Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2013 Oct 4;8(10):e74815. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074815

Four Theorems on the Psychometric Function

Keith A May 1,*, Joshua A Solomon 1
Editor: Hans A Kestler2
PMCID: PMC3790801  PMID: 24124456

Abstract

In a 2-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) discrimination task, observers choose which of two stimuli has the higher value. The psychometric function for this task gives the probability of a correct response for a given stimulus difference, Inline graphic. This paper proves four theorems about the psychometric function. Assuming the observer applies a transducer and adds noise, Theorem 1 derives a convenient general expression for the psychometric function. Discrimination data are often fitted with a Weibull function. Theorem 2 proves that the Weibull “slope” parameter, Inline graphic, can be approximated by Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is the Inline graphic of the Weibull function that fits best to the cumulative noise distribution, and Inline graphic depends on the transducer. We derive general expressions for Inline graphic and Inline graphic, from which we derive expressions for specific cases. One case that follows naturally from our general analysis is Pelli's finding that, when Inline graphic, Inline graphic. We also consider two limiting cases. Theorem 3 proves that, as sensitivity improves, 2AFC performance will usually approach that for a linear transducer, whatever the actual transducer; we show that this does not apply at signal levels where the transducer gradient is zero, which explains why it does not apply to contrast detection. Theorem 4 proves that, when the exponent of a power-function transducer approaches zero, 2AFC performance approaches that of a logarithmic transducer. We show that the power-function exponents of 0.4–0.5 fitted to suprathreshold contrast discrimination data are close enough to zero for the fitted psychometric function to be practically indistinguishable from that of a log transducer. Finally, Weibull Inline graphic reflects the shape of the noise distribution, and we used our results to assess the recent claim that internal noise has higher kurtosis than a Gaussian. Our analysis of Inline graphic for contrast discrimination suggests that, if internal noise is stimulus-independent, it has lower kurtosis than a Gaussian.

Introduction

On each trial of a 2-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) discrimination task, observers are presented with two stimuli, one (often called the pedestal) with stimulus value Inline graphic, and one (the target) with value Inline graphic, where Inline graphic represents a value along some stimulus dimension, such as contrast, luminance, frequency, sound intensity, etc., and Inline graphic represents a (usually) positive increment in Inline graphic. The observer has to say which stimulus contained the higher value, Inline graphic. For this task, the function relating stimulus difference, Inline graphic, to the probability of a correct response, Inline graphic, is called the psychometric function. The form of the psychometric function can reveal characteristics of the underlying mechanisms, helping to constrain the set of possible models. In this paper we present four theorems that help us to understand the properties of the psychometric function and clarify the relationship between the psychometric function and the underlying model.

In order to fit the psychometric function to data, we need a mathematical function whose parameters can be adjusted to fit the kind of data set usually obtained. A widely used function is the Weibull function, and two of our theorems relate specifically to this function. Letting Inline graphic represent the Weibull function, and letting Inline graphic represent its output (i.e., the predicted proportion correct), the Weibull function is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e023.jpg (1)

Inline graphic is the “threshold” parameter, the stimulus increment that gives rise to a proportion correct of Inline graphic. In what follows, we will frequently refer to this threshold performance level as Inline graphic, so this term should be read as the constant, Inline graphic. Inline graphic is often referred to as the “slope” parameter, because it is proportional to the gradient of the Weibull function at Inline graphic when Inline graphic is plotted on a log abscissa.

In 2AFC visual contrast discrimination experiments where the contrasts of both stimuli are at least as high as the detection threshold, Inline graphic usually falls between 1 and 2, with a median of around 1.4 (see Table 1 and Figure 1). As the pedestal contrast approaches zero (making it a 2AFC contrast detection task), Inline graphic increases to a value of around 3 [1][5].

Table 1. Fitted Weibull function parameters for 2AFC contrast discrimination.

λ = 0 λ fitted
Study Condition/observer Pedestal α β W α β λ W
Bird et al. [34] CMB 0.03 0.00735 1.12 0.245 0.00735 1.12 5×10−13 0.245
CMB 0.3 0.0779 1.11 0.260 0.0692 1.21 0.0309 0.231
GBH 0.03 0.00737 0.734 0.246 0.00643 0.832 0.0251 0.214
GBH 0.3 0.0574 0.952 0.191 0.0541 0.993 0.0128 0.180
Foley & Legge [1] JMF, 0.5 cpd 0.00400 0.00165 1.35 0.412 0.00165 1.35 3×10−9 0.412
JMF, 2 cpd 0.00230 0.00111 1.56 0.484 0.00111 1.56 5×10−12 0.484
JMF, 8 cpd 0.00300 0.00125 1.44 0.418 0.00123 1.46 0.0071 0.410
GW, 0.5 cpd 0.00400 0.00134 1.50 0.335 0.00134 1.50 2×10−12 0.335
GW, 2 cpd 0.00229 0.000923 1.58 0.404 0.000923 1.58 1×10−12 0.404
GW, 8 cpd 0.00330 0.00117 1.40 0.353 0.000996 1.94 0.0544 0.301
Henning et al. [47] CMB 2.09 cpd 0.15 0.0421 1.49 0.281 0.0421 1.49 1×10−12 0.281
CMB 8.37 cpd 0.15 0.0461 1.81 0.307 0.0379 2.21 0.0796 0.253
GBH 2.09 cpd 0.15 0.0363 1.49 0.242 0.0363 1.49 2×10−12 0.242
GBH 8.37 cpd 0.15 0.0401 1.21 0.267 0.0244 6.70 0.0645 0.163
Henning & Wichman [40] GBH* 0* 0.0219* 4.26* –*
GBH* 0.01* 0.0102* 13.1* 1.02*
GBH* 0.02* 0.00562* 1.67* 0.281*
GBH 0.04 0.00705 0.987 0.176
GBH 0.08 0.0156 1.16 0.195
GBH 0.16 0.0322 1.75 0.201
GBH 0.32 0.0773 1.45 0.241
NAL* 0* 0.00619* 4.84* –*
NAL* 0.00141* 0.00492* 5.90* 3.48*
NAL* 0.00283* 0.00407* 2.28* 1.44*
NAL* 0.00566* 0.00224* 1.43* 0.395*
NAL 0.0113 0.00272 0.902 0.241
NAL 0.0226 0.00707 0.990 0.312
NAL 0.0453 0.0150 0.943 0.331
NAL 0.0905 0.0233 1.28 0.257
NAL 0.181 0.0424 1.59 0.234
NAL 0.362 0.0658 1.33 0.182
TCC* 0* 0.00838* 6.38* –*
TCC* 0.005* 0.00443* 2.14* 0.886*
TCC 0.01 0.00339 0.912 0.339
TCC 0.016 0.00787 1.17 0.492
TCC 0.032 0.0126 1.52 0.393
TCC 0.08 0.0301 1.64 0.377
TCC 0.16 0.0381 1.27 0.238
TCC 0.32 0.0686 1.10 0.214
Meese et al. [4] Pedestal −∞ dB* 0* 0.00855* 3.32* –*
Pedestal −10 dB* 0.00316* 0.00557* 2.44* 1.76*
Pedestal −5 dB* 0.00562* 0.00346* 1.47* 0.615*
Pedestal 0 dB 0.01 0.00340 1.47 0.340
Pedestal 5 dB 0.0178 0.00654 1.48 0.368
Pedestal 10 dB 0.0316 0.0110 1.40 0.348
Pedestal 15 dB 0.0562 0.0176 1.58 0.313
Pedestal 20 dB 0.1 0.0233 1.47 0.233
Pedestal 25 dB 0.178 0.0339 1.47 0.191
Pedestal 30 dB 0.316 0.0536 1.36 0.170
Nachmias & Sansbury [6] CS 0.0079 0.00387 1.27 0.489
Mean of suprathreshold (non-starred) conditions 1.32 0.298 1.82 0.297
Median of suprathreshold conditions 1.38 0.274 1.49 0.267

This table shows Weibull parameters fitted to 2AFC contrast discrimination data from six studies. The data from Meese et al. [4] are for their Binocular condition (plotted as squares in their Figure 5); these data were kindly provided by Tim Meese. For the other five papers, we read off the data points from digital scans of the figures (Bird et al. [34], Figure 1; Foley and Legge [1], Figure 1; Henning et al. [47], Figure 4 (sine wave stimuli only); Henning & Wichmann [40], Figure 4; Nachmias & Sansbury [6], Figure 2). In most cases, these figures plotted the proportion correct, Inline graphic, for several different contrast differences, Inline graphic, and we fitted the Weibull function using a maximum-likelihood method; specifically, we fitted the Weibull function by maximizing the expression Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is the Weibull function whose parameters were being fitted. In Henning & Wichmann's [40] paper, the figures plotted the Inline graphic values corresponding to 60%, 75%, and 90% correct on the fitted psychometric functions, so we had to fit Weibull functions to points sampled from Henning & Wichmann's own fitted psychometric functions, rather than to the raw data. Where possible, we fitted both the lapse-free Weibull function of Equation (1), and the Weibull function of Equation (2), which includes a fitted lapse rate parameter, Inline graphic. Parameters for the former fit appear under the heading “Inline graphic”, and those for the latter appear under the heading “Inline graphic fitted”. In many cases, the data did not sufficiently constrain Inline graphic because there were no data points on the saturating portion of the psychometric function; in addition, Meese et al.'s Weibull fits did not include a lapse rate parameter. The Weber fraction, Inline graphic, is given by Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is the pedestal value. The means and medians at the bottom of the table are calculated from those studies for which the pedestal level exceeds the detection threshold, so that both stimuli were clearly visible. The cases where the pedestal is below detection threshold are starred in the table, and these were excluded from the means and medians.

Figure 1. Distribution of fitted Weibull Inline graphic values in Table 1.

Figure 1

The fitted Inline graphic values from the suprathreshold (non-starred) conditions of Table 1 were dropped into bins with edges that stepped from 0.8 to 2.4 in jumps of 0.2 (the histogram thus excludes one outlier, the value 6.70 for Henning et al. 's [47] observer GBH at 8.37 cpd). For this histogram, we used the Inline graphic values that had been fitted using a nonzero lapse rate parameter where available, as this is more likely to reflect the true Inline graphic. The median of this hybrid population (some including a lapse rate parameter, some not) was 1.43 (indicated by the vertical dashed line).

When Inline graphic is plotted on a log abscissa, changing the value of Inline graphic shifts the function horizontally, but otherwise leaves it unchanged (Figure 2A), and changing the value of Inline graphic linearly stretches or compresses the function horizontally, leading to a change of slope (Figure 2B). On this log abscissa, the Weibull function always has the same basic shape, up to a linear horizontal scaling. When Inline graphic is plotted on a linear abscissa, changing the value of Inline graphic linearly stretches or compresses the function horizontally as well as changing the threshold (Figure 2C), while changing the value of Inline graphic changes the shape of the function in a way that cannot be described as a linear horizontal scaling (Figure 2D).

Figure 2. Effect of varying Weibull Inline graphic and Inline graphic on log and linear abscissas.

Figure 2

(A) Varying Inline graphic on a log abscissa: The curve shifts horizontally. (B) Varying Inline graphic on a log abscissa: The curve is linearly stretched or compressed horizontally. (C) Varying Inline graphic on a linear abscissa: The curve undergoes a linear horizontal stretch and a change of threshold. (D) Varying Inline graphic on a linear abscissa: The shape changes in a way that cannot be described as a linear scaling.

Since Inline graphic is proportional to the slope of the Weibull function on a log abscissa, the low value of Inline graphic for contrast discrimination (compared with detection) often leads to the psychometric function for discrimination being described as “shallow”, and that for detection as “steep”. However, psychometric functions for contrast discrimination can be steeper than for detection when plotted on a linear contrast abscissa (e.g., Nachmias & Sansbury [6], Figure 2; Foley & Legge [1], Figure 1). We must therefore be vigilant not to be misled by the common practice of referring to Inline graphic as the “slope” parameter. Inline graphic does control the slope of the Weibull function on a log abscissa, and this fact plays a key role in the proof of Theorem 2 of this paper, but the psychometric function is often plotted on a linear abscissa, and, in this case, Inline graphic and Inline graphic both affect the slope (Figures 2C and 2D); on a linear abscissa, Inline graphic additionally controls the threshold and Inline graphic additionally controls the overall shape of the psychometric function. Thus, when considering a linear abscissa, it would be more appropriate to describe Inline graphic as the “shape” parameter, rather than the “slope” parameter.

The Weibull function defined in Equation (1) asymptotes to perfect performance (Inline graphic). This is rarely achieved by human observers due to lapses of concentration, etc., and this can lead to a dramatic underestimation of Inline graphic if the observer makes just one lapse on an easy trial [7]. Because of this problem, many researchers use a version of the Weibull function that includes a “lapse rate” parameter, Inline graphic:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e073.jpg (2)

This function asymptotes to Inline graphic, and reduces to Equation (1) in the case of Inline graphic. The psychometric function described by Equation (2) would result if the observer performed according to Equation (1) on a proportion Inline graphic of trials, and guessed randomly on the remaining trials.

The Weibull function was originally proposed by Weibull [8] as a useful, general-purpose statistical distribution. Its widespread use as a psychometric function can be traced back to Quick [9], who was apparently unaware of Weibull's prior work. Quick proposed this function because, given certain assumptions, the Weibull function makes it easy to calculate how detection performance will be affected by adding extra stimulus components or increasing the size or duration of the stimulus, an approach that has become known as probability summation [10][13]. Quick focused on yes/no detection tasks, where the observer has to make a binary decision about a single stimulus (as opposed to the 2AFC tasks that we consider in this paper, in which the observer makes a binary decision about a pair of stimuli), but a similar analysis can be applied to 2AFC tasks [2].

Most treatments of probability summation with the Weibull function invoke the “high threshold assumption” that a zero-contrast stimulus never elicits a response in the detection mechanism, so detection errors are always unlucky guesses. This assumption makes a number of predictions that have turned out to be false [2], [14][16]. Furthermore, the convenient mathematics of probability summation with the Weibull function only applies to detection. For suprathreshold discrimination, where both stimuli are easily detectable, these computational benefits do not apply. Despite this, many researchers have continued to use the Weibull function to fit data from both detection and discrimination experiments for three perfectly valid reasons: it is well-known, fits well to the data, and is built into QUEST [17], probably the most widely used adaptive psychophysical method.

Different models of visual processing will deliver different mathematical forms for the psychometric function. Therefore, because of the widespread practice of fitting a Weibull function to data, it is of interest to know what happens when we fit a Weibull function to a psychometric function that is not a Weibull. In Theorem 2 of this paper, we derive a general analytical expression that gives a very accurate approximation of Inline graphic when the Weibull function is fitted to non-Weibull psychometric functions.

Although the usage of the Weibull function has its origin in outdated theoretical views, the Weibull function has very recently become more relevant again, due to the work of Neri [18]. He argues that the internal noise on the decision variable has a Laplace distribution, which, as we explain later in this Introduction, can lead to a psychometric function that has the form of a Weibull function with Inline graphic.

First, we consider how the psychometric function might arise from the properties of the observer. In 2AFC discrimination experiments, the observer can be modelled using a transducer, followed by constant additive noise. The transducer converts the stimulus value, Inline graphic, into some internal scalar signal value, Inline graphic. Inline graphic is called the transducer function. A noise sample from a stationary, stimulus-invariant distribution is then added to the internal signal, Inline graphic, to give a noisy internal signal value. If the noise has zero mean, then Inline graphic will be the mean internal signal for stimulus value Inline graphic. The observer compares the noisy internal signal values from the two stimuli, and chooses the stimulus that gave the higher value.

From the experimenter's perspective, the observer behaves as if a sample of noise, Inline graphic, is added to the difference of mean signals, Inline graphic, given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e087.jpg (3)

The observer is correct when Inline graphic, i.e. when Inline graphic. The probability, Inline graphic, of this happening is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e091.jpg (4)

where Inline graphic is the probability density function (PDF) of the noise, Inline graphic. This integral corresponds to the shaded area in Figure 3A. Inline graphic has to be even-symmetric, even if the noise added to the output of the transducer is not. This is because the noise sample on Inline graphic is equal to the noise sample on the target minus the noise sample on the nontarget. This is equivalent to swapping the sign of the nontarget noise sample and adding it to the target noise sample. The sign-reversed noise sample on the nontarget will have a PDF with mirror symmetry relative to the PDF of the noise sample on the target, so the sum of these two values will have an even-symmetric PDF. From the even symmetry of Inline graphic we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e097.jpg (5)

where Inline graphic is the cumulative distribution function (CDF) of the observer's noise on the internal difference signal, and corresponds to the shaded area in Figure 3B. So the psychometric function for 2AFC discrimination, expressed as a function of Inline graphic, will trace out the positive half of the internal noise CDF, increasing from 0.5 to 1 as Inline graphic increases from 0 to Inline graphic.

Figure 3. Graphical representation of the probability of a correct response.

Figure 3

The shaded areas in A and B correspond to the integrals in Equations (4) and (5), respectively. The smooth curves trace out the PDF of the noise, Inline graphic, on the internal difference signal, Inline graphic. As explained in the text, Inline graphic has to be even-symmetric, and this means that the two integrals in Equations (4) and (5) are equal. The shaded areas correspond to the probability of a correct response. The psychometric function (expressed as a function of Inline graphic) is the CDF of the noise, increasing from 0.5 to 1 as Inline graphic increases from 0 to Inline graphic.

Figure 4 plots the CDFs and PDFs for several different forms of noise distribution (the mathematical definitions of these distributions will be given later). These CDFs (plotted as functions of Inline graphic) do not have a sigmoidal shape: The point of inflection is at zero on the abscissa. This is because the point of inflection corresponds to the peak of the derivative, and the derivative of these functions is the noise PDF, which peaks at 0 in each case.

Figure 4. CDFs and PDFs of four different noise distributions.

Figure 4

The top row shows noise CDFs, Inline graphic, for (A) a Laplace distribution (generalized Gaussian with Inline graphic), (B) a Gaussian distribution (generalized Gaussian with Inline graphic), (C) a generalized Gaussian with Inline graphic, (D) a logistic distribution. Each panel in the bottom row shows the PDF, Inline graphic, corresponding to the CDF above it. Only the positive halves of the distributions are shown (i.e. Inline graphic). Note that the use of these colours for the different noise distributions is maintained in Figures 7, 8, 10, 11, 12, 15, and 16.

In summary, Inline graphic is the CDF of the internal noise, and takes an input of Inline graphic (Equation (5)); Inline graphic is the output of Inline graphic, a function that is determined by the transducer and pedestal, and takes an input of Inline graphic (Equation (3)). The composition of these two functions, Inline graphic, gives the observer's psychometric function when it is plotted as a function of Inline graphic. We use Inline graphic to represent this composition of functions:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e123.jpg (6)

If we fit the Weibull function, Inline graphic, of Equation (1) to the psychometric function, Inline graphic, of Equation (6), then the Weibull slope parameter, Inline graphic, will be determined by both the noise CDF, Inline graphic, and the transducer, Inline graphic. In Theorem 2, we show that, to a good approximation, Inline graphic can be partitioned into a product of two factors, Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Inline graphic estimates the Inline graphic of the Weibull function that fits best to the noise CDF, while Inline graphic depends on the transducer function. Weibull Inline graphic is found by multiplying these two factors together. We derive general analytical formulae for both factors, and then derive, from these formulae, specific expressions for Inline graphic for a variety of noise distributions, and specific expressions for Inline graphic for several commonly used transducer functions.

Our work greatly extends a result previously published by Pelli [19]. He showed that, for 2AFC detection or discrimination,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e138.jpg (7)

where Inline graphic is the slope of Inline graphic [20] against Inline graphic on log-log axes. Pelli derived this relationship using the concrete example of contrast detection, but it is a purely mathematical relationship (outlined in his “Analysis” section, Ref. [19], p. 121), which makes no assumptions about the underlying model, and could equally well be applied to discrimination along any unspecified stimulus dimension by replacing the contrast term, Inline graphic, with Inline graphic in his Equations (14) to (21).

Pelli's analysis ran as follows. Given the definition of Inline graphic for 2AFC,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e145.jpg (8)

(where Inline graphic is the cumulative Gaussian), and the observation or assumption Inline graphic (where Inline graphic is the value of Inline graphic corresponding to a proportion correct of Inline graphic, giving Inline graphic, and Inline graphic is the log-log slope of Inline graphic against Inline graphic), we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e155.jpg (9)

Note that Equation (9) has the same form as Equation (6) if the pedestal, Inline graphic, is zero, the transducer is a power function, Inline graphic, and the internal noise CDF is the cumulative Gaussian (as is usually assumed). If we let Inline graphic represent the Inline graphic of the Weibull function, Inline graphic, that fits best to the cumulative Gaussian, Inline graphic, then, substituting this Weibull function for Inline graphic in Equation (9) yields a Weibull function with Inline graphic given by Inline graphic, which is Relation (7).

In our terms, the “Inline graphic” part of Relation (7) is Inline graphic, the factor determined by the transducer; we will show that, in the case of a power-function transducer and zero pedestal, our general expression for Inline graphic reduces to Inline graphic. We obtain Weibull Inline graphic by multiplying Inline graphic and Inline graphic together, resulting in an estimated Inline graphic given by Inline graphic, which is equal to Inline graphic in the scenario just described. In this paper, we derive general analytical expressions for Inline graphic and Inline graphic so that we can easily estimate Weibull Inline graphic for any combination of noise distribution and transducer function, not just the specific case considered by Pelli.

In many situations, the observer can be modelled using a linear filter. This is equivalent to using a linear transducer, Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is a constant. For this transducer, Equation (6) gives

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e180.jpg (10)

Thus, the linear observer's psychometric function (plotted on a linear abscissa, Inline graphic) will have the same basic shape as the internal noise CDF, Inline graphic, just differing by a horizontal scaling factor, Inline graphic. So, if the observer behaves in a linear fashion, the psychometric function plotted on linear axes gives us a direct plot of the shape of the internal noise CDF. In this situation, since Inline graphic controls the Weibull function's shape on linear axes, the Inline graphic that fits best to the psychometric function will be the Inline graphic that fits best to the noise CDF (the sensitivity parameter, Inline graphic, will determine the best-fitting Inline graphic, since Inline graphic controls the Weibull function's horizontal scaling on linear axes).

The internal noise is usually assumed to be Gaussian, but Neri [18] has recently disputed this assumption. Using reverse correlation methods, he attempted to measure both the “deterministic transformation” (in our terms, the transducer function for contrast), and the shape of the internal noise distribution. He concluded that, for temporal 2AFC detection of a bright bar in noise, the contrast transducer was linear, and the internal noise had a Laplace distribution (whose CDF and PDF are given in Figures 4A and 4E, respectively). This is a radical departure from the Gaussian assumption that has usually been made since the invention of signal detection theory in the 1950s [14]. The Laplace distribution has higher kurtosis (i.e., has a sharper peak and heavier tails) than the Gaussian (compare Figure 4E with 4F). As we shall see later on, for positive Inline graphic, the Laplace distribution has a CDF that takes the form of a Weibull function with Inline graphic. Since the psychometric function has the same shape as the internal noise CDF for a linear observer, Neri's proposal that the transducer is linear and the internal noise has a stimulus-independent Laplace distribution predicts that the observer's psychometric function should, like the Laplace CDF, be a Weibull function with Inline graphic. As noted earlier (and shown in Table 1 and Figure 1), this does not generally seem to be the case – with noise-free stimuli, Inline graphic is around 3 for contrast detection and, even for suprathreshold contrast discrimination, where Inline graphic is substantially lower, it is still usually found to be greater than 1; later, we shall show that, assuming additive noise, these Inline graphic values are more consistent with a distribution that has lower kurtosis than a Gaussian.

Although the whole of this paper is couched in terms of the transducer model, it is not necessary to accept the transducer model to find the results useful; we just have to assume that the psychometric function has a form consistent with a particular combination of internal noise distribution and transducer function. For example, the intrinsic uncertainty model produces psychometric functions that are consistent with additive noise following an expansive power-function transducer with exponent that increases with channel uncertainty [21], but the model itself has no explicit transducer. Alternatively, suppose the observer carries out the discrimination task by making noisy estimates of each stimulus value and comparing them. Due to the noise, repeated presentations of the same stimulus value, Inline graphic, will give a distribution of estimated values, Inline graphic, around the mean estimate. If we can find a function, Inline graphic, such that the shape and width of the distribution of Inline graphic is independent of Inline graphic, then the observer is equivalent to a transducer model with additive noise. In this class of model, the stimulus value, Inline graphic, is transduced to give Inline graphic, and then stimulus-independent noise is added to the signal. But we do not have to assume that this is literally how the observer works – the noisy estimates of the stimulus values could have arisen from all sorts of mechanisms, not just a transducer followed by additive noise.

In keeping with our terminology of Inline graphic for the threshold performance level, we introduce the terms Inline graphic and Inline graphic to represent the values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic at threshold, i.e. the values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic when the proportion correct is Inline graphic, which we define as Inline graphic.

Theorem 1: A General Expression for the Psychometric Function in Terms of the Stimulus Values and the Threshold

Introduction

Equation (6) gives a general equation for the psychometric function in terms of the transducer function, Inline graphic, and the noise CDF, Inline graphic. The sensitivity of the system (which determines the discrimination threshold, Inline graphic) can be adjusted either by changing the gain of the transducer function (i.e., stretching or compressing Inline graphic along its vertical axis), or by adjusting the spread of the noise CDF (i.e., stretching or compressing Inline graphic along its horizontal axis), or both. Since the units in which we express the internal signal are arbitrary, researchers will usually either (1) fix the spread of the noise CDF at some convenient standard value (say, unit variance), and vary the transducer gain to achieve the desired threshold, or (2) fix the gain of the transducer at some convenient standard value (say, unit gain), and vary the spread of the noise CDF to achieve the desired threshold. However, for our purposes, it is more convenient to reformulate Equation (6) so that both the spread of the noise CDF and the gain of the transducer are set to convenient values, and the threshold is specified directly. This allows us to consider general forms of the transducer and noise, without having to worry about specifying the gain of the transducer or spread of the noise correctly – the reformulated equation will take care of the spread of the psychometric function automatically. Theorem 1 derives an expression for the psychometric function that meets these requirements.

Statement of Theorem 1

Theorem 1 has three parts:

  1. The expression for the psychometric function, Inline graphic, in Equation (6) can be rewritten as
    graphic file with name pone.0074815.e218.jpg (11)
    where Inline graphic is the stimulus difference corresponding to a performance level of Inline graphic.
  2. If we change the gain of the transducer by replacing the function Inline graphic with Inline graphic, this will have no effect on the psychometric function, Inline graphic, in Equation (11).

  3. Similarly, if we change the spread of the noise CDF by replacing the function Inline graphic with Inline graphic, this will have no effect on Inline graphic in Equation (11).

Proof of Theorem 1

First, let us substitute the threshold values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic into Equation (6):

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e229.jpg (12)

Equation (12) can be rearranged to give

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e230.jpg (13)

Since the left hand side of Equation (13) is equal to 1, we can multiply anything by this expression, and leave it unchanged. Multiplying the argument of Inline graphic in (6) by this expression, we obtain Equation (11), which proves Part 1 of the theorem. If we replace the transducer, Inline graphic, in Equation (11) with one that has a different gain, Inline graphic, the Inline graphic's will obviously cancel out, leaving the psychometric function, Inline graphic, unchanged, which proves Part 2 of the theorem. To prove Part 3 of the theorem, consider what happens if we replace the function, Inline graphic, in Equation (11) with one that has a different spread, Inline graphic. Then the inverse function is given by Inline graphic, and the Inline graphic's cancel out:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e240.jpg

which is identical to Equation (11).□

Discussion of Theorem 1

Equation (11) gives us an expression for the psychometric function (parameterized by the threshold, Inline graphic) in which we can use any convenient standard form of the transducer function or noise distribution, without having to worry about setting the right gain or spread.

Although, for most of this paper, we define the threshold as the stimulus difference that gives rise to a performance level, Inline graphic, defined as Inline graphic, Theorem 1 actually holds for any value that Inline graphic could have taken.

Note that, in the special case of a zero pedestal (Inline graphic) and a transducer that gives zero output for zero input (Inline graphic), Equation (11) reduces to

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e247.jpg (14)

Theorem 2. An Expression That Estimates the Best-Fitting Weibull β for Unspecified Noise and Transducer

Statement of Theorem 2

Theorem 2 has two parts:

  1. If the parameters of the Weibull function, Inline graphic, of Equation (1) can be set to provide a good fit to Equation (6), then the best-fitting beta will be well approximated by
    graphic file with name pone.0074815.e249.jpg (15)
    where Inline graphic and Inline graphic are given by the following expressions:
    graphic file with name pone.0074815.e252.jpg (16)
    graphic file with name pone.0074815.e253.jpg (17)
    and Inline graphic and Inline graphic are the derivatives of, respectively, Inline graphic and Inline graphic with respect to their inputs.
  2. Inline graphic is an estimate of the Inline graphic of the Weibull function that fits best to the noise CDF, Inline graphic, in Equation (6).

Proof of Theorem 2

By assumption, the Weibull function provides a close fit to Inline graphic of Equation (6), so the gradient of Inline graphic at threshold will closely match the gradient of the best-fitting Weibull function at threshold. Therefore, since Inline graphic is proportional to the gradient of the Weibull function at threshold with an abscissa of Inline graphic, we can derive a close approximation to Inline graphic from the gradient of Inline graphic at threshold on this abscissa. To create a log abscissa, let Inline graphic, so that

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e268.jpg (18)

If we substitute Inline graphic for Inline graphic in Equation (1), we find that the gradient of the Weibull function on the log abscissa, Inline graphic, is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e272.jpg (19)

For the Weibull function at threshold performance (Inline graphic), it follows that Inline graphic. Substituting Inline graphic for Inline graphic in (19) gives

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e277.jpg (20)

and so

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e278.jpg (21)

To evaluate Equation (21), we use the chain rule to expand the derivative:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e279.jpg (22)

As noted above, the assumed good fit of the Weibull function, Inline graphic, of Equation (1) to Inline graphic of Equation (6) means that the output, Inline graphic, of the Weibull function is close to the output of Inline graphic, which is the proportion correct, Inline graphic. Substituting Inline graphic for Inline graphic in Equation (22) therefore gives us a good estimate of Inline graphic, which we call Inline graphic:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e289.jpg (23)

From Equation (6), we see that Inline graphic, so Inline graphic is given by Inline graphic, the noise PDF (which is the derivative of Inline graphic with respect to Inline graphic). At threshold, Inline graphic, and so,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e296.jpg (24)

We will see that the first part of Equation (24), Inline graphic, is proportional to Inline graphic, the Inline graphic-estimate of the Weibull function that fits best to the noise CDF, and the second part, Inline graphic at threshold, is proportional to Inline graphic defined above. Most of the work involves deriving an expression for Inline graphic at threshold.

Using Equation (18) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (3), we get

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e304.jpg (25)

Let us define Inline graphic as the target stimulus value:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e306.jpg (26)

Using Equation (26) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (25), we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e308.jpg (27)

Then,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e309.jpg (28)

where Inline graphic is the derivative of Inline graphic with respect to its input. At threshold, we can substitute Inline graphic for Inline graphic in Equation (28), giving

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e314.jpg (29)

Using Equation (29) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (24), we obtain

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e316.jpg (30)

From Equation (6), we have Inline graphic, so, considering the values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic at threshold,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e320.jpg (31)

Using Equation (31) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (30), we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e322.jpg (32)

To evaluate this expression as written in Equation (32), we need to know the gain of the transducer and the spread of the noise CDF, or at least their ratio. However, if we know the shape of the transducer (apart from the gain), and we know the shape of the noise CDF (apart from the spread), we can work out the ratio of gain to spread from Inline graphic. But it is much more convenient to reformulate Equation (32) so that this is taken care of, and we can arbitrarily set the spread of the noise CDF and the gain of the transducer to any convenient values. We can use the same trick that we used in Theorem 1: We multiply the expression in Equation (32) by the left hand side of Equation (13), which equals 1. After doing this, and rearranging the terms, we obtain

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e324.jpg (33)

Equation (33) can be written in the form given by Equations (15) to (17), which proves the Part 1 of the theorem.

We now prove Part 2, that Inline graphic is the estimated β of the Weibull function that fits best to the noise CDF, Inline graphic. First, note that all linear transducers have the form Inline graphic. This gives Inline graphic, and so, from Equation (17), Inline graphic, regardless of the value of Inline graphic, Inline graphic or Inline graphic. Therefore, from (15), Inline graphic for a linear transducer. Now, consider the linear transducer Inline graphic. For this transducer, Equation (6) gives Inline graphic. The estimate of β when the Weibull function, Inline graphic, is fitted to Inline graphic is given by Inline graphic, as it will be for any linear transducer. Since, in this case, Inline graphic, the Weibull function has also been fitted to the noise CDF, and the estimated Inline graphic of this fitted function is given by Inline graphic.□

Discussion of Theorem 2

To get an intuition into how Weibull Inline graphic is partitioned into the two terms, Inline graphic and Inline graphic, let us refer back to Equation (21). This equation shows that Inline graphic is proportional to Inline graphic at threshold. We used the chain rule to express Inline graphic as Inline graphic, which is approximately equal to Inline graphic. Inline graphic depends only on the noise distribution, and is proportional to Inline graphic; Inline graphic at threshold generally depends on the transducer, the pedestal and the threshold, and is proportional to Inline graphic; their product is proportional to Weibull Inline graphic. This is essentially where Equations (15) to (17) come from. The equations were tidied up by specifying the constants of proportionality, and defining Inline graphic and Inline graphic in such a way that they are independent of any horizontal scaling of the noise distribution, or any vertical scaling of the transducer function. Thus, the Inline graphic term will be the same for, for example, all Gaussian distributions, whatever the spread, and the Inline graphic term will be the same for, for example, all power functions with a particular exponent, whatever the gain.

Equation (17) expresses Inline graphic as a function of the threshold stimulus difference, Inline graphic. Alternatively, for nonzero pedestals, we can reformulate Equation (17) as a function of the Weber fraction, Inline graphic, defined as the ratio Inline graphic at threshold:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e363.jpg (34)

From Equation (34), we obtain Inline graphic, and, using this expression to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (17), we can rewrite the expression for Inline graphic in terms of Inline graphic:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e368.jpg (35)

The Weber fraction can only be defined if Inline graphic. If Inline graphic and Inline graphic, Equation (17) reduces to

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e372.jpg (36)

When the stimulus dimension of interest is contrast, a discrimination experiment with a zero pedestal is called a contrast detection experiment.

One important property of Inline graphic is that it is always greater than 1 for a fully expansive transducer function (i.e., one for which the slope always increases away from zero with increasing input), and is always less than 1 for a fully compressive transducer function (i.e., one for which the slope always decreases towards zero with increasing input). Here we provide a geometrical argument (illustrated in Figure 5) to explain why this is the case.

Figure 5. Geometrical interpretation of the expression for Inline graphic.

Figure 5

In each panel, the thick, magenta curve represents the transducer function. The horizontal axes represent the transducer input, and the vertical axes represent the transducer output. Inline graphic is the pedestal level, and Inline graphic is the discrimination threshold. The gradient of the blue line, Inline graphic, is equal to Inline graphic, defined in Equation (39). The green line is the tangent to the transducer at point Inline graphic; its gradient is equal to Inline graphic, defined in Equation (38). The ratio Inline graphic is equal to Inline graphic. For an expansive transducer (panel A), Inline graphic, so Inline graphic. For a compressive transducer (panel B), Inline graphic, so Inline graphic. For a linear transducer (panel C), Inline graphic, so Inline graphic.

First, note that we can rewrite Equation (17) as

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e389.jpg (37)

where

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e390.jpg (38)

and

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e391.jpg (39)

with

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e392.jpg (40)

These quantities are illustrated for an expansive transducer in Figure 5A, where the thick, magenta curve represents the transducer. The filled circles mark points Inline graphic and Inline graphic. The gradient of the blue line connecting these two points is Inline graphic, defined in Equation (39). The short, green, line segment is the tangent to the transducer at Inline graphic; its gradient is Inline graphic, defined in Equation (38). It is clear from the diagram that, for an expansive transducer, like the one illustrated, the gradient of the transducer at Inline graphic must always be steeper than the blue line, because, as we travel along the transducer function from Inline graphic to Inline graphic, the transducer approaches the second point from below the blue line. Therefore, Inline graphic must always be greater than Inline graphic, so, from Equation (37), Inline graphic must always be greater than 1.

Figure 5B illustrates the situation for a compressive transducer. Here, as we travel along the transducer function from Inline graphic to Inline graphic, the transducer approaches the second point from above the blue line, and so the gradient of the transducer at the second point must be lower than the gradient of the blue line. Thus, Inline graphic must always be less than Inline graphic, so, from Equation (37), Inline graphic must always be less than 1.

Finally, Figure 5C illustrates the situation for a linear transducer, i.e. one that is neither expansive nor compressive. Here, the gradient of the transducer is equal to the gradient of the blue line, so Inline graphic, and therefore Inline graphic. This provides a geometrical insight into the previously proved fact that Inline graphic for a linear transducer.

In conclusion, Weibull Inline graphic can be partitioned into two factors: Inline graphic (Equation (16)), which estimates the Inline graphic of the Weibull function that fits best to the noise CDF, Inline graphic; and Inline graphic (Equation (17), (35) or (36)), which is determined partly (or, as we shall see, sometimes completely) by the shape of the transducer function, Inline graphic. Inline graphic is greater than 1 for an expansive transducer, less than 1 for a compressive transducer, and equal to 1 for a linear transducer. Inline graphic is independent of the spread (i.e. horizontal scaling) of the CDF (analogously, Weibull β is independent of the spread of the Weibull function on linear axes); Inline graphic is independent of the gain (i.e. vertical scaling) of the transducer. Multiplying Inline graphic and Inline graphic together gives us Inline graphic, the estimate of Weibull Inline graphic. The expressions for Inline graphic and Inline graphic derived above are completely general. In later sections, we derive values for Inline graphic given specific noise distributions, and expressions for Inline graphic given specific transducers.

There are two possible sources of error in the Weibull Inline graphic estimate, Inline graphic. Firstly, the derivation of the expression for Inline graphic relies on the use of Inline graphic as an approximation of Inline graphic at threshold in the step from Equation (22) to (23), where Inline graphic is the output of the psychometric function, Inline graphic, and Inline graphic is the output of the best-fitting Weibull function. The accuracy of Inline graphic relies on these two slopes being close at the threshold performance level. A second potential source of inaccuracy is that, even if these two slopes are very close at the threshold level, the overall psychometric function, Inline graphic, might still not be well fit by a Weibull function, in which case the best-fitting Weibull Inline graphic could deviate substantially from Inline graphic. However, as we will show, in the range of conditions usually encountered, the Weibull function does provide a good fit to the psychometric function, so Inline graphic is accurate. In cases where Inline graphic is a Weibull function, the best-fitting Weibull function will fit exactly, and Inline graphic gives the exact value of the best-fitting Weibull Inline graphic.

Deriving Inline graphic for Specific Noise Distributions

As proved in Theorem 2, Inline graphic is an estimate of the Inline graphic of the Weibull function that fits best to the noise CDF. In this section, we evaluate the analytical expression for Inline graphic (Equation (16)) for several different noise distributions. We also compare each value with the Inline graphic value obtained by fitting the Weibull function to the noise CDF numerically. There is of course no single correct answer to the question of what is the best-fitting Inline graphic – it depends on both the fitting criterion and the points on the psychometric function that are sampled. When Pelli [19] fitted the Weibull function to the Gaussian CDF, he minimized the maximum error over all positive inputs. We instead performed a maximum-likelihood fit over all inputs from 0 to twice the threshold (actually, we approximated this by sampling the psychometric function in discrete steps of one thousandth of the threshold). Our rationale for this approach was that fitting the psychometric function is usually done by maximum likelihood, and the threshold usually falls around the middle of the set of stimulus values.

Evaluating Inline graphic for a generalized Gaussian noise CDF

Most psychophysical models use Gaussian noise. This is partly because the Gaussian is often easy to handle analytically, but also because, according to the Central Limit Theorem, the sum of independent sources of noise tends towards a Gaussian-distributed random variable, whatever the distribution of the individual noise sources. However, as noted earlier, Neri [18] has recently argued that internal sensory noise is closer to a Laplace distribution. Both the Gaussian and the Laplace are parameterizations of the generalized Gaussian, which we consider in this section.

The generalized Gaussian CDF is given by the following expression, with horizontal scaling (i.e. spread) determined by Inline graphic, and shape determined by Inline graphic:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e454.jpg (41)

where Inline graphic for Inline graphic and Inline graphic for Inline graphic, and Inline graphic is the lower incomplete gamma function, defined as

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e460.jpg (42)

Inline graphic in Equation (42) is the gamma function, which is a continuous generalization of the factorial, given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e462.jpg (43)

Note, the lower incomplete gamma function is often defined without the normalization term, Inline graphic, but it is more convenient for us to define it as in Equation (42), because otherwise we would just have to divide by Inline graphic anyway, complicating the expression for the generalized Gaussian in Equation (41); in addition, the MATLAB function gammainc evaluates the function as defined in Equation (42).

The variance of the generalized Gaussian distribution is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e465.jpg (44)

We use the subscript, Inline graphic, in Equation (44) to indicate that this is the variance of the noise on the difference of mean signals, Inline graphic, as opposed to the variance of the noise on the transducer outputs, which we could call Inline graphic. As long as the noise on the two transducer outputs within a trial is uncorrelated and has zero mean, then we have Inline graphic, and so Inline graphic, whatever form the noise CDF takes.

The PDF of the generalized Gaussian distribution is given by the derivative of the CDF:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e471.jpg (45)

As noted above, the shape of the distribution is determined by the parameter, Inline graphic. When Inline graphic, Equation (45) describes the Gaussian PDF:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e474.jpg (46)

When Inline graphic, Equation (45) describes the Laplace PDF:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e476.jpg (47)

For positive Inline graphic, the inverse of the generalized Gaussian CDF is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e478.jpg (48)

(we don't need to worry about negative Inline graphic, because, for any monotonically increasing transducer, and positive Inline graphic, Inline graphic as defined in Equation (3) is always positive). The inverse of the lower incomplete gamma function, Inline graphic, in Equation (48) can be evaluated using the MATLAB function gammaincinv. At threshold, Inline graphic. Substituting these values into Equation (48), we get

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e484.jpg (49)

We can use the expression for Inline graphic in Equation (49) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (16), and we can use the expression for Inline graphic in Equation (45) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (16). The different instances of Inline graphic cancel out, giving us an expression for Inline graphic for the generalized Gaussian noise distribution that is a function of Inline graphic:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e492.jpg (50)

where

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e493.jpg (51)

The subscript, “Gen.Gaussian”, on Inline graphic in Equation (50) indicates the general form of the noise CDF.

Figure 6 plots Inline graphic as a function of Inline graphic. As proved in Appendix S1, Inline graphic as Inline graphic. Values of Inline graphic for Inline graphic = 1, 2, and 4 are given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e501.jpg (52)
graphic file with name pone.0074815.e502.jpg (53)
graphic file with name pone.0074815.e503.jpg (54)

The value of Inline graphic for the Laplace distribution (Equation (52)) is exactly 1. This is because the positive half of its CDF is a Weibull function with Inline graphic. This can be seen from the fact that Inline graphic, and so Equation (41) gives, for positive Inline graphic,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e508.jpg (55)

The Weibull function with Inline graphic therefore gives an exact fit to the Laplacian noise CDF, and the estimated Weibull Inline graphic, given by Inline graphic, is exactly correct.

Figure 6. Inline graphic plotted as a function of Inline graphic.

Figure 6

This curve plots the predicted Inline graphic when the Weibull function is fitted to the CDF of generalized Gaussian distributions with a range of different Inline graphic values. The graph asymptotes to a value of Inline graphic (see Appendix S1), indicated by the horizontal dashed line. The shape of the generalized Gaussian distribution is determined by Inline graphic. Inline graphic-values of 1 and 2 are special cases: Inline graphic gives a Laplace distribution, and Inline graphic gives a Gaussian distribution.

The coloured curves in Figures 7A, 7B, and 7C show the generalized Gaussian noise CDFs for Inline graphic = 1, 2, and 4, respectively, and the thick, black curves show the best-fitting Weibull functions (maximum-likelihood fit over inputs from 0 to twice the threshold). Also shown in each panel is the appropriate value of Inline graphic from equations (52) to (54), and the best-fitting Weibull Inline graphic, which we call Inline graphic. As explained above, the match between Inline graphic and Inline graphic is exact for the Laplace (Inline graphic, Figure 7A), but the match is also very good for the other distributions. For the Gaussian (Inline graphic, Figure 7B), Inline graphic, very close to our analytically derived value of Inline graphic. As discussed earlier, Pelli [19] fitted the Weibull to a Gaussian CDF using a different fitting method: He minimized the maximum error over all positive inputs. The Inline graphic value he obtained from this fit was 1.247. As noted earlier, there is no single “correct” answer, but our maximum-likelihood fitting paradigm is probably more representative of the process of fitting a function to psychophysical data, and our obtained Inline graphic of 1.295 is very close to the analytically obtained value. The match between Inline graphic and Inline graphic for Inline graphic (Figure 7C) is also close, the deviation being far smaller than the margin of error usually encountered when measuring Weibull Inline graphic [22][27].

Figure 7. Noise CDFs from Figure 4 plotted against the best-fitting Weibull functions.

Figure 7

The thin, coloured curves shown in (A) to (D) are the CDFs from Figures 4A to 4D, respectively. The thick, black curves are the Weibull functions that give the best (maximum-likelihood) fit across the range of inputs shown on the horizontal axis. This fit was carried out by maximizing the expression Inline graphic Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is the noise CDF, and Inline graphic is the Weibull function whose parameters were being fitted. The Weibull function provides a perfect fit to the Laplace CDF (A), an excellent fit to the Gaussian (B), and logistic (D) CDFs, and an acceptable fit to the generalized Gaussian with Inline graphic (C); this partly justifies our use of Inline graphic as an estimate of Inline graphic in Equation (23). The Inline graphic values are the Inline graphic parameters of the fitted Weibull functions. The Inline graphic values are our analytical estimates of Inline graphic, given by Equations (52) to (54) for panels (A) to (C), respectively, and Equation (60) for panel (D). In each case, Inline graphic provides a close match to Inline graphic. The parameter in brackets in each Inline graphic term is the shape parameter, Inline graphic (see Equation (50)). As noted in the text, the CDFs all have a point of inflection at zero. With the exception of panel A, the best-fitting Weibull functions have a point of inflection slightly above zero (Inline graphic would have to be 1 or less for the steepest point to occur at zero). Nevertheless, the Weibull functions still provide good fits.

Evaluating Inline graphic for the logistic noise CDF

Sometimes, the logistic function is used instead of the Gaussian, for computational convenience (e.g. Ref. [28]). The logistic function is very similar in shape to the Gaussian. Its CDF is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e554.jpg (56)

As noted by Strasburger [29], this function is identical to the hyperbolic tangent function, given by Inline graphic. Its variance is Inline graphic. The PDF of the logistic distribution is given by the derivative of the CDF:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e557.jpg (57)

The inverse of the logistic CDF is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e558.jpg (58)

At threshold, Inline graphic. Substituting these values into Equation (58), we get

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e560.jpg (59)

We can use the expression for Inline graphic in Equation (59) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (16), and we can use the expression for Inline graphic in Equation (57) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (16). This gives

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e565.jpg (60)

As before, the subscript on Inline graphic indicates the form of noise CDF. The accuracy of this approximation is confirmed in Figure 7D. The Inline graphic parameter of the fitted Weibull function (Inline graphic) is very close to the estimated value from Equation (60).

Theorem 3. Tendency towards Linear Behaviour with Non-Zero Pedestals

Introduction

As shown earlier, for a linear transducer, Inline graphic, and so Inline graphic, which takes a value of around 1.3 for Gaussian internal noise. So, if a transducer model has additive Gaussian noise and generates psychometric functions with a Weibull Inline graphic of about 1.3, that might seem to suggest that it contains a linear transducer. However, a transducer model with additive Gaussian noise can in fact generate psychometric functions with Inline graphic for suprathreshold contrast discrimination even when the transducer departs wildly from a linear function [4]. Theorem 3 explains how this occurs.

Statement of Theorem 3

If the gradient of the transducer is not 0 or Inline graphic at the pedestal level, then, as Inline graphic, Inline graphic.

Proof of Theorem 3

As noted earlier, Inline graphic, where Inline graphic and Inline graphic are given in Equations (38) and (39), respectively. The limit of Inline graphic as Inline graphic is the derivative of Inline graphic at Inline graphic, i.e.Inline graphic, by definition of the derivative, and the limit of Inline graphic as Inline graphic is obviously Inline graphic, so we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e587.jpg (61)

Then, provided that Inline graphic is not 0 or Inline graphic, we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e590.jpg (62)

If Inline graphic is 0 or Inline graphic, then Inline graphic is an indeterminate form, Inline graphic or Inline graphic, and cannot be evaluated. In this case, we cannot evaluate the limit of Inline graphic by dividing the limit of Inline graphic by the limit of Inline graphic. The limit must instead be evaluated in some other way that will depend on the form of the transducer, and the limit in this case will not necessarily be 1.

Discussion of Theorem 3

Theorem 3 shows that, whatever the transducer function, as long as its gradient is not 0 or Inline graphic at the pedestal level, Weibull Inline graphic will approach that for a linear transducer as sensitivity improves. Virtually all proposed transducers do have a finite, nonzero gradient for nonzero inputs; therefore, if the internal noise is approximately Gaussian, we would expect Weibull Inline graphic to be close to 1.3 for suprathreshold contrast discrimination. Detection and discrimination data are often fitted with a power-function transducer or a Legge-Foley transducer (both considered below), and, with these transducers, the gradient is 0 or Inline graphic at an input level of zero. Thus, for these transducers, when the pedestal level is zero, Equation (62) does not apply, and Inline graphic does not necessarily approach that for a linear transducer as sensitivity improves. This explains why, for contrast detection experiments (i.e. when the pedestal is zero), Weibull Inline graphic has been found to deviate greatly from the value of 1.3 expected from a linear transducer with Gaussian noise.

Consider what happens in general when the pedestal approaches zero. If we assume that Inline graphic, then, as Inline graphic drops below Inline graphic, both Inline graphic (Equation (38)) and Inline graphic (Equation (39)) become dominated by the Inline graphic term, and Inline graphic approaches the value given in Equation (36), which is not, in general, equal to 1. Thus, we would expect Weibull Inline graphic to deviate substantially from the linear case for low pedestals. Meese, Georgeson and Baker [4] showed that this is indeed the case for visual contrast discrimination, and we examine their work in more detail later, in the section on the Legge-Foley transducer.

Deriving Psychometric Functions and Weibull β for Specific Nonlinear Transducers

As shown earlier, Inline graphic for any linear transducer. For a nonlinear transducer, Inline graphic will deviate from 1, and this is how the transducer has its effect on Inline graphic, the estimated Weibull Inline graphic. Starting with one of the general expressions for Inline graphic (Equation (17), (35) or (36), as appropriate), we can substitute a specific transducer function for the general function, Inline graphic, to give a specific expression that describes Inline graphic for that transducer. Similarly, starting with one of the general expressions for the psychometric function (Equation (11) or (14), as appropriate), we can substitute a specific transducer function for the general function, Inline graphic, to give a specific expression for the psychometric function. In this section, we consider five commonly used scenarios: a power function with zero or nonzero pedestal, a log function, and a Legge-Foley function [30] with zero or nonzero pedestal.

Power-function transducers have been used to account for visual contrast discrimination data. As the pedestal increases from 0, the discrimination threshold first decreases, and then starts to increase with further increases in pedestal; this function, giving contrast discrimination threshold at each pedestal level, is known as a “dipper function”. The initial dip can be explained by an expansive power function (i.e., one with exponent greater than 1) at low contrasts [1], [6], while the increase in contrast discrimination threshold for larger pedestals can be explained by a compressive power function (i.e., one with exponent less than 1) at high contrasts. The Legge-Foley transducer approximates an expansive power function at low contrasts and a compressive power function at high contrasts, and accounts for the whole dipper function [4], [30]. We also include the log transducer in our analysis, firstly because discrimination at high pedestal levels has often been found to adhere closely to Weber's law in many different perceptual dimensions and sensory modalities [31][35] (a prediction of the log transducer with additive noise), and, secondly, because we have discovered an interesting link between the power function and the log transducer, which is presented in Theorem 4.

Power function and zero pedestal

The first case that we consider is the one examined by Pelli [19], i.e. Inline graphic. As noted earlier, this relationship between Inline graphic and Inline graphic is consistent with a power function transducer (Inline graphic) and zero pedestal (Inline graphic). In this case, we can use Equation (36) to derive Inline graphic, and it follows easily that

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e627.jpg (63)

(as with Inline graphic, the subscript on Inline graphic describes the specific case). Thus, using Equation (63) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (15), we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e631.jpg (64)

which is the relationship derived by Pelli [19] (Relation (7) of this paper).

From Equation (14), it follows that, for a power function transducer and zero pedestal, the model's true psychometric function is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e632.jpg (65)

with the subscript on Inline graphic describing the specific case. Equation (65) gives us the option of expressing the stimulus difference in absolute units, Inline graphic, or in “threshold units”, Inline graphic – the two options differ only in a linear horizontal scaling. The latter is useful when dealing with general cases where the threshold is not specified; the psychometric function is often expressed in this way [19], [29], [36]. The coloured curves in Figure 8 show the psychometric function of Equation (65). Different rows of panels show psychometric functions for different noise CDFs, Inline graphic, as indicated on the right of the figure. Different columns of panels show psychometric functions for different transducer exponents, Inline graphic. The thick, black curves show the best-fitting Weibull functions. These provide a good fit to the true psychometric functions, justifying the premise of Theorem 2, which is that the Weibull function provides a good fit.

Figure 8. Psychometric functions resulting from power-function transducers and zero pedestal.

Figure 8

The thin, coloured curves show the psychometric function of Equation (65), plotted as a function of Inline graphic. Different rows of panels show psychometric functions with different noise CDFs, Inline graphic, given by the Laplace distribution (top row of panels), the Gaussian (second row), the generalized Gaussian with Inline graphic (third row) or logistic (bottom row). Different columns of panels show psychometric functions for different transducer exponents, Inline graphic, as indicated at the top of the figure. The thick, black curves show the best-fitting (maximum-likelihood) Weibull functions. The curves in the middle column (Inline graphic, top to bottom) are identical to Figures 7A to 7D, respectively. This is because Inline graphic gives a linear transducer, and so the psychometric functions for Inline graphic will have the same shape and same fitted Inline graphic as the CDF (see Equation (10)). Each panel displays the Inline graphic value of the best-fitting Weibull function (Inline graphic) and the estimate, Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is given by Equation (52), (53), (54) or (60), as appropriate.

Each panel of Figure 8 also compares Inline graphic of the best-fitting Weibull function (Inline graphic) with the estimate, Inline graphic, given by Inline graphic. In every case, Inline graphic is very close to the fitted value, the discrepancy being far smaller than the margin of error usually encountered in psychophysical measurements of psychometric function slope [22][27]. For each transducer (i.e. each column of Figure 8), the difference in Inline graphic between the different noise CDFs (i.e. between the different rows of Figure 8) is caused entirely by the different values of Inline graphic. For example, the value of Inline graphic for the Gaussian will always exceed that for the Laplace by a factor Inline graphic.

Power function and nonzero pedestal

We now consider the case of a power function and any pedestal value, a generalization of the previous case. First, starting with Equation (17), we trivially obtain

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e659.jpg (66)

which reduces to Equation (63) when Inline graphic. When Inline graphic, we can start with Equation (35), from which it follows straightforwardly that

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e662.jpg (67)

Figure 9 plots Inline graphic as a function of the Weber fraction, Inline graphic (defined in Equation (34)), for several values of the transducer exponent, Inline graphic. These curves all converge to a value of 1 towards the left. This is because, for a power function transducer with nonzero pedestal, the gradient of the transducer at the pedestal level is not 0 or Inline graphic, and so, as proved in Theorem 3, Inline graphic as Inline graphic.

Figure 9. Inline graphic for the power-function transducer with nonzero pedestal, plotted as a function of Weber fraction.

Figure 9

Each curve gives Inline graphic for a different transducer exponent, Inline graphic. Inline graphic asymptotes towards 1 as Inline graphic decreases, and towards Inline graphic as Inline graphic increases. For typical Weber fractions of less than 0.3 (see Table 1), Inline graphic does not deviate much from 1. The bottom curve, in black, shows the limiting case, as Inline graphic. All the plotted functions except the one for Inline graphic are given by Equation (67). In Theorem 4B, we prove that the limiting case as Inline graphic is identical to the curve corresponding to a logarithmic transducer; this curve is given by Equation (70).

From Equation (11), we can see that, for a power function transducer with unspecified pedestal, the model's true psychometric function is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e680.jpg (68)

If Inline graphic, then we can divide through by Inline graphic, and rewrite Equation (68) in terms of the Weber fraction, Inline graphic:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e684.jpg (69)

Equation (68) is a general formula for the psychometric function, given a power-function transducer and any pedestal value. Equations (65) and (69) are simpler expressions for this psychometric function in the cases of zero and nonzero pedestals, respectively. As with Equation (65), Equation (69) gives us the option of expressing the stimulus difference in absolute units, Inline graphic, or threshold units, Inline graphic. The coloured curves in Figure 10 show the psychometric function of Equation (69) for a range of Weber fractions and a transducer exponent of 2. Different rows of panels show psychometric functions for different noise CDFs, and different columns of panels show psychometric functions for different Weber fractions, Inline graphic. The thick, black curves show the best-fitting Weibull functions. Each panel also compares Inline graphic of the best-fitting Weibull function (Inline graphic) with the estimate, Inline graphic, given by Inline graphic. Figure 11 is the same as Figure 10 except that the transducer exponent is 0.5. In every case, the Weibull function fits well to the true psychometric function, and the agreement between Inline graphic and Inline graphic is very good.

Figure 10. Psychometric functions resulting from transducer Inline graphic and nonzero pedestal.

Figure 10

The thin, coloured curves show the psychometric function of Equation (69) with Inline graphic. Different rows of panels show psychometric functions with different noise CDFs, as indicated on the right of the figure. Different columns of panels show psychometric functions for different Weber fractions, Inline graphic. The thick, black curves show the best-fitting (maximum-likelihood) Weibull functions. Each panel displays the Inline graphic value of the best-fitting Weibull function (Inline graphic) and the estimate, Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is given by Equation (67), and Inline graphic is given by Equation (52), (53), (54) or (60), as appropriate.

Figure 11. Psychometric functions resulting from transducer Inline graphic and nonzero pedestal.

Figure 11

All details are the same as in Figure 10, except that the transducer exponent is 0.5.

It is interesting to compare the psychometric functions in Figures 10 and 11 with those for the same transducer when the pedestal is zero; these are given in the columns of Figure 8 headed “Inline graphic” and “Inline graphic”, respectively. It is clear that, even with the rather large Weber fraction of 0.8, the existence of a nonzero pedestal brings Weibull Inline graphic much closer to the linear case (the case of a linear transducer is shown in the column of Figure 8 headed “Inline graphic”).

Logarithmic transducer

The log function is undefined for zero inputs, so we can only consider a log transducer for nonzero pedestals. If the transducer takes a logarithmic shape for all inputs greater than the pedestal value, then it is effectively logarithmic for the whole of the range of stimulus values being considered.

For a logarithmic transducer, Inline graphic, Equation (35) leads to

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e708.jpg (70)

for any base of logarithm, Inline graphic. In Theorem 4B, below, we show that, for any Weber fraction, Inline graphic is the limiting value of Inline graphic as Inline graphic. The bottom (black) curve in Figure 9 plots Inline graphic as a function of Inline graphic.

Starting with Equation (11), it is straightforward to show that, for a log transducer, the model's true psychometric function is given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e715.jpg (71)

The coloured curves in Figure 12 show the psychometric function of Equation (71) for a range of Weber fractions. Different rows of panels show psychometric functions for different noise CDFs, and different columns of panels show psychometric functions for different Weber fractions. The thick, black curves show the best-fitting Weibull functions. Each panel also compares Inline graphic of the best-fitting Weibull function (Inline graphic) with the estimate, Inline graphic, given by Inline graphic. The Weibull function gives an excellent fit to the true psychometric function in every case, and the agreement between Inline graphic and Inline graphic is very good. As the Weber fraction decreases, Weibull Inline graphic approaches that for the linear case.

Figure 12. Psychometric functions resulting from a logarithmic transducer.

Figure 12

The thin, coloured curves show the psychometric function of Equation (71). Different rows of panels show psychometric functions with different noise CDFs, as indicated on the right of the figure. Different columns of panels show psychometric functions for different Weber fractions, Inline graphic. The thick, black curves show the best-fitting (maximum-likelihood) Weibull functions. Each panel displays the Inline graphic value of the best-fitting Weibull function (Inline graphic) and the estimate, Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is given by Equation (70), and Inline graphic is given by Equation (52), (53), (54) or (60), as appropriate.

Legge-Foley transducer

If the noise is additive, then no single power-function transducer can fit contrast discrimination data across the whole contrast range, because we need an expansive function to explain facilitation at low contrasts, and a compressive function to explain the rise in threshold with pedestal at high contrasts. Legge and Foley [30] used a sigmoid transducer that was expansive at low contrasts and compressive at high contrasts, as required:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e729.jpg (72)

where Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic and Inline graphic are constants, all greater than zero. The transducer in Equation (72) seems to have been first used in psychophysics by Stromeyer and Klein [37], and it is sometimes referred to as the Stromeyer-Foley function [36], [38], but we use the term “Legge-Foley transducer”, as Legge and Foley's use of this transducer is probably better known. For low inputs, Inline graphic, the Legge-Foley transducer approximates a power function with exponent Inline graphic; for large inputs, it approximates a power function with exponent Inline graphic. Legge and Foley had Inline graphic and Inline graphic, so the transducer was an accelerating power function (with exponent Inline graphic) for low inputs, and a compressive power function (with exponent Inline graphic) for high inputs. The point of inflection (at which the transducer changes from expansive to compressive) occurs at an Inline graphic value close to Inline graphic for typical values of the fitted parameters (see Appendix S2 for a derivation of the formula for calculating the position of the point of inflection).

Assuming a zero pedestal, we can substitute Inline graphic for Inline graphic in Equation (36), giving

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e745.jpg (73)

If the threshold, Inline graphic, is much less than Inline graphic, the right hand side of Equation (73) approaches Inline graphic, as we would expect: The Legge-Foley transducer in this case approximates a power-function transducer with exponent Inline graphic, and, for the latter transducer with zero pedestal, Inline graphic is simply equal to the exponent, as in Equation (63).

For nonzero pedestals, we can substitute Inline graphic for Inline graphic in Equation (17) to obtain, after some work,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e753.jpg (74)

Figure 13 plots Inline graphic as defined in Equation (74) as a function of Inline graphic, for typical ranges of Inline graphic, Inline graphic, and Inline graphic. The middle panel of the left column (Inline graphic, Inline graphic) is very close to Legge and Foley's [30] parameters, while the middle panel of the middle column (Inline graphic, Inline graphic) is very close to Meese et al.'s [4] fitted parameters, which we describe in detail later.

Figure 13. Inline graphic for the Legge-Foley transducer with nonzero pedestal.

Figure 13

The curves were generated using Equation (74). Each column of panels has a particular value for Inline graphic, and each row of panels has a particular value for the difference Inline graphic. Within the panels, the Weber fraction, Inline graphic, is indicated by the colour of the curve (the legend in the top-left panel applies to all panels). The curves approach horizontal asymptotes on the right (indicated by dotted lines), with vertical position given by Equation (67) with Inline graphic. This is because, as mentioned earlier, as the input signal increases, the Legge-Foley transducer approaches a power function with exponent Inline graphic. This asymptote can also be derived from Equation (74) by setting Inline graphic to 0, which gives the limit as Inline graphic. On the left, the curves come close to approaching an asymptote with vertical position given by Equation (67) with Inline graphic because, at low contrasts, the Legge-Foley transducer approximates a power function with exponent Inline graphic. These near-asymptotes are indicated by dotted lines on the left of each panel. They are not true asymptotes because, even for Inline graphic, the Legge-Foley transducer is not exactly equal to a power function over a finite range of inputs. The horizontal, dashed lines indicate Inline graphic. The vertical dashed lines indicate the value of Inline graphic corresponding to the point of inflection of the Legge-Foley transducer. An expression for this quantity is derived in Appendix S2. For typical values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic, including those in this figure, the point of inflection occurs very close to an input of Inline graphic, giving Inline graphic. For pedestals above this value, both the target and pedestal will lie in the compressive region of the Legge-Foley transducer, so Inline graphic must be less than 1. For this reason, none of the curves enter the top-right quadrant in any of the panels.

One striking feature of these functions is that they all have a dipper shape – as the pedestal increases, Inline graphic dips down to a minimum and then increases slightly before approaching its asymptote on the right. While it is well known that the discrimination threshold, Weibull Inline graphic, traces out a dipper function as Inline graphic increases from zero [4], [6], [30], [34], [39], to the best of our knowledge no one has ever reported a dipper function for Weibull Inline graphic before, so we set out to see if there was evidence for one in the previous literature. We found such a dipper function for Inline graphic in the data of Henning and Wichmann [40] (see Figure 14 and Table 1). Henning and Wichmann did not report Weibull Inline graphic, but they reported all thresholds at three different performance levels, allowing us to fit Weibull functions to their data.

Figure 14. Dipper functions for Weibull Inline graphic from Henning and Wichmann's data.

Figure 14

Weibull Inline graphic was fitted to Henning and Wichmann's [40] published data as described in the legend of Table 1. These Inline graphic values are plotted in black lines and symbols, excluding observer GBH's Inline graphic value of 13.1 for a pedestal contrast of 0.01, which is obviously an outlier. In each case, the function mapping pedestal contrast to Inline graphic has a dipper shape. To see whether the dip occurred in the predicted location, we fitted a Legge-Foley transducer model to Henning and Wichmann's data separately for each observer. The model's predicted proportion correct, Inline graphic, was given by Equation (6) with the transducer function, Inline graphic, given by the 4-parameter Legge-Foley transducer (Equation (72)), and the noise CDF, Inline graphic, given by the generalized Gaussian (Equation (41)), which had Inline graphic as a free parameter, and Inline graphic set so that Inline graphic, using Equation (44) (thus we adjusted sensitivity by adjusting the transducer gain, rather than the noise CDF spread). For each pedestal value, Henning and Wichmann reported the contrast differences, Inline graphic, corresponding to three different performance levels (proportion correct, Inline graphic = 0.6, 0.75, or 0.9), sampled from their fitted psychometric functions. We performed a maximum-likelihood fit of the Legge-Foley transducer model to the data, by adjusting the parameters to maximize the likelihood, Inline graphic. Fitted model parameter sets (Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic) were (3.78, 3.38, 0.0322, 15.3, 0.947) for GBH, (3.36, 3.02, 0.00968, 22.7, 2.09) for NAL, and (3.93, 3.51, 0.0102, 18.9, 2.15) for TCC. For each observer and pedestal value, we used a numerical search method to find the threshold, Inline graphic, corresponding to a proportion correct of Inline graphic, and then calculated the Weber fraction, Inline graphic, using Equation (34). We then found Inline graphic using Equation (74), and Inline graphic using Equation (50). The analytical prediction of Weibull Inline graphic is then given by Inline graphic, and this is plotted in magenta in the figure. Each observer's global minimum in Weibull Inline graphic was close to that in the analytical prediction.

For the log and power-function transducers, Inline graphic is a function with one or two arguments, so it was practical to test the accuracy of the expressions for a range of plausible arguments. In contrast, Inline graphic has five arguments, corresponding to three of the transducer parameters, as well as the pedestal and either the Weber fraction or the threshold. To constrain the argument space so that we can test the accuracy of Equations (73) and (74), it is helpful to use values for these arguments that have occurred in real experiments. Legge and Foley's study is not suitable for this because their model had several channels, and did not have the straightforward relationship between stimulus and probability of a correct response described by Equation (6). However, the psychometric function generated by Meese et al.'s [4] preferred model really is a parameterization of Equation (6), so we can assess the accuracy of Equations (73) and (74) for their stimulus values and transducer parameters.

Meese et al.'s study was on binocular integration, and their data were best fit by a model that they called the “twin summation” model. This model has a transducer that extends Legge and Foley's transducer so that it can handle inputs from left and right eyes:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e816.jpg (75)

where Inline graphic is the stimulus contrast in the left eye, and Inline graphic is the contrast in the right eye. In Equation (75), we use upper-case Inline graphic in place of the lower-case Inline graphic that Meese et al. used, to avoid confusion with our own Inline graphic, defined in Equation (3). In Meese et al.'s fully binocular condition (Inline graphic), Equation (75) reduces to the standard Legge-Foley transducer of Equation (72), with

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e823.jpg (76)
graphic file with name pone.0074815.e824.jpg (77)
graphic file with name pone.0074815.e825.jpg (78)
graphic file with name pone.0074815.e826.jpg (79)

In Meese et al.'s fully monocular condition (Inline graphic and Inline graphic or vice-versa), Equation (75) reduces to the Legge-Foley transducer with Inline graphic and Inline graphic defined as in Equations (76) and (77), but with the other parameters given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e831.jpg (80)
graphic file with name pone.0074815.e832.jpg (81)

The fitted values of Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic, and Inline graphic appear in the bottom line of Meese et al.'s Table 2. Using these values, we can specify the equivalent Legge-Foley transducer in the binocular or monocular conditions using Equation (72) with Inline graphic and Inline graphic given by Equations (76) and (77), and Inline graphic and Inline graphic given by Equations (78) and (79) for the binocular condition, and by Equations (80) and (81) in the monocular condition. The values of Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic and Inline graphic are given in the legend to our Figure 15 for the binocular condition, and Figure 16 for the monocular condition. In summary, although Meese et al. fitted a single transducer function across all their conditions, the equivalent Legge-Foley transducer differs between the binocular and monocular conditions. In both cases, Inline graphic and Inline graphic, giving Inline graphic, so, although the effective exponent at low contrasts was substantially higher than that of Legge and Foley [30], the effective exponent at high contrasts was similar.

Figure 15. Psychometric functions resulting from a Legge-Foley transducer in Meese et al.'s binocular condition.

Figure 15

The thin, green curves show the psychometric functions generated by Meese et al.'s [4] twin-summation model in their binocular condition; in this condition, their transducer is equivalent to the Legge-Foley transducer of Equation (72) with Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Inline graphic. Note Inline graphic is in units of % contrast, as used by Meese et al.; to convert to units of Michelson contrast, Inline graphic should be divided by 100. The CDF of the noise on the internal difference signal, Inline graphic, is a cumulative Gaussian with standard deviation given by Inline graphic. Each panel gives the model's psychometric function for a different pedestal contrast, Inline graphic, in Meese et al.'s binocular condition. The thick, black curves show the best-fitting (maximum-likelihood) Weibull functions. Each panel displays the Inline graphic value of the best-fitting Weibull function (Inline graphic) and the estimate, Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is given by Equation (73) for Inline graphic, and by Equation (74) for the other pedestal levels. The Weber fraction, Inline graphic, in these equations was calculated from the model's threshold, found by inverting the model's psychometric function using a numerical search method, as explained in the text. Because this model fitted well to Meese et al.'s data, these Weber fractions are close to (but not exactly equal to) the actual Weber fractions obtained in the experiment, given in Table 1.

Figure 16. Psychometric functions resulting from a Legge-Foley transducer in Meese et al.'s monocular condition.

Figure 16

The same as Figure 15, but for Meese et al.'s monocular condition. In this condition, their twin summation model is equivalent to the Legge-Foley transducer of Equation (72) with the same parameters as those given in the legend to Figure 15, except with Inline graphic and Inline graphic.

Meese et al. had one further parameter, the standard deviation, Inline graphic, of the noise on the transducer output, which took a fitted value of 0.259. Meese et al. assumed uncorrelated zero-mean Gaussian noise on each signal, so the two sources of noise on each trial would combine to produce Gaussian noise on the internal difference signal with standard deviation given by Inline graphic. We can therefore define the noise CDF, Inline graphic, as the cumulative Gaussian with standard deviation Inline graphic. This is equivalent to Equation (45) with Inline graphic and Inline graphic.

After we have defined the transducer, Inline graphic, and the noise CDF, Inline graphic, the psychometric function (i.e., the mapping from Inline graphic onto probability correct, Inline graphic) is fully defined by Equation (6). For the Legge-Foley transducer, the psychometric function cannot be inverted algebraically, but the fitted model's threshold, Inline graphic, can be found by searching for the contrast difference, Inline graphic, that gives rise to a probability correct of Inline graphic. The Weber fraction, Inline graphic, is then given by Equation (34), and the obtained values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic can be used in Equations (73) or (74) (depending on whether or not Inline graphic), along with Inline graphic, Inline graphic, and Inline graphic, to calculate a value for Inline graphic for each pedestal level.

Each panel of Figure 15 shows (in green) the psychometric function generated by Meese et al's twin summation model for a particular pedestal level in their binocular condition. The thick, black curves show the best-fitting Weibull functions. Each panel also compares Inline graphic of the best-fitting Weibull function (Inline graphic) with the estimate, Inline graphic, given by Inline graphic, with Inline graphic given by Equation (53) (the model's Weber fractions, Inline graphic, which are used to calculate Inline graphic, are given in the individual panels). The Weibull fits are good, and the agreement between Inline graphic and Inline graphic is mostly excellent. Note how Weibull Inline graphic begins to deviate substantially from the linear value (1.3) as the pedestal drops to very low levels, as explained at the end of the Discussion of Theorem 3. Note, too, how both Inline graphic and Inline graphic show a dipper function, with the lowest value falling at a pedestal level just above Inline graphic (Inline graphic); the finding of a dipper function for Inline graphic in close agreement with that of Inline graphic verifies that the predicted dipper function for Inline graphic shown in Figures 13 and 14 is a real prediction of the Legge-Foley transducer, and not just a peculiarity of our analytical approximation. Unlike the data of Henning and Wichmann [40], Meese et al.'s [4] data (given in Table 1) do not actually show a dipper for Inline graphic, but this is not a serious concern because, as mentioned earlier, Weibull Inline graphic is difficult to measure accurately, and such a small effect on Inline graphic could easily be lost in the experimental noise. Figure 16 shows the same analysis for Meese et al's monocular condition.

Theorem 4. For Nonzero Pedestals, 2AFC Performance for a Power-Function Transducer Approaches That for a Log Transducer as the Exponent Approaches Zero

Introduction

We stated earlier that, for a nonzero pedestal, as the exponent of a power function transducer approaches zero, Inline graphic approaches that for a logarithmic transducer, whatever the Weber fraction. This seems a remarkable finding, because the expressions for Inline graphic and Inline graphic (given by Equations (67) and (70), respectively) appear quite different. In fact, for a given threshold level, the whole 2AFC psychometric function for the power-function transducer and nonzero pedestal (given by Equation (69)) approaches that for a log transducer (Equation (71)) as the exponent, Inline graphic, in Equation (69) approaches zero.

Both of these results stem from a more fundamental result: As the exponent, Inline graphic, of a power function approaches zero, the function converges towards a log function plus a constant. Because 2AFC performance in the transducer model is based on the difference of transducer outputs, this constant cancels out, and, in the limit as Inline graphic, the difference of power functions equals the difference of log functions.

This can be understood from Lemma 1, below.

Lemma 1

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e913.jpg (82)

Proof. To begin with, note that, for Inline graphic,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e915.jpg (83)

Therefore,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e916.jpg

From Lemma 1, we can see that, when Inline graphic is small,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e918.jpg (84)

which explains why the power function converges towards a log function plus a constant, Inline graphic, as the exponent, Inline graphic, approaches zero. When the transducer outputs are subtracted to make the decision in a 2AFC task, this constant cancels out, so we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e921.jpg (85)

and this is why 2AFC performance for the power function approaches that for a log function as the transducer exponent approaches 0. This does not apply to a zero pedestal because the log function is undefined for zero input.

For those readers who find this informal argument unconvincing, we now prove directly that the two psychometric functions are identical (Theorem 4A) and that the Inline graphic values in the two cases are also identical (Theorem 4B).

Theorem 4A

Statement of Theorem 4A

Inline graphic, defined in Equation (69), approaches Inline graphic, defined in Equation (71), as the power-function exponent, Inline graphic, in Equation (69) tends to zero.

Proof

From Equation (69),

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e926.jpg (86)

So,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e927.jpg (87)

Applying Lemma 1 to the numerator and denominator,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e928.jpg (88)

Theorem 4B

Statement of Theorem 4B

As Inline graphic, Inline graphic.

Proof

We can rewrite Equation (67) as

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e931.jpg (89)

The limit of the numerator of Equation (89) as Inline graphic is simply Inline graphic, and the limit of the denominator as Inline graphic is given by Lemma 1, so we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e935.jpg (90)
graphic file with name pone.0074815.e936.jpg (91)
graphic file with name pone.0074815.e937.jpg (92)

Discussion of Theorem 4

Theorem 4 shows that, for nonzero pedestals, 2AFC performance with a power function transducer approaches that of a log transducer as the power-function exponent approaches zero. As noted earlier, contrast discrimination data have previously been fit with the Legge-Foley transducer of Equation (72) with parameters set so that, at high contrasts, the transducer was approximately a power function with an exponent of around 0.4–0.5 [4], [30]. Figure 17 shows that an exponent of 0.5 is close enough to zero to make the psychometric function very similar to that from a log function (compare the blue and black curves in Figure 17).

Figure 17. Psychometric functions for the power-function transducer with nonzero pedestal.

Figure 17

The psychometric function for Inline graphic was generated using Equation (71); the others were generated using Equation (69). In both cases, we assumed Gaussian internal noise (i.e. Inline graphic is the cumulative Gaussian). All the psychometric functions go through the point Inline graphic, by definition of the threshold (the abscissa is in threshold units, i.e. Inline graphic). Each panel shows psychometric functions for a particular Weber fraction. Each curve within a panel shows the psychometric function for a particular transducer exponent, Inline graphic. The orange curve (Inline graphic) is the psychometric function plotted in green in the second-to-top row of Figure 10. The blue curve (Inline graphic) is the psychometric function plotted in green in the second-to-top row of Figure 11. The black line shows the limit as Inline graphic. As proved in Theorem 4A, this limiting case is identical to the psychometric function for a log transducer. This is the psychometric function plotted in green in the second-to-top row of Figure 12. This figure illustrates two effects. Within each panel, we see how the psychometric function for the power-function transducer converges towards that for a log transducer as the exponent decreases (Theorem 4). Across panels (right-to-left), we see a demonstration of the effect proved in Theorem 3, whereby, with a nonzero pedestal, all psychometric functions converge towards that for a linear transducer as the discrimination threshold decreases (in this case, since we are plotting psychometric functions for Gaussian noise, the functions converge towards the pure noise CDF of Figure 4B as the Weber fraction decreases).

General Discussion

In 2AFC discrimination experiments, the observer can be modelled using a transducer, followed by constant, additive noise. In this paradigm, the psychometric function is given by Equation (6), where Inline graphic is the CDF of the internal noise, and Inline graphic is the transducer function. The model's sensitivity to a stimulus difference, which determines its threshold, can be adjusted by setting the gain on the transducer (i.e. stretching or compressing Inline graphic vertically) while keeping the spread of the noise CDF constant at some convenient level, or by setting the spread of the noise CDF (i.e. stretching or compressing Inline graphic horizontally) while keeping the gain of the transducer constant at some convenient level. Theorem 1 reformulates Equation (6) so that both the transducer gain and the spread of the noise CDF can be set to convenient levels, and the threshold can be set directly.

Although the presentation of the theorems in this paper makes heavy use of the theoretical framework in which the stimulus signal is put through a transducer, and stimulus-independent noise is added, it is not necessary to accept this model to find the theorems useful: All we need to assume is that the psychometric function has a form consistent with such a model. For example, the intrinsic uncertainty model contains no transducer, but generates a psychometric function that closely approximates that of a power-function transducer with additive Gaussian noise [21], so the theorems in this paper can be applied to that model as if was a transducer model.

Nevertheless, the theorems do have added value if we go along with the transducer model, because they give an insight into the roles played by the different elements of the transducer model in determining the form of the psychometric function. In the next section, we give a summary of some of the insights that we have gained into the Weibull function. The sections after that examine some of the issues in more detail; each of these detailed sections is self-contained, and any of them can be skipped without affecting the intelligibility of the other sections.

The Weibull Function

Functions of proportion correct against stimulus difference are often fitted with a Weibull function, which has two parameters of interest: the threshold, Inline graphic, and “slope” or “shape” parameter, Inline graphic. Most psychophysical research has focussed on the threshold, but Inline graphic can be informative too, and has proved useful when competing models make quite similar predictions of threshold [4]. Theorem 2 shows what happens to Inline graphic when the Weibull function is fitted to the psychometric function for the transducer model, given in Equation (6). This theorem shows that Inline graphic can be partitioned into two factors: Inline graphic, which depends only on the shape of the internal noise distribution, and Inline graphic, which depends on the transducer function, and can also depend on the pedestal level and the observer's threshold. Weibull Inline graphic is estimated by multiplying these two factors together. Inline graphic is the estimate of the Inline graphic of the Weibull function that fits best to the noise CDF. We found that, for all the noise CDFs in Figure 7, Inline graphic accurately estimates the best-fitting Weibull Inline graphic, which validates the accuracy of our general expression for Inline graphic (Equation (16)) for a range of noise CDFs. From our general expressions for Inline graphic and Inline graphic, we derived expressions for several specific cases. In each case, these specific expressions provided accurate estimates of the fitted Weibull Inline graphics, and will do so in any other situation in which we can express the observer's or model's true psychometric function in the form of Equation (6), and the Weibull function provides a good fit (this is because the only premise of Theorem 2 is that the Weibull function can be adjusted to provide a good fit to Equation (6)).

As well as providing a convenient formula to estimate Weibull Inline graphic, our theorems give many insights into the genesis of this parameter. By partitioning the expression for Inline graphic into the two factors, Inline graphic and Inline graphic, we can understand the separate contributions made by the noise distribution and the transducer.

One insight is that Pelli's [19] finding (that Inline graphic for a power-function transducer and zero pedestal) is a specific instance of the more general expression (Equation (15)) that we derived in Theorem 2. In our terms, the “Inline graphic” part of Pelli's relation is Inline graphic, and our general expression for Inline graphic reduces to Inline graphic for a power function transducer and zero pedestal.

Another insight relates to Inline graphic: Since Inline graphic is a number that depends on the noise distribution, changing the noise distribution simply changes all the Weibull Inline graphics by a fixed proportion. For example, we showed that Inline graphic for Gaussian noise is larger than Inline graphic for Laplacian noise by a factor Inline graphic (see Equations (52) and (53)); therefore, changing from Laplacian to Gaussian noise without any other change will increase Weibull Inline graphic in every situation by a factor 1.302.

A further insight relates to Inline graphic: Theorem 3 proves that, as long as the gradient of the transducer function is not 0 or Inline graphic at the pedestal level, Inline graphic approaches 1 as the discrimination threshold decreases. We showed that the Weber fractions generally obtained for contrast discrimination between two easily visible stimuli are small enough to make Inline graphic close to 1, the value for a linear transducer. Therefore, in this case, Weibull Inline graphic is close to Inline graphic, which is about 1.3 for Gaussian noise. Since the Central Limit Theorem provides a good reason for assuming that the noise should be approximately Gaussian, this explains why Weibull Inline graphic turns out to be close to 1.3 for suprathreshold contrast discrimination (although, as explained below, in the section headed “The shape of the internal noise distribution”, the fitted Inline graphic values in Table 1 are in general slightly too high to be consistent with a Gaussian, suggesting a distribution with lower kurtosis).

The linearizing effects of the pedestal have been noted before [4]. Given that all differentiable functions are “locally linear”, one might argue that the surprising thing is not that performance becomes linear with decreasing discrimination threshold, but that there are cases where this does not happen. A commonly encountered example of the latter is the case of a power-function transducer and zero pedestal, analysed previously by Pelli [19]. Here, Inline graphic is always equal to the power-function exponent, so psychophysical performance never becomes linear, however small the threshold gets. It is not that the power function disobeys the rule that all differentiable functions are locally linear, but rather that the definition of local linearity used in the definition of a differentiable function is too weak for our purposes. In the next section, we introduce a different definition of local linearity that is strong enough to determine whether or not linear behaviour will emerge as the discrimination threshold decreases. We show that this “strong local linearity” is not shown by the power function at Inline graphic.

“Local linearity” and Weibull β

One might think that the tendency towards linear behaviour with decreasing discrimination threshold is just a trivial consequence of the fact that any differentiable function is “locally linear”: The definition of differentiability requires that a function be “well approximated” by a linear function near the point of interest. However, the definition of “locally linear” that appears in the test of differentiability is not sufficient to guarantee linear psychophysical discrimination behaviour for small thresholds. As we saw earlier, for a power-function transducer, Inline graphic, and zero pedestal, Inline graphic is always equal to Inline graphic, however small the threshold gets. Theorem 3 does not apply in this case (except when Inline graphic), because, when Inline graphic, the gradient of the transducer is 0 or Inline graphic at a pedestal level of zero. For Inline graphic, the power function is differentiable at Inline graphic, and so it is locally linear in the sense required by the definition of differentiability, but it does not generate linear behaviour for small thresholds. We can see this in Figure 18, which shows an expansive power-function transducer with exponent 2. As the pedestal value increases from zero, the function mapping the stimulus difference, Inline graphic, onto the internal difference signal, Inline graphic, appears increasingly linear, and this linearizing effect becomes more pronounced as the range of inputs decreases. But, when the pedestal value is zero, the mapping from Inline graphic to Inline graphic always has the same form as the transducer, Inline graphic, regardless of the range of inputs. However much we zoom into the power function at Inline graphic, it still looks like a power function with the same exponent. So there is clearly a sense in which an expansive power function is not locally linear at Inline graphic. What is going on?

Figure 18. Effect of a pedestal on the linearity of an expansive power-function transducer.

Figure 18

Each panel in the rightmost column shows the same expansive power-function transducer given by Inline graphic. The panels to the left show parts of this transducer sampled over different ranges of inputs: The width of the range is varied across columns of panels, and the lower limit of the range is varied across rows of panels. The lower limit would correspond to the pedestal value, Inline graphic, in a discrimination experiment. The abscissa of the curves on the left is the stimulus difference, Inline graphic, and the ordinate is Inline graphic, the difference in internal signal values after transduction. The Inline graphic-value given in each panel is the exponent of the power function that fits best (least squares) to these curves. Each coloured box drawn on a transducer in the right column indicates the part of the transducer that is sampled by the correspondingly coloured curve given in a panel to the left on the same row. It can be seen that, as the pedestal increases, the best-fitting exponent quickly approaches 1, giving an approximately linear mapping from Inline graphic to Inline graphic. This linearizing effect is enhanced as the width of the range decreases. Inline graphic, Inline graphic, and Inline graphic are given in arbitrary units: For a given transducer, the best-fitting exponent is determined by the ratio of the pedestal value to the width of the input range. For example, with the transducer shown here, when the pedestal value is equal to the width of the range, the best-fitting exponent is always 1.227; when the pedestal is twice the width of the range, the best-fitting exponent is always 1.126. For a zero pedestal (top row), the best-fitting exponent is always 2, regardless of the width of the input range, and in this sense the power function is not “strongly locally linear” at Inline graphic.

To make sense of this, we need to consider exactly what we mean when we say that a differentiable function must be locally linear. What follows is equivalent to the definition of differentiability given by Hasselblatt and Katok (Ref. [41], p. 400), but simplified to deal with functions of one variable only. For a function, Inline graphic, to be differentiable at Inline graphic, there must be some straight line, Inline graphic, through the point Inline graphic, such that Inline graphic approaches zero more quickly than Inline graphic does. More formally, Inline graphic is differentiable at Inline graphic if and only if there exists a number Inline graphic such that, if we define Inline graphic, then

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1028.jpg (93)

If this condition is satisfied, then Inline graphic is differentiable at Inline graphic, and Inline graphic is the derivative of Inline graphic at that point. An expansive power function clearly satisfies this condition for Inline graphic. In this case, Inline graphic and Inline graphic, so Inline graphic for all Inline graphic, and Inline graphic. Thus,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1039.jpg (94)

and, for Inline graphic, the limit in Equation (94) is zero.

So the expansive power function is locally linear at Inline graphic in the sense required for differentiability. However, when we look at the top row of Figure 18, we can see that it will never look like a straight line, however much we zoom in. To capture this behaviour, we need a different definition of “locally linear”, and the key property of linear functions that we need to appeal to is the fact that the gradient of a linear function is constant. For any function, Inline graphic, Let Inline graphic be the slope of the secant between the points Inline graphic and Inline graphic, and let Inline graphic be the slope of the secant between the points Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Figure 19 illustrates these secants for three types of function over the range Inline graphic to Inline graphic: an expansive power function where Inline graphic (Figure 19A), an expansive power function where Inline graphic (Figure 19B), and a straight line (Figure 19C). The slopes, Inline graphic and Inline graphic, of these secants are given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1055.jpg (95)

and

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1056.jpg (96)

We define the curve's “index of acceleration”, Inline graphic, as

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1058.jpg (97)

For an expansive function, the slope increases towards the right, so Inline graphic, and Inline graphic; for a compressive function, Inline graphic; and, for a linear (or, strictly speaking, affine) function, Inline graphic. We can therefore take the limit of Inline graphic as Inline graphic to indicate whether the function is locally expansive, compressive, or linear at Inline graphic. We classify a function as being “strongly locally linear” at Inline graphic if Inline graphic as Inline graphic. This precisely captures the kind of local linearity that is relevant to Weibull Inline graphic. In general, the numerator and denominator on the right hand side of Equation (97) both approach the derivative, Inline graphic, as Inline graphic, and so, as long as the gradient of Inline graphic at Inline graphic is not 0 or Inline graphic, we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1075.jpg (98)

Thus, all differentiable functions are “strongly locally linear” at Inline graphic except those with zero gradient at Inline graphic (those with infinite gradient at Inline graphic are not differentiable at Inline graphic anyway, and are not locally linear by either definition). If the gradient at Inline graphic is zero, then Equation (98) gives us the indeterminate form Inline graphic, so the limit of Inline graphic cannot be evaluated using Equation (98), and the function will not necessarily be strongly locally linear at Inline graphic. The conditions necessary for Equation (98) to apply are the premises of Theorem 3. Thus, we can now see what is happening in Theorem 3. In all cases for which the premises of Theorem 3 are satisfied, the transducer function is strongly locally linear at the pedestal level, and so linear behaviour will be expected to emerge as the threshold decreases, and we sample a progressively smaller range of inputs. For the case of a power function, Inline graphic, with Inline graphic (Figure 19B), we can go back to Equation (97) to derive the limit of Inline graphic. In this case, we have

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1087.jpg (99)

Thus, the index of acceleration is Inline graphic, whatever the value of Inline graphic. The limit of Inline graphic as Inline graphic is therefore not 1 (unless Inline graphic), and so the power function is not strongly locally linear at Inline graphic. The ratio of the slopes, Inline graphic and Inline graphic, of the secants is unchanged as Inline graphic, and so the shape of the power function does not become any more linear as we zoom in, as shown in Figure 18.

Figure 19. Index of acceleration, Inline graphic.

Figure 19

(A) The wide, magenta curve shows an expansive power function sampled over a range of inputs from Inline graphic to Inline graphic, where Inline graphic. The horizontal blue lines both have length Inline graphic, and the vertical blue lines have length Inline graphic and Inline graphic as indicated. The slope, Inline graphic, of the secant (the oblique line) across the left half of the curve is given by Inline graphic, and the slope, Inline graphic, of the secant across the right half of the curve is given by Inline graphic. Our index of acceleration, Inline graphic, is given by Inline graphic. For the power function, when Inline graphic, Inline graphic as Inline graphic, so the curve is “strongly locally linear” at Inline graphic. (B) The same as A, but with the bottom of the range of inputs, Inline graphic, equal to zero. In this case, Inline graphic depends only on the exponent of the power function, and so it does not approach 1 as Inline graphic approaches zero. The power function is not “strongly locally linear” at Inline graphic. (C) The same as A, but for a straight line function. Here, Inline graphic for all Inline graphic and Inline graphic.

In summary, the definition of local linearity embodied in the definition of a differentiable function is not strong enough to explain why discrimination performance does not always approach that for a linear transducer as the discrimination threshold decreases. We introduced a different definition of local linearity, which we call “strong local linearity”, and it is only when the transducer conforms to this stronger definition of local linearity at the pedestal level that we should start to see linear behaviour as the discrimination threshold decreases.

Relationship between Weibull β and log-log slope of d′ against stimulus level

As mentioned earlier, in a detection task (i.e. where the pedestal is zero), if Inline graphic is a power function of stimulus level (with exponent Inline graphic), then the resulting psychometric function is given by Equation (9), which has the same form as Equation (6) with a power-function transducer and Gaussian noise. In this scenario, if Inline graphic is plotted against Inline graphic, the resulting function is a straight line with slope Inline graphic. Pelli [21] was the first to appreciate the relationship between Inline graphic and Weibull Inline graphic, showing that, for the intrinsic uncertainty model,

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1128.jpg (100)

He later realised that this relationship is not specific to the uncertainty model, but instead applies to any model for which Inline graphic is a power function of stimulus level [19]; the intrinsic uncertainty model shows this relationship because Inline graphic is approximately a power function of stimulus level in this model.

In his earlier paper, Pelli [21] derived Relation (100) from the uncertainty model, for which the psychometric function does not fit perfectly to either the Weibull function, or Equation (9) (for which Inline graphic is a power function of stimulus level). In his later paper [19], he assumed a model for which the psychometric function was precisely that of Equation (9), and found the best-fitting Weibull function. This resulted in the relationship, Inline graphic, which can be inverted to give Inline graphic, which is the same as Relation (100) within the specified margin of error. So, in Pelli's earlier analysis [21], using the uncertainty model, both the Weibull function and Equation (9) were approximations, whereas his later analysis [19] assumed Equation (9) to be precisely correct, and the Weibull function to be an approximation. Strasburger [29] took the one remaining option, which is to assume that the Weibull function is precisely correct, and Equation (9) is an approximation. For several different Weibull functions (with different Inline graphic values), he plotted Inline graphic against stimulus level. Because, in Strasburger's analysis, Equation (9) was an approximation, the log-log plots of Inline graphic against stimulus level were not exactly straight lines, but they were nearly straight for Inline graphic. Strasburger found the change in log stimulus level between Inline graphic and Inline graphic, and used this to define the slope, and this resulted in a similar relationship to that of Pelli, but with a slightly higher constant of proportionality: Inline graphic.

Our equations give an alternative approach to formulating this relationship. If we assume, like Pelli [19], that Equation (9) is precisely correct, then the “true” psychometric function is identical to that from a power-function transducer, zero pedestal, and additive Gaussian noise. In this case, Inline graphic is given by Equation (63), and Inline graphic is given by Equation (53), giving Inline graphic, or Inline graphic. The reason why our equations yield a lower constant of proportionality than Strasburger's is that our expressions for Weibull Inline graphic are based on the psychometric function at the performance level Inline graphic. From Equation (8), this corresponds to a Inline graphic level of 1.27, where Strasburger's log-log plots of Inline graphic against stimulus level start to become noticeably shallower. Strasburger's slopes were derived between Inline graphic levels of 0.1 and 1, which correspond to performance levels of 0.53 and 0.76, respectively, i.e. approximately the bottom half of the psychometric function; if we fitted the Weibull function to the bottom half of the true psychometric function, we should expect to get a different value for Inline graphic than if we fitted across a wide range of performance levels.

So far, we have focussed on the relationship between Weibull Inline graphic and the exponent of a simple power function. Klein [36] examined the relationship between Weibull Inline graphic and the exponent of the numerator of the Legge-Foley transducer (Inline graphic in Equation (72)). He expressed the Legge-Foley transducer slightly differently from Equation (72):

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1154.jpg (101)

where Inline graphic is a constant, and Inline graphic is the stimulus level, Inline graphic, divided by the stimulus level that gives Inline graphic. The subscript, “1”, on Inline graphic and Inline graphic in Equation (101) indicates the value of Inline graphic that we obtain when Inline graphic; it is easily seen that, if Inline graphic in Equation (101), then Inline graphic, giving a performance level of 0.76. Equation (101) can produce log-log plots of Inline graphic against stimulus level very much like those derived by Strasburger for the Weibull model, becoming more shallow with increasing stimulus level. This suggests that the model described in Equation (101) is a better approximation of the Weibull model than the simple power-function transducer.

To find the psychometric function for Klein's model (defined in Equation (101)), we can use Equation (101) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (8), and obtain

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1167.jpg (102)

where

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1168.jpg (103)

and

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1169.jpg (104)

Equation (102) is the psychometric function that would arise from a Legge-Foley transducer with zero pedestal, and additive, unit-variance, Gaussian noise on the internal difference signal, Inline graphic.

Klein constrained the transducer parameters so that Inline graphic, and Inline graphic. Thus, the only free parameter of the model was Inline graphic. He found that, for any Inline graphic, and any stimulus level, Inline graphic, the psychometric function defined by Equation (102) was extremely close to a Weibull function with Inline graphic. Klein remarked that he was very surprised to discover that this fixed relationship between Inline graphic and Inline graphic held for all values of Inline graphic without having to change the other parameters of the Legge-Foley transducer. But we can explain this surprising finding by using our expression for Inline graphic for the Legge-Foley transducer and zero pedestal (Equation (73)). First, let Inline graphic be the threshold value of Inline graphic corresponding to a performance level of Inline graphic. For any value of Inline graphic, Inline graphic is determined by Inline graphic, and both Inline graphic and Inline graphic are determined by Inline graphic, and we can find Inline graphic by numerical search; we can then plug these values of Inline graphic, Inline graphic and Inline graphic into Equation (73) to obtain an expression for Inline graphic in terms of Inline graphic. When we do this, we always obtain Inline graphic (to 10 decimal places). We estimate Weibull Inline graphic by multiplying Inline graphic by Inline graphic, and as already noted, the model defined in Equation (101) implies Gaussian noise, so Inline graphic is given by Equation (53). When Equation (53) is evaluated to 14 decimal places, we find Inline graphic (to 10 decimal places) for any Inline graphic, supporting what Klein found.

To understand why Inline graphic is a fixed multiple of Inline graphic, we need to express the stimulus in different units. Let Inline graphic denote the stimulus level, Inline graphic, divided by the stimulus level that gives Inline graphic, where

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1208.jpg (105)

with Inline graphic. We can then define the Inline graphic function as

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1211.jpg (106)

Equation (106) has the same form as Equation (101), but with the stimulus expressed in different units. When Inline graphic, Equations (106) and (105) give Inline graphic, and so, from Equation (8), the proportion correct is Inline graphic. Thus, when we express the stimulus in units such that the stimulus level is Inline graphic, the threshold value, Inline graphic (which we have defined to be the stimulus value corresponding to a performance level of Inline graphic), is simply given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1218.jpg (107)

rather than having to be found by numerical search.

Using Equation (106) to substitute for Inline graphic in Equation (8), we obtain

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1220.jpg (108)

where

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1221.jpg (109)

and

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1222.jpg (110)

Let us again assume that Inline graphic is some fixed multiple, Inline graphic, of Inline graphic, as it is in Klein's example:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1226.jpg (111)

for constant Inline graphic. Using Equations (107), (109) and (111) to substitute for the terms in Equation (73), and simplifying, we find

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1228.jpg (112)

and so, for constant Inline graphic and jD, Inline graphic is a fixed multiple of Inline graphic, given by

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1232.jpg (113)

This explains Klein's surprising finding that, with the constraint that Inline graphic is a fixed multiple of Inline graphic, there is a fixed multiplicative relationship between Inline graphic and Inline graphic that holds for all values of Inline graphic when the other Legge-Foley transducer parameters are held constant.

The shape of the internal noise distribution

For several decades, the internal noise in psychophysical models has usually been assumed to be Gaussian, but recently, Neri [18] argued that it has a Laplace distribution, which has considerably higher kurtosis than a Gaussian. This conclusion was reached using reverse correlation techniques to investigate detection of bar stimuli embedded in noise.

But do Neri's conclusions about the internal noise also hold for noise-free stimuli? It is not possible to use Neri's methods to study the internal noise when the stimuli are noise-free because these methods require substantial amounts of noise to be added to the stimuli. For noise-free stimuli, we can learn something about the internal noise from the Inline graphic of the fitted Weibull psychometric function, because the shape of the noise distribution affects Weibull Inline graphic through the factor Inline graphic in Equation (15). If we knew the value of Inline graphic, that would greatly narrow down the set of possible internal noise distributions. The key difficulty is that, as noted by Neri [18], the internal noise distribution is confounded with the deterministic transformation, i.e. the transducer. This confound is made explicit in Equation (15), where Weibull Inline graphic is shown to be the product of Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Since psychophysical measurements are generally affected by both the internal noise and the transducer, we are limited in the conclusions that we can draw about the internal noise distribution. For example, a Weibull Inline graphic of 1.3 is consistent with Gaussian internal noise and a linear transducer, because Inline graphic for the Gaussian is 1.3 (Equation (53)), and Inline graphic for a linear transducer is 1; but, since Inline graphic for Laplacian noise is 1 (Equation (52)), a Weibull Inline graphic of 1.3 is also consistent with Laplacian internal noise and a combination of transducer, pedestal, and threshold that yields Inline graphic. A partial solution to this problem is to focus on experimental situations where it is likely that Inline graphic; then it follows from Equation (15) that Inline graphic. In this case, the fitted Weibull Inline graphic places a lower bound on Inline graphic, and possible internal noise distributions will be those for which Inline graphic.

One situation where we can be reasonably sure that Inline graphic is suprathreshold contrast discrimination. If the internal noise is additive, then the transducer in the suprathreshold region of the contrast axis has to be compressive to account for the rise in discrimination threshold with increasing pedestal for suprathreshold pedestals, as found by numerous researchers [4], [6], [30][32], [34], [39], [40], [42]. As explained in the discussion of Theorem 2, and Figure 5, a compressive transducer will give rise to Inline graphic. Thus, for suprathreshold contrast discrimination, although we cannot determine the exact value of Inline graphic from the psychometric function, we know it must be greater than the fitted Weibull Inline graphic. Looking at Table 1, most of the Weibull Inline graphic values for suprathreshold contrast discrimination fall above 1, and so Inline graphic in these cases must be greater than 1, and therefore inconsistent with a Laplace distribution. Out of 38 suprathreshold discrimination conditions (i.e. where the pedestal is greater than the detection threshold), 31 conditions gave a fitted Inline graphic that was greater than 1.

In general, there will be many different pairs of noise distribution and transducer function that are consistent with the data. Suppose we just consider generalized Gaussian noise distributions (parameterized by the shape parameter, Inline graphic) and power-function transducers, parameterized by the exponent, Inline graphic; most transducers would usually be well-approximated by a simple power function over the limited range of inputs spanned by the psychometric function. For a given empirically obtained psychometric function, we could then plot a contour of all the possible pairs of Inline graphic that are consistent with the empirical data. We will now do this for the data in Table 1.

For generalized Gaussian noise, Inline graphic is given by Inline graphic in Equation (50), which is determined by the shape parameter, Inline graphic. For a power function transducer, Inline graphic is given by Inline graphic in Equation (67), which is determined by the transducer exponent, Inline graphic, and the Weber fraction, Inline graphic. For these forms of noise and transducer, the fitted Weibull Inline graphic, which we call Inline graphic, should be related to Inline graphic and Inline graphic according to the following approximation:

graphic file with name pone.0074815.e1277.jpg (114)

In Equation (114), we explicitly indicate that Inline graphic is a function of Inline graphic, and Inline graphic is a function of Inline graphic and Inline graphic. For a given empirically obtained psychometric function, we know the fitted Weibull Inline graphic, i.e. Inline graphic, and the fitted Weber fraction, Inline graphic (these values are given in Table 1), and we can plug these values into Equation (114), to give an equation with two unknowns, Inline graphic and Inline graphic. It is not possible to rearrange this equation algebraically to make either Inline graphic or Inline graphic the subject; however, for any Inline graphic, we can search for the Inline graphic that satisfies the equation. This allows us to trace out a contour of all the possible pairs Inline graphic that are consistent with the fitted Inline graphic and Inline graphic.

Figure 20 plots the Inline graphic contours for the suprathreshold conditions given in Table 1 (i.e., the non-starred conditions). If we assume the noise is Laplacian, then the transducer exponent consistent with the data can be read off by seeing where the contour for that condition intersects the vertical dashed line (corresponding to Inline graphic). For most conditions, the exponent, Inline graphic, would have to be substantially greater than 1 to be consistent with both the data and the Laplacian assumption. As argued earlier, the transducer for suprathreshold contrast discrimination should be compressive, and would fit best to a power function with Inline graphic, so the range of possible Inline graphic pairs are those that lie below horizontal dashed line (corresponding to Inline graphic). The lowest possible values of Inline graphic consistent with a compressive transducer are those where the contours intersect the horizontal dashed line. For the conditions in Figure 20 that do intersect the horizontal dashed line, the median point of intersection is given by Inline graphic, implying a distribution that has lower kurtosis than a Gaussian, the opposite of Neri's proposal. Furthermore, note that 2.55 is the median of the minimum possible Inline graphic values, corresponding to the limit as the compressive transducer approaches linearity. For a more substantially compressive transducer (i.e. Inline graphic substantially less than 1), the Inline graphic values plotted in Figure 20 are higher, corresponding to distributions with substantially lower kurtosis than a Gaussian.

Figure 20. Pairs of noise distribution and transducer exponent consistent with the Weibull parameters for contrast discrimination.

Figure 20

Inline graphic is the generalized Gaussian CDF shape parameter, and Inline graphic is the power-function transducer exponent. Each curve plots the set of Inline graphic pairs consistent with one of the fitted psychometric functions for suprathreshold contrast discrimination given in Table 1 (non-starred conditions). Where available, we used the fitted Inline graphic and Inline graphic parameters from the Weibull fit that included the lapse rate parameter, Inline graphic. Note that the contour for Henning et al.'s subject GBH in the 8.37 cpd condition lies out of range of the axes in this figure, and so is not visible. This is because the fitted Weibull β of 6.70 is much higher than usually found – almost certainly an unreliable measurement.

How can we reconcile these results with those of Neri, which suggest that the noise has higher kurtosis than a Gaussian? One possibility is that the shape of the noise distribution is dependent on the stimuli, with noisy stimuli somehow inducing a Laplacian internal noise distribution, while noise-free stimuli induce an internal noise distribution that has much lower kurtosis. Another possibility is that the assumption of additive, stimulus-independent, noise may be incorrect. For example, Kontsevich and Tyler [43] argued that the transducer is an expansive power function (with exponent 2–2.7) over the whole contrast range, and the increase in contrast discrimination threshold with increasing pedestal is caused by an increase in the noise variance with increasing contrast. While the additive and variable noise models are barely distinguishable on the basis of Kontsevich and Tyler's data (see Ref. [44]), the possibility of variable noise has some support from 2-response 4AFC experiments [15], [45], and might resolve the apparent conflict between Neri's results and those in Table 1. If Kontsevich and Tyler are correct that the transducer is an expansive power function across the whole contrast range, then this would result in a higher Weibull Inline graphic than a compressive function, and in this case, the Inline graphic values of around 1.4 obtained for suprathreshold contrast discrimination may well be consistent with Laplacian noise with variance that increases with contrast. Further consideration of this hypothesis falls outside the scope of this paper, because here we are mainly concerned with formal relationships between models and psychometric functions within the theoretical framework of a transducer and additive noise.

Relationship between power function and log transducers

Theorem 4 showed that, as the exponent of a power function transducer approaches zero, 2AFC behaviour approaches that for a log transducer. This comes about because, in the limit as the exponent tends to zero, the difference of power functions becomes proportional to the difference of logs. This gives us an insight into what determines the difference between the two fitted exponents in the Legge-Foley transducer. Recall that, for high inputs, the Legge-Foley transducer approaches a simple power function with exponent Inline graphic. For typical Weber fractions of around 0.3, the transducer exponent makes little difference to the predicted Weibull Inline graphic (see Figure 9), so the fitted exponent is more strongly constrained by the threshold, Inline graphic, that it predicts for each pedestal level. If threshold is proportional to the pedestal, then we have Weber's law (i.e. the Weber fraction, Inline graphic is constant, so that a plot of Inline graphic against Inline graphic is a straight line with a slope of 1 on log-log axes). A logarithmic transducer would generate Weber's law [46]; this is because, for additive noise, the discrimination threshold, Inline graphic, corresponds to a constant internal difference signal, Inline graphic, and a logarithmic transducer would give Inline graphic, implying Inline graphic constant, which is Weber's law. On the other hand, a linear transducer would cause Inline graphic to be constant with respect to Inline graphic, so that the plot of Inline graphic against Inline graphic was a straight line with a slope of 0. As the exponent of the power function transducer increases from infinitesimally above zero (giving the same performance as a log transducer) to 1 (giving a linear transducer), the slope of the plot of Inline graphic against Inline graphic on log-log axes will gradually decrease from 1 to 0. Actual slopes obtained in the literature usually fall between 1 and 0.6 [4], [6], [32], [34], and this would require an exponent between 0 and 1, which explains why the difference between the fitted exponents in the Legge-Foley transducer [4], [30] falls in this range. A corollary of Theorem 4 is that no power function transducer could generate a log-log slope of Inline graphic against Inline graphic that was greater than 1: As the exponent increases from zero, the slope decreases from 1. This also applies to any transducer that approximates a power function for high inputs, such as the Legge-Foley transducer.

Lapse rate

As noted earlier, psychophysical observers sometimes respond incorrectly, even on easy trials. This may be due to lapses of concentration, so that the observer either did not look at the stimuli, or cannot remember which interval contained the target; on such trials, the proportion correct will be 0.5. Suppose the proportion correct on non-lapse trials is given by Inline graphic. Then, if lapse trials occur with probability Inline graphic, the probability of a correct response overall will be given by Inline graphic. The effect of Inline graphic is to linearly compress the psychometric function vertically so that the upper asymptote is Inline graphic. For simplicity, our analytical results regarding Weibull Inline graphic are derived assuming Inline graphic, but it is important to realize that these results still apply for non-zero lapse rates. To understand why, note that, if we change the true psychometric function so that the lapse rate is non-zero, the psychometric function will be vertically compressed but otherwise unchanged. Thus, the best-fitting Weibull function will be one that is vertically compressed but otherwise unchanged. This change to the Weibull function is achieved by increasing Inline graphic while keeping Inline graphic and Inline graphic the same, so the Inline graphic of the best-fitting Weibull function is unchanged by introducing a non-zero lapse rate. Our results about Weibull Inline graphic therefore suffer no loss of generality by being derived under the assumption of Inline graphic.

Conclusions

We analyzed the psychometric function within the theoretical framework of a transducer and additive noise. We showed that, for a variety of commonly used transducers and noise distributions, the true psychometric function was well fit by a Weibull function. We showed that Weibull Inline graphic, which controls the Weibull function's shape on a linear abscissa, can be partitioned into two factors. One, which we call Inline graphic, is the Inline graphic of the Weibull function that fits best to the CDF of the noise on the internal difference signal. The other factor, which we call Inline graphic, depends on the transducer function and pedestal level, and can also depend on the observer's threshold. To a close approximation, the Inline graphic of the Weibull function that fits best to the true psychometric function will be given by Inline graphic. We derived general expressions for Inline graphic and Inline graphic, and, from these, derived specific expressions for particular noise distributions and particular transducers. We showed that, for a wide range of noise distributions and transducers, the fitted Weibull Inline graphic was closely matched by Inline graphic. For a power function transducer with exponent Inline graphic, and zero pedestal, Inline graphic, which gives us the relationship between Weibull Inline graphic and Inline graphic derived by Pelli [19]. The power of our approach is that it can easily be applied to any noise distribution and any transducer, provided that the Weibull function provides a good fit to the psychometric function.

We also explained why, as the discrimination threshold decreases, 2AFC behaviour will approach that for a linear transducer for suprathreshold discrimination, but not for detection. Although most transducer functions are differentiable (and therefore locally linear in one sense), we showed that, at the point at which the gradient of a nonlinear function is zero, the function fails a stronger test of local linearity, and it is this stronger kind of local linearity that is critical for determining whether or not behaviour becomes linear with decreasing threshold. For detection experiments, the transducer usually has zero gradient at the (zero) pedestal level, and is not “strongly locally linear” in the sense that we defined, and this prevents the psychophysical behaviour from approaching that for a linear transducer as the threshold decreases.

In Theorem 4, we showed that, as the exponent of a power function approaches zero, psychophysical behaviour approaches that for a logarithmic transducer. A corollary of this theorem is that the log-log slope of the threshold vs pedestal curve can never exceed 1 for a power-function transducer and additive noise.

Finally, an understanding of the factors that determine Weibull beta gives us some insight into the shape of the noise distribution. In apparent contrast to a recent claim [18] that the internal noise has considerably higher kurtosis than a Gaussian distribution (based on experiments on detection of a bar embedded in noise), our analysis of suprathreshold contrast discrimination with noise-free stimuli suggests that the internal noise does not have higher kurtosis than a Gaussian; if anything, the internal noise appears to have lower kurtosis than a Gaussian. Both our analysis and that of Neri [18] made the assumption of additive, stimulus-independent noise, and we suggest that one possible resolution of this apparent contradiction might be to drop that assumption.

Supporting Information

Appendix S1

Proof that Inline graphic asymptotes to Inline graphic as Inline graphic .

(PDF)

Appendix S2

Point of inflection of the Legge-Foley transducer.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank our reviewers, Hans Strasburger and Daniel Baker, for their insightful comments and careful scrutiny of our manuscript.

Funding Statement

This work was funded by EPSRC grant EP/H033955/1 to Joshua Solomon (http://www.epsrc.ac.uk/). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Foley JM, Legge GE (1981) Contrast detection and near-threshold discrimination in human vision. Vision Res 21: 1041–1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Nachmias J (1981) On the psychometric function for contrast detection. Vision Res 21: 215–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Mayer MJ, Tyler CW (1986) Invariance of the slope of the psychometric function with spatial summation. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 3: 1166–1172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Meese T, Georgeson MA, Baker DH (2006) Binocular contrast vision at and above threshold. J Vis 6: 1224–1243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Wallis SA, Baker DH, Meese TS, Georgeson MA (2013) The slope of the psychometric function and non-stationarity of thresholds in spatiotemporal contrast vision. Vision Res 76: 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Nachmias J, Sansbury RV (1974) Grating contrast: discrimination may be better than detection. Vision Res 14: 1039–1042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Wichmann FA, Hill NJ (2001) The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Percept Psychophys 63: 1293–1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Weibull W (1951) A statistical distribution function of wide applicability. Journal of Applied Mechanics 18: 293–297. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Quick RF (1974) A vector-magnitude model of contrast detection. Kybernetik 16: 65–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Legge GE (1978) Sustained and transient mechanisms in human vision: Temporal and spatial properties. Vision Res 18: 69–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Legge GE (1978) Space domain properties of a spatial frequency channel in human vision. Vision Res 18: 959–969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Watson AB (1979) Probability summation over time. Vision Res 19: 515–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Robson JG, Graham N (1981) Probability summation and regional variation in contrast sensitivity across the visual field. Vision Res 21: 409–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Tanner WP, Swets JA (1954) A decision-making theory of visual detection. Psychol Rev 61: 401–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Swets JA, Tanner WP, Birdsall TG (1961) Decision processes in perception. Psychol Rev 68: 301–340. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Laming D (2013) Probability summation-a critique. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 30: 300–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Watson AB, Pelli DG (1983) QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys 33: 113–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Neri P (2013) The statistical distribution of noisy transmission in human sensors. Journal of Neural Engineering 10: 016014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Pelli DG (1987) On the relation between summation and facilitation. Vision Res 27: 119–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Tanner WP, Birdsall TG (1958) Definitions of d′ and η as psychophysical measures. J Acoust Soc Am 30: 922–928. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Pelli DG (1985) Uncertainty explains many aspects of visual contrast detection and discrimination. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 2: 1508–1532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. O'Regan JK, Humbert R (1989) Estimating psychometric functions in forced-choice situations: Significant biases found in threshold and slope estimations when small samples are used. Percept Psychophys 46: 434–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Leek MR, Hanna TE, Marshall L (1992) Estimation of psychometric functions from adaptive tracking procedures. Percept Psychophys 51: 247–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. King-Smith PE, Rose D (1997) Principles of an adaptive method for measuring the slope of the psychometric function. Vision Res 37: 1595–1604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Treutwein B, Strasburger H (1999) Fitting the psychometric function. Percept Psychophys 61: 87–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Kontsevich LL, Tyler CW (1999) Bayesian adaptive estimation of psychometric slope and threshold. Vision Res 39: 2729–2737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Kaernbach C (2001) Slope bias of psychometric functions derived from adaptive data. Percept Psychophys 63: 1389–1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. McIlhagga WH, May KA (2012) Optimal edge filters explain human blur detection. J Vis 12: 10/9/1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Strasburger H (2001) Converting between measures of slope of the psychometric function. Percept Psychophys 63: 1348–1355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Legge GE, Foley JM (1980) Contrast masking in human vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America 70: 1458–1471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Campbell FW, Kulikowski JJ (1966) Orientation selectivity of the human visual system. Journal of Physiology 187: 437–445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Swift DJ, Smith RA (1983) Spatial frequency masking and Weber's law. Vision Res 23: 495–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Laming D (1989) Experimental evidence for Fechner's and Stevens's laws. Behav Brain Sci 12: 277–281. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Bird CM, Henning GB, Wichmann FA (2002) Contrast discrimination with sinusoidal gratings of different spatial frequency. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 19: 1267–1273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Solomon JA (2009) The history of dipper functions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 71: 435–443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Klein S (2001) Measuring, estimating, and understanding the psychometric function: A commentary. Percept Psychophys 63: 1421–1455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Stromeyer CF, Klein S (1974) Spatial frequency channels in human vision as asymmetric (edge) mechanisms. Vision Res 14: 1409–1420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Yu C, Klein SA, Levi DM (2003) Cross- and Iso- oriented surrounds modulate the contrast response function: The effect of surround contrast. J Vis 3: 527–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Wilson HR (1980) A transducer function for threshold and suprathreshold human vision. Biol Cybern 38: 171–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Henning GB, Wichmann FA (2007) Some observations on the pedestal effect. J Vis 7: 1/3/1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hasselblatt B, Katok A (2003) A First Course in Dynamics: With a Panorama of Recent Developments. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Legge GE (1981) A power law for contrast discrimination. Vision Res 21: 457–467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Kontsevich LL, Chen C-C, Tyler CW (2002) Separating the effects of response nonlinearity and internal noise psychophysically. Vision Res 42: 1771–1784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Georgeson MA, Meese TS (2006) Fixed or variable noise in contrast discrimination? The jury's still out…. Vision Res 46: 4294–4303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Solomon JA (2007) Intrinsic uncertainty explains second responses. Spat Vis 20: 45–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Fechner GT (1912) In: Rand B, editor. The classical psychologists: Selections illustrating psychology from Anaxagoras to Wundt. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. (Original work published 1860). [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Henning GB, Bird CM, Wichmann FA (2002) Contrast discrimination with pulse trains in pink noise. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 19: 1259–1266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix S1

Proof that Inline graphic asymptotes to Inline graphic as Inline graphic .

(PDF)

Appendix S2

Point of inflection of the Legge-Foley transducer.

(PDF)


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES