Abstract
Cutaneous flexion reflexes are amongst the first behavioural responses to develop and are essential for the protection and survival of the newborn organism. Despite this, there has been no detailed, quantitative study of their maturation in human neonates. Here we use surface electromyographic (EMG) recording of biceps femoris activity in preterm (<37 weeks gestation, GA) and term (≥37 weeks GA) human infants, less than 14 days old, in response to tactile, punctate and clinically required skin-breaking lance stimulation of the heel. We show that all infants display a robust and long duration flexion reflex (>4 seconds) to a single noxious skin lance which decreases significantly with gestational age. This reflex is not restricted to the stimulated limb: heel lance evokes equal ipsilateral and contralateral reflexes in preterm and term infants. We further show that infant flexion withdrawal reflexes are not always nociceptive specific: in 29% of preterm infants, tactile stimulation evokes EMG activity that is indistinguishable from noxious stimulation. In 40% of term infants, tactile responses are also present but significantly smaller than nociceptive reflexes. Infant flexion reflexes are also evoked by application of calibrated punctate von Frey hairs (vFh), 0.8–17.2 g, to the heel. Von Frey hair thresholds increase significantly with gestational age and the magnitude of vFh evoked reflexes are significantly greater in preterm than term infants. Furthermore flexion reflexes in both groups are sensitized by repeated vFh stimulation. Thus human infant flexion reflexes differ in temporal, modality and spatial characteristics from those in adults. Reflex magnitude and tactile sensitivity decreases and nociceptive specificity and spatial organisation increases with gestational age. Strong, relatively non-specific, reflex sensitivity in early life may be important for driving postnatal activity dependent maturation of targeted spinal cord sensory circuits.
Introduction
The flexion withdrawal reflex is a fundamental sensory behaviour that protects the body from harm. In adults, it is characterised by a brief ipsilateral contraction of flexor muscles, leading to a brisk, withdrawal of the limb from the source of potential or actual damage on the body surface [1]–[3]. The mature reflex has a high stimulus threshold, normally in the noxious range, and as a result is used extensively in pain research in rodents [4], [5] and in adult man [6], [7]. The flexion reflex reflects the excitability of spinal nociceptive circuits and, as such, has led to much of our current understanding of pain processing including peripheral and central sensitization [8], [9]and descending inhibition [10], [11].
Newborn mammals display clear reflex withdrawal behaviour from noxious stimulation at birth[12] and indeed this reflex has been used to demonstrate hyperalgesia in the presence of tissue injury in human infants and rat pups [13]–[15]. Nevertheless, numerous anatomical and electrophysiological studies in rat pups have shown that the properties of nociceptive reflexes and their underlying dorsal horn nociceptive circuits differ in early postnatal life from those in adults [12], [16]–[18]. Most notably, withdrawal reflexes in rat pups have lower thresholds, in the innocuous range, and are greater in amplitude and duration than adult rats and the underlying dorsal horn neurons and reflex motoneurones have larger, disorganised cutaneous receptive fields [16], [19]–[22]. The changing properties of newborn rodent reflexes are due, at least in part, to the postnatal development of spinal sensory circuits. Newborn rat dorsal horn neurons receive stronger synaptic input from low threshold mechanoreceptor afferents than adults [21], [23] due to exuberant A fibre terminals[17], [24] (Beggs et al., 2002; Granmo et al., 2008) and immature glycinergic signalling [18], [25]. In addition, sensitization of dorsal horn cells [21] and increased fos expression in lamina II [26] are induced by repeated tactile or A fibre stimulation, both of which only occur following noxious stimulation in adults [27].
The implication of these findings for human infants is unclear. While lower cutaneous reflex thresholds have been observed in preterm infants [20], [28], [29], no quantitative analysis has been undertaken. Here, we have performed direct EMG recordings of flexor withdrawal reflex activity evoked by tactile, punctate and (clinically required) noxious stimuli in preterm and full term infants. We show that human flexion reflexes change substantially with gestational age and only gradually become nociceptive specific over postnatal life.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Participants were in-patients admitted to the Neonatal Unit and Postnatal ward at University College Hospital (UCH), London, UK. Ethical approval was obtained from the UCH research ethics committee and informed written parental consent given for each infant. For noxious stimulation studies, a clinically required heel lance for blood sampling was performed. No additional heel lances were performed for research purposes.
Table 1 summarises the demographic characteristics of the infants used in this study. Reason for admission included prematurity, respiratory distress or tachypnea, infection, jaundice, and to monitor blood sugar or establish feeds. Infants were divided into two age groups, according to the standard definition of “term” (≥ 37 weeks gestational age at the time of study) and “preterm” infants (<37 weeks gestational age at the time of study). All infants were less than 14 days postnatal age, were neurologically healthy i.e. with no suspected or confirmed clinical neurological disorders or injury and were not prescribed analgesics or sedatives at the time of the study. Infants were not eligible for inclusion in a study if there were signs of tissue damage on the lower limbs, were born with known congenital malformations or genetic conditions or in-utero opioid exposure.
Table 1. Demographic characterisation of the neonatal population.
Age at time of study | ||||
Preterm | Term | |||
<37 weeks GA | ≥37 weeks | |||
Mean (SD) | Median; range | Mean (SD) | Median; range | |
Total number in cohort | 24 | 25 | ||
Mean GA at birth, weeks | 33.1 (2.2) | 33.2; range,28.4–36.0 | 39.2 (1.7) | 38.6; range, 35.9–41.6 |
Mean GA at time of study, weeks | 34.1 (2.0) | 34.3; range,30.1–36.4 | 39.8 (1.7) | 39.0; range, 37.0–42.3 |
Mean postnatal age, days | 6.7 (3.7) | 6; range, 1–14 | 3.8 (2.8) | 3; range, 0–10 |
Mean weight at birth, g | 1911 (496) | 1838; range, 872–2758 | 3218 (682) | 3295; range, 2014–4900 |
Mean weight at time of study, g | 1855 (483) | 1825; range, 930–2780 | 3128 (688) | 3285; range, 1969–4900 |
Number of males | 17 | 12 | ||
Number of multiple gestation births | 15 | 1 | ||
No. caesarean deliveries | 22 | 11 | ||
No. spontaneous vaginal deliveries | 2 | 14 | ||
Condition at birth | ||||
Mean Apgar score @1 min | 7.1 (2.2)* | 8; range, 5–10* | 8.4 (1.6)+ | 9; range, 3–10+ |
Mean Apgar score @5 min | 9.2 (0.9)* | 9; range, 6–10* | 9.4 (1.0) | 10; range, 6–10+ |
Mean no. days on mechanical ventilator | 1.0 (n = 2) | 1; range,1 |
SDs are in parentheses. GA, Gestational Age.
Information unavailable for 2 infants. +Information unavailable for 4 infants.
Electromyography (EMG)
Flexion withdrawal reflex activity was measured from the lower limb using surface EMG. Self-adhesive bipolar surface silver/silver-chloride electrodes (Cardinal Health, USA) were positioned over the biceps femoris of each limb (including the contralateral muscle where possible). The EMG signal was amplified (x10,000), filtered between 0–500 Hz and sampled at 2 kHz using a 32 bit recording system (Neuroscan Synamps 2, Compumedics, USA). A notch filter was applied at 50 Hz to reduce interference from external electrical signals. A chest electrode served to ground the signal. Surface electrodes were positioned at least 5 minutes prior to cutaneous stimulation to assess EMG recording quality and for baseline data acquisition. EMG recordings were excluded from the data analysis for low signal to noise ratio, that is less than 10% above baseline, or technical artifact.
Cutaneous sensory stimulation
Figure 1 shows the numbers of infants included for each study. It was not possible to perform all tests on all infants for clinical reasons; noxious heel lancing was only performed as part of standard medical care.
All studies were performed with the participant lying on the side or in the supine position. Three types of stimulus were applied to the foot. (1) Noxious stimulation was a clinically required heel lance performed for blood collection (Tenderfoot; ITC, USA); (Figure 1, inset ‘A’). A lancet device was held against the outer aspect of the heel, a spring-loaded blade was released from the device to break the surface of the skin. (2) Innocuous mechanical “tactile” stimulation was performed in the same group of infants (Figure 1, inset ‘B’) before the lance was applied. Here, the lancet device was rotated by 90° and placed against the heel so that when the blade was released it did not contact the heel nor break the skin. Therefore, infants experienced the tactile sensation and a click associated with the blade release from the lancet device but no skin incision/noxious stimulation. For all infants, the tactile stimulus was performed prior to blood sampling and on the same site on the foot as the noxious heel lance. The time of noxious and tactile stimulation was automatically event-marked on the EMG recording by electronically linking the lancet device to the recording equipment [30] (3) “Punctate” stimuli, performed in a separate study from the lance and tactile stimulation using a series of calibrated von Frey hairs of differing stimulus intensities (Figure 1, inset ‘C’). The von Frey hair threshold was established by applying hairs in ascending order of intensity to the plantar surface of the foot until clear, brisk, leg withdrawal was observed. The observed withdrawal threshold was later confirmed as the threshold that produced an EMG response. The suprathreshold von Frey EMG response was established using a von Frey hair two grades above threshold (mean force applied 6.2±6.0 g, range 0.8 – 17.2 g, at 10 second intervals). Three stimuli were applied and the maximum response used for analysis. To test whether sensitization to repeated punctate stimulation occurred, the EMG response to the threshold von Frey hair was measured before and 30 seconds after a train of 12 suprathreshold stimuli applied at 10 sec intervals (mean ISI, 10.6±0.9 s, range 10.0–12.4 s). Application of each von Frey hair punctate stimulus was event-marked using a custom-built manual push button trigger system linked electronically to the recording equipment.
EMG Analysis
Raw EMG recordings were filtered with a 10 Hz high-pass filter to eliminate signal drift and ensure signal composition contained frequencies associated with motor activity. Recordings were analyzed from 1000 ms before the stimulus (baseline) to 5000 ms after the stimulus. To identify the onset of the EMG response, the standard deviation was calculated in a 50 ms sliding window from the time of stimulation; the time at which the standard deviation exceeded three times the baseline standard deviation, was defined as the reflex onset. For analysis of the reflex magnitude, the EMG signal was divided into 250 ms time-bins from the onset of the response and the root mean square (RMS) of EMG activity in each time bin calculated. Since the baseline EMG, recorded in the 1000 ms epoch before the noxious stimulus, increased significantly with gestational age [<34 wks: 5.66±3.83 µV (n = 8), 34−<37 wks: 8.10±5.89 µV (n = 11), 37−<40 wks: 11.53±4.05 µV (n = 10), <43 wks: 17.85±4.30 µV (n = 10); One-way ANOVA, **p = 0.003], evoked EMG activity was expressed as fold changes over baseline. Data points with fold changes greater than 100 were excluded as artifact.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted using GraphPad Prism (GraphPad Software, USA). Data expressed as mean (standard error of the mean, SEM), unless otherwise stated. All groups of data were tested for normality using the D′Agostino-Pearson normality test, where the data did not follow a Gaussian distribution an appropriate non-parametric test was applied (specified in the text parenthesis). Differences in the patterns of EMG activity were compared for (1) gestational age –preterm versus full term infants, (2) laterality – ipsilateral versus contralateral biceps femoris activity, and (3) stimulus specificity – non-noxious tactile stimulus versus noxious stimulus, using a two-way ANOVA with repeated measures. Posthoc analyses of these comparisons were conducted using appropriate t-tests (specified in the text parenthesis). Significant differences were assumed at p<0.05. T-tests are reported with degrees of freedom, t-statistic and significance level. ANOVAs are reported with degrees of freedom (between groups, within group), F-statistic and significance level. Asterisks indicate the significance level where: *, p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001.
Results
Nociceptive flexion reflexes are greater in magnitude and duration in younger infants
To test whether infant nociceptive flexion reflex responses change with gestational age, we compared the biceps femoris EMG activity evoked by a clinically required time-locked heel lance in preterm (<37 weeks gestation at time of study, n = 19) and term infants (≥ 37 weeks gestation at time of study, n = 20). In all infants, a robust biceps femoris EMG response was evoked by heel lance, but the overall nociceptive flexion reflex response was significantly greater in preterm compared to term infants due to more sustained EMG activity in the younger group (Figure 2).
Figure 2B shows that while the lance evoked EMG activity in both groups peaks within the first 1000 ms, the magnitude and time course of activity is significantly different in preterm compared to term infants [two-way ANOVA: age group, F1,629 = 5.3,*p = 0.04; and time course of activity, F16,629 = 5.9, ***p = 0.0001 time]. The nociceptive reflex activity in the 1000–1500 ms time period post stimulus is significantly greater in the younger babies (Welch-corrected unpaired t-test, 1000–1250 ms, t24 = 1.8, *p = 0.04; 1250–1500 ms, t24 = 1.7 *p = 0.048). Furthermore, Fig 1C shows that lance evoked EMG activity in individual infants at 1000–1250 ms post-lance is correlated with gestational age at study (R2 = 0.1, *p = 0.025, one-tailed). Taken together, this data shows that cutaneous noxious evoked flexion reflex EMG activity decreases with gestational age in human infants.
Noxious stimulation evokes equal ipsilateral and contralateral flexion reflexes in newborn infants
To test the spatial characteristics of human infant nociceptive reflexes, we performed paired EMG recordings from the ipsilateral and contralateral biceps femoris following noxious heel lance (preterm, n = 11; term n = 13). In both preterm and term infants, noxious stimulation of the heel evoked EMG activity in the contralateral limb that was not significantly different in magnitude and duration from the ipsilateral flexor reflex EMG activity (Figures 3A & B). This data shows that in both preterm and term infants, nociceptive flexion reflexes are not restricted to the stimulated limb.
Flexion reflexes are not always noxious specific in infants
To test whether human infant flexion reflexes are nociceptive specific, the reflex response to a non-noxious tactile stimulus was recorded (where the lancet device was rotated by 90° and placed against the heel so that when the blade was released it did not contact the heel nor break the skin) and compared to the response to a heel lance, in the same infants. In both age groups, some, but not all, infants displayed a significant flexion reflex response to innocuous tactile stimulus. The occurrence of these responses to non-noxious, tactile stimulation of the heel did not differ in the two gestational age groups [term: 40% (n = 8/20); preterm 29% (n = 5/17) respond to tactile stimulation]. Furthermore, there was no significant difference in the magnitude or duration of the noxious evoked EMG response in those infants that displayed a tactile response (‘tactile responders’ – infants who responded to tactile stimulation with a brisk leg withdrawal) and those that did not (‘tactile non-responders’ – infants who did not respond to tactile stimulation with a brisk leg withdrawal), (term infants: two way ANOVA, tactile sensitivity, F1,288 = 0.2, p = 0.65; time course of activity. F15,288 = 1.7, p = 0.06).
Fig.4 shows paired comparisons of noxious and tactile flexion reflexes in ‘tactile responders’. Figure 4A shows that, in term infants (n = 7), the tactile evoked EMG response was significantly smaller than the noxious evoked EMG response, but Figure 4B shows that in preterm infants (n = 5), the magnitude and duration of evoked response to tactile stimulation was not significantly different from that evoked by noxious stimulation (Term infants, two-way ANOVA, **p = 0.006 time, ****p<0.0001 stimulus. Fisher's t-test at 250 ms: *p = 0.014, at 500 ms: ***p = 0.0005, at 750 ms: **p = 0.006). Furthermore, tactile stimulation of the heel evoked both ipsilateral and contralateral reflexes, but contralateral reflexes were significantly smaller in magnitude than ipsilateral responses [Term infants, two way ANOVA: side, F1,64 = 6.1, *p = 0.01; time course of activity, F15,322 = 2.0, *p = 0.01, n = 7; no recordings for preterm infants].
These results show that flexion reflex EMG activity is not necessarily modality specific in newborn infants and can be evoked by non-noxious tactile stimulation. Importantly, in preterm infants, tactile reflexes, when they occur, are not significantly different from noxious lance evoked responses, whereas in term infants noxious evoked reflexes are significantly larger than tactile evoked reflexes.
Infant flexion reflexes are sensitized by brief, low frequency punctate stimulation
To explore the cutaneous sensitivity of infant flexion reflexes further, we applied calibrated punctate von Frey hairs to the foot (vFh) in a separate group of infants (n = 11, Fig.5). Application of a known force to the skin surface allowed precise measurements of cutaneous reflex thresholds and reflex excitability. Von Frey thresholds were initially recorded using visual observation of leg movement and confirmed from post hoc EMG analysis. Fig 5A shows that the von Frey threshold of infant flexor reflexes significantly increase with gestational age (R2 0.46, **p = 0.008, one-tailed). The median (IQR) threshold in preterm infants was 1.23 (0.43–2.75 g, n = 6) and in term infants was 2.1 g (1.67–3.88 g, n = 5). In addition, the magnitude of flexion EMG responses to suprathreshold vFh punctate stimulation (2 grades above threshold) is significant greater in amplitude and duration in preterm infants (n = 6) compared to term infants (n = 5) (Two-way ANOVA, age, F1,154 = 7.4, **p<0.007; time course of activity, F16,154 = 5.3, ***p = <0.0001; Fisher's unpaired t-test, 250 ms, *p = 0.02; 500 ms, ***p<0.0001, 750 ms, ***p<0.0001).
We also tested whether von Frey hair evoked flexion reflexes could be sensitized by repeated stimulation. To do this a suprathreshold hair (2 grades above threshold) was applied to the foot repeatedly at a rate of 1/10 s for two minutes (n = 6 preterm, n = 5 term). A reflex response was evoked an average of 5 times by these 12 repeated stimuli in both age groups. Figure 5E shows that 10 seconds after repeated stimulation, the reflex EMG response to a threshold hair was significantly larger than the response to the same threshold hair when tested 10 seconds before the train (Two-way ANOVA, *p = 0.04 treatment,***p = 0.002, time; Fisher's paired t-test, 250 ms, *p = 0,002, n = 11, pooled term and preterm, first 1000 ms).
These results show that the flexion reflex in preterm infants is more sensitive to cutaneous punctate von Frey hair stimulation than term infants and that infant flexion reflexes are sensitized by repeated innocuous punctate stimulation.
Discussion
These results demonstrate that human infant spinal nociceptive reflexes undergo substantial changes in modality, temporal and spatial specificity with gestational age. In contrast to adults, flexion reflex EMG responses to cutaneous noxious stimulation are remarkably prolonged and widespread, and display marked sensitivity to single and repeated non-noxious tactile and punctate stimulation. These data provide new insight into the functional maturation of neural pathways underlying early human pain behaviour.
Differences in adult and neonatal nociceptive reflexes in man
In adult man, the lower limb flexion reflex is a brief response that results in limb withdrawal from a noxious or potentially noxious cutaneous stimulus and is a reliable and objective reflection of an individual's pain experience [6]. Stimulation of small diameter nociceptive fibres is required to produce this relatively short duration reflex in healthy adults [31], [32]. Thus high intensity cutaneous nerve stimulation in adults produces a distinct burst in biceps femoris, which is linearly correlated with pain sensation and returns to baseline 100–120 ms poststimulus [33]. Thermal laser stimulation of the foot also elicits a very brief (∼100 ms), long latency nociceptive reflex that is well correlated with pricking pain sensation [34].
In contrast, we show here that the infant flexion reflex is remarkably long duration. A single heel lance evoked biceps femoris activity for at least 2–4 seconds, decreasing significantly between the preterm and term period, but remaining longer in duration than adult reflexes. Furthermore, the reflex does not always require a noxious stimulus in infants, but can also be evoked by tactile and punctate innocuous skin stimulation. Newborn infants are known to have low cutaneous thresholds [29], [35] but here we show, using within infant comparisons, that the reflex response to touch can be as great as that to noxious stimulation in the youngest babies. In addition, the infant contralateral flexion response to noxious stimulation is as large as the ipsilateral response, while in adults it is entirely ipsilateral [36]. Finally, repeated punctate innocuous stimulation in infants caused a significant increase in reflex magnitude and a drop in threshold, which are the hallmarks of sensitization [28]. In contrast, flexion reflex temporal summation or sensitization occurs in adults only after repeated C fibre or noxious stimulation [37], [38] and is associated with increased pain ratings [39].
The neural basis for infant human nociceptive reflex properties
The results reported here have a clear translational link with laboratory animal data. The first seven days in rodent life approximates to the preterm period in man from 24 weeks gestation to term equivalent. Rat pups show a postnatal decline in the amplitude and duration of flexion reflex response to a single strong mechanical skin stimulus [40]; at birth the reflex response in rats can last for up to 20 seconds but by the third postnatal week, even the highest intensity stimulation never produces reflexes of that duration. This pattern is likely to reflect lack of inhibitory control in cutaneous, rather than motor, spinal circuits [12] since it is not reflected in proprioceptive reflexes [41], [42]. While the excitatory and inhibitory locomotor circuitry in newborn rats closely mimics that of adults [43], cutaneous reflex receptive fields are large and disorganised at birth and take several weeks to mature [16], [22]. Neonatal dorsal horn cells also have larger receptive fields and display prolonged firing to noxious stimuli [19], [44]. Furthermore, the balance of descending control from the brainstem onto neonatal nociceptive dorsal horn circuits is excitatory rather than inhibitory which also enhances reflex sensitivity [45]. Repetitive activation of low-threshold A-fibres sensitizes neonatal dorsal horn neurons [21], evokes fos expression[26]and NMDA dependent LTP [46] in the dorsal horn all of which require high threshold stimulation in adult animals. There is evidence for a greater synaptic input from cutaneous A fibre myelinated afferents in the newborn rat [23], [47]–[49], due to diffuse A fibre terminal fields [17], [24]and immature inhibitory signaling, in particular from glycinergic networks, in the dorsal horn[25]. In adults, Aβ fibres synapse directly onto glycinergic interneurons [50], [51]and glycine receptor antagonists enhance tactile behavioural sensitivity [52], [53]and increase A fibre excitation of dorsal horn neurons [54], [55]. However in the newborn cord, glycinergic synaptic activity is weak [56] and for the first two postnatal weeks, glycine facilitates rather than inhibits dorsal horn wide dynamic range neuronal responses to low threshold skin stimulation [25].
Developmental implications of nociceptive reflex immaturity in healthy neonates
The developing nervous system depends upon sensory input for tuning and organising sensory and motor circuits and we propose that the sensitivity of newborn reflexes to low threshold skin stimulation reflects this need. The newborn mammal is almost continuously exposed to tactile skin stimulation, be it through maternal contact, huddling or spontaneous twitching [16]. Maternal licking and grooming during a critical period in early life modifies neural development in newborn rat pups [57]and in human infants, there is evidence that skin contact supports normal growth and development [58]. The maturation of directed nociceptive reflex behaviour between the second and third postnatal week, requires intact low threshold inputs [16] and the postnatal organisation of primary afferent terminals in the dorsal horn is prevented if animals are raised in a high ‘tactile noise’ environment [17]. The first postnatal weeks in rodents, when many spinal sensory synaptic and circuit changes take place, are equivalent to infancy and early childhood in man [59]. Thus we propose that the cutaneous sensitivity of human infant reflexes reported here will maximise sensory drive at critical stage of somatosensory and pain circuit development [18].
Do flexion reflexes reflect pain perception in neonates?
The implications of these findings are considerable for understanding human somatosensory development and for clinical pain assessment of neonates. In adults nociceptive flexion reflexes are a useful measure of central pain processing: they are sensitized by repeated C fibre stimulation [38] enhanced in chronic pain states [60], [61], modulated by distraction [62] and emotional arousal [63], and reflect ethnic differences in pain report [64]. The size of the reflex receptive field area in adults can be modulated by the cognitive state demonstrating a link between the cognitive activity and the descending control of spinal withdrawal reflex pathways [65] and long-term potentiation of the flexion reflex has been proposed as a target for pain therapy [66]. The correlation of flexion reflex threshold and magnitude with subjective pain threshold and intensity in adults, suggests that such a link may also apply in infants. The immaturity of the infant brain makes the concept of subjective pain experience merely speculative, but it is possible to draw comparisons between pain processing at the spinal and cortical levels in preterm and term infants. Thus the sensitivity of the preterm flexion reflex to tactile inputs is entirely consistent with the lack of differentiation between tactile and noxious cortical activity before 35 weeks gestation [67]. It also agrees with reports that the behavioural and physiological reactivity of preterm neonates to tactile stimulation can be the same as that to painful stimulation [68], [69]. Caution should therefore be used when using behavioural indices as a measure of pain in infancy. Our results confirm the importance of quantitative measurement of the neural activity underlying early human tactile and pain behaviour.
Funding Statement
The research was supported by the Wellcome Trust and Medical Research Council. LC was funded by an MRC DTA PhD studentship. This research was carried out at the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) UCLH Clinical Research Facility. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Sherrington CS (1910) Flexion-reflex of the limb, crossed extension-reflex, and reflex stepping and standing. J Physiol (Lond) 40: 28–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Schouenborg J, Kalliomäki J (1990) Functional organization of the nociceptive withdrawal reflexes. I. Activation of hindlimb muscles in the rat. Exp Brain Res 83: 67–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Sandrini G, Serrao M, Rossi P, Romaniello A, Cruccu G, et al. (2005) The lower limb flexion reflex in humans. Prog Neurobiol 77: 353–395 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Le Bars D, Gozariu M, Cadden SW (2001) Animal models of nociception. Pharmacol Rev 53: 597–652. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Allen JW, Yaksh TL (2004) Assessment of acute thermal nociception in laboratory animals. Methods Mol Med 99: 11–23 10.1385/1-59259-770-X:011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Skljarevski V, Ramadan NM (2002) The nociceptive flexion reflex in humans -- review article. Pain 96: 3–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Rhudy JL, France CR (2011) Reliability and validity of a brief method to assess nociceptive flexion reflex (NFR) threshold. J Pain 12: 782–791 10.1016/j.jpain.2011.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Woolf CJ (2007) Central sensitization: uncovering the relation between pain and plasticity. Anesthesiology 106: 864–867 10.1097/01.anes.0000264769.87038.55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Basbaum AI, Bautista DM, Scherrer G, Julius D (2009) Cellular and molecular mechanisms of pain. Cell 139: 267–284 10.1016/j.cell.2009.09.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Fields HL (2000) Pain modulation: expectation, opioid analgesia and virtual pain. Prog Brain Res 122: 245–253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Heinricher MM, Tavares I, Leith JL, Lumb BM (2009) Descending control of nociception: Specificity, recruitment and plasticity. Brain Res Rev 60: 214–225 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.12.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Fitzgerald M (2005) The development of nociceptive circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci 6: 507–520 10.1038/nrn1701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Fitzgerald M, Millard C, MacIntosh N (1988) Hyperalgesia in premature infants. Lancet 1: 292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Fitzgerald M, Millard C, McIntosh N (1989) Cutaneous hypersensitivity following peripheral tissue damage in newborn infants and its reversal with topical anaesthesia. Pain 39: 31–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Walker SM, Meredith-Middleton J, Lickiss T, Moss A, Fitzgerald M (2007) Primary and secondary hyperalgesia can be differentiated by postnatal age and ERK activation in the spinal dorsal horn of the rat pup. Pain 128: 157–168 10.1016/j.pain.2006.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Waldenström A, Thelin J, Thimansson E, Levinsson A, Schouenborg J (2003) Developmental learning in a pain-related system: evidence for a cross-modality mechanism. J Neurosci 23: 7719–7725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Granmo M, Petersson P, Schouenborg J (2008) Action-based body maps in the spinal cord emerge from a transitory floating organization. J Neurosci 28: 5494–5503 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0651-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Koch SC, Fitzgerald M (2013) Activity-dependent development of tactile and nociceptive spinal cord circuits. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1279: 97–102 10.1111/nyas.12033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Fitzgerald M (1985) The post-natal development of cutaneous afferent fibre input and receptive field organization in the rat dorsal horn. J Physiol (Lond) 364: 1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Fitzgerald M, Shaw A, MacIntosh N (1988) Postnatal development of the cutaneous flexor reflex: comparative study of preterm infants and newborn rat pups. Dev Med Child Neurol 30: 520–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Jennings E, Fitzgerald M (1998) Postnatal changes in responses of rat dorsal horn cells to afferent stimulation: a fibre-induced sensitization. J Physiol (Lond) 509 (Pt 3): 859–868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Levinsson A, Luo XL, Holmberg H, Schouenborg J (1999) Developmental tuning in a spinal nociceptive system: effects of neonatal spinalization. J Neurosci 19: 10397–10403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Park JS, Nakatsuka T, Nagata K, Higashi H, Yoshimura M (1999) Reorganization of the primary afferent termination in the rat spinal dorsal horn during post-natal development. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 113: 29–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Beggs S, Torsney C, Drew LJ, Fitzgerald M (2002) The postnatal reorganization of primary afferent input and dorsal horn cell receptive fields in the rat spinal cord is an activity-dependent process. Eur J Neurosci 16: 1249–1258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Koch SC, Tochiki KK, Hirschberg S, Fitzgerald M (2012) C-fiber activity-dependent maturation of glycinergic inhibition in the spinal dorsal horn of the postnatal rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109: 12201–12206 10.1073/pnas.1118960109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Jennings E, Fitzgerald M (1996) C-fos can be induced in the neonatal rat spinal cord by both noxious and innocuous peripheral stimulation. Pain 68: 301–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Williams S, Evan G, Hunt SP (1990) Spinal c-fos induction by sensory stimulation in neonatal rats. Neurosci Lett 109: 309–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Andrews K, Fitzgerald M (1994) The cutaneous withdrawal reflex in human neonates: sensitization, receptive fields, and the effects of contralateral stimulation. Pain 56: 95–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Andrews K, Fitzgerald M (1999) Cutaneous flexion reflex in human neonates: a quantitative study of threshold and stimulus-response characteristics after single and repeated stimuli. Dev Med Child Neurol 41: 696–703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Worley A, Fabrizi L, Boyd S, Slater R (2012) Multi-modal pain measurements in infants. J Neurosci Methods 205: 252–257 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.01.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Kugelberg E, Eklund K, Grimby L (1960) An Electromyographic Study of the Nociceptive Reflexes of the Lower Limb. Mechanism of the Plantar Responses. Brain 83: 394–410 10.1093/brain/83.3.394 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z, Hallin RG, Persson A (1984) Do large diameter cutaneous afferents have a role in the transmission of nociceptive messages? Brain Research 311: 375–379 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90104-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Chan C, Dallaire M (1989) Subjective pain senation is linearly correlated with the flexion reflex in man. Brain Research 479: 145–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Willer JC, Boureau F, Berny J (1979) Nociceptive flexion reflexes elicited by noxious laser radiant heat in man. Pain 7: 15–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Fitzgerald M, Shaw A, MacIntosh N (1988) Postnatal development of the cutaneous flexor reflex: comparative study of preterm infants and newborn rat pups. Dev Med Child Neurol 30: 520–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Decchi B, Zalaffi A, Spidalieri R, Arrigucci U, Di Troia AM, et al. (1997) Spinal reflex pattern to foot nociceptive stimulation in standing humans. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 105: 484–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Arendt-Nielsen L, Brennum J, Sindrup S, Bak P (1994) Electrophysiological and psychophysical quantification of temporal summation in the human nociceptive system. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 68: 266–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Arendt-Nielsen L, Sonnenborg FA, Andersen OK (2000) Facilitation of the withdrawal reflex by repeated transcutaneous electrical stimulation: An experimental study on central integration in humans. European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology 81: 165–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Price DD (1972) Characteristics of second pain and flexion reflexes indicative of prolonged central summation. Exp Neurol 37: 371–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Fitzgerald M, Gibson S (1984) The postnatal physiological and neurochemical development of peripheral sensory C fibres. Neuroscience 13: 933–944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Skoglund S (1960) Hind-limb reflexes in the kitten. Nature 187: 132–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ekholm J (n.d.) Postnatal development of excitatory and inh. [Acta Soc Med Ups. 1967] - PubMed - NCBI. 1967. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6030193. Accessed 2012 Sep 10.
- 43. Hochman S, Gozal EA, Hayes HB, Anderson JT, DeWeerth SP, et al. (2012) Enabling techniques for in vitro studies on mammalian spinal locomotor mechanisms. Front Biosci 17: 2158–2180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Ririe DG, Bremner LR, Fitzgerald M (2008) Comparison of the immediate effects of surgical incision on dorsal horn neuronal receptive field size and responses during postnatal development. Anesthesiology 109: 698–706 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181870a32 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Hathway GJ, Koch S, Low L, Fitzgerald M (2009) The changing balance of brainstem-spinal cord modulation of pain processing over the first weeks of rat postnatal life. J Physiol (Lond) 587: 2927–2935 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.168013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Wu J, Hu Q, Huang D, Chen X, Chen J (2012) Effect of electrical stimulation of sciatic nerve on synaptic plasticity of spinal dorsal horn and spinal c-fos expression in neonatal, juvenile and adult rats. Brain Research 1448: 11–19 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Fitzgerald M, Jennings E (1999) The postnatal development of spinal sensory processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96: 7719–7722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Nakatsuka T, Ataka T, Kumamoto E, Tamaki T, Yoshimura M (2000) Alteration in synaptic inputs through C-afferent fibers to substantia gelatinosa neurons of the rat spinal dorsal horn during postnatal development. Neuroscience 99: 549–556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Daniele CA, MacDermott AB (2009) Low-threshold primary afferent drive onto GABAergic interneurons in the superficial dorsal horn of the mouse. J Neurosci 29: 686–695 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5120-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Todd AJ (1990) An electron microscope study of glycine-like immunoreactivity in laminae I-III of the spinal dorsal horn of the rat. Neuroscience 39: 387–394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Narikawa K, Furue H, Kumamoto E, Yoshimura M (2000) In vivo patch-clamp analysis of IPSCs evoked in rat substantia gelatinosa neurons by cutaneous mechanical stimulation. J Neurophysiol 84: 2171–2174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Yaksh TL (1989) Behavioral and autonomic correlates of the tactile evoked allodynia produced by spinal glycine inhibition: effects of modulatory receptor systems and excitatory amino acid antagonists. Pain 37: 111–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Sherman SE, Loomis CW (1996) Strychnine-sensitive modulation is selective for non-noxious somatosensory input in the spinal cord of the rat. Pain 66: 321–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Sivilotti L, Woolf CJ (1994) The contribution of GABAA and glycine receptors to central sensitization: disinhibition and touch-evoked allodynia in the spinal cord. J Neurophysiol 72: 169–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Miraucourt LS, Moisset X, Dallel R, Voisin DL (2009) Glycine inhibitory dysfunction induces a selectively dynamic, morphine-resistant, and neurokinin 1 receptor- independent mechanical allodynia. J Neurosci 29: 2519–2527 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3923-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Baccei ML, Fitzgerald M (2004) Development of GABAergic and glycinergic transmission in the neonatal rat dorsal horn. J Neurosci 24: 4749–4757 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5211-03.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Kaffman A, Meaney MJ (2007) Neurodevelopmental sequelae of postnatal maternal care in rodents: clinical and research implications of molecular insights. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48: 224–244 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01730.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Moore ER, Anderson GC, Bergman N, Dowswell T (2012) Early skin-to-skin contact for mothers and their healthy newborn infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 5: CD003519 10.1002/14651858.CD003519.pub3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. McCutcheon JE, Marinelli M (2009) Age matters. Eur J Neurosci 29: 997–1014 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06648.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Banic B, Petersen-Felix S, Andersen OK, Radanov BP, Villiger PM, et al. (2004) Evidence for spinal cord hypersensitivity in chronic pain after whiplash injury and in fibromyalgia. Pain 107: 7–15 10.1016/j.pain.2003.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Neziri AY, Haesler S, Petersen-Felix S, Müller M, Arendt-Nielsen L, et al. (2010) Generalized expansion of nociceptive reflex receptive fields in chronic pain patients. Pain 151: 798–805 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Ruscheweyh R, Kreusch A, Albers C, Sommer J, Marziniak M (2011) The effect of distraction strategies on pain perception and the nociceptive flexor reflex (RIII reflex). Pain 152: 2662–2671 10.1016/j.pain.2011.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Roy M, Lebuis A, Hugueville L, Peretz I, Rainville P (2012) Spinal modulation of nociception by music. European Journal of Pain 16: 870–877 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2011.00030.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Campbell CM, France CR, Robinson ME, Logan HL, Geffken GR, et al. (2008) Ethnic differences in the nociceptive flexion reflex (NFR). Pain 134: 91–96 10.1016/j.pain.2007.03.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Bjerre L, Andersen AT, Hagelskjær MT, Ge N, Mørch CD, et al. (2011) Dynamic tuning of human withdrawal reflex receptive fields during cognitive attention and distraction tasks. Eur J Pain 15: 816–821 10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.01.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Ruscheweyh R, Wilder-Smith O, Drdla R, Liu X-G, Sandkühler J (2011) Long-term potentiation in spinal nociceptive pathways as a novel target for pain therapy. Mol Pain 7: 20 10.1186/1744-8069-7-20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Fabrizi L, Slater R, Worley A, Meek J, Boyd S, et al. (2011) A shift in sensory processing that enables the developing human brain to discriminate touch from pain. Curr Biol 21: 1552–1558 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Hellerud BC, Storm H (2002) Skin conductance and behaviour during sensory stimulation of preterm and term infants. Early Hum Dev 70: 35–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Gaspardo CM, Chimello JT, Cugler TS, Martinez FE, Linhares MBM (2008) Pain and tactile stimuli during arterial puncture in preterm neonates. Pain 140: 58–64 10.1016/j.pain.2008.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]