Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Sep 27.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2008 Jan 29;38(4):511–522. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002778

Risk factors for Post Traumatic Stress Disorder amongst United Kingdom Armed Forces personnel

Amy C Iversen 1,*, Nicola T Fear 2, Anke Ehlers 3, Jamie Hacker Hughes 2, Lisa Hull 1, Mark Earnshaw 2, Neil Greenberg 1, Roberto Rona 1, Simon Wessely 1, Matthew Hotopf 1
PMCID: PMC3785135  EMSID: EMS53525  PMID: 18226287

Abstract

Background

Understanding the factors which increase the risk of PTSD for military personnel is important. This study aims to investigate the relative contribution of pre-deployment, peri-deployment, and post deployment variables to the prevalence of post traumatic stress symptoms in UK Armed Forces personnel who have been deployed in Iraq since 2003.

Method

Data are drawn from stage 1 of a retrospective cohort study comparing a random sample of UK military personnel deployed to the 2003 Iraq War with a control group who were not deployed to the initial phase of war fighting (response rate 61%). The analyses are limited to 4762 regular service individuals who responded and who deployed to Iraq since 2003.

Results

Post traumatic stress symptoms were associated with lower rank, being unmarried, low educational attainment and a history of childhood adversity. Exposure to potentially traumatising events was associated with post traumatic stress symptoms. Appraisals of the experience as involving threat to life or that work in theatre was above an individual’s trade and experience were strongly associated with post traumatic stress symptoms Low morale, poor social support within the unit and non-receipt of a homecoming brief were associated with greater risk of post traumatic stress symptoms.

Conclusions

These results support that there are modifiable occupational factors which may influence an individual’s risk of PTSD. Personal appraisal of threat to life during the trauma emerged as the strongest predictor of symptoms, and therefore interventions focused on reinstating a sense of control are an important focus for treatment.

Introduction

There is a consistent body of research showing that exposure to combat is associated with an increased risk of post-deployment psychiatric injury (Kulka et al. 1990; Lee et al. 1995). After the recent conflict in Iraq, high rates of post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) have been reported in US combat troops (Hoge et al. 2006). UK personnel returning from Iraq have a lower prevalence of PTSD than their US counterparts but there is a clear association between combat role and increased risk of PTSD (Hotopf et al. 2006). PTSD is a major concern to the military because of the considerable disability and co-morbidity associated with the disorder (Zatzick et al. 1997; Kessler 2000), which has implications both for the individual affected, for the employer, and for society in general. More research is needed to understand modifiable factors in the military environment which reduce the risk of PTSD.

The introduction of the diagnostic category of PTSD in 1980 stated that war trauma alone was a sufficient cause of long term disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 1980). Like its predecessors that view was too absolute, and since 1980 there has been a gradual re-exploration of the importance of individual factors associated with PTSD. Meta-analyses have identified several risk factors for PTSD in both the military and the general population.

In terms of pre-trauma vulnerability factors, Brewin et al have identified factors such as previous psychiatric history, early childhood adversity and a family history of mental illness as independent risk factors for PTSD(Brewin et al. 2000). Other individual factors such as low education, previous trauma, female gender, ethnic minority status and younger age, have also been shown to be associated with PTSD in both the military (Riddle et al. 2007) and in the general population (Brewin et al. 2000; Ozer et al. 2003). Several authors report that degree of exposure to potentially traumatic events and severity of trauma is associated with risk of PTSD (Brewin et al. 2000; Hoge et al. 2004). Objective degree of trauma exposure may, however, be less predictive of PTSD than the individuals’ appraisals of the trauma and its aftermath (Ehlers and Clark 2000). Such appraisals have been shown to predict PTSD over and above objective trauma severity (Halligan et al. 2003) and perceived threat to life during trauma is among the best established predictors of PTSD (Ozer et al. 2003). There is also evidence to suggest that an individual’s sense of unpredictability and uncontrollability during a traumatic situation increases the risk of PTSD (Basoglu et al. 2005; Basoglu et al. 2007). Conversely a sense of preparedness in political torture victims is related to less perceived distress during torture and less severe psychological sequelae afterwards (Basoglu et al. 1997).

The recovery environment after trauma exposure may act as a protective factor. Social support is associated with lower PTSD risk in the general population (Brewin et al. 2000; Ozer et al. 2003) and military settings (Green and Berlin 1987; Solomon et al. 1988; Green et al. 1990; Solomon et al. 1990; Solomon et al. 1991; Sutker et al. 1995; King et al. 1998; Neria et al. 1998), as well as non-military occupational settings such as the police (Carlier et al. 1997).

In this study we used a large, randomly selected, military cohort to examine systematically the factors that are associated with PTSD in the UK Armed Forces. We investigated the relationship between PTSD and pre-trauma vulnerability factors (such as childhood adversity), objective deployment variables (such as number of potentially traumatic events to self and others) and their subjective appraisals (such as perceived threat to life), and homecoming experiences. We were especially interested in the role of organisational factors specific to the military including what impact group-cohesiveness and morale within the unit, the receipt of a homecoming brief and the practice of ‘decompression’ (time spent in base location before discharge) have on the risk of PTSD.

Methods

Study design and sample

The methods have been described in a previous paper (Hotopf et al. 2006). Briefly, the study is the first wave of a retrospective cohort study comparing UK military personnel who were deployed on the first phase of the Iraq War between 18th January and 28th April 2003 with personnel serving in the Armed Forces on 31st March 2003, but not deployed at this time (the ‘Era’ cohort). Sampling was stratified by Service (Naval Services, Army and Royal Air Force) and enlistment status (regulars and reservists) with reservists being oversampled by a ratio of 2:1. Because we have previously shown an interaction between reservist status and deployment (Hotopf et al. 2006), explored in greater depth in a subsequent paper (Browne et al. 2006), we limit the present analyses to regular personnel. Further, since deployment to Iraq was the focus of interest for the present paper, we have excluded those from the ‘Era’ cohort who did not deploy to Iraq on any of the subsequent roulements since 28th April 2003.

Data collection and follow up

Participants were approached irrespective of whether or not they had left the Armed Forces, either by a postal questionnaire, or where feasible, with visits to over 80 military bases. A range of approaches including three mailings and multiple base visits were used to follow up non-participants (see (Hotopf et al. 2006) for full details). The final participation rate was 61%, with younger, lower rank and male Service personnel being least likely to respond.

Variables

Participants were sent a detailed 26 page questionnaire booklet. This included information that participation in the survey was voluntary, and that the research was being conducted independently of the UK Ministry of Defence. For the ‘Era’ cohort, the participant was asked to complete the questionnaire for their most recent deployment, thus it was possible to gain information on deployment experiences for individuals who had served on later Iraq deployments.

For the purposes of the present paper, we focus mainly on variables collected which cover experiences on deployment; experiences following deployment, information on current health; and background information, including past medical history and adversity in childhood. Questions about experiences on deployment included information on the individual’s main duty in theatre, which is classified as combat (e.g. armour, infantry and artillery), combat support (e.g. signals, engineering) and combat service support (e.g. medical, administrative, and logistic). Participants were asked how long they had served in a “forward” area in close contact with the enemy. They were then asked about a series of potentially traumatic experiences: e.g. discharging their weapon in direct combat, handling bodies, aiding the wounded and seeing personnel wounded or killed, experiencing landmine attacks, coming under mortar or artillery fire, or experiencing hostility from civilians. Two questions aimed at assessing appraisals that may increase the risk of PTSD were included; “thinking that I might be killed” (perceived threat to life) and “work in theatre matched my trade and experiences” (perceived preparedness). We classified the trauma variables into three groups of experience: (1) “high risk to self events” (e.g. coming under small arm fire); (2) “trauma to others events” (e.g. aiding wounded, or handling bodies) and (3) “appraisal of deployment experiences” (‘perceived preparedness’ and “perceived threat to life”).

Participants were also asked a series of questions about morale and the level of support and sense of comradeship they experienced within their unit adapted from the US Deployment Experiences Survey (2002) including statements such as “I felt a sense of comradeship (or closeness) between myself and other people in my unit” which were endorsed on a Likert scale. Relevant items post deployment included whether the individual spent time in barracks after returning from deployment before going home (known as decompression), whether they received a homecoming brief, and a number of questions related to the end of deployment including feeling proud of the contribution made on whilst deployed.

Finally, a series of questions were included about experiences in childhood. These have been described in depth in another paper (Iversen et al. 2007) but consist of 16 questions with the stem: “when I was growing up…”. Participants were given a choice of answering true or false to each item. Care was taken to include both protective and adverse experiences in childhood, with examples including: coming from a close family, playing truant from school, or being hit by parents or caregivers regularly. For the purposes of this study we use a composite score of adverse childhood events with higher scores indicating greater adversity (items were reverse scored as appropriate).

Symptoms of PTSD were measured using the 17 item National Center for PTSD Checklist (PCL-C)(Blanchard et al. 1996). We defined cases on the PCL as scoring positive if the total score was 50 or more(Weathers et al. 1993). This cut-off was chosen because it is the standard cut-off for this scale, and also because we have used a cut-off of 50 in our previous large scale UK military studies, allowing direct comparison with data from previous conflicts. For brevity’s sake, we refer in the remainder of this article to those who score above this threshold as having PTSD, whilst acknowledging that a proportion of such cases would not have met criteria for the diagnosis in a semi-structured clinical interview. We used a slightly different definition of PCL ‘caseness’ compared to that used in the main previous US study of Iraq veterans(Hoge et al. 2004) which required a score of greater than 50, that the participant scored moderate or above on one of the re-experiencing symptoms, three avoidance symptoms, and two hyperarousal symptoms. We have analysed the main findings of our cohort study using both our own UK definition of PCL ‘caseness’ and the US version of PCL ‘caseness’ described above, and there were no significant differences in our results between these two approaches(Hotopf et al. 2006).

Statistical analyses

Analyses were performed in STATA 9. The analysis presented compares individuals who score above threshold of 50 on the PCL-C with individuals who score below the threshold (controls). The relative distribution of independent variables (socio-demographic, trauma, morale and childhood) between PCL-cases and controls are expressed as percentages and odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals, derived from logistic regression analysis. Multivariate models controlled for known confounders and other exposures. Model adequacy was tested using a specification test (linktest command in STATA) and a goodness of fit using the Hosmer-Lemeshow test. Where these indicated that the logistic model did not provide an adequate description of the data, we performed further analyses, including quadratic and cubic terms for continuous variables in the models. As there were a large number of potential exposure variables, we simplified some by using principal components analysis and deriving factor loadings.

Results

The final sample consisted of the 4762 individuals who responded to our survey, were regulars, and served in any Iraq War deployment.

Socio-demographic characteristics and pre-enlistment vulnerability are described in Table 1. This shows strong associations between PTSD and younger age, low rank, low educational attainment, and not being in a relationship. There is also an association between PTSD and Service, with those in the Royal Navy and Army experiencing greater symptoms than those in the Royal Air Force. The Royal Marine Commandos (elite Naval Service combat troops) had a relatively low prevalence of PTSD. There was no gender difference in the distribution of PTSD. There is a strong association between total adverse childhood experiences and PTSD. In the multivariable model there remained relatively powerful effects of Service, rank, educational status, and marital status.

Table 1. Pre-Deployment: Distribution of participants and PTSD cases by socio-demographic variables and childhood adversity, number (%) and odds ratios (OR) (and 95% confidence intervals (CI)).

Number of
participants (%)
Number of PTSD
cases (%)
Unadjusted OR
(95% CI)
Adjusted# OR
(95% CI)

Age (yrs)
<25 1003 (21.5) 57 (5.7) 1 1
25-29 1105 (23.7) 48 (4.3) 0.75 (0.51-1.12) 1.03 (0.66-1.59)
30-34 1050 (22.5) 31 (3.0) 0.50 (0.32-0.79) 0.70 (0.41-1.20)
35-39 876 (18.8) 25 (2.9) 0.49 (0.30-0.79) 0.64 (0.35-1.16)
40+ 628 (13.5) 16 (2.6) 0.43 (0.25-0.76) 0.93 (0.46-1.85)
P-trend <0.0001 0.209

Sex
Male 4300 (92.2) 165 (3.8) 1 1
Female 362 (7.8) 12 (3.3) 0.85 (0.47-1.56) 0.99 (0.54-1.83)

Service
Royal Navy 476 (10.2) 19 (4.0) 0.85 (0.52-1.39) 0.90 (0.53-1.52)
Royal Marines 245 (5.3) 5 (2.0) 0.42 (0.17-1.05) 0.42 (0.17-1.05)~
Army 3009 (64.5) 140 (4.7) 1 1
Royal Air Force 932 (20.0) 13 (1.4) 0.29 (0.16-0.51) 0.33 (0.18-0.61)

Rank
Other ranks 923 (20.0) 48 (5.2) 5.14 (2.41-10.9) 1.91 (0.81-4.51)
Non-commissioned
officer
2942 (63.7) 118 (4.0) 3.91 (1.90-8.04) 2.39 (0.92-6.24)
Commissioned officer 757 (16.4) 8 (1.1) 1 1

Educational status*
None 347 (7.8) 29 (8.4) 2.04 (1.32-3.16) 2.07 (1.32-3.23)
GCSE or equivalent 2062 (46.4) 88 (4.3) 1 1
‘A’ level of equivalent 1362 (30.7) 45 (3.3) 0.77 (0.53-1.11) 0.99 (0.65-1.40)
Degree or above 669 (15.1) 7 (1.1) 0.24 (0.11-0.51) 0.48 (0.19-1.19)
P-trend <0.0001 0.002

Marital status
Married/cohabiting 3527 (75.9) 113 (3.2) 1 1
Single 876 (18.9) 48 (5.5) 1.75 (1.23-2.48) 1.36 (0.92-2.00)
Divorced, widowed or
separated
243 (5.2) 15 (6.2) 1.99 (1.14-3.46) 2.06 (1.16-3.65)

Childhood adversity
score
0/1 1055 (23.8) 23 (2.2) 1.00 1
2/3 1436 (32.3) 27 (1.9) 0.86 (0.49-1.51) 0.81 (0.45-1.45)
4/5 876 (19.7) 39 (4.5) 2.09 (1.23-3.52) 1.53 (0.87-2.69)
6+ 1074 (24.2) 78 (7.3) 3.51 (2.19-5.63) 2.39 (1.41-4.00)
*

GCSEs are examinations usually taken at 16. A levels are usually taken at 18 and are required for entry to university

#

Model adjusts for all variables in table simultaneously except for childhood adversity.Childhood Adversity is adjusted for age (continuous), sex, rank, educational status, Service,and marital status

~

P=0.063

Table 2 describes deployment experiences in Iraq, potentially traumatic events, and appraisal of events. Deploying in a combat role was associated with PTSD, as were deploying to a forward area in contact with the enemy. Potentially traumatic experiences that involved threat to the self or others were related to PTSD, with odds ratios ranging from 1.6-3.1 and all but four being statistically significant. There was little discernable difference in the strength of “risk to self” as opposed to “trauma to others” events. There was a dose-response effect with ‘increased time in a forward area’ and increased ‘risk to self’ events being associated with higher odds of PTSD. The multivariable analysis made little difference to these associations. Perceived threat to life and ill-preparedness for the experiences during deployment were strongly related to PTSD, with odds ratios of 5.1 and 3.8.

Table 2. During Deployment: Experiences during deployment and their association with PTSD, number (%) of participants, number (%) of PTSD cases and odds ratios (OR) (and 95% confidence intervals (CI)).

Number of
participants (%)
Number of PTSD
cases (%)
Unadjusted OR
(95% CI)
Adjusted* OR
(95% CI)

Nature of duty
Combat 1306 (28.7) 75 (5.7) 1 1
Combat support 554 (12.2) 14 (2.5) 0.43 (0.24-0.76) 0.60 (0.33-1.09)
Combat service support 2693 (59.2) 84 (3.1) 0.53 (0.38-0.73) 0.71 (0.50-1.01)

Deployed with parent unit
No 1449 (32.1) 54 (3.7) 1 1
Yes 3060 (67.9) 117 (3.8) 1.03 (0.73-1.42) 0.74 (0.54-1.06)~

Time spent in a “forward area”
None 1858 (41.7) 24 (1.3) 1 1
Up to 1 week 577 (12.9) 25 (4.3) 3.46 (1.96-6.12) 2.71 (1.47-5.00)
1 week-1 month 865 (19.4) 41 (4.7) 3.80 (2.28-6.33) 3.35 (1.95-5.75)
>1 month 1158 (26.0) 79 (6.8) 5.59 (3.52-8.89) 4.52 (2.75-7.43)

“Risk to self” events

Discharged weapon in direct combat 775 (17.1) 62 (8.0) 2.96 (2.14-4.10) 2.57 (1.81-3.66)

Came under small arm fire 1600 (34.3) 107 (6.7) 3.11 (2.28-4.24) 2.65 (1.85-3.79)

Came under mortar, SCUD, artillery
fire
2371 (50.9) 108 (4.6) 1.56 (1.14-2.12) 1.39 (0.99-1.95)

Experienced landmine strike 207 (4.4) 12 (5.8) 1.61 (0.88-2.94) 1.25 (0.66-2.37)

Experienced hostility from civilians 2121 (45.5) 121 (5.7) 2.73 (1.97-3.79) 2.14 (1.49-3.09)

Number of “risk to self” events
0 1207 (26.7) 21 (1.7) 1.00 1
1 1258 (27.8) 34 (2.7) 1.57 (0.91-2.72) 1.63 (0.91-2.95)
2 940 (20.8) 33 (3.5) 2.05 (1.18-3.58) 1.90 (1.01-3.57)
≥3 1121 (24.8) 81 (7.2) 4.40 (2.70-7.16) 3.70 (2.06-6.64)

“Trauma involving others” events

Saw personnel killed or wounded (any) 2076 (44.5) 113 (5.4) 2.30 (1.68-3.16) 2.04 (1.45-2.88)
Saw UK/allied forces killed/wounded 1152 (24.7) 62 (5.4) 1.69 (1.23-2.32) 1.58 (1.13-2.20)
Saw enemy forces killed or wounded 1136 (28.7) 77 (5.8) 1.99 (1.46-2.70) 1.81 (1.30-2.53)
Saw civilians killed or wounded 1220 (26.2) 79 (6.5) 2.39 (1.75-3.24) 2.13 (1.53-2.95)

Handled bodies (any) 675 (14.5) 43 (6.4) 1.97 (1.38-2.81) 1.82 (1.26-2.64)
Handled UK/allied forces bodies 283 (6.1) 16 (6.6) 1.58 (0.93-2.68) 1.73 (1.01-2.96)
Handled enemy forces bodies 413 (8.9) 30 (7.3) 2.20 (1.46-3.31) 1.96 (1.27-3.01)
Handled civilian bodies 428 (9.2) 32 (7.5) 2.29 (1.54-3.41) 2.05 (1.35-3.10)

Aided wounded (any) 785 (16.8) 58 (7.4) 2.54 (1.84-3.52) 2.49 (1.76-3.51)
Aided UK/allied forces wounded 559 (12.0) 45 (8.1) 2.65 (1.87-3.77) 2.82 (1.96-4.07)
Aided enemy forces wounded 304 (6.5) 21 (6.9) 2.01 (1.25-3.22) 2.14 (1.30-3.53)
Aided civilian wounded 577 (12.4) 40 (6.9) 2.16 (1.50-3.11) 2.14 (1.46-3.13)

Appraisals of deployment experiences

Thought might be killed 2583 (57.0) 149 (5.8) 5.11 (3.28-8.00) 4.56 (2.86-7.28)

Work in theatre matched trade and
experiences
Yes 3513 (84.0) 114 (3.3) 1 1
No, above 185 (4.4) 21 (11.4) 3.82 (2.34-6.23) 3.38 (2.02-5.67)
No, below 322 (7.7) 14 (4.4) 1.35 (0.77-2.39) 1.38 (0.77-2.45)
No, outside 161 (3.9) 10 (6.2) 1.97 (1.01-3.85) 1.83 (0.93-3.61)
*

Adjusted for age (continuous), sex, rank, educational status, Service, marital status

Table 3 describes the associations between variables relating to perceived social support and morale during deployment, homecoming experiences and PTSD. For each morale variable, there is a relatively strong association with PTSD, and the single factor generated from principal components analysis showed a strong, dose response relationship with PTSD, which was not greatly altered by controlling for demographic variables. There was an association between non-receipt of a homecoming brief and PTSD, with those who did not receive it having more PTSD symptoms. There was no association between PTSD and spending time at a military base before being granted leave – so called “decompression” Individuals who reported going home to their families directly after deployment were found to have the same prevalence of PTSD as those who stayed on bases.

Table 3. Post-Deployment: Social support or morale on deployment, homecoming experiences and their association with PTSD, number (%) of participants, number (%) of PTSD cases and odds ratios (OR) (and 95% confidence intervals (CI)).

Number of
participants (%)
Number of PTSD
cases (%)
Unadjusted Odds
ratio (95% CI)
Adjusted* OR
(95% CI)

Social support and morale

I felt a sense of
comradeship with
unit
Agree/strong agree 3769 (82.8) 130 (3.5) 1 1
Neutral 533 (11.7) 24 (4.5) 1.32 (0.85-2.06) 1.22 (0.76-1.96)
Disagree/strongly
disagree
250 (5.5) 20 (8.0) 2.43 (1.49-3.97) 2.31 (1.37-3.90)

I could go to most
people with a
personal problem
Agree/strong agree 2238 (49.2) 60 (2.7) 1 1
Neutral 1057 (23.2) 33 (3.1) 1.17 (0.76-1.80) 1.27 (0.82-1.97)
Disagree/strongly
disagree
1256 (27.6) 81 (6.5) 2.50 (1.78-3.52) 2.54 (1.77-3.65)

My seniors were
interested in what I
did or thought
Agree/strong agree 2559 (56.3) 51 (2.0) 1 1
Neutral 935 (20.6) 41 (4.4) 2.25 (1.48-3.42) 2.04 (1.33-3.15)
Disagree/strongly
disagree
1054 (23.2) 82 (7.8) 4.15 (2.90-5.93) 3.47 (2.38-5.05)

I felt well informed
about what was
going on
Agree/strong agree 2532 (55.6) 62 (2.5) 1 1
Neutral 827 (18.2) 34 (4.1) 1.71 (1.11-2.61) 1.50 (0.96-2.35)
Disagree/strongly
disagree
1195 (26.2) 78 (6.5) 2.78 (1.98-3.91) 2.39 (1.67-3.41)

Tertiles of “morale”
factor
Highest 1633 (36.0) 32 (2.0) 1.00 1
Middle 1409 (31.1) 38 (2.7) 1.39 (0.86-2.23) 1.41 (0.87-2.29)
Lowest 1496 (33.0) 104 (7.0) 3.74 (2.50-5.59) 3.23 (2.13-4.90)

Homecoming

Time spent on base
location before
deployment leave
straight on leave
1 week or less 1685 (37.8) 51 (3.0) 1 1
1-2 weeks 1261 (28.3) 52 (4.1) 1.38 (0.93-2.04) 1.00 (0.64-1.55)
2+ weeks 982 (22.0) 45 (4.6) 1.54 (1.02-2.32) 0.98 (0.61-1.56)
529 (11.9) 16 (3.0) 1.00 (0.56-1.77) 0.65 (0.35-1.22)

Non-receipt of
homecoming brief
1678 (45.5) 77 (4.6) 1.29 (0.93-1.80) 1.84 (1.30-2.62)
*

Adjusted for age (continuous), sex, rank, educational status, Service, marital status

We derived an interaction term to determine whether any specific groups would be at increased risk from being in a forward area for a prolonged period of time. Because of the risk of type 1 error from testing multiple interactions we limited the analyses to three interaction terms (rank, morale and childhood adversity by time in forward area). No statistically significant interaction was detected for morale and childhood adversity, but a statistically significant interaction was found between rank and duration of time in a forward area – those from lower ranks having a greater risk of PTSD if in a forward area for a prolonged time than commissioned officers (chi2= 11.9, 2df, p=0.003).

Table 4 shows a series of models involving pre-deployment, deployment, and post-deployment variables. A single hierarchical regression analysis involving 3 models is presented. Pre-deployment variables were entered in model 1, deployment variables in model 2, and post-trauma variables (homecoming experiences) in model 3. Due to the presence of a statistically significant interaction with rank and time spent in a forward area, we present the results of this analysis excluding officers. The full model shows that the following variables are independently associated with PTSD symptoms: spending longer in a forward area; perceiving one’s work to be above one’s trade and experience; perceiving that one might be killed; witnessing trauma to others; experiencing low morale on deployment; and having more adverse experiences in childhood. It is noteworthy that the effect of experiencing specific events which placed the individual at risk was no longer associated in the full model. This variable was strongly correlated with time in a forward area (p<0.001), however there was no effect for self-risk events even when time in a forward area was omitted from the model (data not shown).

Table 4. Full model~: Sequential Multiple Regression including pre-deployment variables (Model 1), deployment variables (Model 2) and post-deployment variables (Model 3), odds ratios (OR) (and 95% confidence intervals (CI)).

Model 1: pre-
deployment factors
Model 2: Model 1
plus deployment
factors
Model 3: Model 2
plus post-
deployment factors

Childhood adversity
0/1 1 1 1
2/3 0.95 (0.55-1.61) 0.91 (0.52-1.62) 0.87 (0.48-1.56)
4/5 1.84 (1.14-2.94) 1.37 (0.80-2.33) 1.26 (0.73-2.18)
6+ 3.22 (2.01-5.15)* 2.35 (1.39-3.98) 2.34 (1.38-4.00)

Time spent in a
“forward area”
None 1 1
Up to 1 week 2.16 (1.05-4.44) 2.05 (0.99-4.25)
1 week-1 month 2.45 (1.25-4.80) 2.05 (1.03-4.08)
>1 month 3.33 (1.75-6.36) 3.10 (1.60-5.97)

Work in theatre
matched trade and
experience
Yes 1 1
No, above 2.65 (1.50-4.70) 2.72 (1.51-4.90)
No, below 1.31 (0.70-2.47) 1.18 (0.59-2.32)
No, outside 1.06 (0.47-2.39) 0.89 (0.37-2.11)

Thought might be
killed
No 1 1
Yes 2.81 (1.66-4.78) 3.01 (1.73-5.24)

Number of “risk to
self” events
0 1 1
1 1.11 (0.55-2.26) 0.98 (0.48-2.02)
2 0.76 (0.35-1.67) 0.68 (0.30-1.51)
≥3 1.00 (0.45-2.22) 0.95 (0.42-2.12)

Number of “trauma
involving others”
events
0 1 1
1 1.67 (0.94-2.96) 1.76 (0.97—3.19)
2 1.69 (0.91-3.12) 1.79 (0.94-3.40)
≥3 2.18 (1.25-3.82) 2.35 (1.32-4.20)

Tertiles of “morale”
Highest 1 1
Middle 1.54 (0.89-2.66) 1.52 (0.87-2.63)
Lowest 3.19 (1.95-5.21) 3.15 (1.92-5.18)

Deployed with parent
unit
No 1 1
Yes 0.88 (0.59-1.32) 0.96 (0.63-1.45)

Nature of duty
Combat 1 1
Combat support 1.07 (0.54-2.10) 1.0 (0.50-2.00)
Combat service
support
1.01 (0.64-1.59) 0.93 (0.58-1.47)

Non-receipt of
Homecoming brief
0.93 (0.86-1.02)

Time spent on base
location before
deployment leave
0.83 (0.68-1.02)
~

Officers were excluded due the presence of statistically significant interaction

*

NB these estimates are different from those presented in table 1 because officers were excluded from the analysis.

Model 1 is adjusted for age, sex, rank, service arm, education, and marital status.

Model 2 is adjusted for model 1 plus all variables in model 2

Model 3 is adjusted for model 1, model 2, and all variables in model 3.

Discussion

The principal findings are as follows. PTSD is more frequent in those of lower rank, who are single or separated/divorced, have lower educational attainment and a history of childhood adversity. An individual’s appraisals of their deployment experiences, both in terms of perceived that to life (might have been killed) and in terms of not being prepared for the work in theatre (work above trade and experience) showed particularly strong associations, with odds ratios of 4.56 and 3.38 respectively. In terms of post-deployment experiences, low morale, perceived disinterest by superiors, and non-receipt of a homecoming brief were all associated with PTSD. When all variables were considered together in one model, childhood adversity, time spent in forward area, low perceived preparedness for work in theatre, perceived threat to life, number of traumas involving others, and low morale remained significant predictors.

The demographic associations reported here are consistent with previous research in the general population. Brewin et al. (2000) have previously shown that lower educational attainment is associated with PTSD. In the military, a recent large epidemiological study in the United States has indicated that a range of mental health problems including PTSD are more common in female, younger, less educated, single, white, short-term service, lower rank, Army personnel(Riddle et al. 2007). In keeping with our previous studies we found no additional risk of PTSD in females in the Armed Forces (Rona et al. 2006) in contrast to the US military studies (Adler et al. 2005; Riddle et al. 2007). This may be because females in the US military are more likely to be engaged in front-line duties than their UK counterparts.

The association of childhood adversity with PTSD has been reported in both of the large meta-analyses of PTSD (Brewin et al. 2000; Ozer et al. 2003), and is also described in both the UK (Iversen et al. 2007) and the United States military population (Bremner et al. 1993; Cabrera et al. 2007). This may be explained by psychosocial (Koenen et al. 2007), genetic (Koenen et al. 2003) and neurobiological mechanisms (De Bellis et al. 1999). New traumatic experiences may also reactivate earlier memories of childhood trauma. Previous work suggests that early adversity may predispose an individual to PTSD by a ‘double-hit’; not only are they more likely to get PTSD with any given traumatic exposure, but they are also more likely to be exposed to trauma in a combat situation (King et al. 1996).

In common with published meta-analyses (Brewin et al. 2000; Ozer et al. 2003), in the full model, peri-trauma variables emerge as more important than the individual pre-trauma variables discussed above. Time spent in a forward area has an important association with PTSD in our study; more important than having a combat role. It may be that being in a forward area increases the risk of PTSD risk by biological (e.g., exhaustion, sleep deprivation) and psychological (e.g. high states of vigilance) mechanisms. The effect of time spent in a forward area is more marked for lower ranks than for officers. We have reported previously that elite forces (paratroopers) have low rates of PTSD (Hacker Hughes et al. 2005). This is probably because both officers and members of elite forces are in general more experienced combatants with higher level of group cohesion, fitness and general motivation. There may also be a differential selection process – officers who are having difficulty coping are taken away from the front line more promptly than other ranks. It is important to note that as we use a self report of ‘time in forward area’ without further qualifying our definition of what that meant. It may be that what we are measuring here is appraisal of risk in a combat situation.

As in Ozer’s metaanalysis, perceived threat to life emerged as one of the most important predictors of PTSD. When perceived threat to life and number of risk to self events were considered together in the full model, only threat to life remained significant. This is line with earlier research suggesting that subjective appraisals of threat are more important for the development and maintenance of PTSD than objective trauma severity (Dunmore et al. 1999; Ehlers & Clark, 2000; Halligan et al., 2003); and with findings from other studies of war, torture, and natural disaster survivors (Basoglu et al. 1997; Basoglu et al. 2005; Basoglu et al. 2007). A further important psychological dimension appears to be perceived loss of control during the trauma and in its aftermath. Low perceived preparedness, as indicated by the perception that work in the theatre was above one’s experience, was associated with greater risk of PTSD. This may be because feeling unprepared in this way is associated with a sense of loss of control or a perceived threat to one’s autonomy at the time of trauma. Both responses are associated with higher rates of PTSD (Baum et al. 1993; Ehlers et al. 2000). The importance of loss of control as a central concept in PTSD is also supported by findings that post-trauma loss of control, and associated perceived continued threat to safety, was the most significant predictor of PTSD in war survivors, including combat veterans (Basoglu et al. 2002; Livanou et al. 2002; Basoglu et al. 2004; Basoglu et al. 2005; Salcioglu et al. 2007).

In this study, those whose work in the theatre was above their experience had a greater risk of PTSD. This may be because feeling unprepared in this way is associated with a sense of loss of control or a perceived threat to one’s autonomy at the time of trauma. Both responses are associated with higher rates of PTSD (Baum et al. 1993; Ehlers et al. 2000), and perceived preparedness reduces the risk of PTSD in torture survivors (Basoglu et al. 1997).

The finding that social support, feeling well informed and part of a group and good leadership were associated with PTSD has practical implications because they are potentially modifiable factors within the occupational environment. Social support is likely to help individuals cope with potentially traumatic experiences because positive and close-knit social environments provide confirmation of social identity, instrumental aid, and readily available sources of advice and feedback to group members (Cobb 1976; Cohen and Willis 1985). Effective leadership within a unit increases role clarity, self-efficacy, and job engagement and thus mitigates the effects of potential stressors (Britt et al. 2004). US work on contextual factors in the Army has shown that ‘collective efficacy’ within a unit was significantly related to reduced levels of psychological strain, job satisfaction, and commitment (Jex et al. 2001).Some of our morale and social support variables may have been tapping into other concepts. ‘A sense of comradeship’ might also reflect commitment to a group and the value system shared by the group, and this may in turn increase of psychological preparedness at times of trauma. Similarly, ‘feeling well informed about what was going on’ may result in an enhanced sense of control during trauma, which reduces the risk of PTSD. The knowledge that one could “go to most people with a personal problem” may exert its impact through enhancing one’s sense of personal efficacy. This is consistent with the additional evidence that this study provides regarding the role of preparedness and sense of control in PTSD. However, it is important to note that the association of PTSD with poorer perceived social support may also be because those who were unwell were more likely to perceive their levels of support as less helpful than those who did not develop problems. The cross-sectional nature of the data means that we are unable to discern the direction of causation.

In terms of other occupational policies, decompression (time spent in base location before discharge) appeared to make little difference to prevalence of PTSD. A period of decompression after deployment is currently standard procedure both in the UK and US military, despite little evidence for its efficacy in reducing post deployment distress or ill-health. Our own qualitative work in UK personnel has shown that it is often perceived negatively by returnees who typically described it as ‘a waste of time’(French and Dandeker, October 2005) and describe frustration and boredom. Further work is needed to ascertain whether there are any useful aspects of decompression and whether the timing of decompression makes any difference to its effectiveness.

A homecoming brief is a series of talks given to returnees which covers various aspects of post-deployment functioning including health and readjustment to life with family and friends. The talks typically last around 2 hours and are delivered either by the Chain of Command or by Community Mental Health Nurses, Padres, or Consultant Psychiatrists or Clinical Psychologists. They cover a broad range of topics including homecoming, how to reintegrate with family, common symptoms of stress, and where to seek help. They are compulsory but in exceptional circumstance an individual may be too unwell to attend. This is not the same as debriefing which encourages specific and individual exploration/reliving of any traumatic experiences. In this observational study, receipt of a homecoming brief is associated with lower prevalence of PTSD, although this association disappears in the full model. Ehlers and Clark have highlighted that catastrophic cognitions about common symptoms after trauma predispose to PTSD (e.g. ‘I am going mad’, ‘My brain must be damaged’). It may be that psycho-education at an early stage works by normalising symptoms and experiences. It is important to note that the association of PTSD with lack of a homecoming brief may also be because those who were unwell were less likely to attend such an event. The cross-sectional nature of the data means that we are unable to discern the direction of causation.

Strength and Limitations

This study is the first systematic examination of the risk factors associated with PTSD in UK Armed Forces personnel. The sample is large, random and representative of all three Services.

Our response rate of 60% is comparable to that achieved by other population based studies, especially those of urban populations dominated by young men (Eaker et al. 1998). Furthermore, we have already presented data suggesting that reduced response rates in military cohorts are largely due to practical difficulties in finding people or participant inertia (Iversen et al. 2006). A detailed analysis of non-response(Tate et al. 2007 (in press)) has demonstrated no difference between responders and non-responders in terms of medical downgrading (whether an individual is fit for duty or not) in the two years following the start of the Iraq War, and similarly a limited intensive follow up study showed that persistent non-responders did not have different health outcomes when compared with responders(Hotopf et al. 2006).

As these data are derived from cross-sectional analysis and a major limitation of this study is that is not possible to comment on the direction of causation. Future longitudinal data from the cohort will address this.

There is potential recall bias. We have previously shown that test-retest reliability of recall of deployment related hazards is poor (Wessely et al. 2003) and that changes in recall are non-random, being affected by changes in health status. However, some of the most important variables identified here are inherently subjective ones – perceived morale, perceived risk etc – rather than specific exposures/hazards. The criterion validity for the PCL are 0.82 (sensitivity) and 0.83 (specificity)(Weathers et al.1993). Despite this there are limitations to the validity of any questionnaire-based instrument for PTSD, and we intend to validate the PCL in this population in a subsequent clinical study using the gold standard for PTSD diagnosis, the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS).

Implications

Three factors reported here emerge as potential areas for intervention. The first is the increased risk of PTSD for individuals who are deployed in a role above their perceived trade and experience. More careful matching of personnel’s expertise to role in theatre, and ensuring that all troops undergo sufficiently robust training prior to deployment, may help to reduce rates of PTSD. Second, personal perceptions of threat to life during trauma appear to be robustly related to PTSD in this and earlier studies (Ozer et al., 2003).This is in line with the emphasis on threat to life in the DSM-IV definition of trauma. It remains to be tested whether there are interventions that lessen the impact of life threat. In line with the work on preparedness, one could argue that systematic preparation for such extreme situations may lessen their psychological impact. Finally, this study suggests that good leadership, fostering a sense of individual and group efficacy, and general social support within the unit may be associated with a decreased risk of PTSD.

Acknowledgements

M Earnshaw is a member of the Defence Medical Services currently seconded to King’s College London. N Greenberg is a full time active service medical officer who has been seconded to King’s College Centre for Military Health Research as a liaison officer; although paid from Ministry of Defence funds, he was not directed in any way by the Ministry in relation to this publication. J Hacker Hughes is a member of the Defence Medical Services currently seconded to King’s College London. S Wessely is Honorary Civilian Consultant Advisor on Psychiatry to the British Army.

This study was funded by the UK Ministry of Defence (MOD). We are grateful to MOD for their cooperation in the execution of this study; in particular we thank the Defence Medical Services Department, the Defence Analytical Services Agency, the single services, AFPAA and the Veteran’s Policy Unit. Although the MOD has facilitated data collection, it has had no input in the hypotheses under study, design, analysis, interpretation and publication of this study.

Footnotes

Disclosures

All other authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Adler AB, Huffman AH, Bliese PD, Castro CA. The impact of deployment length and experience on the well-being of male and female soldiers. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology. 2005;10:121–37. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.10.2.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed. Spitzer; Washington, DC: 1980. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Basoglu M, Kilic C, Salcioglu E, Livanou M. Prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder and comorbid depression in earthquake survivors in Turkey: an epidemiological study. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2004;17:133–41. doi: 10.1023/B:JOTS.0000022619.31615.e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Basoglu M, Livanou M, Crnobaric C. Torture versus other cruel, inhuman, and degrading treatment: is the distinction real or apparent? Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:277–85. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.3.277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Basoglu M, Livanou M, Crnobaric C, Franciskovic T, Suljic E, Duric D, Vranesic M. Psychiatric and cognitive effects of war in former Yugoslavia: association of lack of redress for trauma and posttraumatic stress reactions. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2005;294:580–90. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.5.580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Basoglu M, Mineka S, Paker M, Aker T, Livanou M, Gok S. Psychological preparedness for trauma as a protective factor in survivors of torture. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27:1421–33. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Basoglu M, Salcioglu E, Livanou M. Traumatic stress responses in earthquake survivors in Turkey. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2002;15:269–76. doi: 10.1023/A:1016241826589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baum A, Cohen L, Hall M. Control and intrusive memories as possible determinants of chronic stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1993;55:274–86. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199305000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blanchard EB, Jones A, Buckley T, Forneros C. Psychometric properties of the PTSD check list (PCL) Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1996;74:669–673. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(96)00033-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bremner J, Southwick S, Johnson D, Yehuda R, Charney D. Childhood physical abuse and combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;150:235–239. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.2.235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brewin C, Andrews B, Valentine J. Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma exposed adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:748–766. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.68.5.748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Britt TW, Davison J, Bliese PD, Castro CA. How leaders can influence the impact that stressors have on soldiers. Military Medicine. 2004;169:541–5. doi: 10.7205/milmed.169.7.541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Browne T, Hull L, Horn O, Murphy D, Fear N, Greenberg N, French C, Rona R, Wessely S, Hotopf M. Explanations for the increase in mental health problems in UK Reserve Forces who have served in Iraq. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;190:484–489. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.030544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cabrera OA, Hoge CW, Bliese P, Castro CA, Messer SC. Childhood Adversity and Combat: Effects on Depression and Post Traumatic Stress. American Journal of Preventative Medicine. 2007;33:77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Carlier IV, Lamberts RD, Gersons BP. Risk factors for posttraumatic stress symptomatology in police officers: a prospective analysis. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1997;185:498–506. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199708000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cobb S. Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1976;38:300–314. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197609000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cohen S, Willis TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin. 1985;98:310–357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. De Bellis MD, Baum AS, Birmaher B, Keshavan MS, Eccard CH, Boring AM, Jenkins FJ, Ryan ND. “A.E. Bennett Research Award. Developmental traumatology. Part I: Biological stress systems. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;45:1259–70. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00044-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dunmore E, Clark DM, Ehlers A. Cognitive factors involved in the onset and maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after physical or sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1999;37:809–29. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00181-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Eaker S, Bergstron R, Bergstrom A, Adami H, Nyren O. Response rate to mailed epidemiologic questionnaires: a population-based randomized trial of variations in design and mailing routines. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1998;147:74–82. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ehlers A, Clark DM. A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2000;38:319–45. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00123-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ehlers A, Maercker A, Boos A. Posttraumatic stress disorder following political imprisonment: the role of mental defeat, alienation, and perceived permanent change. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109:45–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. French C, Dandeker C. UK military families and the deployments to Iraq: Preliminary findings from a pre-, during-, and post-deployment study of the British Army; Inter-University Seminar on Armed Forces and Society; Chicago, USA. October 2005. [Google Scholar]
  24. Green B, Grace M, Lindy J, Gleser G, Leonard A. Risk factors for PTSD and other diagnoses in a general sample of Vietnam veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1990;147:729–733. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.6.729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Green MA, Berlin MA. Five psychosocial variables related to the existence of post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1987;66:507–512. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198711)43:6<643::aid-jclp2270430602>3.0.co;2-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Halligan SL, Michael T, Clark DM, Ehlers A. Posttraumatic stress disorder following assault: the role of cognitive processing, trauma memory, and appraisals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71:419–31. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.71.3.419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hoge CW, Auchterlonie JL, Milliken CS. Mental health problems, use of mental health services, and attrition from military service after returning from deployment to Iraq or Afghanistan. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2006;295:1023–32. doi: 10.1001/jama.295.9.1023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hoge CW, Castro CA, Messer SC, McGurk D, Cotting DI, Koffman RL. Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. New England Journal of Medicine. 2004;351:13–22. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa040603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hotopf M, Hull L, Fear NT, Browne T, Horn O, Iversen A, Jones M, Murphy D, Bland D, Earnshaw M, Greenberg N, Hacker Hughes J, Tate AR, Dandeker C, Rona R, Wessely S. The health of UK military personnel who deployed to the 2003 Iraq war: a cohort study. Lancet. 2006;367:1731–41. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68662-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hacker Hughes J, Cameron F, Eldridge R, Devon M, Wessely S, Greenberg N. Going to war does not have to hurt: preliminary findings from the British deployment to Iraq. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;186:536–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.6.536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Iversen A, Fear NT, Hull L, Greenberg N, Jones M, Browne T, Murphy D, Hotopf M, Rona R, Wessely S. Pre-enlistment Vulnerability Factors and their Influence on Health Outcomes amongst UK Military Personnel. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2007 in press. [Google Scholar]
  32. Iversen A, Liddell K, Fear N, Hotopf M, Wessely S. Consent, confidentiality, and the Data Protection Act. British Medical Journal. 2006;332:165–9. doi: 10.1136/bmj.332.7534.165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jex SM, Bliese PD, Buzzell S, Primeau J. The impact of self-efficacy on stressor-strain relations: coping style as an explanatory mechanism. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2001;86:401–9. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kessler RC. Posttraumatic stress disorder: the burden to the individual and to society. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2000;61(Suppl 5):4–12. discussion 13-4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. King DW, King LA, Foy DW, Gudanowski DM. Prewar factors in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder: structural equation modeling with a national sample of female and male Vietnam veterans. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:520–31. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.3.520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. King LA, King DW, Fairbank JA, Keane TM, Adams GA. Resilience-recovery factors in post-traumatic stress disorder among female and male Vietnam veterans: hardiness, postwar social support, and additional stressful life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:420–34. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.2.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Koenen KC, Lyons MJ, Goldberg J, Simpson J, Williams WM, Toomey R, Eisen SA, True WR, Cloitre M, Wolfe J, Tsuang MT. A high risk twin study of combat-related PTSD comorbidity. Twin Research. 2003;6:218–26. doi: 10.1375/136905203765693870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Poulton R, Martin J, Caspi A. Early childhood factors associated with the development of post-traumatic stress disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:181–92. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kulka R, Schlenger W, Fairbank J, Hough R, Jordan BK, Marmar C, Weiss D. Trauma and the Vietnam War Generation: Report of Findings From the National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study. Brunner/Mazel; New York, NY: 1990. [Google Scholar]
  40. Lee K, Vaillant G, Torrey W, Elder G. A 50-year Prospective Study of the Psychological Sequelae of World War II Combat. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1995;152:516–522. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.4.516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Livanou M, Basoglu M, Marks IM, De SP, Noshirvani H, Lovell K, Thrasher S. Beliefs, sense of control and treatment outcome in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32:157–65. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701004767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Neria Y, Solomon Z, Dekel R. An eighteen-year follow-up study of Israeli prisoners of war and combat veterans. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1998;186:174–82. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199803000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ozer EJ, Best SR, Lipsey TL, Weiss DS. Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:52–73. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Riddle JR, Smith TC, Smith B, Corbeil TE, Engel CC, Wells TS, Hoge CW, Adkins J, Zamorski M, Blazer D. Millennium Cohort: the 2001-2003 baseline prevalence of mental disorders in the U.S. military. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2007;60:192–201. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rona RJ, Fear NT, Hull L, Wessely S. Women in novel occupational roles: mental health trends in the UK Armed Forces. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2006 doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Salcioglu E, Basoglu M, Livanou M. Post-traumatic stress disorder and comorbid depression among survivors of the 1999 earthquake in Turkey. Disasters. 2007;31:115–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7717.2007.01000.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Solomon Z, Margalit C, Waysman M, Bleich A. In the shadow of the Gulf war: psychological distress, social support and coping among Israeli soldiers in a high risk area. Israel Journal of Medical Sciences. 1991;27:687–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Solomon Z, Mikulincer M, Avitzur E. Coping, locus of control, social support, and combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;55:279–285. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.55.2.279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Solomon Z, Mikulincer M, Habershaim N. Life-events, coping strategies, social resources, and somatic complaints among combat stress reaction casualties. British Journal of Medical Psychology. 1990;63(Pt 2):137–48. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1990.tb01607.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sutker PB, Davis JM, Uddo M, Ditta SR. Assessment of psychological distress in Persian Gulf troops: ethnicity and gender. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1995;64:415–27. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6403_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tate AR, Jones M, Hull L, Fear NT, Rona R, Wessely S, Hotopf M. How many mailouts: could attempts to increase the response rate in the Iraq cohort study be counter-productive? BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2007 doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-7-51. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Weathers FW, Litz BT, Herman DS, Huska JA, Keane TM. The PTSD Checklist (PCL): reliability, validity, and diagnostic Utility; Annual Meeting of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies; San Antonio, Texas. 1993; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  53. Wessely S, Unwin C, Hotopf M, Hull L, Ismail K, Nicolaou V, David A. Stability of recall of military hazards over time. Evidence from the Persian Gulf War of 1991. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;183:314–22. doi: 10.1192/bjp.183.4.314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Zatzick DF, Marmar CR, Weiss DS, Browner WS, Metzler TJ, Golding JM, Stewart A, Schlenger WE, Wells KB. Posttraumatic stress disorder and functioning and quality of life outcomes in a nationally representative sample of male Vietnam veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154:1690–5. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.12.1690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES