Abstract
BACKGROUND
Race consciousness (the frequency with which one thinks about his or her own race) is a measure that may be useful in assessing whether racial discrimination negatively impacts blood pressure (BP). However, the relation between race consciousness and BP has yet to be empirically tested, especially within the context of the patient–physician relationship and medication adherence.
METHODS
Race-stratified generalized estimating equations were used to assess the relationship of race consciousness on BP, measures of the patient–physician relationship, and self-reported medication adherence, controlling for patients being nested within physicians and for patient age and sex.
RESULTS
The mean age of the patients was 61.3 years, 62% were black, and 65% were women. Black patients were more likely to ever think about race than were white patients (49% vs. 21%; P < 0.001). Race-conscious blacks had significantly higher diastolic BP (79.4 vs. 74.5mm Hg; P = 0.004) and somewhat higher systolic BP (138.8 vs. 134.7mm Hg; P = 0.13) than blacks who were not race conscious. Race-conscious whites were more likely to perceive respect from their physician (57.1% vs. 25.8%; P = 0.01) but had lower medication adherence (62.4% vs. 82.9%; P = 0.05) than whites who were not race-conscious.
CONCLUSIONS
Among blacks, race consciousness was associated with higher diastolic BP. In contrast, among whites, there was no association between race consciousness and BP, but race consciousness was associated with poor ratings of adherence, despite more favorable ratings of the patient–physician relationship. Future work should explore disparities in race consciousness and its impact on health and health-care disparities.
Keywords: adherence, blood pressure, blood pressure discrimination, disparities, hypertension, perceived quality of care, race, racism.
Blacks are disproportionately affected by hypertension, having the highest prevalence in the United States and one of the highest rates worldwide.1,2 Compared with whites, blacks are more likely to have hypertension and be treated with antihypertensive medications but are less likely to have controlled blood pressure (BP).3,4 Furthermore, blacks report lower rates of antihypertensive medication adherence.5,6
Perceived discrimination4,7–9 and impaired patient–physician relationships10,11 are 2 potential mechanisms for racial disparities in adherence and hypertension control. It has been suggested that blacks who face discrimination on a day-to-day basis may “ready themselves” to cope with overt and covert forms of racism through adoption of survival strategies.7,12,13 Clark and colleagues14 conceptualize racism as an environmental stressor that, depending on how it is perceived, activates an individual’s coping responses, resulting in a cascade of psychological and physiological responses (allostatic load) that then contribute to health outcomes. The resultant elevation of environmental vigilance and anticipatory threat may contribute to racial disparities in health generally7,8,15 and specifically to risk of hypertension among blacks.7 Furthermore, psychological distress among blacks has been independently predictive of the development of hypertension;16 thus the constant psychological vigil and heightened physiological response associated with racial discrimination among blacks may increase their risk of hypertension.7
Although perceived discrimination and individual reactions to it may have a direct impact on BP, there is also ample support for an indirect influence on BP through effects of perceived discrimination on patient adherence8 and the patient–physician relationship.10,17,18 The existing body of literature does not comprehensively examine the associations between reactions to discrimination and adherence to antihypertensive regimens, patient–provider relationships, and BP among persons with hypertension. Therefore, further investigation is necessary to elucidate these potential mechanisms for racial disparities.
Race consciousness is a novel measure of racial climate at the individual level, originally described by Jones as one potential mechanism to characterize and assess the impact of multiple dimensions of racial discrimination on health disparities.19 Among blacks, race consciousness is postulated as a heightened awareness of one’s stigmatized status, resulting in heightened vigilance and anticipatory stress toward the threat of discrimination, which could contribute to elevated rates of hypertension and other cardiovascular health issues.7,8 Indeed, recent work links anticipation of or perseveration on possible discrimination (termed “racism-related vigilance”) to disparities in sleep difficulty between blacks and whites20 and hypertension prevalence and large arterial elasticity among blacks.21,22
There is less knowledge about the meaning of race consciousness and its potential effects among whites. The experience may differ from that for blacks; however, both racial groups may experience psychological stress as a result, leading to poor health. One theory suggests that a strong white racial identity has both progressive and defensive components; thus, race consciousness could have varied meaning and reflect polarized reactions to the social environment among whites of different backgrounds.23 Given the socially dominant status of whites in the United States, higher levels of race consciousness could reflect greater awareness of white privilege.24 Another explanation, particularly among whites who reside in areas with a high black population, is that race consciousness represents heightened fear of victimization, an anxiety-provoking stressor.25 Scholars of critical race theory are still debating whether race consciousness enhances or adversely affects the health of whites.26
The clinical relevance of race consciousness and its contribution to racial disparities in hypertension is relatively unexplored. Most studies use indirect measures such as perceived racial discrimination or racial identity.27 We sought to explore race consciousness and its effect on the patient–physician relationship, medication adherence, and BP control among urban primary care patients. We hypothesized that race consciousness is a measure of vigilance and anticipatory stress among blacks and anxiety-provoked stress among whites, both of which are associated with poorer ratings of the patient–physician relationship, lower medication adherence, and higher BP.
METHODS
Study design and population
We analyzed enrollment data from the Patient–Physician Partnership (Triple P) Study, a randomized controlled trial of patient and physician interventions to improve patient–physician communication and adherence to therapy among hypertensive patients in 14 urban community-based primary care clinics in Baltimore, Maryland, serving primarily low-income and/or ethnic minority patients.28 Approximately 60%–100% of the patients in participating sites were black, and 35%–55% earned <200% of the federally defined poverty guidelines. Patients were enrolled from September 2003 to August 2005. The trial received approval from the Johns Hopkins Institutional Review Board. Informed written consent was obtained from all participating physicians and patients. The Triple P Study enrolled 279 patients with hypertension (36% white, 62% black) and 41 primary care physicians (44% white, 29% black, 24% Asian, 2% Hispanic). For this analysis, we excluded 3 Asian and 2 Native American patients and limited our analysis to black and white patients. Eight patients did not respond to the race-consciousness question, resulting in a sample size of 266 patients.
Study measures
Patient characteristics.
At enrollment, patients completed surveys on health status, sociodemographics (age, race/ethnicity, sex, educational level, income and employment status, insurance status), health literacy, experiences of stress (felt stressed sometimes, fairly often, or very often in the last month because of daily hassles or personal problems in one’s life), adherence to recommended therapies for hypertension, and trust in and perceived respect from their clinician. Patients also had a physical exam, which included height, weight, and BP measurements. Patients self-identified as belonging to 1 of 6 racial/ethnic groups: Asian, black/African American, Latino/Latin American or Hispanic, Native American/American Indian, Pacific Islander, or white). Literacy was assessed by the rapid estimate of adult literacy in medicine and was analyzed as a dichotomous variable defined a priori as ≥9th grade or <9th grade.29
Race consciousness.
The primary exposure variable was race consciousness of patients, assessed using an item from the 2002 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System “Reactions to Race” module developed by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.19 Patients were asked how often they think about their race. Two categories of race consciousness were created: ever think about one’s race (at least once per year, at least once per month, at least once per week, at least once per day, and constantly) and never think about one’s race (never category). Patients who responded “not sure” were categorized as ever think about race; we reasoned that if respondents were unsure how often, they must think about race at least sometimes.
Patient adherence.
Patient adherence was measured by self-report using a modified version of the 4-item Morisky medication adherence scale examined as a dichotomous variable of adherent (answered “no” to each of the items) or nonadherent (answered “yes” to at least 1 of the items). The Morisky scale has a sensitivity of 72% and specificity of 74% for ≥80% medication compliance.30
Patient–physician relationship.
Patient perceptions of clinicians were measured by postvisit surveys. Patients rated their attitude toward their primary care physician as “I trust this doctor to look out for my best interests” and their primary care physician’s attitude toward them as “My doctor has a great deal of respect for me” on a 5-point Likert scale. Because the responses were positively skewed, we dichotomized responses as the highest category (i.e., strongly agree) vs. all others, as has been done in previous research using these items.31
Outcome assessment: BP.
The primary outcome, BP, was measured before the baseline visit by trained and certified observers using an automatic oscillometric monitor (Omron HEM 907, Bannockburn, IL). This device programs a 5-minute delay before activation and has a 30-second delay between the triplicate measurements. The average of the 3 measurements was used. BP control was dichotomized as uncontrolled (systolic BP (SBP) ≥ 140mm Hg or diastolic BP (DBP) ≥ 90mm Hg, or SBP ≥ 130mm Hg or DBP ≥ 80mm Hg if history of diabetes or chronic kidney disease) or controlled.
Statistical analyses
Analyses were conducted on black and white patients separately. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the patients were summarized and compared across race- consciousness groups using χ2, Fisher exact, or 2-sample t tests as appropriate. Generalized estimating equations were used to control for patients being nested within physicians. We examined variables associated with BP (i.e., age, income) and the patient–physician relationship (i.e., race concordance) in previous work and included them as covariables only if they were statistically significantly associated with at least 1 outcome for the overall sample. We also conducted analyses to test for potential interactions between race consciousness and feeling stressed on the associations between race consciousness and BP. Finally, we tested whether medication adherence and perceived respect from physicians mediated the association between race consciousness and BP.
Models were initially adjusted for patient’s age and race concordance with the physician, then for age and income. Neither race concordance nor income significantly changed the estimates for race consciousness; therefore they were excluded from the final models. Race consciousness was significantly associated with sex among blacks and with stress within the last month among whites. We chose not to include stress in the final models given its potential role in the causal pathway of race consciousness and BP. The final models are adjusted for patient age and sex. Mean estimates for the continuous outcomes of BP and predicted probabilities from the logit models for dichotomous outcomes are reported. Confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated using robust estimates of the standard error. Analyses were performed using SAS versions 9.2 and 9.3 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). All reported P values are 2-sided, and significance was set at P < 0.05.
RESULTS
Characteristics of study sample
Among the 266 participants, 62% were black, and 65% were women. The majority of patients had an income <$35,000 per year (71.4%) and had a form of health insurance (90.6%) or prescription plan (93.6%). The overall mean age was 61.3 years.
Race consciousness
Almost half of black patients were likely to ever think about their race compared with about 1 in 5 white patients (49.4% vs. 21.0%; P < 0.001). Sociodemographic characteristics for blacks and whites by race consciousness status are presented in Table 1. As mentioned previously, sex was significantly associated with race consciousness among black patients. For whites, there was a significant association between race consciousness and the report of at least some stress within the last month. No other patient characteristics significantly differed by race-consciousness status.
Table 1.
Characteristic | Black patients | White patients | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ever (n = 82) | Never (n = 84) | P value | Ever (n = 21) | Never (n = 79) | P value | |
Age, years | 59.4 (10.9) | 61.0 (12.7) | 0.39 | 60.4 (9.3) | 63.9 (12.4) | 0.23 |
Sex, female | 49 (60) | 63 (75) | 0.047 | 12 (57) | 50 (63) | 0.62 |
Marital status, married | 26 (32) | 17 (20) | 0.11 | 11 (52) | 40 (51) | 1.0 |
Education | ||||||
<High school graduate | 28 (35) | 30 (36) | 1.0 | 4 (19) | 21 (27) | 0.58 |
Years | 11.9 (2.5) | 11.6 (2.4) | 0.50 | 12.6 (2.6) | 11.9 (2.1) | 0.18 |
REALM, <9th grade | 38 (48) | 34 (41) | 0.53 | 3 (14) | 18 (23) | 0.55 |
Income | ||||||
<$10,000 | 32 (41) | 36 (46) | 0.63 | 2 (10) | 22 (30) | 0.09 |
<$35,000 | 56 (72) | 64 (81) | 0.19 | 11 (52) | 49 (66) | 0.31 |
Employed | 0.53 | 0.08 | ||||
Full time | 20 (25) | 12 (15) | 5 (24) | 12 (15) | ||
Part time | 4 (5) | 6 (8) | 1 (5) | 4 (5) | ||
Retired | 23 (28) | 30 (38) | 3 (14) | 36 (46) | ||
Disabled | 19 (23) | 21 (26) | 6 (29) | 10 (13) | ||
Healthcare insurance | 75 (93) | 75 (89) | 0.59 | 21 (100) | 69 (87) | 0.12 |
Medicaid | 25 (31) | 36 (43) | 0.06 | 5 (24) | 12 (15) | 0.48 |
Medicare | 27 (34) | 38 (46) | 0.17 | 6 (29) | 31 (39) | 0.45 |
Other | 40 (49) | 36 (44) | 0.53 | 11 (55) | 51 (65) | 0.45 |
Prescription plan | 77 (96) | 78 (93) | 0.50 | 20 (95) | 72 (91) | 1.00 |
MOS-SF-12 physical component | 41.8 (11.4) | 39.3 (13.5) | 0.20 | 36.8 (13.3) | 40.2 (11.3) | 0.25 |
MOS-SF-12 mental component | 50.9 (10.8) | 52.7 (9.7) | 0.28 | 47.1 (12.7) | 49.8 (10.6) | 0.33 |
Felt stress in the last month at least sometimes | 47 (59) | 40 (49) | 0.21 | 17 (85) | 44 (56) | 0.02 |
Diabetic | 43 (54) | 44 (53) | 1.0 | 9 (43) | 20 (25) | 0.17 |
Body mass index, kg/m2 | 32.4 (7.1) | 33.9 (8.8) | 0.25 | 31.7 (7.4) | 32.7 (8.1) | 0.61 |
Race concordant clinician | 32 (39) | 29 (35) | 0.63 | 15 (71) | 45 (57) | 0.32 |
Results are mean (SD) or number (%). P values are from Fisher exact test or two-sample t-test.
Abbreviations: REALM, rapid estimate of adult literacy in medicine; MOS-SF-12, Medical Outcomes Study 12-item short form.
Association of race consciousness with BP levels and control
Table 2 shows the association of race consciousness with BP stratified by racial group. Race-conscious black patients had significantly higher DBP and somewhat, though not statistically significant, higher SBP than those who were not race conscious. There were no differences between the 2 groups among whites in SBP or DBP. Furthermore, there was no statistically significant difference in the adjusted predicted probabilities of BP control among blacks or whites who ever think about their race and those who do not (Table 3).
Table 2.
Outcome | Black patients | White patients | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ever (n = 82) | Never (n = 84) | P value | Ever (n = 21) | Never (n = 79) | P value | |
SBP, mm Hg | 138.8 (134.0–143.5) | 134.7 (131.4–137.9) | 0.13 | 139.7 (125.1–134.3) | 132.7 (127.3–138.1) | 0.47 |
DBP, mm Hg | 79.4 (76.9–81.9) | 74.5 (72.2–76.9) | 0.004 | 72.3 (68.6–76.1) | 73.4 (70.6–76.2) | 0.64 |
Data are mean estimates and 95% confidence intervals from generalized estimating equations analysis adjusting for patient age and sex and controlling for nesting of patients within physician.
Abbreviations: DBP, diastolic blood pressure; SBP, systolic blood pressure.
Table 3.
Outcome | Black patients | White patients | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ever (n = 82) | Never (n = 84) | P value | Ever (n = 21) | Never(n = 79) | P value | |
Controlled BP | 41.6 (31.6–52.3) | 42.8 (33.7–52.4) | 0.88 | 62.3 (39.9–80.5) | 60.2 (48.5–70.8) | 0.86 |
Adherent on Morisky | 52.8 (42.4–63.1) | 61.5 (50.8–71.2) | 0.14 | 62.4 (42.9–78.5) | 82.9 (77.6–87.2) | 0.05 |
Trust in clinician | 67.4 (56.5–76.7) | 59.2 (47.1–70.2) | 0.30 | 84.5 (64.3–94.3) | 68.7 (59.8–76.4) | 0.15 |
Perceived respect | 34.8 (25.3–45.7) | 32.5 (19.9–48.3) | 0.75 | 57.1 (34.0–77.5) | 25.8 (17.2–36.8) | 0.01 |
Data are predicted probabilities and 95% confidence intervals from generalized estimating equations regression analysis adjusting for patient age and sex and controlling for nesting of patients within physician.
Abbreviation: BP, blood pressure.
Association of race consciousness with self-reported medication adherence
Race-conscious black patients had somewhat lower medication adherence than black patients who were not race conscious, but this difference was not statistically significant (Table 3). Race-conscious whites had lower medication adherence than those who were not race conscious.
Association of race consciousness with the patient–physician relationship
Among blacks and whites, there were no statistically significant differences in trust between patients who were and were not race conscious. There were no differences in perceived respect between blacks who were and were not race conscious. A higher percentage of race conscious white patients perceived respect from their clinicians than whites who were not race conscious (Table 3).
Additional analyses
There were no statistically significant interactions of stress with race consciousness on the outcomes of SBP or DBP among blacks or whites. Additionally, neither medication adherence nor perceived respect mediated the association between race consciousness and BP among blacks or whites (data not shown).
DISCUSSION
Race consciousness is associated with higher DBP and may be associated with higher SBP among black but not white patients with hypertension. In contrast, race consciousness is associated with ratings of the patient–physician relationship and adherence among whites but not blacks. Our findings provide novel insights that advance current knowledge of race consciousness among black and white primary care patients and the potential role it plays in influencing cardiovascular health and the patient–physician relationship.
Similar to previous work, we found that black patients reported a higher level of race consciousness than their white counterparts. According to Jones,19 this discrepancy in itself captures the discordant nature of the racial climate that the blacks and whites in our society experience, an environment that not only confers racial disadvantage to blacks but may also accord racial advantage to whites. Interestingly, among whites in this study, race consciousness was associated with higher ratings of the patient–physician relationship but lower medication adherence. Taken together, these findings reflect the complexity in understanding race consciousness among blacks and whites. Recent studies have supported the assertion that blacks experience race consciousness as hypervigilance, whereas whites often experience it as an anxiety-provoking stressor.8,20–22 Although the experience of this measure may be discrepant for the 2 racial groups, both may similarly experience psychological stress as a result, leading to negative health behaviors and outcomes.
Our findings related to race consciousness and BP add support to an emerging body of evidence examining the association between the psychological stress related to racial discrimination and BP. Several studies have suggested biological mechanisms linking chronic social stress and cardiovascular reactivity.15,22,32,33 In particular, stress may result in sustained BP elevations through increased sympathetic tone resulting in norepinephrine-induced vasoconstriction.34 Furthermore, different types of chronic stressors, including perceived discrimination, may preferentially affect SBP and DBP.35 For instance, tasks that require active coping efforts may lead to an increase in heart rate and SBP, whereas tasks that require quiet attentiveness and vigilance may lead to decreased cardiac output as well as increased total peripheral resistance and DBP. These biological mechanisms may explain our findings and lend support to our hypothesis that in persons for whom race consciousness represents increased vigilance to situations, there may be enhanced diastolic responses, whereas in those for whom race consciousness represents efforts to cope with perceived discrimination of their ethnic group, there is greater vascular reactivity.
Black patients in this study had lower rates of antihypertensive medication adherence than their white counterparts. Although race-conscious blacks had the lowest self-reported adherence of all study groups, the association of race-consciousness and adherence was not statistically significant among blacks. Paradoxically, although race-conscious whites reported higher quality ratings of the patient–physician relationship, they also had lower levels of medication adherence than their counterparts who were not race conscious. McIntosh describes the theory of racial privilege as an existence in which whites unintentially benefit and develop a sense of entitlement from biases within contemporary American society stemming from the foundational roots of racism in US history.24 Racial privilege by whites has been associated with poor physical and mental health.36 If race consciousness among whites is associated with awareness of their privileged status in society, guilt and moral ambivalence are potential mechanisms for the negative health behaviors reported in our sample. Alternatively, race-conscious whites in our study may have experienced greater physical and social environmental stresses and fears of victimization that have been observed among whites living in areas that are predominantly black.25 This anxiety and stress may have contributed to poorer medication adherence.
Our findings on the influence of race consciousness on the patient–physician relationship differ from those demonstrated by other studies examining race itself. In these studies, minority patients report lower-quality physician interactions.10 Contrary to our hypothesis, there was no association between race consciousness and measures of the patient–physician relationship among blacks. Race-conscious blacks may not have reported stronger opinions about their physicians out of concern for the possible negative impact on ongoing health care. It has also been suggested that blacks may have developed a normative perception of racism and discrimination because of their repetitive exposures in their natural environments.7 White patients who were race conscious perceived higher levels of respect from their physicians than their counterparts who were not race conscious. Similar to the explanation we proposed previously, one could infer that some race-conscious white patients have a greater awareness of their institutional privilege within the health-care system.
Our study findings should be interpreted in the context of the following limitations. This is a cross-sectional analysis where both race consciousness and our outcome variables were measured at 1 clinical encounter. We may not have captured patients’ lifetime exposure and reactions to racism or their overall health-care experiences and health status. This limits our ability to draw causal inferences between race consciousness and the patient–physician relationship, medication adherence, or BP control. Because of the relatively small sample size and limited response distributions, the magnitude or dose–response relationship of race consciousness as a construct was not assessed. There may also have been recall bias and social desirability response bias in the self-reported measures. Our results may have limited external validity. The study included an urban population, where many individuals tend to reside in fairly racially homogenous settings with potentially segregated neighborhoods and work environments and where acculturation may influence the likelihood to report perceived racism.37 Our sample of black patients is older, has higher educational attainment, and has a higher proportion of women and our sample of white patients is older, has lower educational attainment, and has a higher proportion of women than national samples of blacks and whites with hypertension, respectively.38 Additionally, the study sample included community-based physicians enrolled in a clinical trial who may have been more motivated about communication skills and caring for minority patients than are other physicians. Moreover, patients and physicians were reasonably well known to each other, and reports of the relationship were positively skewed. Finally, our study was not designed to assess the full spectrum of biopsychosocial mechanisms connected to race consciousness that might explain our findings.
Despite these limitations, this is the first study to our knowledge to examine associations between race consciousness and the patient–physician relationship, medication adherence, and BP among patients with hypertension in urban primary care clinics. There remains no consensus on the optimum manner to measure experiences of and reactions to discrimination; prior studies using everyday measures of discriminatory experiences have shown inconsistent results on hypertension outcomes.13,39,40 However, here we provide support for the use of a measure of race consciousness to assess associations between racial climate, patient experience in actual clinical encounters, adherence to medications, and BP outcomes. Future research should seek to replicate and better understand these findings and to explore these relationships over time. Additionally, research is crucial to identify biological influences of race consciousness, including those related to upregulated sympathetic activity and stress. Identifying the magnitude of effect and mechanisms of influence could inform the development of interventions. Strategies that reduce exposures and responses to psychosocial stress among blacks and whites could be used to mitigate the influence of race consciousness on medication adherence among whites and on BP among blacks with hypertension.
DISCLOSURE
The authors reported no conflict of interest.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The findings from this study were presented at the 2012 Society of General Internal Medicine Annual Meeting (Orlando, FL) and the 2012 AcademyHealth Annual Research Meeting (Orlando, FL). This work is supported by grants from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (K24 HL083113, P50 HL0105187, and R01HL069403).
REFERENCES
- 1. American Heart Association Heart disease and stroke statistics—2012 update. A report from the American Heart Association. Circulation 2012; 125:188–197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Fields LE, Burt VL, Cutler JA, Hughes J, Roccella EJ, Sorlie P. The burden of adult hypertension in the United States 1999 to 2000: a rising tide. Hypertension 2004; 44:398–404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Ong KL, Cheung BM, Man YB, Lau CP, Lam KSL. Prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control of hypertension among United States adults 1999–2004. Hypertension 2007; 49:69–75 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Brondolo E, Love EE, Pencille M, Schoenthaler A, Ogedegbe G. Racism and hypertension: a review of the empirical evidence and implications for clinical practice. Am J Hypertens 2011; 24:518–529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Bosworth HB, Powers B, Grubber JM, Thorpe CT, Olsen MK, Orr M, Oddone EZ. Racial differences in blood pressure control: potential explanatory factors. J Gen Intern Med 2008; 23:692–698 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Hyre AD, Krousel-Wood MA, Muntner P, Kawasaki L, DeSalvo KB. Prevalence and predictors of poor antihypertensive medication adherence in an urban health clinic setting. J Clin Hypertens (Greenwich) 2007; 9:179–186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Williams DR, Neighbors H. Racism, discrimination and hypertension: evidence and needed research. Ethn Dis 2001; 11:800–816 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Williams DR, Mohammed SA. Discrimination and racial disparities in health: evidence and needed research. J Behav Med 2009; 32:20–47 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Albert MA, Williams DR. Invited commentary: discrimination—an emerging target for reducing risk of cardiovascular disease? Am J Epidemiol 2011; 173:1240–1243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. LaVeist TA, Nickerson KJ, Bowie JV. Attitudes about racism, medical mistrust, and satisfaction with care among African American and white cardiac patients. Med Care Res Rev 2000; 57:146–161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Saha S, Arbelaez JJ, Cooper LA. Patient–physician relationships and racial disparities in the quality of health care. Am J Public Health 2003; 93:1713–1719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Dressler WW, Bindon JR, Neggers YH. John Henryism, gender, and arterial blood pressure in an African American community. Psychosom Med 1998; 60:620–624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Peters RM. Racism and hypertension among African Americans. West J Nurs Res 2004; 26:612–631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, Williams DR. Racism as a stressor for African Americans: a biopsychosocial model. Am Psychol 1999; 54:805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. J Health Psychol 1997; 2:335–351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Matthews KA, Katholi CR, McCreath H, Whooley MA, Williams DR, Zhu S, Markowitz JH. Blood pressure reactivity to psychological stress predicts hypertension in the CARDIA study. Circulation 2004; 110:74–78 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Kressin NR, Orner MB, Manze M, Glickman ME, Berlowitz D. Understanding contributors to racial disparities in blood pressure control. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes 2010; 3:173–180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Greer TM. Perceived racial discrimination in clinical encounters among African American hypertensive patients. J Health Care Poor Underserved 2010; 21:251–263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Jones C. Confronting institutionalized racism. Phylon 2002; 50:7–22 [Google Scholar]
- 20. Hicken MT, Lee H, Ailshire J, Burgard SA, Williams DR. “Every Shut Eye, Ain’t Sleep”: the role of racism-related vigilance in racial/ethnic disparities in sleep difficulty. Race Soc Probl 2013; 5:100–112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Hicken MT, Lee H, Morenoff J, House JS, Williams DR. Racial/ ethnic disparities in hypertension prevalence: reconsidering the role of chronic stress. Am J Public Healthin press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Clark R, Benkert RA, Flack JM. Large arterial elasticity varies as a function of gender and racism-related vigilance in black youth. J Adolesc Health 2006; 39:562–569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Croll Paul R. Modeling determinants of white racial identity: results from a new national survey. Social Forces 2007; 86:613–642 [Google Scholar]
- 24. McIntosh P. White privilege: unpacking the invisible knapsack. Independent School 1990;49:31–35 [Google Scholar]
- 25. Schulz AJ, Zenk SN, Israel BA, Mentz G, Stokes C, Galea S. Do neighborhood economic characteristics, racial composition, and residential stability predict perceptions of stress associated with the physical and social environment? Findings from a multilevel analysis in Detroit. J Urban Health 2008; 85:642–661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Brown TN. Race, racism, and mental health: elaboration of critical race theory’s contribution to the sociology of mental health. Contemporary Justice Review 2008; 11:53–62 [Google Scholar]
- 27. Wyatt SB, Williams DR, Calvin R, Henderson FC, Walker ER, Winters K. Racism and cardiovascular disease in African Americans. Am J Med Sci 2003; 325:315–331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Cooper LA, Roter DL, Bone LR, Larson SM, Miller ER, Barr MS, Carson KA, Levine DM. A randomized controlled trial of interventions to enhance patient-physician partnership, patient adherence and high blood pressure control among ethnic minorities and poor persons: study protocol NCT00123045. Implement Sci 2009; 4:7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Davis TC, Crouch MA, Long SW, Jackson RH, Bates P, George RB, Bairnsfather LE. Rapid assessment of literacy levels of adult primary care patients. Fam Med 1991; 23:433–435 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Morisky DE, Green LW, Levine DM. Concurrent and predictive validity of a self-reported measure of medication adherence. Med Care 1986; 24(1)67–74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Cooper LA, Roter DL, Carson KA, Beach MC, Sabin JA, Greenwald AG, Inui TS. The associations of clinicians’ implicit attitudes about race with medical visit communication and patient ratings of interpersonal care. Am J Public Health 2012; 102:979–987 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Steffen PR, McNeilly M, Anderson N, Sherwood A. Effects of perceived racism and anger inhibition on ambulatory blood pressure in African Americans. Psychosom Med 2003; 65:746–750 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Pieper S, Brosschot JF. Prolonged stress-related cardiovascular activation: is there any? Ann Behav Med 2005; 30:91–103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Strogatz DS, Croft JB, James SA, Keenan NL, Browning SR, Garrett JM, Curtis AB. Social support, stress, and blood pressure in black adults. Epidemiology 1997; 8:482–487 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Thomas KS, Nelesen RA, Malcarne VL, Ziegler MG, Dimsdale JE. Ethnicity, perceived discrimination, and vascular reactivity to phenylephrine. Psychosom Med 2006; 68:692–697 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Fujishiro K. Is perceived racial privilege associated with health? Findings from the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. Soc Sci Med 2009; 68:840–844 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Finch BK, Vega WA. Acculturation stress, social support, and self-rated health among Latinos in California. J Immigr Health 2003; 5:181–186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Hertz RP, Unger AN, Cornell JA, Saunders E. Racial disparities in hypertension prevalence, awareness, and management. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165:2098–2104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Brown C, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT, Chang Y. The relation between perceived unfair treatment and blood pressure in a racially/ethnically diverse sample of women. Am J Epidemiol 2006; 164:257–262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Sims M, Diez-Roux AV, Dudley A, Gebreab S, Wyatt SB, Bruce MA, James SA, Robinson JC, Williams DR, Taylor HA. Perceived discrimination and hypertension among African Americans in the Jackson Heart Study. Am J Public Health 2012; 102:258–265 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]