species Res 10: 93–106. [Google Scholar]
- 53. Pinaud D (2008) Quantifying search effort of moving animals at several spatial scales using first-passage time analysis: effect of the structure of environment and tracking systems. Journal of Applied Ecology 45: 91. [Google Scholar]
- 54. Bradshaw CJA, Sims DW, Hays GC (2007) Measurement error causes scale-dependent threshold erosion of biological signals in animal movement data. Ecological Applications 17: 628–638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Bailleul F, Lesage V, Hammill MO (2010) Spherical First Passage Time: A tool to investigate area-restricted search in three-dimensional movements. Ecological Modelling.
- 56. Hamer K, Humphreys E, Magalhaes M, Garthe S, Hennicke J, et al. (2009) Fine scale foraging behaviour of a medium ranging marine predator. Journal of Animal Ecology 78: 880–889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Votier SC, Grecian WJ, Patrick S, Newton J (2011) Inter-colony movements, at-sea behaviour and foraging in an immature seabird: results from GPS-PPT tracking, radio-tracking and stable isotope analysis. Marine Biology: 1–8.
- 58. Horne JS, Garton EO, Krone SM, Lewis JS (2007) Analyzing animal movements using Brownian bridges. Ecology 88: 2354–2363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Calenge C (2011) Home Range Estimation in R: the adehabitatHR Package.
- 60.Whiteway T (2009) Australian Bathymetry and Topography Grid, June 2009. Geoscience Australia Record 21.
- 61. Brainerd KE, Gregg MC (1995) Surface mixed and mixing layer depths. Deep Sea Research Part I: Oceanographic Research Papers 42: 1521–1543. [Google Scholar]
- 62. Nilsen J, Falck E (2006) Variations of mixed layer properties in the Norwegian sea for the period 1948–1999. Progress in Oceanography 70: 58–90. [Google Scholar]
- 63.McDougall T, Feistel R, Millero F, Jackett D, Wright D, et al.. (2010) The International Thermodynamic Equation Of Sea water 2010 (TEOS-10): Calculation and Use of Thermodynamic Properties, Global Ship-based Repeat Hydrography Manual. IOCCP Report.
- 64. Rudnick DL, Ferrari R (1999) Compensation of horizontal temperature and salinity gradients in the ocean mixed layer. Science 283: 526–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Huyer A, Fleischbein JH, Keister J, Kosro PM, Perlin N, et al. (2005) Two coastal upwelling domains in the northern California Current system. Journal of Marine Research 63: 901–929. [Google Scholar]
- 66. Schlitzer R (2002) Interactive analysis and visualization of geoscience data with Ocean Data View. Computers & geosciences 28: 1211–1218. [Google Scholar]
- 67. Wood SN (2000) Modelling and smoothing parameter estimation with multiple quadratic penalties. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Statistical Methodology) 62: 413–428. [Google Scholar]
- 68. Freitas C, Kovacs KM, Ims RA, Fedak MA, Lydersen C (2009) Deep into the ice: over-wintering and habitat selection in male Atlantic walruses. Marine Ecology Progress Series 375: 247–261. [Google Scholar]
- 69.Nordstrom CA, Battaile BC, Cotté C, Trites AW (2012) Foraging habitats of lactating northern fur seals are structured by thermocline depths and submesoscale fronts in the eastern Bering Sea. Deep Sea Research Part II: Topical Studies in Oceanography.
- 70. Freitas C, Kovacs KM, Lydersen C, Ims RA (2008) A novel method for quantifying habitat selection and predicting habitat use. Journal of Applied Ecology 45: 1213–1220. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Box G, Jenkins G, Reinsel G (1994) Time series analysis, forecasting and control. Eaglewood Cliffs, NJ.: Prentice Hall.
- 72. Gardner G, Harvey A, Phillips G (1980) Algorithm AS 154: An Algorithm for Exact Maximum Likelihood Estimation of Autoregressive-Moving Average Models by Means of Kalman Filtering. Applied Statistics 29: 311–322. [Google Scholar]
- 73. Ljung GM, Box GEP (1978) On a measure of lack of fit in time series models. Biometrika 65: 297. [Google Scholar]
- 74. Hurvich CM, Tsai CL (1989) Regression and time series model selection in small samples. Biometrika 76: 297. [Google Scholar]
- 75. McClatchie S, Middleton JF, Ward TM (2006) Water mass analysis and alongshore variation in upwelling intensity in the eastern Great Australian Bight. Journal of Geophysical Research 111: C08007. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Rochford D (1977) A review of a possible upwelling situation off Port Macdonnell SA [South Australia]. Report-CSIRO Division of Fisheries and Oceanography.
- 77. Kämpf J, Doubell M, Griffin D, Matthews RL, Ward TM (2004) Evidence of a large seasonal coastal upwelling system along the southern shelf of Australia. Geophysical Research Letters 31: L09310. [Google Scholar]
- 78. Menge BA, Blanchette C, Raimondi P, Freidenburg T, Gaines S, et al. (2004) Species interaction strength: testing model predictions along an upwelling gradient. Ecological Monographs 74: 663–684. [Google Scholar]
- 79. Menge BA, Sanford E, Daley BA, Freidenburg TL, Hudson G, et al. (2002) Inter-hemispheric comparison of bottom-up effects on community structure: Insights revealed using the comparative-experimental approach. Ecological Research 17: 1–16. [Google Scholar]
- 80. Guinet C, Dubroca L, Lea MA, Goldsworthy S, Cherel Y, et al. (2001) Spatial distribution of foraging in female Antarctic fur seals Arctocephalus gazella in relation to oceanographic variables: a scale-dependent approach using geographic information systems. Marine Ecology Progress Series 219: 251–264. [Google Scholar]
- 81. Croll DA, Marinovic B, Benson S, Chavez FP, Black N, et al. (2005) From wind to whales: trophic links in a coastal upwelling system. Marine Ecology Progress Series 289: 117–130. [Google Scholar]
- 82. Schoenherr JR (1991) Blue whales feeding on high concentrations of euphausiids around Monterey Submarine Canyon. Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 583–594. [Google Scholar]
- 83. Middleton JF, Bye JAT (2007) A review of the shelf-slope circulation along Australia's southern shelves: Cape Leeuwin to Portland. Progress in Oceanography 75: 1–41. [Google Scholar]
- 84. Hanson CE, Pattiaratchi CB, Waite AM (2005) Seasonal production regimes off south-western Australia: influence of the Capes and Leeuwin Currents on phytoplankton dynamics. Marine and Freshwater Research 56: 1011–1026. [Google Scholar]
- 85. Greaves DK, Hammill MO, Eddington JD, Pettipas D, Schreer JF (2004) Growth rate and shedding of vibrissae in the gray seal, Halichoerus grypus: A cautionary note for stable isotope diet analysis. Marine Mammal Science 20: 296–304. [Google Scholar]
- 86. Phillips RA, Bearhop S, Mcgill RAR, Dawson DA (2009) Stable isotopes reveal individual variation in migration strategies and habitat preferences in a suite of seabirds during the nonbreeding period. Oecologia 160: 795–806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Quillfeldt P, McGill RAR, Furness RW (2005) Diet and foraging areas of Southern Ocean seabirds and their prey inferred from stable isotopes: review and case study of Wilsons storm-petrel. Marine Ecology Progress Series 295: 295–304. [Google Scholar]
- 88. Page B, McKenzie J, Goldsworthy SD (2005) Dietary resource partitioning among sympatric New Zealand and Australian fur seals. Marine Ecology Progress Series 293: 283–302. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.