Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Oct 7.
Published in final edited form as: Ann Epidemiol. 2012 Aug 15;22(10):738–743. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2012.07.004

Lead exposure and educational proficiency: moderate lead exposure and educational proficiency on end-of-grade examinations

Michael S Amato MS 1,*, Colleen F Moore 1, Sheryl Magzamen 1, Pamela Imm 1, Jeffrey A Havlena 1, Henry A Anderson 1, Marty S Kanarek 1
PMCID: PMC3791858  NIHMSID: NIHMS473460  PMID: 22902043

Abstract

Purpose

To investigate and quantify the impact of moderate lead exposure on students' ability to score at the “proficient” level on end-of-grade standardized tests.

Methods

We compared the scores of 3757 fourth grade students from Milwaukee, Wisconsin, on the Wisconsin Knowledge and Concepts Exam (WKCE). The sample consisted of children with a blood lead test before age 3 years that was either unquantifiable at the time of testing (<5 μg/dL) or in the range of moderate exposure (10–19 μg/dL).

Results

After controlling for gender, poverty, English language learner status, race/ethnicity, school disciplinary actions, and attendance percentage, results showed a significant negative effect of moderate lead exposure on academic achievement for all 5 subtests of the WKCE. Test score deficits owing to lead exposure were equal to 22% of the interval between student categorization at the “proficient” or “basic” levels in Reading, and 42% of the interval in Mathematics.

Conclusions

Children exposed to amounts of lead before age 3 years that are insufficient to trigger intervention under current policies in many states are nonetheless at a considerable educational disadvantage compared with their unexposed peers 7 to 8 years later. Exposed students are at greater risk of scoring below the proficient level, an outcome with serious negative consequences for both the student and the school.

Keywords: Lead poisoning, Lead exposure, Childhood, School, Testing, Environmental pollutants, Environmental pollution, Environmental policy


Childhood lead exposure has been recognized as a major public health issue for over a century. Lead exposure is associated with a wide variety of negative health impacts [1], including protracted physical development and smaller stature [2,3], gastrointestinal disorders [4], lower cognitive function [57], antisocial behavior and delinquency [813], and in severe cases death [14]. The preponderance of studies demonstrate a consistent albeit modest effect of early childhood lead exposure on cognitive function, often measured by intelligence quotient (IQ) tests [1519]. Partly because IQ test scores are a familiar measure with proven sensitivity, the association of early life lead exposure with IQ scores has been frequently used in risk analyses and for defining risk levels [1]. Although IQ scores are often statistically correlated with academic and professional success in life, until recently few studies have investigated the relationship of lead exposure with more direct educational outcomes such as end-of-grade examinations, which can deterministically influence students' academic trajectories.

School achievement tests have taken on increasing importance in the United States following federal policies that evaluate schools and teachers according to the performance of students on standardized tests administered by each state. The assignment of proficiency categories based on students' scores is emphasized. Schools are required to achieve target percentages of children performing at or above the “proficient” level in reading and math. Schools failing to meet benchmarks risk cuts to funding or closure, making these tests a high-stakes outcome for students, schools, and their communities. The extent to which lead exposure is negatively associated with performance on standardized tests has important implications for American educational policy, as well as public health.

The upper Midwest of the United States, which includes Wisconsin, has a relatively high risk of lead exposure. According to surveillance conducted by the Wisconsin Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention Program (WCLPPP), in 2006 the statewide prevalence of lead exposure over the level of concern at the time (blood lead level [BLL] ≥10 μg/dL) was 2.6% for tested children under 2 years of age; more than twice the national prevalence. Approximately 5% of children who entered the Wisconsin public school system in 2006 had at least one test result above the level of concern [20]. Despite this high prevalence, state public health interventions in Wisconsin and many other states are currently mandated only for children with elevated BLL defined as 20 μg/dL or greater. There are neither state-mandated interventions nor secondary prevention efforts targeted to children with BLLs that are considered elevated but less than the 20 μg/dL threshold, although some municipalities and counties provide resources. In 2012, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) accepted the recommendation of the Advisory Committee on Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention [21], and replaced the previous level of concern (10 μg/dL) with a new reference level calculated every 4 years as the 97.5th percentile of BLLs in in the two most recent National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey datasets. This change was made in response to an accumulation of evidence suggesting that there is no safe level of lead exposure [22], with the effect of increasing the number of children that the CDC considers at risk for negative consequences of lead exposure. The present study examines the educational impact of lead exposure, in terms of meeting or failing to meet the “proficient” level on an end-of-grade examination, for children with BLL test results that are elevated but below the threshold for mandated intervention (20 μg/dL).

Methods

We used WCLPPP data to select children from Milwaukee with a BLL test of 10 μg/dL or greater and less than 20 μg/dL before the age of 3. We compared these children having moderately elevated BLL with those with a blood lead test before age 3 but with BLL considered unquantifiable by the Wisconsin State Laboratory of Hygiene (WSLH) at the time of testing (<5 μg/dL). We examined data from children who resided in Milwaukee at the time of their blood test owing to sample size considerations: In addition to being the largest city in Wisconsin, Milwaukee has the highest prevalence of elevated BLLs in children (6.1%) of any municipality in the state [20]. The protocol for this study was approved by the University of Wisconsin–Madison Education Research Institutional Review Board.

Blood lead levels

In 2006, approximately 20% of Wisconsin children under 6 years of age had their blood lead levels tested. Many parents have their children tested voluntarily; however, Wisconsin mandates lead screening for children enrolled in Medicaid and Women, Infants, and Children at their 1st and 2nd birthdays. Sixty-seven percent of children tested in Wisconsin in 2006 were enrolled in one of those programs. Health care providers are required by Wisconsin Administrative Code (HFS 181) to report BLL results to WCLPPP, or to direct the analyzing laboratories to do so. Children in Wisconsin have their blood drawn in clinics, doctor's offices, and hospitals, and their samples are tested by a number of different laboratories. There is substantial variation in the limits of quantification established by individual laboratories. The WSLH, which analyzes more than one quarter of all samples, established a limit of quantification of 5 μg/dL for all blood lead analysis before 2000. Although many laboratories, including the WSLH, are currently able to reliably test below this level, we use it to anchor the low end of the quantifiable lead exposure spectrum in our study because more than half of the BLL results in our sample were tested during this period, many at the WSLH. Our definition of “not exposed” is based on the WSLH limits of quantification at the time of testing, which is also the current CDC reference level [22].

Academic performance

The Wisconsin Knowledge and Concepts Examination (WKCE) is a Wisconsin standardized examination administered to public school students in grades 3 through 8 and 10. Every grade is tested on Mathematics and Reading; in 4th, 8th, and 10th grade students are also tested on Language Arts, Social Studies, and Science. The 4th-grade WKCE is one factor in deciding whether individual students advance to 5th grade [23]. The 4th-grade examination was selected for study, because it is the most comprehensive and high-stakes exam given during the elementary years. Students receive a raw score in each subject area based on the number of questions answered correctly, which is converted to a scaled score intended to maintain a similar distribution of scores across multiple years. The scaled scores are the primary score of interest to Wisconsin educators and policymakers to determine whether schools are meeting federal standards; for that reason, we conducted our analyses using the scaled scores.

Study sample construction

Our sample was constructed by combining Milwaukee Public Schools (MPS) records of academic performance with BLL data maintained by WCLPPP. The sample consisted of children who met the following eligibility criteria: (1) Born between January 1,1996, and December 31, 2000; (2) BLL tested by capillary or venous test before their 3rd birthday and results reported to WCLPPP; (3) Milwaukee address at the time of one or more blood lead tests; and (4) confirmed by the Wisconsin Department of Public Instruction to have taken the 4th-grade WKCE. Children were defined as either “exposed” (children tested at least once before their 3rd birthday with a result of ≥10 and <20 μg/dL, interpreted for study purposes as “moderately elevated BLL”), or “not exposed” (children tested between 18 and 36 months of age with a BLL of <5 μg/dL; interpreted for study purposes as “never exposed”). A child could only be classified as exposed if they never had a result 20 μg/dL or greater in their lifetime. Similarly, a child classified as not exposed could not have a BLL result of 5 μg/dL or higher at any age.

The BLL records of children in the WCLPPP database who met these definitions were matched with Department of Public Instruction records of 4th-grade WKCE completion. This match produced a list of 5779 children who had taken the WKCE, defined as exposed (3616) or unexposed (2163). Study staff provided this list of students (along with each student's date of birth and BLL information in a coded string indecipherable to non-study personnel) to an MPS data manager to obtain WKCE scores. For the 5779 eligible children, MPS was able to match 3757 in the district with 4th-grade WKCE scores. Those children for whom MPS was unable to find a match had most likely moved out of the district before the 4th grade. Figure 1 shows a flowchart of sample construction.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Participant flow diagram for Milwaukee Public Schools analysis.

Student data and characteristics

The MPS returned a deidentified dataset to study researchers. The anonymized dataset of student records included WKCE scores, attendance and suspension data, and information on seven student characteristics including special education status, English Language Learner (ELL) status, race/ethnicity, gender, school attended, and year of birth. All students were listed with a single race/ethnicity status. Eligibility for free or reduced lunch during 4th grade was used as a marker for poverty, and was the only socioeconomic status variable available for analysis.

The leftmost columns in Table 1 show the distributions of student characteristics in our sample. Although recent research suggests important relationships between lead exposure and specific diagnoses associated with special education [24,25], our sample sizes for specific special education designations and uncertainty about the criteria used to determine them prevents reliable conclusions from being drawn. Students receiving any form of special education were therefore excluded from the regression analysis. Native American and Asian students were also excluded from further analysis owing to small sample sizes. Although only 7% of students in the overall sample were ELL, all but 4 of these students were identified as Hispanic. Among Hispanic students, the proportion of ELL was 38%.

Table 1. Student characteristics and sample comparison.

WKCE sample MPS 2008–2009 all 4th–9th graders


n % n %
Total 3757 100 37,526 100
Lead exposure
 Exposed 2672 71 NA NA
 Unexposed 1085 29 NA NA
Gender
 Male 1985 53 19,392 52
 Female 1772 47 18,134 48
Race/ethnicity
 Asian 10 0 1674 5
 Black/African-American 2489 66 21,631 60
 Hispanic 775 21 8302 23
 Native American 22 1 304 1
 White 461 12 4409 12
Poverty indicator
 No assisted lunch 647 17 9132 24
 Assisted lunch program 3110 83 28,394 76
Language indicator
 Native speaker 3509 93 33,746 90
 English language learner 248 7 3780 10
Education designation
 Non-special education 2782 74 30,521 81
 Special education 975 26 7365 19
Suspension incidents
 Suspended in 4th grade 957 25 NA NA
 Not suspended in 4th grade 2800 75 NA NA

MPS = Milwaukee Public Schools; NA = not applicable; WKCE = Wisconsin Knowledge and Concepts Examination.

Analysis

We used Chi-square tests of independence to investigate the bivariate relationship of each student characteristic with lead exposure. Effect sizes were calculated Φ = √(χ 2/N). The Chi-square test for ELL and lead exposure considered Hispanic students only, to provide an unconfounded test of this important educational construct. The relationship between attendance rate and lead exposure was tested with a two-sample t-test, presented with effect size Cohen's d. Analyses were conducted using R version 2.12.2.

We conducted two sequential ordinary least-squares regressions for each of the five WKCE subject areas. The dependent variable for the initial models was student scaled score in that subject area. All covariates, but not lead exposure group, were included as predictor variables in the initial models: Male, assisted lunch eligibility, Black, Hispanic, ELL, suspension during the 4th grade, and attendance percentage. All predictor variables were binary coded, with the exception of attendance percentage, which was entered as a continuous variable.

Students' residual scores from the initial models were entered as the dependent variable for the secondary models. The only predictor in the secondary models was lead exposure. Exposure was coded as a binary variable (exposed/unexposed) because the size of our sample was insufficient to accurately test the specific dose-response of lead exposure for each academic subject at each level of BLL. This sequential regression analysis strategy removes all variance that can be attributed to covariates before looking for an effect of lead. Any effect found will provide evidence for the relationship between moderate lead exposure during child development and subsequent academic performance. A similar pair of multivariate regressions, testing the effects of covariates and exposure on the set of students' five WKCE scores was also conducted by approximating Pillai's trace to the F distribution.

Mean scores on the five WKCE subjects for students in the exposed and unexposed groups, adjusted to remove the effects of all covariates listed above, were obtained for graphical presentation using analysis of covariance.

Results

Sample representativeness

Table 1 evaluates sample representativeness by comparison to publicly available demographic data for all 4th- to 9th-grade students in MPS during 2008 and 2009 [26]. That cohort approximates the population from which our sample was drawn (4th graders from 2005 to 2010). A typical 9th-grade student in 2008 and 2009 would have taken the 4th-grade WKCE in 2004, only 1 year before the first WKCE scores in our sample. The demographic distributions in our sample approximate those present in the larger population.

Lead exposure associations with student characteristics

The distribution of student characteristics for the exposed and unexposed groups is shown in Table 2. In addition to the categorical associations in Table 2, students in the unexposed group had a higher mean attendance rate (95%) than students in the exposed group (92%), d = 0.40. All covariates were significantly associated with exposure at the P < .0001 level except for gender, which was significant at P < .05. Among the categorical covariates, race and poverty had the strongest relationships with lead exposure. Black/African-American students and Hispanic students were more likely to have been exposed to lead than White students, and students who were living in poverty were more likely to have been exposed than students who were not.

Table 2. Lead exposure covariates for WKCE sample.

Characteristic Unexposed Exposed Effect Size



n % n % Φ
Total 1066 29 2659 71
Gender* .03
 Male 534 27 1432 73
 Female 532 30 1227 70
Race/ethnicity .31
 Black/African-American 501 20 1988 80
 Hispanic 277 36 498 64
 White 288 62 173 38
Poverty indicators .31
 No assisted lunch 336 64 193 36
 Assisted lunch program 730 23 2466 77
Language (Hispanic students only) .17
 Native speaker 203 42 277 58
 English language learner 74 25 221 75
Education designation .12
 Non-special education 877 32 1879 68
 Special education 187 19 780 81
Suspension incidents .21
 Suspended in 4th grade 114 12 834 88
 Not suspended in 4th grade 952 34 1825 66
*

P < .05 for Chi-square test of independence.

P < .0001 for Chi-square test of independence.

Effect of lead on WKCE scores

Models were fit for each of the five WKCE subject areas separately. Results of the initial models are shown in Table 3. Results of the secondary models are shown in Table 4. Beta coefficients in both models should be interpreted as the expected difference in test score owing to the presence of that characteristic. For example, based on Table 3, a male child would be expected to perform 4.51 points lower than a female child in language arts.

Table 3. Effect of covariates on WKCE scores (model 1).

Predictor Language arts Mathematics Reading Science Social studies Multivariate






Beta P Beta P Beta P Beta P Beta P Pillai P
Intercept 245.70 *** 368.04 *** 426.09 *** 251.44 *** 263.47 ***
Male −4.51 *** 7.87 *** −1.24 .41 2.47 .02 0.13 .89 0.0379 ***
Assisted lunch −8.80 *** −10.79 *** −16.52 *** −10.15 *** −11.56 *** 0.0291 ***
ELL −13.24 *** −11.88 ** −22.44 *** −7.22 * −7.20 ** 0.0227 ***
Black/African American −15.41 *** −27.78 *** −25.69 *** −20.28 *** −16.12 *** 0.0598 ***
Hispanic −5.95 * −10.84 ** −13.63 *** −8.68 *** −7.15 ** 0.0092 **
Suspension −5.35 ** −6.55 * −7.26 *** −4.90 ** −4.38 ** 0.0070 *
Attendance % 0.64 *** 1.10 *** 0.71 *** 0.55 *** 0.45 *** 0.0293 ***
F(7, 2748) 56.27 66.61 65.92 72.60 68.09
Adjusted R2 0.1236 0.1430 0.1418 0.1545 0.1461

ELL = English language learner.

*

P < . 01;

**

P < .001;

***

P < .0001.

Table 4. Unique effect of lead exposure on WKCE scores (model 2).

Predictor Language arts Mathematics Reading Science Social studies Multivariate






Beta P Beta P Beta P Beta P Beta P Pillai P
Intercept 3.71 ** 4.86 ** 6.66 *** 3.80 *** 3.55 ***
Lead exposure −5.44 *** −7.14 *** −9.77 *** −5.58 *** −5.21 *** 0.0149 ***
F(1, 2754) 22.84 18.80 38.62 27.01 28.48
Adjusted R2 0.0079 0.0064 0.0135 0.0094 0.0099
*

P < .01;

**

P < .001;

***

P < .0001.

Children exposed to moderate amounts of lead performed significantly worse on all five sections of the WKCE compared with unexposed children (P < .0001 for each), even after removing all variance that could be explained by gender, poverty, racial/ethnic group, suspensions, and attendance. WKCE scale scores are not directly comparable across sections, however the effect of lead exposure was numerically greatest for the reading section, on which exposed students scored an average of 9.77 points lower than unexposed students (Table 4).

Adjusted mean scores for the exposed and unexposed groups are presented in Figure 2. The figure also shows the score cutoff thresholds for three of the four proficiency categories used by Wisconsin Department of Public Instruction: Basic, proficient, and advanced. The cutoff for minimal proficiency is below the plotted area and not shown. The graphs indicate that children in the exposed group are at greater risk of scoring in the basic range for language arts, mathematics, and reading. In the subject of science, a student scoring at the adjusted mean of the unexposed group would be classified as proficient, whereas a student scoring at the adjusted mean of the exposed group would be classified as basic. The disadvantage of an exposed student relative to an unexposed student can be contextualized as percentages of the intervals between the basic and proficient categories. Group differences in mean test score owing to moderate lead exposure range from 19% of the basic–proficient interval in science, to 42% of the basic–proficient interval in mathematics.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Adjusted mean scores and standard errors for exposed and unexposed students. Horizontal lines show cutoff thresholds for three of four proficiency categories used by the Wisconsin Department of Public Instruction.

Discussion

The results of the current study show that a BLL of 10 to less than 20 μg/dL before age 3 years is associated with significantly lower standardized test scores in all five content areas of the 4th-grade WKCE after removing all variance attributable to gender, poverty, ELL status, race/ethnicity, suspension, and attendance percentage. The adjusted mean difference in scores for students in the exposed and unexposed groups was equivalent to 22% of the interval between categorization as “proficient” or “basic” in reading, and 42% of the interval in mathematics. Exposed students were more likely to score below the “proficient” level in both subjects, an outcome with negative consequences for both the individual students and their school.

These results punctuate the importance of environmental investigation and lead remediation for children with BLLs in this range. Wisconsin is not alone in having a standard for intervention that excludes some children with BLLs above the CDC reference level. Neighboring states Michigan and Iowa both use guidelines similar to Wisconsin's, and Minnesota requires investigation of BLLs at or above 15 μg/dL. Although data have consistently indicated that lead exposure in any amount is harmful, our study is one of only a few in the recent literature to quantify the relationship of moderate (often unremediated) lead exposure with classroom administered standardized test scores. Miranda et al [27] studied more than 8000 children in North Carolina, and revealed that performance on 4th-grade standardized tests of reading and math declined as BLL increased before 5 years of age. Miranda et al [28] concluded that lead exposure had the largest negative effect on school achievement test scores when combined with other risk factors (e.g., poverty, parental education level). The North Carolina researchers also found that increased BLLs were associated with an elevated risk of special education requirements [25]. Researchers in Detroit found robust associations of lead exposure and lower test scores in the 3rd, 5th, and 8th grades [29]. A study analyzing county-level data in New York State found the percentage of preschoolers with elevated BLL (>10 μg/dL) predicted 8% to 16% of the differences among counties in the percentage of their students scoring in the lowest category on 3rd- and 8th-grade English and mathematics tests [30]. Finally, an analysis of school-level data in Massachusetts found that schools experiencing the greatest reduction in mean student BLL from 2002 to 2009 also experienced the greatest increase in end-of-grade test scores over the same period, after controlling for covariates [31].

Although lead exposure has most typically been addressed as a public health or environmental issue, taken together these studies suggest that it should also be understood and studied as an educational issue with direct consequences for policy outcomes. Milwaukee is typical of many industrial Midwestern cities in the problems it faces controlling lead exposure in its large stock of older housing and contaminated soils. It is also typical of many urban areas in the complexity of issues faced by school districts, and pressures exerted on school districts to improve educational achievement. It is noteworthy that the racial/ethnic covariates account for considerably more variance in scores than does the lead exposure variable. That result is not an artifact of our particular statistical method, nor is it uncommon in the literature. There is, however, a crucial difference in how the variables in the model relate to phenomena in the world. Lead is a neurotoxin, and although more study is needed to fully understand the exact mechanisms by which it impairs cognitive function, several linking hypotheses have been put forward by researchers in the field, such as disrupted sensory gating mechanisms [32]. In contrast, the racial/ethnic covariates in our model are indirect markers for unmeasured characteristics of the children in our sample. The strong association between race/ethnicity and test scores inform us that a serious problem exists, but the covariates are frustratingly silent regarding the cause of the problem. Proposed approaches for reducing the racial achievement gap are numerous and often highly controversial. However, remediation strategies for lead exposure enjoy near consensus; funding must be provided to remove it from housing, the primary source of lead in children's environments [20,33,34]. The CDC Advisory Committee on Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention makes the claim that “primary prevention is necessary because the effects of lead appear to be irreversible” [21], based on the finding that reducing BLLs in children postexposure using chelation therapy yields no benefit for cognitive outcomes [35]. Given the disproportionately high number of African-American and Hispanic students with elevated blood lead levels in our study, and the demonstrated negative relationship between lead exposure and standardized test scores, one strategy for reducing the racial achievement gap in schools would be to increase efforts to remove lead from housing.

We have been deliberately conservative in both our operationalization of ‘moderate’ lead exposure, and in the statistical methodology used to test its effect on end of grade test scores. The sequential regression analysis removed all variance that could be attributed to the covariates before testing for an effect of exposure, a method that was a priori biased against finding an effect of exposure. Additionally, the inclusion of suspension and attendance data as covariates likely further biased our test against finding an effect of exposure; research has suggested that both behavior problems and reduced immune system function may themselves be causally linked with lead exposure. Regardless, the effects are robust; performance was worse for children in the exposed group for each subject of the WKCE. The observed effects likely underestimate the magnitude of the true effects in the population. However, if we interpret our results as a lower bound on the true population effects, we may do so with greater confidence than if we had included all variables in a single model. They suggest a direct, quantifiable impact on educational outcomes that cannot be remediated through changes to school policy alone.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Margie Coons and the Wisconsin Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention Program; to Milwaukee Public Schools; to the Wisconsin Department of Public Instruction; to Noel Stanton, WI State Lab of Hygiene; to Matt Michala; and to Jennifer Kiser. This research was made possible by funding from the Wisconsin Partnership Program of the University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health.

References

  • 1.Bellinger DC. The Protean toxicities of lead: New chapters in a familiar story. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2011;8:2593–628. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8072593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Schwartz J, Angle C, Pitcher M. Relationship between childhood blood lead levels and stature. Pediatrics. 1986;77:153–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Selevan S, Rice D, Hogan K, Euling S, Pfahles-Hutchens A, Bethel J. Blood lead concentration and delayed puberty in girls. N Engl J Med. 2003;348:1527–36. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa020880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Markowitz M. Lead poisoning: a disease for the next millennium. Curr Prob Pediatrics. 2000;30:62–70. doi: 10.1067/mps.2000.104053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Needleman H, Gunnoe C, Leviton A, Reed R, Peresie H, Maher C, et al. Deficits in psychologic and classroom performance of children with elevated dentine lead levels. N Engl J Med. 1979;300:689–95. doi: 10.1056/NEJM197903293001301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bellinger D, Sloman J, Leviton A, Rabinowitz M, Needleman H, Walternaux C. Low-level lead exposures and children's cognitive function in the preschool years. Pediatrics. 1991;87:219–27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.McMichael A, Baghurst P, Wigg N, Vimpani G, Robertson E, Roberts R. Port Pirie Cohort Study: environmental exposure to lead and children's abilities at the age of four years. N Engl J Med. 1988;319:468–75. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198808253190803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mendelsohn A, Dreyer B, Fierman A. Low level lead exposure and behavior in early childhood. Pediatrics. 1998;101:E10. doi: 10.1542/peds.101.3.e10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Needleman H, Riess J, Tobin M, Biesecker G, Greenhouse J. Bone lead levels and delinquent behavior. JAMA. 1996;275:363–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Needleman H, McFarland C, Ness R, Fienberg S, Tobin M. Bone lead levels in adjudicated delinquents: a case control study. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24:711–7. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(02)00269-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Nevin R. How lead exposure relates to temporal changes in IQ, violent crime, and unwed pregnancy. Environ Res. 2000;83:1–22. doi: 10.1006/enrs.1999.4045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Stretesky P, Lynch M. The relationship between lead and crime. J Health Soc Behav. 2004;45:214–29. doi: 10.1177/002214650404500207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wright JP, Dietrich KN, Ris MD, Hornung RW, Wessel SD, Lanphear BP, et al. Association of prenatal and childhood blood lead concentrations with criminal arrests in early adulthood. PLoS Med. 2008;5:1–9. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Berney B. Round and round it goes: the epidemiology of childhood lead poisoning, 1950–1990. Milbank Q. 1993;71:3–39. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Baghurst PA, McMichael AJ, Wigg NR, Vimpani GV, Robertson EF, Roberts RJ, et al. Environmental exposure to lead and children's intelligence at the age of seven years. N Engl J Med. 1992;327:1279–84. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199210293271805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Bellinger DC, Stiles KM, Needleman HL. Low-level lead exposure, intelligence and academic achievement: a long-term follow-up study. Pediatrics. 1992;90:855–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Dietrich KN, Succop PA, Berger OG, Hammond PB, Bornschein RL. Lead exposure and the cognitive development of urban preschool children: the Cincinnati Lead Study cohort at age 4 years. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1991;13:203–11. doi: 10.1016/0892-0362(91)90012-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lanphear BP, Hornun R, Khoury J, Yolton K, Baghurst P, Bellinger D, et al. Low-level environmental lead exposure and children's intellectual function: an international pooled analysis. Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113(7):894–9. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Chen A, Dietrich KN, Ware JH, Radcliffe J, Rogan WJ. IQ and blood lead from 2 to 7 years of age: are the effects in older children the residual of high blood lead concentrations in 2-year-olds? Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113:597–601. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wisconsin DHFS, Department of Health and Family Services . The legacy of lead: the report on childhood lead poisoning in Wisconsin. Madison: Author; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.ACCLPP (Advisory Committee on Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention) Low level lead exposure harms children: a renewed call for primary prevention. Atlanta: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention) CDC response to Advisory Committee on Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention recommendations in “Low Level Lead Exposure Harms Children: A Renewed Call of Primary Prevention”. Atlanta: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wisconsin DPI, Department of Public Instruction Wisconsin knowledge and concepts examinations. http://dpi.wi.gov/oea/wkce.html.n.d. accessed 20.02.12.
  • 24.Braun JM, Kahn RS, Froelich T, Auinger P, Lanphear BP. Exposures to environmental toxicants and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in U.S. children. Environ Health Perspect. 2006;114(12):1904–9. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Miranda ML, Maxson P, Kim D. Early childhood lead exposure and exceptionality designations for students. Int J Child Health Hum Dev. 2010;3:77–84. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hinojosa T, Hoogstra L. Milwaukee Public Schools, Milwaukee, WI. Chicago: Learning Point Associates; 2010. The effects of Enrollment in the 2010 Milwaukee Public Schools Summer School Program. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Miranda ML, Kim D, Overstreet Galeano MA, Paul CJ, Hull AP, Morgan SP. The relationship between early childhood blood lead levels and performance on end-of-grade tests. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115(8):1242–7. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Miranda ML, Kim D, Reiter J, Overstreet Galeano MA, Maxson P. Environmental contributors to the achievement gap. Neurotoxicology. 2009;30:1019–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2009.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Tarr H, Raymond RE, Tufts M. Department of Health and Wellness Promotion. Detroit: Detroit Public Schools; The effects of lead exposure on school outcome among children living and attending public schools in Detroit, MI. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Strayhorn JC, Strayhorn JM., Jr Lead exposure and the 2010 achievement test scores of children in New York counties. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Ment Health. 2012;6(1):4. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-6-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Reyes JW. New England Public Policy Center. Boston: Federal Reserve Bank of Boston; 2011. Childhood lead and academic performance in Massachusetts. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Moore CF, Gajewski LL, Laughlin NK, Luck ML, Larson JA, Schneider ML. Developmental lead exposure induces tactile defensiveness in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Env Health Perspect. 2008;116(10):1322–6. doi: 10.1289/ehp.11203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Needleman HL. Childhood lead poisoning: the promise and abandonment of primary prevention. Am J Public Health. 1998;88:1871–7. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.12.1871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Jacobs DE, Clickner RP, Zhou JY, Viet SM, Marker DA, Rogers JW, Zeldin DC, et al. The prevalence of lead-based paint hazards in U.S. housing. Environ Health Perspect. 2002;110(10):A599–606. doi: 10.1289/ehp.021100599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dietrich KN, Ware JH, Salganik M, Radcliffe J, Rogan WJ, Rhoads GG, et al. Effect of chelation therapy on the neuropsychological and behavioral development of lead-exposed children after school entry. Pediatrics. 2004;114:19–26. doi: 10.1542/peds.114.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES