Abstract
The sense of smell is found widely in the animal kingdom. Human and animal studies show that odor perception is modulated by experience and/or physiological state (such as hunger), and that some odors can arouse emotion, and can lead to the recall of emotional memories. Further, odors can influence psychological and physiological states. Individual odorants are mapped via gene-specified receptors to corresponding glomeruli in the olfactory bulb, which directly projects to the piriform cortex and the amygdala without a thalamic relay. The odors to which a glomerulus responds reflect the chemical structure of the odorant. The piriform cortex and the amygdala both project to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) which with the amygdala is involved in emotion and associative learning, and to the entorhinal/hippocampal system which is involved in long-term memory including episodic memory. Evidence that some odors can modulate emotion and cognition is described, and the possible implications for the treatment of psychological problems, for example in reducing the effects of stress, are considered.
Keywords: odor, emotion, amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex
Introduction
Inputs received through the sensory systems, individually or together, can produce emotion and influence cognition (LeDoux, 1998; Rolls, 2005; Adolphs, 2010). Emotions can be elicited by stimuli that are instrumental reinforcers (Rolls, 2005). For example, the sight of an impending painful stimulus produces the emotional state of fear, and instrumental actions are performed to avoid the stimulus. Emotional states are generally associated with autonomic responses such as a change in heart rate, and endocrine responses, which are adaptive in that they prepare the body for action. Different reinforcing stimuli arouse different emotional states. Rewarding stimuli are associated with pleasant subjective states, and punishing stimuli with unpleasant subjective states. Human imaging studies have shown that reinforcing visual, auditory, olfactory and taste stimuli activate brain areas such as the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and that the activations are linearly correlated with the subjectively reported pleasantness or unpleasantness of the stimuli (Phan et al., 2002; Anderson et al., 2003; Kringelbach et al., 2003; Zald, 2003; Grabenhorst et al., 2007; Anders et al., 2008; Rolls et al., 2008). Odors may be particularly effective stimuli for the recall of memories including emotional memories (Herz, 1996).
The present review addresses two specific questions. First, what is the evidence for a strong link between odors and emotion? Second, what aspects of olfaction and the olfactory system predispose it to produce emotional responses? I begin by reviewing current definitions of emotion, and current views about the neurobiology of odor perception. The unique neuroanatomy and corresponding evolutionary history of the mammalian olfactory system (Lledo et al., 2005) suggest that it could be a very effective, often sub-conscious, driver of emotional responses. Further, the use of pleasant olfactory stimuli, acting as safety signals or reminders of a safe or pleasant event or place, may be useful in the treatment of psychiatric and related conditions, for example reducing anxiety (Lehrner et al., 2000, 2005).
What is emotion?
Darwin described expressions and gestures that are associated with emotion in humans and other animals (Darwin, 1872). Emotions are also associated with adaptive changes of physiological state including heart rate and endocrine responses (Darwin, 1872; LeDoux, 1998; Rolls, 2005; Adolphs, 2010; LeDoux, 2012). Emotions can usefully be defined as states elicited by stimuli that are rewards or punishers, that is which have value (Rolls, 2005). For example, the “fear” evoked by the smell of a predator is associated with actions to avoid the predator, changes in heart-rate, increased blood flow directed to skeletal muscles, adrenaline release, etc. that maximize the ability to expend energy to escape. Humans might report that this constellation of adaptive autonomic, endocrine, and behavioral responses is associated with an introspective or conscious label of “fear”. Although Ekman suggested that there is a set of basic emotions, fear, anger, enjoyment, sadness, disgust, and surprise (Ekman, 1992), a much larger range of emotions can be accounted for if we take into account the rewarding or punishing effects of many different types of reinforcer, such as the taste of food, pleasant touch or pain, and the sight of a beautiful person or scene. Humans and non-human animals alike will perform actions to obtain a stimulus which is a reward, and will perform actions to avoid or escape from a punisher, and these reinforced actions are fundamental to survival and reproductive success (Rolls, 2005).
Perception of odor
Odor perception can be influenced by learning and memory (Wilson and Stevenson, 2003a,b, 2006), for example exposure to wine or beer significantly improves discrimination (Owen and Machamer, 1979; Peron and Allen, 1988).
The piriform cortex plays a role in odor perception by recoding odor mixtures in such a way that some neurons can respond to an odorant mixture but not to the components (Kadohisa and Wilson, 2006b; Wilson and Sullivan, 2011). In particular, the anterior piriform cortex (aPC) encodes the identity of mixtures of odorants separately from the components, while the posterior piriform cortex (pPC) encodes the similarity between mixtures and their components (Gottfried et al., 2006; Kadohisa and Wilson, 2006b; Wilson and Sullivan, 2011). The implication is that the aPC enables complex mixtures to be represented separately from their components and from each other, while the pPC may enable generalization to similar odors. Another aspect of odor perception is short or long term habituation. aPC neurons adapt to a repeated or prolonged odor rapidly (Wilson, 1998; Wilson and Linster, 2008), which facilitates segmentation of an odor from the background odor, that is, aPC neurons detect a new odor against an adapted background odor (Kadohisa and Wilson, 2006a). This neurophysiological adaptation may be useful in drawing attention to harmful chemicals in adapted background odor environments.
Physiological state influences the perception of odor. For example, the odor of a food will be pleasant during hunger, and its pleasantness will decline as the food is being eaten to satiety (Rolls and Rolls, 1997; Yeshurun and Sobel, 2010). This can be a sensory-specific decrease in the pleasantness of the odor of a food eaten to satiety. In addition, the perception of odors can lead to physiological responses, such as salivation and the release of insulin (Lee and Linden, 1992; Yeomans, 2006; Savigner et al., 2009). Thus physiological state and the effects of odors can interact to influence motivation, for example whether to eat or not. In addition, odors influence autonomic responses, for example pleasant and novel odors can decrease heart rate (Fletcher and Wilson, 2001; Wilson, 2009), while the more sympathetically arousing the odor, the more the skin conductance response increases (Alaoui-Ismaili et al., 1997; Bensafi et al., 2002).
Thus, odor perception is neurophysiologically modulated by experience to facilitate identification of individual odorants including odor mixtures, and to filter out an odor by adaptation, and is also modified by physiological states such as hunger.
Olfactory processing streams in the brain
There are two different olfactory systems. So far, I have considered the main olfactory epithelium (MOE), which projects to the main olfactory bulb (MOB). In addition, there is an accessory olfactory system with its peripheral receptors located in the vomeronasal organ (VNO) which projects to the accessory olfactory bulb (AOB). The VNO contributes to the detection of pheromones while the MOB detects volatile odors including pheromones. The VNO of mice detects opposite-sex urinary volatiles, in contrast to the MOB which detects volatiles from both sexes (Martel and Baum, 2007; Baum, 2012). The VNOs of humans and old world monkeys are non-functional (Liman and Innan, 2003; Zhang and Webb, 2003; Brennan and Zufall, 2006) although pheromones play important roles in eliciting social and reproductive behavior. In humans, the MOB may be responsible for pheromone detection (Preti et al., 2003; Wysocki and Preti, 2004).
Considering the main olfactory system, the glomerular layer of the MOB forms odorant receptor maps which reflect individual odorant molecular features (Mori et al., 2006) and the behavioral effects such as escape that they produce (Kobayakawa et al., 2007; Mainen, 2007; Matsumoto et al., 2010; Mori and Sakano, 2011).
Odor information reaches the piriform cortex, amygdala, and entorhinal cortex from the MOB. As shown above, the aPC separates representations of odor mixtures from their components. The piriform cortex projects to the amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC) (OFC in primates) (Barbas and De Olmos, 1990; Carmichael et al., 1994; Carmichael and Price, 1995; Pitkanen et al., 2000). The amygdala and PFC are involved in the processes of emotion including emotion-related learning (Zald and Pardo, 1997; Royet et al., 2003; Rolls, 2004) as described below. The MOB and piriform cortex also project to the entorhinal cortex, which in turn projects to the hippocampus where olfactory information can become incorporated into episodic long-term memory (Rolls, 2010; Small et al., 2011; Olsen et al., 2012) as described below.
Amygdala: The MOB projects directly to the anterior cortical nucleus and medial nucleus of amygdala, and the periamygdaloid cortex in rodents and macaques (Turner et al., 1978; Turner and Mishkin, 1978; Carmichael et al., 1994; McDonald, 1998), while the AOB projects to the medial nucleus of the amygdala in rodents (Keverne, 1999; Pitkanen, 2000; Kang et al., 2009). The medial nucleus of the amygdala is thought to be a place for inputs from the AOB and MOB of rodents to interact with each other (Brennan and Zufall, 2006; Baum, 2012). In addition, the piriform cortex projects directly to the basolateral nucleus of amygdala in both rodents and macaques (Carmichael et al., 1994; McDonald, 1998). Further, the amygdala projects to the hypothalamus (Pitkanen, 2000; Barbas et al., 2003; Gabbott et al., 2012), which may provide a route for olfactory stimuli to influence the autonomic and endocrine systems.
The amygdala plays a key role in some affective responses to stimuli. Amygdala-lesioned animals show a lack of reactions such as freezing, and autonomic responses, to a fear conditioned stimulus (CS; Weiskrantz, 1956; Helmstetter, 1992; Phillips and LeDoux, 1992; Sananes and Davis, 1992), and show abnormal food preferences including reduced neophobia (Rolls and Rolls, 1973; Murray et al., 1996). In addition, the amygdala plays a role in associative learning, for example, some amygdala neurons encode odor cues associated with a positive or negative taste (Schoenbaum et al., 1999).
Hippocampus: The hippocampus receives olfactory information from the amygdala (Petrovich et al., 2001) and from the entorhinal cortex which receives inputs from the MOB and piriform cortex (Price, 1973; Witter and Amaral, 1991; Carmichael et al., 1994).
The hippocampus plays a fundamental role in episodic memory (Lemogne et al., 2006; Rolls, 2010; Small et al., 2011; Olsen et al., 2012). Hippocampal damage impairs odor-place associative learning, and temporal order memory for odor information (Kesner et al., 2002; Rolls and Kesner, 2006). Some neurons in the hippocampus represent the place of odor-related reward (Rolls, 2010; Tort et al., 2011). In addition, stimulation of the hippocampus affects autonomic responses, that is, decreases heart rate and blood pressure (Ruit and Neafsey, 1988).
Prefrontal cortex: The primate OFC receives olfactory information from the piriform cortex (Carmichael and Price, 1994), the amygdala (Amaral and Price, 1984; Ghashghaei and Barbas, 2002; Barbas et al., 2003), parahippocampal cortices (Carmichael and Price, 1995), and from the hypothalamus (Tanabe et al., 1975; Barbas et al., 2003).
The OFC associates olfactory with taste, oral texture, and visual inputs, to produce multimodal representations of reward value, such as the reward value of food (Rolls, 2012). The OFC representations are of the economic value of goods, e.g., of a food (Padoa-Schioppa, 2011). The reward evaluation performed by the OFC is important in both emotion and in autonomic responses such as heart rate and skin conductance responses (Damasio, 1995; Bechara et al., 1996).
In addition, the amygdala, OFC, and hippocampus are also involved in the regulation of stress which causes not only psychological and psychiatric but also physiological problems (see Mcewen, 2007; Arnsten, 2009; Rodrigues et al., 2009; Ulrich-Lai and Herman, 2009). As described below, the olfactory pathways may enable some odorants to influence stress in a therapeutic way.
Odor-evoked emotion and odor-associated emotional memory
There is evidence that odors elicit emotion and are linked to emotional memory. Responses to odors can include emotions, which can usefully be defined as states elicited by stimuli that are rewards and punishers (Rolls, 2005). For examples, a predator’s odor can cause a prey to escape from it, while newborn babies move towards the breast odor of their own mother (Varendi and Porter, 2001). Different odors can elicit individual affective responses which may be pleasant or unpleasant (Chrea et al., 2009; Seubert et al., 2009, 2010). Emotionally arousing odors may influence cognition and emotion. Some odors facilitate the recognition of disgust face expressions (Seubert et al., 2010) and impair working memory (Schneider et al., 2006; Habel et al., 2007) while others reduce anger (Rétiveau et al., 2004) or improve mood (Schiffman et al., 1995). In addition, some affective odors modulate psychological and physiological state. Komori and colleagues (Komori et al., 1995) reported effects of citrus fragrance on the improvement of psychological states and immune function of patients with depression. Green odor emanating from oak leaves is necessary for polyphemus moths to mate (Riddiford, 1967), and elicits pleasantness from humans (Sano et al., 2002). In addition, green odor attenuates the stress-induced elevations of plasma adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), body temperature (Nakashima et al., 2004), and skin-barrier disruption in rats (Fukada et al., 2007). Similarly, rose essential oil inhibits stress-induced skin-barrier disruption and elevation of salivary cortisol in humans (Fukada et al., 2012), and orange and lavender reduce the anxiety of patients in a dental office (Lehrner et al., 2000, 2005).
Odors can become associated by learning with reinforcers such as taste in the OFC and amygdala, and this provides a way for previously neutral odors to produce emotional responses (Herz et al., 2004; Rolls, 2005). Patients frightened by dental treatment respond negatively to the smell of eugenol which is used for treatment (Robin et al., 1998). Odor-evoked autobiographical memory is powerful in reminding humans about past emotional experiences (Chu and Downes, 2002; Larsson and Willander, 2009; Zucco et al., 2012). This odor associative memory and learning begins early in life (Schaal et al., 2000; Larsson and Willander, 2009). For examples, the foods eaten by a pregnant mother can influence the odors liked by her offspring (Schaal et al., 2000), and neonatal rats at ages of less than 3 days learn the odor associated with aversive or rewarded outcomes (Rudy and Cheatle, 1977; Johanson and Hall, 1979). Further, this associative learning regulates behavior, that is, when animals have learned associations between an odor CS and a negative outcome such as foot shock unconditioned stimulus (US), they try to avoid the CS associated with the negative US, or show fear expressions such as freezing (Sullivan et al., 2000; Sacco and Sacchetti, 2010).
The amygdala, OFC and hippocampus are involved in the process of odor-elicited emotion and odor-associated emotional memory. Human imaging studies have demonstrated that the amygdala and OFC are activated by unpleasant or pleasant odors, consistent with their functions in emotion (Zald and Pardo, 1997; Royet et al., 2003; Small et al., 2003). Consistently, activations of the human OFC elicited by odors correlate with the subjective pleasantness or unpleasantness of the odors (Rolls, 2000; Anderson et al., 2003; Grabenhorst et al., 2007). Amygdala lesions impair learned odor preferences in infant rats (Sullivan and Wilson, 1993) while hippocampus and subiculum lesions in rats produce a deficit in social learning of an odor association (Alvarez et al., 2002). In humans, the amygdala and PFC of patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are activated by exposure to the odor related to the traumatic memory (Vermetten et al., 2007) while the hippocampus and PFC are activated by odor cues during autobiographical memory retrieval process (Larsson and Willander, 2009).
The odor mapping performed in the aPC may set up representations of odors that are separate, even when considering odorant mixtures and their components (Kadohisa and Wilson, 2006b; Wilson and Sullivan, 2011). This pattern separation of the representations of even odorant mixtures using non-linear remapping is useful for the pattern association learning of odors to taste and visual representations performed in the OFC and amygdala to represent the reward/emotional value of the odor object (Rolls, 2008), and for the autoassociative formation of episodic memory performed in the hippocampus (Rolls, 2010).
Conclusions
Human and complementary studies in non-human animals provide evidence that odors evoke emotion and autonomic state via pathways to the amygdala and OFC, and become incorporated into episodic memory via the hippocampus.
In addition, it is suggested that some odorants which elicit emotion may have potential to treat patients with psychological problem such as depression.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
I greatly appreciate comments on an earlier draft of this paper provided by Professor D.A. Wilson (NKI and NYU/CNS).
References
- Adolphs R. (2010). Social cognition: feeling voices to recognize emotions. Curr. Biol. 20, R1071–R1072 10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alaoui-Ismaili O., Vernet-Maury E., Dittmar A., Delhomme G., Chanel J. (1997). Odor hedonics: connection with emotional response estimated by autonomic parameters. Chem. Senses 22, 237–248 10.1093/chemse/22.3.237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alvarez P., Wendelken L., Eichenbaum H. (2002). Hippocampal formation lesions impair performance in an odor-odor association task independently of spatial context. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 78, 470–476 10.1006/nlme.2002.4068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amaral D. G., Price J. L. (1984). Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis). J. Comp. Neurol. 230, 465–496 10.1002/cne.902300402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anders S., Eippert F., Weiskopf N., Veit R. (2008). The human amygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and sounds irrespective of arousal: an fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 233–243 10.1093/scan/nsn017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson A. K., Christoff K., Stappen I., Panitz D., Ghahremani D. G., Glover G., et al. (2003). Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 196–202 10.1038/nn1001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnsten A. F. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 410–422 10.1038/nrn2648 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H., De Olmos J. (1990). Projections from the amygdala to basoventral and mediodorsal prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 300, 549–571 10.1002/cne.903000409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H., Saha S., Rempel-Clower N., Ghashghaei T. (2003). Serial pathways from primate prefrontal cortex to autonomic areas may influence emotional expression. BMC Neurosci. 4:25 10.1186/1471-2202-4-25 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baum M. J. (2012). Contribution of pheromones processed by the main olfactory system to mate recognition in female mammals. Front. Neuroanat. 6:20 10.3389/fnana.2012.00020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A., Tranel D., Damasio H., Damasio A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 6, 215–225 10.1093/cercor/6.2.215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bensafi M., Rouby C., Farget V., Bertrand B., Vigouroux M., Holley A. (2002). Autonomic nervous system responses to odours: the role of pleasantness and arousal. Chem. Senses 27, 703–709 10.1093/chemse/27.8.703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan P. A., Zufall F. (2006). Pheromonal communication in vertebrates. Nature 444, 308–315 10.1038/nature05404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmichael S. T., Clugnet M. C., Price J. L. (1994). Central olfactory connections in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 346, 403–434 10.1002/cne.903460306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmichael S. T., Price J. L. (1994). Architectonic subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 346, 366–402 10.1002/cne.903460305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmichael S. T., Price J. L. (1995). Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 363, 615–641 10.1002/cne.903630408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chrea C., Grandjean D., Delplanque S., Cayeux I., Le Calve B., Aymard L., et al. (2009). Mapping the semantic space for the subjective experience of emotional responses to odors. Chem. Senses 34, 49–62 10.1093/chemse/bjn052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chu S., Downes J. J. (2002). Proust nose best: odors are better cues of autobiographical memory. Mem. Cognit. 30, 511–518 10.3758/bf03194952 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio A. R. (1995). On some functions of the human prefrontal cortex. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 769, 241–251 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38142.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darwin C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Leipzig: Amazon [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P. (1992). Facial expressions of emotion: an old controversy and new findings. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 335, 63–69 10.1098/rstb.1992.0008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher M., Wilson D. A. (2001). Ontogeny of odor discrimination: a method to assess novel odor discrimination in neonatal rats. Physiol. Behav. 74, 589–593 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00602-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukada M., Kaidoh T., Ito A., Yano T., Hayashibara C., Watanabe T. (2007). “Green odor” inhalation reduces the skin-barrier disruption induced by chronic restraint stress in rats: physiological and histological examinations. Chem. Senses 32, 633–639 10.1093/chemse/bjm031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukada M., Kano E., Miyoshi M., Komaki R., Watanabe T. (2012). Effect of “rose essential oil” inhalation on stress-induced skin-barrier disruption in rats and humans. Chem. Senses 37, 347–356 10.1093/chemse/bjr108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabbott P., Warner T. A., Brown J., Salway P., Gabbott T., Busby S. (2012). Amygdala afferents monosynaptically innervate corticospinal neurons in rat medial prefrontal cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 520, 2440–2458 10.1002/cne.23047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghashghaei H. T., Barbas H. (2002). Pathways for emotion: interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience 115, 1261–1279 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00446-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottfried J. A., Winston J. S., Dolan R. J. (2006). Dissociable codes of odor quality and odorant structure in human piriform cortex. Neuron 49, 467–479 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabenhorst F., Rolls E. T., Margot C., da Silva M. A., Velazco M. I. (2007). How pleasant and unpleasant stimuli combine in different brain regions: odor mixtures. J. Neurosci. 27, 13532–13540 10.1523/jneurosci.3337-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Habel U., Koch K., Pauly K., Kellermann T., Reske M., Backes V., et al. (2007). The influence of olfactory-induced negative emotion on verbal working memory: individual differences in neurobehavioral findings. Brain Res. 1152, 158–170 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmstetter F. J. (1992). Contribution of the amygdala to learning and performance of conditional fear. Physiol. Behav. 51, 1271–1276 10.1016/0031-9384(92)90320-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herz R. S. (1996). A comparison of olfactory, tactile and visual as associated memory cues. Chem. Senses 21, 614–615 [Google Scholar]
- Herz R. S., Schankler C., Beland S. (2004). Olfaction, emotion and associative learning: effects on motivated behavior. Motiv. Emotion 28, 363–383 10.1007/s11031-004-2389-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Johanson I. B., Hall W. G. (1979). Appetitive learning in 1-day-old rat pups. Science 205, 419–421 10.1126/science.451612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kadohisa M., Wilson D. A. (2006a). Olfactory cortical adaptation facilitates detection of odors against background. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 1888–1896 10.1152/jn.00812.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kadohisa M., Wilson D. A. (2006b). Separate encoding of identity and similarity of complex familiar odors in piriform cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 103, 15206–15211 10.1073/pnas.0604313103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang N., Baum M. J., Cherry J. A. (2009). A direct main olfactory bulb projection to the ‘vomeronasal’ amygdala in female mice selectively responds to volatile pheromones from males. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 624–634 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06638.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kesner R. P., Gilbert P. E., Barua L. A. (2002). The role of the hippocampus in memory for the temporal order of a sequence of odors. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 286–290 10.1037/0735-7044.116.2.286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keverne E. B. (1999). The vomeronasal organ. Science 286, 716–720 10.1126/science.286.5440.716 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayakawa K., Kobayakawa R., Matsumoto H., Oka Y., Imai T., Ikawa M., et al. (2007). Innate versus learned odour processing in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature 450, 503–508 10.1038/nature06281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Komori T., Fujiwara R., Tanida M., Nomura J., Yokoyama M. M. (1995). Effects of citrus fragrance on immune function and depressive states. Neuroimmunomodulation 2, 174–180 10.1159/000096889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach M. L., O’doherty J., Rolls E. T., Andrews C. (2003). Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1064–1071 10.1093/cercor/13.10.1064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsson M., Willander J. (2009). Autobiographical odor memory. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1170, 318–323 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03934.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J. (1998). The Emotional Brain. New York: Phoenix [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J. (2012). Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73, 653–676 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee V. M., Linden R. W. (1992). An olfactory-submandibular salivary reflex in humans. Exp. Physiol. 77, 221–224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehrner J., Eckersberger C., Walla P., Potsch G., Deecke L. (2000). Ambient odor of orange in a dental office reduces anxiety and improves mood in female patients. Physiol. Behav. 71, 83–86 10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00308-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehrner J., Marwinski G., Lehr S., Johren P., Deecke L. (2005). Ambient odors of orange and lavender reduce anxiety and improve mood in a dental office. Physiol. Behav. 86, 92–95 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemogne C., Piolino P., Friszer S., Claret A., Girault N., Jouvent R., et al. (2006). Episodic autobiographical memory in depression: specificity, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective. Conscious Cogn. 15, 258–268 10.1016/j.concog.2005.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liman E. R., Innan H. (2003). Relaxed selective pressure on an essential component of pheromone transduction in primate evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 100, 3328–3332 10.1073/pnas.0636123100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lledo P. M., Gheusi G., Vincent J. D. (2005). Information processing in the mammalian olfactory system. Physiol. Rev. 85, 281–317 10.1152/physrev.00008.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mainen Z. F. (2007). The main olfactory bulb and innate behavior: different perspectives on an olfactory scene. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1511–1512 10.1038/nn1207-1511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martel K. L., Baum M. J. (2007). Sexually dimorphic activation of the accessory, but not the main, olfactory bulb in mice by urinary volatiles. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 463–475 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05651.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto H., Kobayakawa K., Kobayakawa R., Tashiro T., Mori K., Sakano H., et al. (2010). Spatial arrangement of glomerular molecular-feature clusters in the odorant-receptor class domains of the mouse olfactory bulb. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 3490–3500 10.1152/jn.00035.2010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonald A. J. (1998). Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala. Prog. Neurobiol. 55, 257–332 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00003-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mcewen B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol. Rev. 87, 873–904 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori K., Sakano H. (2011). How is the olfactory map formed and interpreted in the mammalian brain? Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 467–499 10.1146/annurev-neuro-112210-112917 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori K., Takahashi Y. K., Igarashi K. M., Yamaguchi M. (2006). Maps of odorant molecular features in the Mammalian olfactory bulb. Physiol. Rev. 86, 409–433 10.1152/physrev.00021.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray E. A., Gaffan E. A., Flint R. W. Jr. (1996). Anterior rhinal cortex and amygdala: dissociation of their contributions to memory and food preference in rhesus monkeys. Behav. Neurosci. 110, 30–42 10.1037/0735-7044.110.1.30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakashima T., Akamatsu M., Hatanaka A., Kiyohara T. (2004). Attenuation of stress-induced elevations in plasma ACTH level and body temperature in rats by green odor. Physiol. Behav. 80, 481–488 10.1016/j.physbeh.2003.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsen R. K., Moses S. N., Riggs L., Ryan J. D. (2012). The hippocampus supports multiple cognitive processes through relational binding and comparison. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:146 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owen D. H., Machamer P. K. (1979). Bias-free improvement in wine discrimination. Perception 8, 199–209 10.1068/p080199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padoa-Schioppa C. (2011). Neurobiology of economic choice: a good-based model. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 333–359 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113648 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peron R. M., Allen G. L. (1988). Attempts to train novices for beer flavor discrimination: a matter of taste. J. Gen. Psychol. 115, 403–418 10.1080/00221309.1988.9710577 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrovich G. D., Canteras N. S., Swanson L. W. (2001). Combinatorial amygdalar inputs to hippocampal domains and hypothalamic behavior systems. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 38, 247–289 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00080-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phan K. L., Wager T., Taylor S. F., Liberzon I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage 16, 331–348 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips R. G., LeDoux J. E. (1992). Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 274–285 10.1037/0735-7044.106.2.274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitkanen A. (2000). The Amygdala. Oxford: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
- Pitkanen A., Pikkarainen M., Nurminen N., Ylinen A. (2000). Reciprocal connections between the amygdala and the hippocampal formation, perirhinal cortex, and postrhinal cortex in rat. A review. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 911, 369–391 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06738.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preti G., Wysocki C. J., Barnhart K. T., Sondheimer S. J., Leyden J. J. (2003). Male axillary extracts contain pheromones that affect pulsatile secretion of luteinizing hormone and mood in women recipients. Biol. Reprod. 68, 2107–2113 10.1095/biolreprod.102.008268 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price J. L. (1973). An autoradiographic study of complementary laminar patterns of termination of afferent fibers to the olfactory cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 150, 87–108 10.1002/cne.901500105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rétiveau A. N., Chambers IV E., Milliken G. A. (2004). Common and specific effects of fine fragrances on the mood of women. J. Sens. Stud. 19, 373–394 10.1111/j.1745-459x.2004.102803.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Riddiford L. M. (1967). Trans-2-hexenal: mating stimulant for polyphemus moths. Science 158, 139–141 10.1126/science.158.3797.139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robin O., Alaoui-Ismaili O., Dittmar A., Vernet-Maury E. (1998). Emotional responses evoked by dental odors: an evaluation from autonomic parameters. J. Dent. Res. 77, 1638–1646 10.1177/00220345980770081201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues S. M., Ledoux J. E., Sapolsky R. M. (2009). The influence of stress hormones on fear circuitry. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 289–313 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135620 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cereb. Cortex 10, 284–294 10.1093/cercor/10.3.284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T. (2004). Convergence of sensory systems in the orbitofrontal cortex in primates and brain design for emotion. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell. Evol. Biol. 281, 1212–1225 10.1002/ar.a.20126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T. (2005). Emotion Explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T. (2008). Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making. Oxford: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T. (2010). A computational theory of episodic memory formation in the hippocampus. Behav. Brain Res. 215, 180–196 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T. (2012). Taste, olfactory and food texture reward processing in the brain and the control of appetite. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 1–14 10.1017/s0029665112000821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T., Grabenhorst F., Parris B. A. (2008). Warm pleasant feelings in the brain. Neuroimage 41, 1504–1513 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T., Kesner R. P. (2006). A computational theory of hippocampal function, and empirical tests of the theory. Prog. Neurobiol. 79, 1–48 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T., Rolls B. J. (1973). Altered food preferences after lesions in the basolateral region of the amygdala in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 83, 248–259 10.1037/h0034414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E. T., Rolls J. H. (1997). Olfactory sensory-specific satiety in humans. Physiol. Behav. 61, 461–473 10.1016/s0031-9384(96)00464-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royet J. P., Plailly J., Delon-Martin C., Kareken D. A., Segebarth C. (2003). fMRI of emotional responses to odors: influence of hedonic valence and judgment, handedness, and gender. Neuroimage 20, 713–728 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00388-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudy J. W., Cheatle M. D. (1977). Odor-aversion learning in neonatal rats. Science 198, 845–846 10.1126/science.918668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruit K. G., Neafsey E. J. (1988). Cardiovascular and respiratory responses to electrical and chemical stimulation of the hippocampus in anesthetized and awake rats. Brain Res. 457, 310–321 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90701-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sacco T., Sacchetti B. (2010). Role of secondary sensory cortices in emotional memory storage and retrieval in rats. Science 329, 649–656 10.1126/science.1183165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sananes C. B., Davis M. (1992). N-methyl-D-aspartate lesions of the lateral and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala block fear-potentiated startle and shock sensitization of startle. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 72–80 10.1037/0735-7044.106.1.72 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sano K., Tsuda Y., Sugano H., Aou S., Hatanaka A. (2002). Concentration effects of green odor on event-related potential (P300) and pleasantness. Chem. Senses 27, 225–230 10.1093/chemse/27.3.225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savigner A., Duchamp-Viret P., Grosmaitre X., Chaput M., Garcia S., Ma M., et al. (2009). Modulation of spontaneous and odorant-evoked activity of rat olfactory sensory neurons by two anorectic peptides, insulin and leptin. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 2898–2906 10.1152/jn.91169.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaal B., Marlier L., Soussignan R. (2000). Human foetuses learn odours from their pregnant mother’s diet. Chem. Senses 25, 729–737 10.1093/chemse/25.6.729 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiffman S. S., Suggs M. S., Sattely-Miller E. A. (1995). Effect of pleasant odors on mood of males at midlife: comparison of African-American and European-American men. Brain Res. Bull. 36, 31–37 10.1016/0361-9230(94)00134-m [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider F., Gur R. C., Koch K., Backes V., Amunts K., Shah N. J., et al. (2006). Impairment in the specificity of emotion processing in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 442–447 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.442 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G., Chiba A. A., Gallagher M. (1999). Neural encoding in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during olfactory discrimination learning. J. Neurosci. 19, 1876–1884 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seubert J., Kellermann T., Loughead J., Boers F., Brensinger C., Schneider F., et al. (2010). Processing of disgusted faces is facilitated by odor primes: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage 53, 746–56 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seubert J., Rea A. F., Loughead J., Habel U. (2009). Mood induction with olfactory stimuli reveals differential affective responses in males and females. Chem. Senses 34, 77–84 10.1093/chemse/bjn054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Small D. M., Jones-Gotman M., Dagher A. (2003). Feeding-induced dopamine release in dorsal striatum correlates with meal pleasantness ratings in healthy human volunteers. Neuroimage 19, 1709–1715 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00253-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Small S. A., Schobel S. A., Buxton R. B., Witter M. P., Barnes C. A. (2011). A pathophysiological framework of hippocampal dysfunction in ageing and disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 585–601 10.1038/nrn3085 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan R. M., Landers M., Yeaman B., Wilson D. A. (2000). Good memories of bad events in infancy. Nature 407, 38–39 10.1038/35024156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan R. M., Wilson D. A. (1993). Role of the amygdala complex in early olfactory associative learning. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 254–263 10.1037/0735-7044.107.2.254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanabe T., Yarita H., Iino M., Ooshima Y., Takagi S. F. (1975). An olfactory projection area in orbitofrontal cortex of the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 38, 1269–1283 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tort A. B., Komorowski R., Kopell N., Eichenbaum H. (2011). A mechanism for the formation of hippocampal neuronal firing patterns that represent what happens where. Learn. Mem. 18, 718–727 10.1101/lm.2307711 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner B. H., Gupta K. C., Mishkin M. (1978). The locus and cytoarchitecture of the projection areas of the olfactory bulb in Macaca mulatta. J. Comp. Neurol. 177, 381–396 10.1002/cne.901770303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner B. H., Mishkin M. (1978). A reassessment of the direct projections of the olfactory bulb. Brain Res. 151, 375–380 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90892-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich-Lai Y. M., Herman J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 397–409 10.1038/nrn2647 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varendi H., Porter R. H. (2001). Breast odour as the only maternal stimulus elicits crawling towards the odour source. Acta Paediatr. 90, 372–375 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2001.tb00434.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vermetten E., Bremner J. D., Skelton L., Spiegel D. (2007). Ptsd and Vietnam Veterans. Science 315, 184–187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiskrantz L. (1956). Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 49, 381–391 10.1037/h0088009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson D. A. (1998). Habituation of odor responses in the rat anterior piriform cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 1425–1440 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson D. A. (2009). Olfaction as a model system for the neurobiology of mammalian short-term habituation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 92, 199–205 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson D. A., Linster C. (2008). Neurobiology of a simple memory. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 2–7 10.1152/jn.90479.2008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson D. A., Stevenson R. J. (2003a). The fundamental role of memory in olfactory perception. Trends Neurosci. 26, 243–247 10.1016/s0166-2236(03)00076-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson D. A., Stevenson R. J. (2003b). Olfactory perceptual learning: the critical role of memory in odor discrimination. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 307–328 10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00050-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson D. A., Stevenson R. J. (2006). Learning to Smell. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press [Google Scholar]
- Wilson D. A., Sullivan R. M. (2011). Cortical processing of odor objects. Neuron 72, 506–519 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witter M. P., Amaral D. G. (1991). Entorhinal cortex of the monkey: V. Projections to the dentate gyrus, hippocampus, and subicular complex. J. Comp. Neurol. 307, 437–459 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wysocki C. J., Preti G. (2004). Facts, fallacies, fears, and frustrations with human pheromones. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell. Evol. Biol. 281, 1201–1211 10.1002/ar.a.20125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeomans M. R. (2006). Olfactory influences on appetite and satiety in humans. Physiol. Behav. 89, 10–14 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeshurun Y., Sobel N. (2010). An odor is not worth a thousand words: from multidimensional odors to unidimensional odor objects. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 219–241 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163639 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zald D. H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 41, 88–123 10.1016/s0165-0173(02)00248-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zald D. H., Pardo J. V. (1997). Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 94, 4119–4124 10.1073/pnas.94.8.4119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang J., Webb D. M. (2003). Evolutionary deterioration of the vomeronasal pheromone transduction pathway in catarrhine primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 100, 8337–8341 10.1073/pnas.1331721100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zucco G. M., Aiello L., Turuani L., Koster E. (2012). Odor-evoked autobiographical memories: age and gender differences along the life span. Chem. Senses 37, 179–189 10.1093/chemse/bjr089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]