Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Oct 21.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Topogr. 2012 Nov 5;26(3):458–467. doi: 10.1007/s10548-012-0264-8

The effect of individual differences in working memory capacity on sentence comprehension: an fMRI study

Sharlene D Newman 1, Evie Malaia 2, Roy Seo 1, Hu Cheng 1
PMCID: PMC3802527  NIHMSID: NIHMS519252  PMID: 23124385

Abstract

This study explores the interaction between working memory systems - episodic and verbal working memory - and language processing by examining how differences in working memory capacity (WMC) modulated neural activation levels and functional connectivity during sentence comprehension. Activation of the inferior frontal gyrus and medial prefrontal cortex was correlated with WMC during the probe phase, suggesting that not all of the activation observed in the left IFG is language-specific, but is instead related to executive processes recruited during comprehension. Posterior cingulate/precuneus activation was negatively correlated with WMC scores during sentence reading. In addition, functional connectivity between the PCC and IFG was positively correlated with WMC while that between the PCC and the inferior parietal cortex was negatively correlated with WMC. Together these results suggest that high-capacity and low capacity readers may utilize different processing strategies with high capacity readers utilizing episodic memory systems during sentence reading to generate a representation of the events (event representation). Both groups use executive working memory processes during the probe phase.

1.0 Introduction

One of the long running debates in the psycholinguistic literature relates to the relationship between language and working memory (WM, Fedorenko et al., 2006; Caplan and Waters, 1999; King and Just, 1991; Gordon et al., 2002; MacDonald & Christiansen, 2002). There are numerous studies that show a link between working memory capacity (WMC) and language processing, particularly sentence comprehension (Caramazza et al., 1981; Friedrich et al., 1984; King & Just, 1991; Lauro et al., 2010; Papagno et al., 2007). For example, in a meta-analysis Daneman and Merikle’s (1996) found that individual differences in WMC significantly predicted reading comprehension, but only for complex span tasks like the reading span and not for storage-only span tasks like forward digit span. Additionally, operation span tasks with numerical stimuli also predict comprehension, not just verbal span tasks. This suggests that the verbal-processing component in these WM measures does not fully account for the working memory-comprehension association (Engle, Cantor, & Carullo, 1992; Kane et al., 2004). The current study attempts to further explore this interaction between working memory and comprehension by examining the involvement of WM systems other than the verbal WM system during sentence comprehension.

Working memory (WM) is considered to be a multi-domain workspace composed of various storage and processing modules (Baddeley et al., 2010; Logie, 2011). There are at least three domain specific modules: memory for verbal sequences; visuo-spatial/motor sequences; and episodic, integrated multidimensional representations. Much of the previous research examining the relationship between language and working memory has focused on its relationship with the verbal WM system. However, it may very well be that language interacts with the visuo-spatial and episodic buffers as well, likely via mental imagery.

This study proposes that the use of event representations in the service of sentence comprehension may vary as a function of WMC. Language is thought to activate internal representations of previously experienced events, or schematic abstractions (Lakoff, 1987; Langacker, 1987; Talmy, 2000). The activation of these internal representations may then used to create an event representation or situation model of the sentence which aids in comprehension. Bergen and colleagues (2007) have shown that mental imagery is triggered by both subject nouns and verbs in the service of sentence comprehension. Also, McRae and colleagues (2005) argue that accessing a verb activates information regarding the event it encodes including the entities that typically participate in the event. Both studies suggest that event information, or complex integrated multidimensional representations may be triggered via language. We argue here that the construction of this event representation requires working memory resources and therefore, may vary as a function of WMC.

It may also be that these different WM modules are more or less involved in comprehension at different points during the task. The typical sentence comprehension task presents a sentence (what we refer to as the online phase) followed by a probe to test comprehension (off-line phase). The online sentence processing is viewed as the interpretive processes of “recognizing words and appreciating their meanings and syntactic features; constructing syntactic and prosodic representations; and assigning thematic roles, and other aspects of propositional and discourse-level semantics” (Caplan and Waters 1999, p.78). On the other hand, off-line processing is thought of as the post-interpretive processes associated with the post-hoc usage of extracted meanings to accomplish the task (e.g., responding to a comprehension probe; Caplan and Waters 1999). As such it is predicted that during sentence reading episodic memory systems are more involved in the activation of event knowledge and the integration of information into an event representation. Conversely, during the comprehension probe executive control processes required to extract the appropriate information from the working memory representation generated during sentence processing are more involved.

Previous studies have used neuroimaging to explore the impact of individual differences in WM on language related activation (Fiebach et al., 2005; Prat et al., 2007). Generally, these studies have found that high capacity readers outperform low WMC readers in that they have faster reading and response times and are more accurate. High capacity readers also elicit less activation. For example, in a recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study Prat and colleagues (2007) explored the impact of WMC differences on neural network properties outlined by Newman and Just (2005) – efficient use of neural resources, adaptability to changing demands, and the coordination or synchronization of nodes within cortical networks. It was found that high capacity readers demonstrated greater neural efficiency in that they elicited less activation in conjunction with better behavioral performance. They also showed better synchronization of the processing nodes within the language network as revealed by increased functional connectivity. This study reveals that working memory has a significant impact on not only behavior but also the neural network that supports language. However, their analysis was limited to regions that have been previously identified as part of the language network rather than performing a whole brain analysis. Therefore, it was not possible to explore how other neuro-cognitive networks (e.g., working memory networks) may be differentially interacting with the language network.

The primary goal of the current study was to explore the impact WMC has on sentence comprehension. To accomplish this goal we examined the interaction between WMC and activation during both sentence reading and when responding to a comprehension probe. In this way the differential impact of WM on these two processing phases could be determined. Additionally, functional connectivity analysis was performed to explore how WMC impacted the connectivity between language-related and working memory related processing regions.

2.0 Methods

2.1. Participants

Fifty participants took part in this study. They were all Indiana University students without any history of neurological disorders (28 female, age = 22.5±3.1). All participants gave signed informed consent which was approved by the Indiana University Institutional Review Board.

2.2 Experimental procedure

This study combines data from three separate studies with overlapping stimuli (Lee & Newman, 2010; Newman et al., 2009, 2010); the non-overlapping stimuli were treated as fillers of no interest. Each study was composed of three sessions, training, imaging and debriefing sessions. During the training session, all participants were administered the Edinburgh handedness inventory and the Daneman and Carpenter (1980) Reading Span Test to obtain a measure of working memory capacity. Participants were all right-handed and their reading span scores ranged from 2 to 5 (Mean = 3.4 ± 0.98). During the training session, participants were also introduced to the sentence comprehension experiment that they would perform and completed 16 practice trials to familiarize them with the experimental procedure. Immediately after the fMRI scan, participants completed a debriefing questionnaire. The questionnaire was designed to determine how the participant performed the task as well as assess their perception of difficulty.

The fMRI experiments used a single trial event-related design in which each trial was treated as an event block (Kruggel and von Cramon 1999; Zarahn et al. 1997; Zarahn, 2000; Fig.1). A trial could be divided into two phases; a sentence reading phase and a responding to a comprehension probe phase. Participants were instructed to read each sentence thoroughly and respond as quickly and accurately as possible to probes that were presented 6 secs later. Participants were told to place a greater weight on accuracy than speed of responding. Sentence materials were taken from Keller et al. (2001) which were derived originally from Just et al. (1996). The object-relative sentences were syntactically more complex (Caplan and Waters, 1999; Just et al. 1996; Keller et al. 2001; Prat et al. 2007). Stimuli were equated across conditions for frequency, word length, sentence length and animacy.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Experimental Design

Probes were constructed by asking if one of the nouns performed the act denoted by one of the two verbs. Thirty-three percent of the probes were false. Example stimuli include:

Conjoined active: The pilot scared the escort and broke the mirror on the closet.

The pilot scared the escort.

Object-relative: The pilot that the escort scared broke the mirror on the closet.

The pilot broke the mirror.

The duration of each trial was 16 sec. A trial began with a sentence being presented in the middle of the screen for 5 secs. After 5 sec, an X was presented on the screen for 6 sec during a delay to allow the hemodynamic response to approach baseline. By inserting the 6 sec delay the online sentence reading phase and off-line comprehension phase could be distinguished. Finally, a comprehension probe was presented for 5 sec with a cue (i.e. F|T). The cue indicated the appropriate response (a right index finger for true and the left index finger for false). After each trial, a 12 sec ITI was added to allow the hemodynamic response to return to baseline.

Each experiment was composed of 4 runs. Each run contained 3, 28 sec fixation periods located at the beginning, middle and end of each run. The baseline hemodynamic response was measured by averaging the signal during the 28 sec fixation periods (fixation to a star sign, *). Stimuli were presented on the screen located behind the scanner and viewed by participants via a mirror attached on the head coil. Fiber optic button boxes in each hand were used to record behavioral responses. Incorrect responses were removed from the response time analysis and the fMRI data analysis.

2.3 fMRI acquisition and analysis

Functional MRI was conducted on a 3T Siemens TRIO scanner with an 8-channel radio frequency head coil located in the Imaging Research Facility at Indiana University. Functional images were obtained in eighteen oblique axial slices with 5mm thickness and a 1mm gap (TR = 1000 msec, TE = 25 msec, flip angle = 60°, matrix size = 64×64, FOV = 240×240 mm2) by a gradient echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence. Before statistical analysis, for all functional images, conventional preprocessing procedures such as slice timing correction, head motion correction by realignment and spatial normalization were conducted using SPM8 software (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). In the spatial normalization step, all functional images were warped to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) EPI template and resampled to 2×2×2 mm3 voxels. A conventional statistical inference was performed on the normalized functional images from each individual by using the general linear model and Gaussian random field theory (Friston et al. 1995). A canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) with trial onsets and durations for each phase (including the delay period) was used to generate a statistical parametric map.

Multiple regression analysis was performed using SPM8 for the sentence and probe phases and object-relative and conjoined active conditions separately as well as for the object-relative minus conjoined active contrast. Reading span was entered as a covariate. This allowed for the examination of regions whose activation was correlated with reading span as well as the exploration of the activation for a given contrast when reading span was co-varied out. In addition, a standard one sample t-test was performed without entering reading span as a covariate. A threshold of p<0.001, uncorrected was used. The extent threshold of 92 voxels was determined using Monte Carlo simulations via AlphaSim (a part of the Afni software package).

2.3.1 Functional connectivity analysis.

The functional connectivity between the regions found to be impacted by working memory capacity during sentence comprehension was analyzed. The regions included the two regions within the left IFG (MNI: −42, 20, 30, BA44/45 and −28, 42, 6, BA45/46), the posterior temporal cortex (−44, −44, −2), the inferior parietal cortex (−36, −52, 48) and the posterior cingulate (−8, −44, 6). These coordinates reflect the centroids of significant activation from the fMRI syntactic complexity contrasts or the correlation analyses with individual reading span scores carried out in SPM8. The ROIs were chosen to be spheres with a 5 mm radius. The center of the sphere was the peak activation voxel. Five participants were excluded from this analysis due not having a complete dataset (one run was removed due to excess motion).

An analysis similar to psychophysiological interactions analysis (PPI) was performed. Instead of examining whether the correlation in activity between two brain areas is different in different psychological contexts (which is what PPI does) we explored whether that activity is different across individuals with different WM capacities. For each participant, the voxel timecourses in the ROIs were regressed against the time series for the motion correction parameters and global signal of the whole brain. Partial correlation analysis (regressing out time series from the other ROIs) was performed on each pair of regions using mean signal intensity1. Z-scores were then computed from the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients for each ROI pair for each participant using Fisher r-to-Z transformation. The correlation between individual reading span scores and pairwise regional connectivity was then assessed using linear regression analysis.

3.0 Results

3.1. Behavioral Results

Fifty participants’ fMRI and behavioral data are included in this study. The reaction time (RT) and error rate for object-relative (OR) and conjoined active (CA) sentences were compared using a paired sample t-test. The results revealed that the object relatives elicited more errors (OR: M=15±11%; CA: M=7±8%; t=7.2, p<0.0001) and a slower RT (OR: M=2405±445ms; CA: M=2053±401ms; t=10.8, p<0.0001). RT and error rate were also correlated with working memory capacity. For CA sentences, only RT was correlated with WMC [error: r=0.26; F(1,49)=3.08, p=0.06; RT: r=0.38; F(1,49)=8.07; p<0.01]. For OR sentences both RT and error were significantly correlated with WMC [error: r=0.45; F(1,49)=12.1, p<0.005; RT: r=0.33; F(1,49)=5.95; p<0.05].

3.2. fMRI Sentence Phase

3.2.1. Syntactic Complexity effect

During the sentence phase object-relative sentences elicited increased activation in the caudate body bilaterally (MNI: 6, 16, 14/ −8, 20, 12; z=4.67/4.16; k=189/202) and the left caudate tail (MNI: −24,−38, 10; z=3.82; k=189). The comparison between object-relative and conjoined active sentences was also performed while regressing out working memory capacity. The same regions were found when working memory capacity was regressed out (the caudate nucleus).

Additionally, we explored the correlation between working memory and the activation related to the syntactic complexity effect, no regions were found to be significantly correlated.

3.2.2. Correlation with object-relative sentences

Working memory capacity was also correlated with each sentence type separately. For the object-relative condition, the traditional working memory-related regions were not found to be correlated with working memory capacity as measured by the reading span task. One region did reveal a significant negative correlation, the posterior cingulate/precuneus region (PCC/MNI coordinates: −8, −44, 6; k=108; z=3.55), see Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Depicts the activation within the posterior cingulate cortex that was correlated with WMC during the sentence phase of object-relative: sentence

To confirm this correlation a two-sample t-test was performed by dividing the participants into two groups: a low capacity group (span ≤ 3, n=27) and a high capacity group (span>3, n=23). The low capacity readers revealed greater activation in the same PCC region which extended into the right hemisphere (MNI coordinates: −8, −44, 6; k=976; z=3.93) and the medial prefrontal cortex (MNI coordinates: 2, 62, 32; k=209; z=3.6).

3.2.3. Correlation with conjoined active sentences

No region reached threshold for the conjoined active condition. However, when the cluster threshold was relaxed a similar posterior cingulate/precuneus region as observed for OR sentences was observed for this condition.

3.3. fMRI Probe Phase

3.3.1. Syntactic Complexity effect

Object-relative and conjoined active sentences were compared when working memory capacity was regressed out and when it was not. Significant overlap in the pattern of activation as well as notable differences were observed for these two analyses (see Table 1 and Figure 3). Syntactic complexity effects were observed in the caudate, the middle frontal gyrus that extended inferiorly, medial prefrontal cortex, the precuneus and the inferior parietal cortex. When working memory was regressed out, activation was also observed in middle temporal cortex and the posterior lobe of the cerebellum.

Table 1.

Syntactic complexity effect during the probe

Region BA k z x y z
OR minus CA without regressing WMC

Left Middle Frontal Gyrus 4/6/44 2181 5.71 −42 20 30
Left Inferior Parietal Cortex 40/39/7 933 4.44 −36 −50 50
Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus 45/46 121 4.09 −28 42 6
Left Caudate Body 501 5.35 −12 −6 22
Right Caudate Body 124 4.3 12 −4 20
Caudate Body 151 4.14 −24 −46 12

OR minus CA regressing WMC

Left Middle Frontal Gyrus 9/6/44 2800 5.95 −42 20 30
Left Inferior Parietal Cortex 40/39/7 1018 4.78 −36 −52 48
Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus 45/46 146 4.19 −28 42 6
Left Superior Frontal Gyrus 6 118 3.79 −2 14 68
Left Posterior Temporal Cortex 22/37 413 4.83 −44 −44 −2
Left Caudate Body 551 5.23 −12 −6 22
Left Caudate Tail 256 4.36 −24 −44 12
Right Caudate Body 137 4.24 12 −4 20
Figure 3.

Figure 3

The figure shows the syntactic complexity (object-relative minus conjoined active). The left figure shows the activation when WMC is not regressed out and the right is when it is regressed. The left temporal activation is present only when WMC is controlled.

Additionally, we explored the correlation between working memory and the activation related to the syntactic complexity effect, no regions were found to be significantly correlated.

3.3.2. Correlation with object-relative sentences

When examining each condition separately, the object-relative sentences revealed a significant negative correlation with working memory capacity in the superior IFG (BA 45, MNI coordinates: −50, 26, 26 k=92; z=3.65) see Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

The figure depicts probe phase activation that is correlated with WMC during conjoined active (left, top and bottom) and object-relative sentences (right, top and bottom).

3.3.3. Correlation with conjoined active sentences

For the conjoined active condition, significant negative correlations were observed in the SMA (MNI: −14, 6, 62 k=98, z=3.75); PCC/precuneus (MNI:−16,−52, 12 k=191, z=3.54); putamen (MNI:−26, −2 −20; k=244; z=3.85); and the right caudate tail (MNI: 26, −24, 16; k=199, z=3.68) regions, the left IFG activation failed to pass the extent threshold (IFG/MNI −50,26,22 k=87; z=3.52).

3.4. Functional connectivity

We examined whether functional connectivity was modulated by WMC. After global signal regression using partial correlation based on the mean signal, BA45/44-PCC and IPL-PCC connectivity were found to be significantly modulated by WMC [r=0.36, F(1, 44)=6.407, p<0.015 and r=−0.30, F(1, 44)=4.228, p<0.046, respectively]. In addition, the connectivity between the more anterior IFG region, BA45/46, and the temporal ROI [r=0.305, F(1, 44)=4.419, p<0.041] as well as between the inferior parietal and temporal ROI [r=0.34, F(1, 44)=4.369, p<0.044] were found to be modulated by WMC after global signal regression and full correlation analysis, using ICA and mean signal, respectively.

4.0 Discussion

There is a rich literature that has explored the relationship between working memory and sentence comprehension. The results presented demonstrate that WMC modulates neural activation during both the sentence reading and the probe phases, however, the cortical region impacted varied as a function of processing phase. Activity of a non-traditional language region, the posterior cingulate/precuneus cortex, was found to be correlated with working memory capacity during sentence reading (particularly for the object-relative sentences). Conversely, the inferior frontal gyrus was correlated with WMC during the probe phase. It was also found that functional connectivity was modulated by WMC. Specifically, the connectivity between the PCC and the IFG region mentioned above as well as the temporal cortex and a more anterior region of the IFG were found to be positively correlated with WMC. These results suggest a strong, but intricate relationship between working memory and sentence comprehension.

4.1. Posterior Cingulate

The posterior cingulate/precuneus (PCC) region was found here to be correlated with WMC for both object-relative and conjoined active conditions, although during different processing phases. The region has been found to be involved in a number of memory processes (Burianova et al., 2010) including semantic, episodic and visuo-spatial memory (Aggleton and Pearce 2001; Binder et al., 2009, 2011; Vincent et al. 2006; Epstein et al. 2007). For example, in a review of 120 neuroimaging studies of semantic processing Binder and colleagues (2009) performed an activation likelihood estimation technique and found that the PCC was strongly related to semantic processing along with the lateral parietal, inferior frontal, lateral temporal, and middle frontal regions. The PCC is most interesting here. Yarkoni et al (2008) also found activation of a similar region of the PCC during the comprehension of story narratives. There it was suggested that readers create a situation model that aids in comprehension and retention of the text. A situation model (or event representation) is a mental representation of the situation described in text which includes the readers’ knowledge of events, characters, goals, etc; information pulled from long-term memory stores. We argue that it is akin to the mental imagery during comprehension mentioned in the introduction. The hypothesis that the PCC is involved in the creation of an event representation fits all of the memory functions that have been attributed to the region including semantic, the temporary storage of integrated information related to episodic memor,y and information related to typical events including the characters that participate in those events.

The PCC is also thought to be part of the default-mode network (Raichle et al., 2001) and has often been found to show decreased activation relative to baseline. In fact, Yarkoni (2008) found increased “deactivation” (negative activation) in the PCC related to the development of a situation model/event representation. We found a similar pattern here in that the PCC activation was negatively correlated with reading span such that the activation was more negative the higher the reading span score (see Figure 2). One possible explanation for these results is that high WMC readers are more likely than low capacity readers to generate an event representation during sentence reading because they have the resources available to do so. Because this event representation facilitates sentence comprehension and retention, high capacity readers may be expected to have better behavioral behavioral performance.

4.2. Inferior Frontal Gyrus

One region of the left IFG (BA 45/44 at the inferior frontal sulcus) revealed a correlation with WMC, but only during the probe. While this region has been found to be involved in language processing previously, it has also been linked to working memory processes. For example, Nee and colleagues (2012) performed an activation likelihood estimation analysis of 36 neuroimaging studies of executive working memory processes. They found that a similar region appears to be involved in post-retrieval selection (i.e., selecting from competing information being held in memory). This region’s correlation with WMC during the probe phase, therefore, is likely related to post-retrieval selection and not language specific processes. The correlation observed is such that the higher the WMC the less activation elicited. This fits our hypothesis presented earlier. The high capacity readers developed an event representation to ease comprehension and retention processes; therefore, post-retrieval selection is also eased.

It is important to note that the left IFG is composed of multiple sub-regions that have been linked to different language processes (e.g., Price et al., 2010). For example, BA 44 has been linked to syntactic/hierarchical processing while BA 45/47 has been linked to semantic processing (Friederici et al., 2012; Newman et al., 2009; Price et al., 2010). However, the left IFG has also been linked to a number of general cognitive processes, particularly those linked to executive functions like working memory, interference and inhibitory processes (Bunge et al., 2003; Derrfuss et al., 2004; Wagner & Smith, 2003). What we suggest here is that at least part of the left IFG is not performing language specific processes but is instead involved in cognitive processing that greatly impacts comprehension, particularly for low WMC participants. It is important to note that this region does not overlap with those that have been linked to language (e.g., BA 44 and BA45/47).

The role of episodic memory during comprehension

According to the definition, the episodic buffer is a “limited capacity temporary store that forms an interface between a range of systems all having different basic memory codes” (Baddeley et al., 2010, p.229). Interestingly, Baddeley and colleagues argue that the episodic buffer is a passive store and is not responsible for the binding of multidimensional information, but that the bound chunks, or in this case the bound event representation, is created within another system. We propose here that the PCC/precuneus region is associated with the episodic buffer and that during sentence comprehension participants generate a multidimensional representation of the sentence that is held in the episodic buffer.

The functional connectivity results may support our hypothesis. Two networks were observed. The first involves the MTG, anterior IFG, and IPL - the connectivity between MTG and the anterior IFG and MTG and IPL was also correlated with WMC. This finding indicates that communication between these regions increased with WMC. These are regions that have long been linked to language comprehension processes, semantic processing in particular (e.g., Binder et al., 2009). Therefore, the high capacity readers showed increased communication within a network involved in semantic processing, suggesting that they were better able to use long-term memory stores to generate a more elaborate representation of the meaning of the sentence. We propose that this increased communication within the semantic network may be related to the binding of the event representation that is to be held in the episodic buffer.

The second network involves the PCC, IFG (BA44/45) and IPL - connectivity between the PCC and IFG (BA44/45) and the PCC and inferior parietal cortex was correlated with WMC. Both the IFG and IPL have been linked to WM, but for different reasons. As discussed above, the IFG region described here has been linked to a control mechanism involved when there is competing information being held online (Nee et al., 2012). While the exact role of the IPL in verbal working memory has recently been disputed (Buchsbaum & D’Esposito, 2008) there is overwhelming evidence of IPL involvement in working memory. The finding that the connectivity between the PCC and these two regions that have been linked to WM was modulated by WMC show that individuals with a higher WMC had increased communication between these regions than those with lower WMC. As mentioned above, while the binding may involve the semantic network a more expanded working memory network, including verbal working memory and the episodic buffer, may be involved in keeping this event representation active.

Limitations

An alternative explanation for the PCC/precuneus activation that is related to the default network deserves attention. It is possible that high capacity readers read the sentences much faster than the 5 seconds it is displayed and then engage the default network earlier, thereby inducing the observed correlation. Although this is a possibility, it is unlikely. The average reading time differences we have observed between object-relative and conjoined active sentences is on the order of 500msec (high: 4.3sec, and low: 4.8sec capacity readers). Given the sluggishness of the hemodynamic response it is doubtful that earlier engagement of the default network is responsible for the correlation between WMC and PCC/precuneus activation.

A second limitation concerns the use of a language comprehension task with three processing phases. Because the hemodynamic delay is slow it is necessary to place a delay between the sentence and the probe. However, this delay induced an unnatural working memory load that has an impact on probe processing and may account for some of the differences observed between the object-relative and conjoined active conditions during the probe.

4.4. Conclusions

The results presented here extend those previously reported in that it appears as though two distinct working memory systems are involved during sentence comprehension and that the use of these systems are dependent on working memory capacity. We propose that high capacity readers are able to utilize episodic memory systems to generate a representation of the events depicted in the sentence and that this is extremely helpful in the comprehension of non-canonical sentences like object-relative constructions. The results also suggest that at least some of the activation observed in the left IFG is not language specific activation but is instead related to executive processes that are recruited during comprehension. Further studies are needed to explore the relationship between comprehension and working memory which takes a closer look at the impact of working memory stores other than verbal working memory.

Acknowledgement:

This work was partially supported by National Institute of Health Grant (R03 HD051579-01).

Footnotes

1

To explore possible within-network effects among selected ROIs, the same analysis was also performed using full correlation for either the mean or the first component of ICA in the signals from individual ROIs.

5.0 References

  • 1.Aggleton JP, Pearce JM. Neural systems underlying episodic memory: insights from animal research. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B. 2001;356:1467–1482. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Baddeley A, Allen RJ, Hitch GJ. Investigating the episodic buffer. Psychologica Belgica. 2010;50:223–243. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bergen BK, Lindsay S, Matlock T, Narayanan S. Spatial and Linguistic Aspects of Visual Imagery in Sentence Comprehension. Cognitive Science. 2007;31:733–764. doi: 10.1080/03640210701530748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Binder JR, Desai RH. The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15:527–536. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19:2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Buchsbaum BR, D’Esposito M. The search for the phonological store: from loop to convolution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20:762–778. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bunge SA, Kahn I, Wallis JD, Miller EK, Wagner AD. Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:3419–3428. doi: 10.1152/jn.00910.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Burianova H, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. A common functional brain network for autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memory retrieval. NeuroImage. 2010;49:865–874. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Caplan D, Waters G. Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behav Brain Sci. 1999;22:114–126. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99001788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Caramazza A, Basili AG, Koller JJ, Berndt RS. An investigation of repetition and language processing in a case of conduction aphasia. Brain and Language. 1981;14:235–271. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(81)90078-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Daneman M, Carpenter PA. Individual differences in working memory. J Verb Learn Verb Behav. 1980;19:450–456. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Daneman M, Merikle PM. Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 1996;3:422–433. doi: 10.3758/BF03214546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Derrfuss J, Brass M, von Cramon DY. Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: Evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control, and working memory. Neuroimage. 2004;23:604–612. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Engle RW, Cantor J, Carullo JJ. Individual differences in working memory and comprehension: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1992;18:972–992. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.18.5.972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Epstein RA, Parker WE, Feiler AM. Where am I now? Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortices in place recognition. J Neurosci. 2007;27:6141–6149. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0799-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fedorenko E, Gibson E, Rohde D. The nature of working memory capacity in sentence comprehension: evidence against domain-specific working memory resources. J Mem Lang. 2006;54:541–553. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fiebach CJ, Schlesewsky M, Lohmann G, von Cramon DY, Friederici AD. Revisiting the role of Broca’s area in sentence processing: syntactic integration versus syntactic working memory. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;24:79–91. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Friederici AD. The coritical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:262–268. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Friedrich F, Glenn C, Martin OSM. Interruption of phonological coding in conduction aphasia. Brain and Language. 1984;22:266–291. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(84)90094-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline JP, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp. 1995;2:189–210. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gordon PC, Hendrick R, Levine WH. Memory load interference in syntactic processing. Psychological Science. 2002;13:425–430. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Just MA, Carpenter PA, Keller TA, Eddy WF, Thulborn KR. Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science. 1996;274:114–116. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5284.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kane MJ, Hambrick DZ, Tuholski SW, Wilhelm O, Payne TW, Engle RW. The domain generality of working-memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and spatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004;133:189–217. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Keller TA, Carpenter PA, Just MA. The neural bases of sentence comprehension: A fMRI examination of syntactic and lexical processing. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:223–237. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.3.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.King J, Just MA. Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language. 1991;30:580–602. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kruggel F, von Cramon DY. Modeling the hemodynamic response in single-trial functional MRI experiments. Magn Reson Med. 1999;42:787–797. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1522-2594(199910)42:4<787::aid-mrm22>3.0.co;2-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lakoff G. Women, fire, and dangerous things. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1987. p. 1073. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lakoff G, Johnson M. Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1980. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lauro LJR, Reis J, Cohen LG, Cecchetto C, Papagno C. A case for the involvement of phonological loop in sentence comprehension. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:4003–4011. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lee D, Newman SD. The effect of presentation paradigm on syntactic processing: An event-related fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping. 2010;31:123–132. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Logie RH. The functional organization and capacity limits of working memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2011;20:240–245. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.MacDonald MC, Christiansen MH. Reassessing working memory: comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996) Psychological Review. 2002;109:35–54. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.109.1.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.McRae K, Hare M, Elman JL, Ferretti TR. A basis for generating expectancies for verbs from nouns. Memory & Cognition. 2005;33:1174–1184. doi: 10.3758/bf03193221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Nee DE, Brown JW, Askren MK, Berman MG, Demiralp E, Krawitz A, Jonides J. A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cerebral Cortex. 2012 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Newman SD, Just MA, Sternberg R, Davidson J, Pretz J. The neural bases of intelligence: a perspective based on functional neuroimaging. Cognition & Intelligence. 2005 [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Newman SD, Lee D, Ratliff KL. Off-line sentence processing: what is involved in answering a comprehension probe? Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2499–2511. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Newman SD, Ikuta T. The effect of the semantic relatedness on sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Brain and Language. 2010;113:51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Newman SD, Ikuta T. The effect of semantic relatedness on syntactic processing. Presented at CUNY 2009 – Conference on Human Sentence Processing; Davis, CA. 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Papagno C, Cecchetto C, Reati F, Bello L. Processing of syntactically complex sentences relies on verbal short-term memory: Evidence from a short term memory patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 2007;24:292. doi: 10.1080/02643290701211928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Prat C, Keller TA, Just MA. Individual differences in sentence comprehension: a functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging investigation of syntactic and lexical processing demands. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1950–1963. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.1950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Price CJ. The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1191:62–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05444.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. PNAS. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Talmy L. Toward a cognitive semantics. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Vincent JL, Snyder AZ, Fox MD, Shannon BJ, Andrews JR, Raichle ME, Buckner RL. Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal--parietal memory network. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:3517–3531. doi: 10.1152/jn.00048.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wagner TD, Smith EE. Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;3:255–274. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.4.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Waters G, Caplan D. The relationship between age, processing speed, working memory capacity, and language comprehension. Memory. 2005;13:403–13. doi: 10.1080/09658210344000459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Yarkoni T, Speer NK, Zacks JM. Neural substrates of narrative comprehension and memory. NeuroImage. 2008;41:1408–1425. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Zarahn E, Aguirre G, D’Esposito M. A trial-based experimental design for fMRI. Neuroimage. 1997;6:122–138. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Zarahn E. Testing for neural responses during temporal components of trials with BOLD fMRI. Neuroimage. 2000;11:783–796. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES