Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Autism Dev Disord. 2013 Nov;43(11):2710–2719. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1833-8

Social Participation Among Young Adults with an Autism Spectrum Disorder

Gael I Orsmond 1, Paul T Shattuck 2, Benjamin P Cooper 2, Paul R Sterzing 3, Kristy A Anderson 4
PMCID: PMC3795788  NIHMSID: NIHMS472487  PMID: 23615687

Abstract

Investigating social participation of young adults with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is important given the increasing number of youth aging into young adulthood. Social participation is an indicator of life quality and overall functioning. Using data from the National Longitudinal Transition Study 2, we examined rates of participation in social activities among young adults who received special education services for autism (ASD group), compared to young adults who received special education for intellectual disability, emotional/behavioral disability, or a learning disability. Young adults with an ASD were significantly more likely to never see friends, never get called by friends, never be invited to activities, and be socially isolated. Among those with an ASD, lower conversation ability, lower functional skills, and living with parent were predictors of less social participation.

Keywords: autistic spectrum disorder, social participation, young adulthood


A recent epidemiological study in England indicated that the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) in adults is similar to the rates recently observed among children (approximately 1 in 100; Brugha et al., 2011). Researchers have documented that the communication and social impairments that characterize the disorder continue throughout adulthood (Billstedt, Gillberg, & Gillberg, 2007; Seltzer et al., 2011; Shattuck et al., 2007). Although investigators have begun to document young adults’ involvement in work and school settings (Shattuck et al., 2012), little is known about these adults’ participation in social settings. Describing the social participation of young adults with an ASD is important as it is a central aspect of their quality of life (Verdugo, Navas, Gómez, & Schalock, 2012), and reflects current theoretical views emphasizing “participation” as a desirable outcome for individuals with behavior and health conditions (World Health Organization, 2001).

The growing number of individuals with an ASD aging into young adulthood increases the urgency to describe and understand social participation outcomes in this population. Accordingly, the Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee (IACC) has called for research aimed at improving the quality of life and functioning of adults with autism, with the overall goal of enabling adults to lead fulfilling and productive lives in the community (IACC, 2012). In this study, we aim to provide one of the first descriptions of the extent of social participation or isolation among young adults with an ASD. This information is necessary for the planning of adult services that can meet the specific needs of this transition-age population.

Social outcomes in adults with an ASD have been documented through longitudinal follow-up studies as well as research examining quality of life in adulthood. This research suggests that adults with an ASD often experience social isolation (Orsmond, Krauss, & Seltzer, 2004), with approximately one-half to two-thirds of this population having no close friendships (Billstedt et al., 2007; Eaves & Ho, 2008; Howlin, Mawhood, & Rutter, 2000; Howlin, 2003; Howlin, Goode, Hutton, & Rutter, 2004; Liptak, Kennedy, & Dosa, 2011; Whitehouse, Watt, Line, & Bishop, 2009). When friendships do occur, they appear to be less close and supportive than in the general population (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2003). The rates for social participation in the community are also low. In studies of Canadian and European populations, fewer than half of adults with an ASD participated in social events in the community, such as attending church or special interest groups or participating in recreational activities that would provide social opportunities (Billstedt et al., 2007; Eaves & Ho, 2008; Howlin et al., 2000). With respect to the predictors of social participation, researchers have documented that individuals with higher cognitive and adaptive skills, however, are more likely to have meaningful social relationships (Farley et al., 2009; Howlin et al., 2004; Liptak et al., 2011).

Although this research literature is fairly consistent in describing the social difficulties of adults with an ASD, few studies have focused exclusively on young adulthood. This life stage represents a potentially crucial turning point in the maintenance and development of social relationships. Oftentimes, friendships from school are severed or require considerable effort to maintain in the context of less frequent personal contact. Alternately, individuals may have the opportunity to develop new friendships or meaningful social relationships in new settings, such as higher education, work, volunteer, or day programs. In prior research, the data presented have been from samples that include individuals from adolescence through middle adulthood. More specific knowledge about young adulthood outcomes will aid in transition planning from school-based to adult services. Additionally, few studies have included comparison groups, so we know little about the unique aspects of social isolation in young adults with an ASD, compared to other disability groups. Moreover, much of the extant information is also based on studies with small samples that are not racially and socioeconomically diverse, nor nationally representative.

In this paper, we present young adult social participation outcomes using data from a large nationally representative cohort study from the United States. We compare social participation in four groups of young adults who formerly received special education services for an ASD (special education designation of autism), intellectual disability (special education designation of mental retardation), emotional disturbance, and learning disability. Our selection of these comparison groups was both pragmatic and theoretical. These groups represent the largest disability populations receiving special education services, and thus our data includes large numbers and our findings are applicable to a large group of students. By comparing rates of social participation across different diagnostic groups, we will be able to determine if ASDs pose unique social challenges at this life stage, or whether disability more broadly contributes to social challenges. Based on prior research, we anticipated that young adults with an ASD would show higher rates of social isolation than the other disability groups.

We addressed three research questions: (1) What are the rates of participation in social activities among young adults with an ASD? (2) How do these participation rates compare to young adults with other types of disabilities? and (3) What personal and contextual factors are associated with limited social participation? In answering these research questions, we anticipate that our findings will provide important needed information that will raise awareness of the unique challenges that young adults with an ASD experience and provide a basis for framing the service and policy needs of this population at this life stage.

Method

Ethics Statement

Use of these data is governed by a data use agreement with the U.S. Department of Education and was deemed exempt by the Washington University Institutional Review Board.

Participants

The National Longitudinal Transition Study-2 (NLTS2) was a prospective study that collected data from multiple sources in five waves, two years apart, from 2001 to 2009. The study began with over 11,000 youth who were receiving special education services at baseline and followed them as they aged into young adulthood. The NLTS2 sampling plan was designed to yield nationally representative estimates that would generalize to all students who were receiving special education services and were 13 through 16 years old on December 1, 2000. This data set provides a unique opportunity to examine social participation in a racially and socioeconomically diverse sample that generalizes to the population.

This paper is primarily based on parent and student surveys collected for Wave 5, in 2009. Some covariates were not measured at Wave 5 and, thus, Wave 4 data were used where noted. Unweighted sample numbers in this report were rounded to the nearest ten, as required by the U.S. Department of Education. The multistage sampling procedure sampled school districts first and then students within districts (SRI International, 2000). Survey weights enable estimates to generalize to the national population of youth who were receiving special education services in the given age range and disability type. Weights were adjusted for attrition at each wave so that estimates always generalize to the same initial population. Full details of the weighting strategy for NLTS2 were previously published (Wagner, Kutash, Duchnowski, & Epstein, 2005).

We compared the social experiences of young adults with an ASD to those who had been served in other special education categories and who had also exited high school by Wave 5: intellectual disability (ID), emotional disturbance (ED), and learning disabilities (LD). The total number of young adults with ASD remaining in the study at Wave 5 was 660. We restricted analyses to those who were no longer in high school at the time of the interviews in 2009, reducing the number used for analysis to 620. We excluded 20 comparison group members (ID, ED, LD) from analyses who also had a parent report of ever receiving an autism-related diagnosis.

For each of these categories (ASD, ID, ED, and LD), determination of eligibility for special education services was made by each student’s school district. Thus, the groups represent young adults who received special education services using school-based criteria. Findings from this study should generalize to young adults who had received special education services through the autism eligibility category. Local schools deem students eligible and do not necessarily adhere to standardized clinical assessment or diagnostic guidelines. Thus, we use the term ASD throughout to refer to individuals who received special education services under the autism eligibility category, as they likely include students with Autistic Disorder, Asperger’s Disorder and Pervasive Developmental Disorder – Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS). Prior research has demonstrated that this labeling process results in very high specificity and moderate sensitivity (Bertrand et al., 2001; Yeargin-Allsopp et al., 2003).

Procedure

Parent/guardian telephone interviews were conducted in 2009. The interview began by identifying the adult who was best able to respond about the sampled youth. Parents/guardians reported on demographics and measures of impairment severity. They were then asked about the young adult’s capacity for answering questions independently. If the parent/guardian indicated that the young adult would not be able to answer questions independently, the interview continued with parents, including the sequence of questions about social participation. If deemed capable by the parent/guardian respondent then youth or youth adult himself or herself was asked to respond to these questions.

Measures

We examined 3 measures of social activity and participation and 1 composite measure created from the 3 measures. Survey respondents were asked to think about “the past 12 months” when answering questions about social participation. Two questions, “getting together with friends” and “friends calling on the phone,” were asked as six-category ordinal questions. The term “friends” was not defined for survey respondents. One question, “being invited to activities,” was dichotomous. We created dichotomous indicators of limited social participation during the past 12 months for our logistic regression models: (1) never got together with friends; (2) friends never called on the phone; (3) was never invited to other friends’ social activities; and (4) the created variable of “social isolation” (young adult never had any of these 3 forms of social participation during the past 12 months). We used this strategy in order to facilitate interpreting all 4 statistical models consistently as correlates of the complete absence of each type of social participation.

Demographic correlates included gender, age, ethnicity and race, and overall health status. Fewer than 10% of young adults with an ASD were living independently, so we also used parent/guardian household income as our measure of socioeconomic position. A 4-category ordinal question asked parents how well their child could carry on a conversation. A functional skills scale was constructed by summing eight 4-category (not at all well, not very well, pretty well, very well) questions about how well a youth could do the following tasks without help: tell time on an analog clock, read and understand common signs, count change, look up telephone numbers and use a telephone, get to places outside the home, use local public transportation, buy their own clothes, arrange plane or train trip out of town (Cronbach’s alpha = .92 in the ASD group). Indicators of functional skill were not measured in wave 5 so wave 4 responses, collected from 2006–2007, were used instead.

The number of years since each young adult left high school (range 0–8 years in the ASD group) was collapsed into 4 categories (<1 – 2, 2 – 4, 4 – 6, 6 – 8). A composite variable indicating each young adult’s current postsecondary educational enrollment status was created from 3 dichotomous survey questions (Is young adult currently attending any of the following: vocational/technical school, 2-year, or 4-year college?). Other current status variables were whether young adult was currently employed, where young adult resided (with parent, alone, or under supervision) and whether they utilized adult day services.

Data Analysis

Rates of missing data per variable ranged from 0% to 21%: household income (21%), functional skills scale (20%), years since high school (16%), social participation measures (14–15%), and all remaining variables (< 8%). We imputed missing data using sequential regression in IVEware (2002; Version 0.1) to create 50 implicates with no missing values (Raghunathan, Lepkowski, Van Hoewyk, & Solenberger, 2001)

We accounted for the complex sampling and the multiple imputations using the “mi svy” procedures available in Stata v 12, which uses standard methods for pooling estimates in the analysis of multiply imputed data (Rubin, 1987). Univariate point estimates and 95% confidence intervals were computed for describing the covariates across 4 groups based on special education designation in high school (see Table 1). Unadjusted rates of social participation were compared across groups (see Table 2). We tested for the significance of differences between the young adults with an ASD compared with each other disability group using logistic regression with dummy coding. In Table 3, we reported odds ratios comparing rates of social participation in the other groups versus the ASD group, adjusting for covariates. Finally, we used logistic regression to estimate the adjusted association between correlates and the 4 indicators of limited social participation among the young adults with an ASD (see Table 4).

Table 1.

Percentage and 95% Confidence Intervals of Population Characteristics Across Groups

Covariates ASD ID ED LD
Sex
  Male 85.0
[79.6, 89.2]
54.7
[48.8, 60.4]
72.3
[65.2, 78.4]
64.5
[58.1, 70.5]
  Female 15.0
[10.8, 20.4]
45.3
[39.6, 51.2]
27.7
[21.6, 34.8]
35.5
[29.5, 41.9]
Age
  21 7.1
[4.9, 10.0]
6.3
[4.0, 9.8]
9.2
[5.6, 14.7]
10.5
[7.3, 15.0]
  22 24.2
[19.6, 29.4]
20.8
[16.3, 26.1]
23.3
[18.1, 29.6]
27.3
[22.2, 33.2]
  23 24.1
[19.6, 29.1]
24.5
[19.5, 30.3]
24.5
[18.3, 32.0]
20.7
[16.3, 25.8]
  24 25.6
[20.0, 32.1]
29.5
[24.2, 35.5]
23.2
[17.7, 29.8]
21.2
[16.6, 26.6]
  25 19.2
[13.6, 26.3]
18.9
[14.2, 24.8]
19.7
[14.6, 26.1]
20.3
[15.5, 26.1]
Hispanic
  No 90.0
[83.7, 94.1]
88.6
[83.4, 92.4]
85.8
[79.6, 90.4]
77.2
[69.0, 83.8]
  Yes 10.0
[5.9, 16.3]
11.4
[7.6, 16.6]
14.2
[9.6, 20.4]
22.8
[16.2, 31.0]
Race
  White 70.0
[63.0, 76.2]
57.2
[49.2, 64.9]
60.1
[48.2, 70.9]
66.8
[56.6, 75.6]
  African-American 18.7
[13.5, 25.3]
31.7
[25.1, 39.0]
23.3
[14.3, 35.6]
15.7
[10.2, 23.2]
  Mixed/other 11.3
[7.9, 16.0]
11.1
[7.4, 16.2]
16.6
[11.9, 22.7]
17.6
[12.4, 24.3]
Parent, guardian household
income
  ≤$25,000 16.5
[11.3, 23.3]
46.4
[39.5, 53.4]
33.0
[25.9, 40.9]
32.5
[26.2, 39.5]
  $25,001–50000 31.5
[25.0, 38.8]
28.4
[22.4, 35.3]
31.9
[25.2, 39.4]
28.4
[22.1, 35.6]
  $50,001-$75,000 34.1
[27.3, 41.5]
21.5
[16.6, 27.3]
27.1
[21.0, 34.1]
31.5
[25.0, 38.8]
  Over $75,000 17.9
[12.9, 24.4]
3.7
[2.2, 6.2]
7.9
[4.7, 13.2]
7.6
[4.8, 11.8]
Years since high school
  < 1–2 18.3
[12.1, 26.6]
10.3
[6.8, 15.2]
7.3
[4.0, 12.5]
5.4
[2.5, 10.7]
  2–4 33.0
[26.7, 40.0]
23.3
[18.3, 29.2]
16.8
[12.7, 21.8]
21.6
[16.6, 27.5]
  4–6 35.3
[28.2, 43.0]
42.3
[36.4, 48.5]
46.8
[40.3, 53.3]
38.6
[32.6, 44.9]
  6–8 13.4
[8.2, 20.9]
24.1
[19.1, 29.9]
29.2
[23.3, 35.9]
34.5
[28.9, 40.5]
Overall health
Excellent 27.9
[21.6, 35.1]
20.9
[16.0, 26.7]
23.9
[18.3, 30.7]
25.4
[20.4, 31.2]
  Very good 37.3
[30.5, 44.6]
29.6
[24.3, 35.5]
25.0
[20.0, 30.7]
30.8
[24.5, 37.8]
  Good 26.5
[20.2, 33.9]
31.0
[25.5, 37.0]
24.9
[19.5, 31.1]
30.8
[25.2, 37.0]
  Fair/poor 8.3
[5.5, 12.5]
18.6
[14.5, 23.5]
26.2
[20.2, 33.2]
13.0
[8.3, 19.6]
How well youth converses
  No trouble 12.0
[8.5, 16.6]
45.3
[38.1, 52.6]
67.2
[59.2, 74.4]
70.4
[62.8, 77.1]
  Little trouble 41.3
[33.7, 49.4]
32.7
[27.2, 38.7]
24.6
[19.4, 30.8]
21.9
[16.7, 28.1]
  Lot of trouble 29.9
[24.3, 36.1]
14.7
[10.6, 20.1]
4.9
[2.1, 10.4]
4.8
[1.8, 11.2]
  Not at all 16.9
[11.7, 23.6]
7.3
[3.6, 13.5]
! 2.9
[0.5, 11.0]
Functional skills scale
  High ability (28–32) 19.8
[13.6, 27.9]
24.0
[17.9, 31.4]
44.8
[38.4, 51.4]
52.3
[45.3, 59.1]
  3 (20–27) 33.8
[26.1, 42.3]
45.9
[37.5, 54.6]
42.6
[29.9, 56.2]
32.3
[22.0, 44.3]
  2 (12–19) 29.5
[23.2, 36.6]
21.3
[14.6, 29.6]
12.6 15.4
  Lowest ability (8–11) 17.0
[12.1, 23.1]
8.7
[5.4, 13.7]
[3.0, 33.7] [6.2, 31.0]
Currently attending
postsecondary school
  No 87.6
[82.2, 91.6]
97.6
[95.7, 98.7]
87.2
[81.9, 91.1]
82.7
[77.4, 86.9]
  Yes 12.4
[8.5, 17.8]
2.4
[1.3, 4.3]
12.8
[8.9, 18.1]
17.3
[13.1, 22.6]
Currently has a paid job
  No 66.5
[57.9, 74.1]
61.7
[55.4, 67.7]
52.2
[45.6, 58.8]
33.0
[26.1, 40.7]
  Yes 33.5
[25.9, 42.1]
38.3
[32.3, 44.6]
47.8
[41.2, 54.4]
67.0
[59.3, 73.9]
Current residence
  With a parent/guardian 82.0
[76.8, 86.1]
70.3
[63.7, 76.1]
46.6
[39.9, 53.5]
48.4
[41.3, 55.5]
  Alone/with a roommate 7.8
[4.9, 12.3]
25.6
[20.5, 31.5]
46.9
[39.7, 54.4]
50.6
[43.1, 58.0]
  Under supervision 10.2
[7.2, 14.3]
4.1
[2.1, 7.9]
6.4
[3.2, 12.5]
1.1
[0.4, 2.9]
Currently attending adult
day program
  No 87.5
[82.5, 91.3]
94.3
[90.3, 96.7]
99.5
[97.3, 99.9]
100
  Yes 12.5
[8.7, 17.5]
5.7
[3.3, 9.7]
! 0
!

Point estimate not reported because of low cell count for this category.

Point estimates for these categories were too low to report individually and were combined.

Note. Source: National Longitudinal Transition Study 2, Wave 5.

Number of multiply imputed data sets = 50. Weighted to population levels. Variances adjusted for sampling method.

Table 2.

Percentage and 95% Confidence Intervals of Social Participation During the Prior 12 Months

ASD ID ED LD
How often sees friends
  Never 38.6
[31.1, 46.7]
19.5***
[15.4, 24.4]
15.9***
[10.8, 22.7]
7.1***
[4.2, 11.6]
  Sometimes, not weekly 16.5
[11.6, 22.9]
20.0
[14.8, 26.3]
8.6*
[5.1, 14.2]
10.7
[6.9, 16.0]
  About once weekly 16.0
[11.4, 22.1]
12.6
[8.8, 17.8]
13.7
[9.4, 19.4]
14.4
[10.0, 20.3]
  > once weekly 28.8
[21.5, 37.2]
47.9**
[40.9, 55.0]
61.8***
[52.9, 69.9]
67.8***
[60.6, 74.2]
How often friends call
  Never 47.2
[38.6, 55.9]
16.5***
[12.1, 22.1]
7.2***
[4.0, 12.5]
4.4***
[2.2, 8.2]
  Less than monthly 19.5
[14.2, 26.0]
17.1
[12.5, 22.9]
12.0
[7.5, 18.4]
5.5***
[2.9, 9.7]
  About once weekly or a few times per
month
15.3
[10.5, 21.7]
25.0*
[20.2, 30.6]
23.6
[17.2, 31.4]
26.3*
[20.4, 33.1]
  > once weekly 18.0
[12.1, 25.9]
41.4***
[35.3, 47.7]
57.2***
[49.1, 65.0]
63.8***
[56.2, 70.9]
Never invited to activities 48.1
[40.4, 56.0]
37.0*
[30.7, 43.7]
22.9***
[16.6, 30.5]
10.4***
[6.5, 16.0]
Socially isolated
[never sees friends, friends never call,
and never invited to activities]
28.1
[21.7, 35.5]
8.9***
[5.9, 13.1]
2.7***
[1.2, 5.7]
2.0***
[0.8, 4.8]
*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001

Note. Source: National Longitudinal Transition Study 2, Wave 5.

Number of multiply imputed data sets = 50. Weighted to population levels. Variances adjusted for sampling method.

Table 3.

Odds Ratios and 95% Confidence Intervals of Social Participation Among Young Adults Compared to Those with Autism, Controlling for Covariates1

ID ED LD
Never sees friends 0.4**
[0.2,0.7]
0.5
[0.2,1.0]
0.2***
[0.1,0.5]
Friends never call 0.3**
[0.1,0.6]
0.2**
[0.1,0.5]
0.2***
[0.1,0.4]
Never invited to
activities
0.8
[0.5,1.5]
0.6
[0.3,1.3]
0.3**
[0.1,0.8]
Socially isolated 0.3*
[0.1,0.8]
0.2*
[0.1,0.7]
0.2**
[0.1,0.6]
*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001

Note. Source: National Longitudinal Transition Study 2, Wave 5.

Number of multiply imputed data sets = 50. Weighted to population levels. Variances adjusted for sampling method.

1

Covariates: gender, age, race, ethnicity, parent household income, years since leaving high school, overall health, how well youth converses, functional skills, currently attending postsecondary school, currently has a paid job, current residence, currently attending adult daycare.

Table 4.

Logistic Regression Odds Ratios and 95% Confidence Intervals of Social Participation Outcomes Among Youth with ASD

Covariate Never sees
friends
Friends never
call
Never invited
to activities
Socially
isolated
Sex
  Male (reference) (reference) (reference) (reference)
  Female 0.7
[0.3,1.9]
0.6
[0.2,1.6]
0.6
[0.2,1.6]
0.6
[0.3,1.6]
Age 0.9
[0.7,1.2]
1
[0.7,1.3]
1
[0.8,1.3]
0.9
[0.7,1.2]
Hispanic 1.2
[0.5,3.1]
0.8
[0.4,2.0]
1.6
[0.6,4.3]
1.4
[0.6,3.4]
Race
  White (reference) (reference) (reference) (reference)
  African-American 0.7
[0.3,1.8]
0.6
[0.2,1.7]
0.5
[0.2,1.4]
0.5
[0.2,1.4]
  Mixed/other 1.3
[0.5,3.0]
0.8
[0.3,1.9]
1
[0.4,2.4]
1
[0.4,2.4]
Parent, guardian
household income
0.9
[0.8,1.1]
0.9
[0.8,1.0]
0.9
[0.8,1.0]
0.9
[0.8,1.0]
Years since high school 0.9
[0.7,1.2]
1
[0.8,1.2]
0.9
[0.7,1.2]
1
[0.7,1.3]
Overall health 1
[0.7,1.4]
0.8
[0.6,1.2]
0.9
[0.6,1.2]
0.9
[0.6,1.3]
How well youth
converses
0.7
[0.5,1.1]
0.5***
[0.3,0.7]
0.8
[0.5,1.2]
0.6*
[0.4,0.9]
Functional skills scale 0.8
[0.5,1.2]
0.5**
[0.3,0.9]
0.7
[0.4,1.0]
0.8
[0.5,1.2]
Currently attending
postsecondary school
0.7
[0.3,1.6]
0.4
[0.2,1.2]
0.7
[0.3,1.6]
0.8
[0.3,2.2]
Currently has a paid job 0.6
[0.3,1.2]
0.5
[0.3,1.1]
0.5*
[0.2,1.0]
0.6
[0.3,1.2]
Current residence
  With a parent/guardian (reference) (reference) (reference) (reference)
  Alone/with a
  roommate
0.2*
[0.1,1.0]
0.8
[0.2,4.0]
0.7
[0.2,2.5]
0.3
[0.0,2.1]
  Under supervision 1.2
[0.6,2.7]
1.2
[0.5,2.7]
1.5
[0.7,3.3]
1.7
[0.7,3.9]
Currently attending adult
day program
1
[0.5,2.1]
0.7
[0.3,1.7]
0.6
[0.3,1.3]
0.9
[0.4,2.0]
*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001

Note. Source: National Longitudinal Transition Study 2, Wave 5.

Number of multiply imputed data sets = 50. Weighted to population levels. Variances adjusted for sampling method.

Results

Compared to young adults with other types of disabilities (see Table 1), those with an ASD were more likely to be male, more concentrated in the highest income category, had a higher rate of significant conversational impairment, and were more likely to live with a parent/guardian or under supervision. The rate of Hispanic ethnicity was similar for ASD and ID but lower than ED and LD.

Young adults with an ASD were significantly more likely to never see friends (38.6%), never get called by friends (47.2%), and never be invited to activities (48.1%) compared to other groups (see Table 2). The rate of being socially isolated (no participation in any of the three activities) in the ASD group (28.1%) ranged from 3 to 14 times higher than other groups. After controlling for other covariates (see Table 3), adjusted odds ratios predicting social participation outcomes revealed that young adults with an ASD had significantly higher odds of friends never calling and of being socially isolated compared to all three disability comparison groups. Additionally, young adults with ASD had higher adjusted odds of never seeing friends compared to young adults in the ID and LD groups. Finally, young adults with ASD had higher adjusted odds of never being invited to activities compared to young adults from the LD group. Focusing on the young adults with ASD (see Table 4) those with more limited conversation ability had significantly higher odds of friends never calling and of being socially isolated. Young adults with lower functional skills also had significantly higher odds of friends never calling. Those who were not living independently had higher odds of never seeing friends.

Discussion

This study examined the social participation outcomes from a nationally representative U.S. sample of young adults who had received special education services for a diagnosis of autism. Our findings illustrate the uniquely high levels of social isolation experienced by those with an ASD. The young adults with an ASD in our study experienced significantly more social isolation than young adults who had received special education services under the categories of intellectual disability, emotional disturbance, or learning disability. Almost 40% of youth with an ASD never got together with friends. This was twice the rate observed for young adults with ID. Similarly, almost 50% of young adults with an ASD never received phone calls from friends and were never invited to activities by friends. In contrast, only 17% of young adults with ID never received phone calls from friends, and 37% were never invited to activities. Overall, almost one-third of young adults with an ASD were socially isolated, with no contact with friends, no phone calls, and no invitations to activities. Less than 10% of young adults with ID were socially isolated, and only 2–3% of young adults with ED or LD were socially isolated.

It was not surprising to see that the unadjusted rates of social isolation were higher among young adults with an ASD compared to the other disability groups, given that difficulty with social interaction is a defining feature of ASDs. However, the odds of social isolation were still much higher for those with an ASD compared to those in other groups even after adjusting for a wide range of correlates including demographics, family income, conversation ability, functional skills, and involvement in contemporaneous activities like work and school. Our previous work found that the adjusted odds of involvement in postsecondary education and employment were also lower among those with an ASD (Shattuck et al., 2012). Individual attributes and level of family resources do not seem to account for all the between-group variability observed.

Information gained from the current study on the prevalence of social isolation in young adulthood adds to a growing body of research documenting consistent social participation impairments from adolescence through adulthood. Similar to the findings from a previous publication on adolescents from this data set (Shattuck, Orsmond, Wagner, & Cooper, 2011) the majority of young adults with an ASD do not have regular contact with friends. Based on this cross-sectional analysis, social participation does not appear to change drastically from the high school years to the early post-high school years, despite the contextual changes that come with transition to adulthood (such as decreased opportunity to socialize with peers from school, and new opportunities to socialize with peers from college or work). Our findings are also consistent with prior research on adult outcomes in autism, based on smaller and less diverse samples. Other researchers have also found that approximately 40% of adults with autism do not experience reciprocal friendships (Kobayashi & Murata, 1998; Howlin et al., 2000). Future longitudinal research is needed to determine whether the same individuals who struggle with friendship and social participation in adolescence are the same who lack friendships and are socially isolated in adulthood.

Among the factors measured that might account for variability in social isolation among young adults with an ASD, we observed very few significant correlates. Not surprisingly, young adults with significant communication and functional impairments were most likely to never receive phone calls from friends. Young adults who lived independently were more likely to get together with friends compared to those who lived with their parents. These findings are similar to, yet slightly different from what was observed for this same population in 2001 using Wave 1 of NLTS2 when these young adults were in their teens (Shattuck et al., 2011). Similar to the previous study, we found that impaired conversation ability and functional cognitive skills were associated with increased odds of social isolation for the “never sees friends” and “never invited to activities” indicators.

This study had some limitations. We did not have measures of the size or composition of friendship and acquaintance networks. Nor did we have information about the participants’ level of satisfaction with their social participation. The adequacy of outcomes in adulthood should ideally take into account the gap between a given outcome and the preferences and goals of the individual (Henninger & Taylor, 2012). We also did not have community-level data on the availability of services and activities, another unmeasured factor that may have contributed to variability in outcomes. The lack of information on services may limit external validity of the findings. Counterbalancing these limitations were many strengths. The large and population-representative nature of the sample increases the generalizability of findings relative to a smaller clinical or community-based sample. Our ability to make between-group comparisons increased our ability to contextualize and interpret the meaning of findings.

In summary, our findings add to the growing body of literature documenting the persistent social participation impairments in adults with an ASD. In our analysis, we focused on the specific life stage of young adulthood, primarily the early years of transition from school-based to adult services. The high rates of social isolation in young adults with an ASD are concerning. Our findings have important implications for quality of life and service delivery in this growing population. Young adulthood is a high-risk developmental period for the onset of mental health conditions, particularly depression and anxiety, in this population (Farley et al., 2009; Hofvander et al., 2009). Moreover, research with children with an ASD has documented an association between lower quality social relationships and heightened levels of anxiety (Eussen et al., 2012; Mazurek & Kanne, 2010).

Creating opportunities for social engagement in this population during adulthood may improve quality of life as well as lessen the impact of comorbid conditions or exacerbated autism symptoms. Supporting social participation in adulthood for individuals with an ASD will require continued individual intervention to refine social and communication skills, as well as resources directed towards families and service providers who provide social opportunities. Addressing social isolation should be a priority for service providers who have contact with this population: vocational service providers, day program providers, and those who have access to creating opportunities for community recreation. Overall, these findings call for continued efforts to develop and evaluate interventions, supports, and community programs to enable young adults on the autism spectrum to participate in social activities with peers and in the community.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by funding to Dr. Shattuck from the Organization for Autism Research, Autism Speaks, the Emch Foundation, and the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH086489). Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health or other funders.

References

  1. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. The Friendship Questionnaire: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2003;33:509–517. doi: 10.1023/a:1025879411971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bertrand J, Mars A, Boyle C, Bove F, Yeargin-Allsopp M, Decoufle P. Prevalence of autism in a United States population: the Brick Township, New Jersey, investigation. Pediatrics. 2001;108:1155–1161. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.5.1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Billstedt E, Gillberg IC, Gillberg C. Autism in adults: Symptom patterns and early childhood predictors. The use of the DISCO in a community sample followed from childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:1102–1110. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01774.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brugha TS, McManus S, Bankart J, Scott F, Purdon S, Smith J, Meltzer H. Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders in adults in the community in England. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2011;68:459–466. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Eaves LC, Ho HH. Young adult outcome of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38:739–747. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0441-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Eussen MLJM, Van Gool AR, Verheij F, De Nijs PFA, Verhulst FC, Greaves-Lord K. The association of quality of social relations, symptom severity and intelligence with anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. 2012 doi: 10.1177/1362361312453882. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Farley MA, McMahon WM, Fombonne E, Jenson WR, Miller J, Gardner M, Coon H. Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and average or near-average cognitive abilities. Autism Research. 2009;2:109–118. doi: 10.1002/aur.69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Henninger NA, Taylor JL. Outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: a historical perspective. Autism. 2013;17:103–116. doi: 10.1177/1362361312441266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Hofvander B, Delorme R, Chaste P, Nydén A, Wentz E, Ståhlberg O, Leboyer M. Psychiatric and psychosocial problems in adults with normal-intelligence autism spectrum disorders. BMC Psychiatry. 2009;9 doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-9-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Howlin P. Outcome in high-functioning adults with autism with and without early language delays: Implications for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2003;33:3–13. doi: 10.1023/a:1022270118899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M. Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:212–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Howlin P, Mawhood L, Rutter M. Autism and developmental receptive language disorder--A follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: Social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41:561–578. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee. IACC Strategic Plan for Autism Spectrum Disorder Research – 2012 Update. 2012 Retrieved from the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee website: http://iacc.hhs.gov/strategic-plan/2012/index.shtml.
  14. IVEware. Imputation and variance estimation software [computer program] Version 1.0. Ann Arbor: Survey Methodology Program, Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  15. Kobayashi R, Murata T. Behavioral characteristics of 187 young adults with autism. Psychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences. 1998;52:383–390. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.1998.00415.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Liptak GS, Kennedy JA, Dosa NP. Social participation in a nationally representative sample of older youth and young adults with autism. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2011;32:277–283. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31820b49fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Mazurek MO, Kanne SM. Friendship and internalizing symptoms among children and adolescents with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40:1512–1520. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1014-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Orsmond GI, Krauss MW, Seltzer MM. Peer relationships and social and recreational activities among adolescents and adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2004;34:245–256. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000029547.96610.df. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Raghunathan TE, Lepkowski JM, Van Hoewyk J, Solenberger PW. A multivariate technique for multiply imputing missing values using a sequence of regression models. Survey Methodology. 2001;27:85–95. [Google Scholar]
  20. Rubin DB. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  21. Seltzer MM, Greenberg JS, Taylor JL, Smith L, Orsmond GI, Esbensen A, Hong J. Adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorders. In: Amaral DG, Dawson G, Geschwind DH, editors. Autism spectrum disorders. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 241–252. [Google Scholar]
  22. Shattuck PT, Seltzer MM, Greenberg JS, Orsmond GI, Bolt D, Kring S, Lord C. Change in autism symptoms and maladaptive behaviors in adolescents and adults with an autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(9):1735–1747. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0307-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Shattuck PT, Narendorf SC, Cooper BP, Sterzing P, Wagner M, Taylor JL. Postsecondary education and employment among youth with an autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics. 2012;129:1042–1049. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Shattuck PT, Orsmond GI, Wagner M, Cooper BP. Participation in social activities among adolescents with an autism spectrum disorder. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(11):e27176. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. SRI International. National Longitudinal Transition Study II (NLTS2) Sampling Plan. Menlo Park, CA: Author; 2000. Feb 25, [Google Scholar]
  26. Verdugo MA, Navas P, Gómez LE, Schalock RL. The concept of quality of life and its role in enhancing human rights in the field of intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2012;56(11):1036–1045. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2012.01585.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Wagner M, Kutash K, Duchnowski AJ, Epstein MH. The Special Education Elementary Longitudinal Study and the National Longitudinal Transition Study: Study designs and implications for children and youth with emotional disturbance. Journal of Emotional & Behavioral Disorders. 2005;13:25–41. [Google Scholar]
  28. Whitehouse AJO, Watt HJ, Line EA, Bishop DVM. Adult psychosocial outcomes of children with specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 2009;44(4):511–528. doi: 10.1080/13682820802708098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. World Health Organization. International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  30. Yeargin-Allsopp M, Rice C, Karapurkar T, Doernberg N, Boyle C, Murphy C. Prevalence of autism in a U.S. metropolitan area. The Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;289:49–55. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES