Skip to main content
Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders EXTRA logoLink to Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders EXTRA
. 2013 Sep 11;3(1):291–300. doi: 10.1159/000354188

Correlates of Subjective and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Depressive Symptoms and CSF Biomarkers

Ramune Grambaite a,c,*, Erik Hessen a,d, Eirik Auning b,e, Dag Aarsland b,f,g, Per Selnes a,e, Tormod Fladby a,e
PMCID: PMC3808228  PMID: 24174924

Abstract

Aims

To improve early diagnosis of dementia disease, this study investigates correlates of cognitive complaints and cognitive test performance in patients with subjective (SCI) and mild (MCI) cognitive impairment.

Methods

Seventy patients from a memory clinic, aged 45-79, with a score of 2 (n = 23) or 3 (n = 47) on the Global Deterioration Scale, were included. CSF biomarkers [Aβ42, total tau (T-tau) and phosphorylated tau (P-tau)], depressive symptoms, cognitive performance, and complaints were examined.

Results

Correlation analysis showed that cognitive complaints increased with decreasing cognitive performance in SCI and decreased with decreasing performance in MCI. Linear regression models revealed that cognitive complaints were associated with depressive symptoms in both groups of patients, while cognitive performance was associated with CSF Aβ42 and P-tau in SCI and with T-tau and P-tau in MCI.

Conclusion

These results suggest that depressive symptoms are associated with cognitive complaints, while degenerative changes are associated with objective cognitive decline in high-risk predementia states.

Key Words
: Mild cognitive impairment, Subjective cognitive impairment, Cognitive complaints, Depression, CSF biomarkers, Degenerative diseases


Introduction

To differentiate between the individual diagnoses of patients presenting at memory clinics with cognitive complaints and often depressive symptoms, more accurate diagnostic measures are needed. The term subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) describes patients with subjective cognitive complaints and occurs before mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [1,2]. MCI patients have objective cognitive impairment in addition to subjective cognitive complaints [3,4]. Both conditions may represent predementia stages. In order to determine which persons should be treated or investigated further, it is important to find reliable correlates of both subjective cognitive complaints and objective cognitive impairment.

Objective cognitive impairment has been found in 11% of nondemented elderly [5], while estimates of memory complaints in nondemented elderly range from 22 to 56% [6]. For early diagnosis of dementia and Alzheimer's disease (AD), it is important to know when cognitive complaints are accurate, i.e. reflect actual performance, but the existing findings regarding accuracy of complaints in MCI are divergent [7,8]. Significant associations between cognitive complaints and performance have been found in a few community-based samples [7,9], while other studies have not found such associations [10,11]. One review study suggests that nondemented individuals are generally able to accurately identify their cognitive problems [12], while patients with AD dementia underestimate their cognitive problems [13,14]. Early AD is typically characterized by neuronal loss in the medial temporal lobes [15] and memory impairment [16]. The degree of awareness of one's cognitive state may decrease as the disease progresses and frontal lobe functions decrease [17].

Combinations of AD-related CSF biomarkers (tau and Aβ proteins) predict the development of dementia in MCI [18,19,20,21]. The findings from one recent study [22] indicate that CSF levels of Aβ42 are already fully decreased at least 5-10 years before conversion to AD dementia, whereas total tau (T-tau) and phosphorylated tau (P-tau) seem to be later markers [23]. Previously, we have found associations between memory and CSF tau levels in the SCI/MCI sample [24,25], and associations between CSF biomarkers and cognition in MCI have been found by other researchers [26,27,28,29]. Still, many aspects regarding the relationship between biological changes related to degeneration in terms of CSF biomarkers and clinical symptoms in predementia stages remain unclear. The association between CSF biomarkers and both cognitive complaints and performance has not been studied in SCI.

Depressive symptoms occur commonly in SCI and MCI [30] and constitute another known risk factor for dementia [31,32,33,34,35]. People that are depressed complain [36,37], and the criteria for depression include SCI and/or objective cognitive impairment [38]. Positive correlations have been found between depression and memory complaints in a few studies of normal and cognitively impaired samples [8,39,40].

Our first hypothesis is that individuals with SCI judge their performance more accurately than MCI patients. The second hypothesis is that cognitive complaints are associated with depressive symptoms and CSF biomarkers are associated with objective cognitive impairment.

Methods

Research Participants

The project was approved by the South-Eastern Norway committee for medical research ethics. The participants' consent was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki [41]. Seventy patients were included in the study. Patients with subjective cognitive complaints which had lasted 6 months or more and who had attended a university-based memory clinic between September 2005 and January 2010 were assessed for inclusion in the study. Inclusion criteria were no or very mild problems with activities of daily living and Global Deterioration Scale (GDS) [42,43] score 2 (SCI, n = 23) or 3 (MCI, n = 47) as determined from a clinical interview and screening tests. Screening tests included parameters 13-20 (memory, disorientation, abstract thinking, visuospatial ability, language, sensory aphasia, visual agnosia, and apraxia) from the stepwise comparative status analysis (STEP) [44,45], word fluency, interference, and numeral-letter items from the I-FLEX [46], and items from the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination (Cognistat) [47] as well as Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [48]. To be classified as GDS 2, subjects had to score ≥28 on MMSE, 0 on STEP variables, a total sum <2 on the elements from I-FLEX, and there had to be a maximum of one domain where they scored 0.5 on the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scale [49]. Subjects classified as GDS 3 scored ≥26 on MMSE, ≤1 on STEP variables, ≤2 on I-FLEX variables, and there were more than one CDR domain where they scored 0.5, albeit none where they scored 1. The following criteria for exclusion were established: psychiatric disorder (including major depressive episode), stroke, cancer, drug abuse, solvent exposure or anoxic brain damage.

Neuropsychological Assessment and Cognitive Variables

Neuropsychological examination was performed not later than 3 months after inclusion in the study. The complete neuropsychological test battery is described elsewhere [50]. The four neuropsychological variables, used in the analyses, were termed: ‘word list memory’ (30-min recall) of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) [51], ‘visual memory’ (30-min recall) of the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT) [52], ‘working memory’ of the Letter-Number Sequencing subtest, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition [53], and ‘response inhibition’ (naming colors of the ink of inconsistently colored names) of the Color-Word Interference Test [54].

Depressive Symptoms

Self-reported depressive symptoms were assessed by the Symptom Checklist-90-R (SCL-90-R) [55] on the same day as the neuropsychological examination was performed. The questionnaire consists of 90 items and is designed to evaluate a broad range of psychological problems and symptoms of psychopathology, including a 13-item subscale for depressive symptoms. The items are rated on a five-point scale of distress from ‘not at all’ to ‘extremely’. In addition to SCL-90-R, depressive symptoms were assessed using a short version of the Geriatric Depression Scale [56], namely GDS-15 [57]. Its items require a yes/no response.

Subjective Cognitive Complaints

SCL-90-R was used for the assessment of subjective memory and concentration complaints. Two of the SCL-90-R items concern memory and concentration complaints, which are rated on a five-point scale of distress from ‘not at all’ (0) to ‘extremely’ (4).

Lumbar Puncture

CSF samples were collected by lumbar puncture through the L3/L4 or L4/L5 inter-space. The lumbar puncture was performed consecutively following the patients' inclusion in the study. CSF concentrations of Aβ42, T-tau, and P-tau were routinely examined using commercially available enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay kits (Innogenetics, Belgium). Raw scores of the CSF variables were used in all analyses. Both raw scores and frequencies of pathological levels are shown in table 1. Aβ42 was considered pathological if ≤550 ng/l. P-tau was considered pathological if ≥80 ng/l. T-tau level was considered abnormal if T-tau ≥300 ng/l for patients under 50 years, >450 ng/l for patients from 50 to 69 years, and ≥500 ng/l for patients from 70 years and above [58].

Table 1.

Characteristics of the patients

Total group SCI (n = 23) MCI (n = 47) p
Age (range 45–79), years 61.7 (7.2) 58.8 (7.2) 63.1 (6.8) 0.03
Education (range 7–18), years 12.5 (2.9) 13.4 (2.8) 12.1 (2.9) 0.08
Gender, n male 35 (50%) 10 (44%) 25 (53%) 0.45
Handedness, n right 66 (94%) 23 (100%) 43 (92%) 0.15
APOE-€4, n carriers 33 (47%) 13 (57%) 20 (43%) 0.32
MMSE1 28.1 (1.4) 29.0 (1.0) 27.6 (1.3) <0.001
Word list memory (RAVLT) 7.8 (4.1) 10.0 (2.9) 6.7 (4.1) <0.001
Visual memory (RCFT) 13.0 (6.8) 17.3 (6.7) 10.9 (5.8) <0.001
Response inhibition (CWIT) 75.6 (47.1) 61.1 (11.5) 82.5 (55.4) 0.02
Working memory (Letter-Number Sequencing) 8.3 (2.2) 9.1 (2.2) 7.9 (2.2) 0.04
Memory complaints (range 0–4) 2.4 (1.1) 2.5 (1.1) 2.3 (1.1) 0.58
Concentration complaints (range 0–4) 1.9 (1.2) 2.1 (1.2) 1.8 (1.2) 0.27
SCL-90-R depressive symptoms 0.9 (0.7) 1.1 (0.8) 0.8 (0.6) 0.28
GDS-15 depressive symptoms 3.3 3.4 3.3 0.22
42, ng/l 842.2 (291.1) 869.1 (264.0) 829.1 (305.4) 0.54
42, n pathological 15 (21%) 3 (13%) 12 (26%) 0.35
T-tau, ng/l 344.8 (201.4) 263.0 (81.9) 384.9 (229.5) 0.01
T-tau, n pathological 16 (23%) 2 (9%) 14 (30%) 0.07
P-tau, ng/l 75.8 (30.1) 65.8 (12.2) 80.7 (34.9) 0.06
P-tau, n pathological 24 (34%) 3 (13%) 21 (45%) 0.02

Values are presented as means (±SD) unless indicated otherwise. Raw scores of all variables are presented. The comparison of gender, handedness, APOE-€4, and pathological levels of CSF biomarkers between the SCI and MCI groups was made with a χ2 test (Pearson χ2 test p value is shown). The independent-samples t test was used to compare the groups for the remaining variables. 1 MMSE: maximum score: 30.

Statistical Analysis

The Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS 16.0) was used for all statistical analyses. To compare characteristics of the patient groups, the independent-samples t test and the χ2 test were used. The Pearson correlation method was used to test for associations between cognitive complaints and test performance. Linear multiple regression analysis was performed to assess the impact of depressive symptoms and CSF biomarkers on subjective cognitive complaints and cognitive performance. The variable of depressive symptoms was entered together with one of three CSF variables at a time as the predictor variables. Only one CSF variable was included per model because of the colinearity among the predictor variables. As there was a significant group difference in age, age was used as a confounder in each model. The term ‘significant’, used in this article, refers to p < 0.05. The term ‘significant at trend level’ refers to p < 0.10.

Results

Characteristics of the Patients

Table 1 presents patient characteristics. Demographic data were similar in SCI and MCI except for age. As expected, cognitive performance (MMSE, word list/visual memory, response inhibition, and working memory) was significantly better in SCI compared to MCI, but both groups reported a similar degree of memory and concentration complaints. Depressive symptoms were not significantly different between the groups, although there was a high variability in depression scores in both groups. CSF Aβ42 levels were higher (but not significantly so) in SCI compared to MCI, while T-tau levels were significantly lower and P-tau levels were lower at trend level in SCI compared to MCI.

Correlations between Cognitive Complaints and Performance

Table 2 shows the correlations between test performance and cognitive complaints. There was a significant negative relationship between visual memory performance and concentration complaints as well as between response inhibition and both memory and concentration complaints in patients with SCI (the better the test performance, the less complaints). In the same group, there were also negative relationships between memory complaints and performance on tests of working memory, word list and visual memory, but they were only significant at trend level. In MCI, significant positive relationships were found between word list/visual memory performance and concentration complaints, i.e. a lower performance was associated with less complaints. No significant associations were found between test performance and memory complaints in MCI.

Table 2.

Correlations between cognitive complaints and test performance

Word list memory Visual memory Response inhibition Working memory
Memory complaints
 SCI −0.361 −0.401 −0.482* −0.390
 MCI −0.039 0.285 −0.030 −0.052
Concentration complaints
 SCI −0.076 −0.433* −0.448* −0.156
 MCI 0.361* 0.466* 0.250 0.017

Pearson correlation coefficients are listed for the SCI (n = 23) and the MCI (n = 47) groups. Inhibition scores have been multiplied with −1, so that a higher score means a better result for each test.

*

p < 0.05.

Linear Regression Analyses

In order to assess the impact of depressive symptoms and CSF biomarkers (explanatory variables) on the reported cognitive complaints and cognitive performance (the dependent variables), linear regression analyses were performed (tables 3, 4). SCL-90-R depressive symptoms, but not CSF biomarkers, predicted subjective memory and concentration complaints in both groups of patients. In contrast, CSF biomarkers (Aβ42, T-tau, and P-tau) were significant predictors of cognitive test performance. In the SCI group (table 3), Aβ42 was a significant predictor of word list/visual memory and P-tau was a significant predictor of response inhibition. In the MCI group (table 4), T-tau was a significant predictor of word list memory (significance at trend level was achieved for visual memory), and P-tau was a significant predictor of visual memory (significance at trend level was achieved for word list memory). We also explored the associations of depression with cognitive complaints and test performance, using GDS-15 as the depression measure. The results were largely similar in SCI, with the exception that the GDS-15 score was negatively associated with response inhibition. In MCI, the GDS-15 score did not correlate with cognitive complaints or test performance (data not shown).

Table 3.

Multiple regression analyses with cognitive complaints and test results as the dependent variables and SCL-90-R depression scale and CSF variables as the predictor variables in SCI

Predictor variables Dependent variables
subjective complaints
test results
memory complaints concentration complaints word list memory visual memory response inhibition working memory
Model 1
 Depression 0.75 (0.001) 0.86 (<0.001) 0.00 (0.999) −0.26 (0.160) −0.33 (0.118) −0.26 (0.316)
 Aβ42 −0.13 (0.505) −0.25 (0.130) 0.42 (0.027) 0.44 (0.022) 0.39 (0.074) 0.26 (0.316)
Model 2
 Depression 0.78 (0.001) 0.91 (<0.001) −0.04 (0.835) −0.30 (0.168) −0.39 (0.087) −0.29 (0.275)
 T-tau 0.09 (0.593) 0.13 (0.402) −0.02 (0.936) 0.04 (0.830) −0.15 (0.473) −0.05 (0.828)
Model 3
 Depression 0.76 (0.001) 0.90 (<0.001) −0.04 (0.830) −0.30 (0.157) −0.39 (0.057) −0.29 (0.279)
 P-tau 0.04 (0.834) 0.26 (0.076) −0.08 (0.649) 0.10 (0.593) 0.41 (0.026) −0.07 (0.780)

β (p) values are shown. Inhibition scores have been multiplied with −1, so that a higher score means a better result for each test (n = 23). Age was included in each model. The italicized numbers indicate a significance level of p < 0.05.

Table 4.

Multiple regression analyses with cognitive complaints and test results as the dependent variables and SCL-90-R depression scale and CSF variables as the predictor variables in MCI

Predictor variables Dependent variables
subjective complaints
objective test results
memory complaints concentration complaints word list memory visual memory response inhibition working memory
Model 1
 Depression 0.40 (0.005) 0.38 (0.002) 0.18 (0.222) 0.20 (0.154) −0.10 (0.491) −0.02 (0.862)
 Aβ42 0.06 (0.685) <0.001 (0.998) 0.15 (0.305) 0.23 (0.093) 0.20 (0.178) −0.05 (0.730)
Model 2
 Depression 0.41 (0.005) 0.36 (0.004) 0.13 (0.369) 0.16 (0.263) −0.13 (0.391) −0.04 (0.815)
 T-tau 0.03 (0.832) −0.17 (0.165) 0.31 (0.039) −0.26 (0.076) −0.18 (0.266) −0.06 (0.727)
Model 3
 Depression 0.40 (0.006) 0.37 (0.003) 0.15 (0.304) 0.16 (0.238) −0.12 (0.453) −0.01 (0.945)
 P-tau −0.02 (0.885) −0.14 (0.222) −0.26 (0.072) 0.32 (0.019) −0.11 (0.492) 0.13 (0.917)

β (p) values are shown. Inhibition scores have been multiplied with −1, so that a higher score means a better result for each test (n = 47). Age was included in each model. The italicized numbers indicate a significance level of p < 0.05.

Discussion

This study has investigated correlates of cognitive complaints and cognitive test performance in patients with SCI and MCI. Depressive symptoms and CSF biomarkers were assessed as possible correlates because of their importance for the differential diagnosis of dementia. Subjects with SCI were included since they constitute a group of particular interest for the early detection of neurodegenerative disease. The study objectives were met and the hypotheses confirmed.

In addition to neuropsychological data, we have studied both memory and concentration complaints, while most of the existing studies have aggregated the complaints into one complaint score instead of analyzing them by domain [59,60,61]. We have used the self-rating questionnaires SCL-90-R and GDS-15 for the assessment of cognitive complaints and depressive symptoms. Relatively high MMSE scores (SCI mean score 29 and MCI mean score 28) imply that most of patients are reliable sources of information. Still, a decrease in reliability is expected with disease progression (e.g., from SCI to MCI). The SCI and MCI groups studied reported a similar amount of memory and concentration complaints. Cognitive complaints increased with decreasing cognitive function in SCI and decreased with decreasing cognitive function in MCI. These results suggest that patients with incipient cognitive difficulties (SCI), not satisfying the clinical criteria for MCI, assess their cognitive abilities more correctly, while patients with MCI tend to underestimate their cognitive difficulties. Similarly, disagreement between patients and their informants was associated with MCI but not SCI in another recent study [62].

Depressive symptoms, but not CSF biomarkers, were significantly associated with cognitive complaints, which is in accord with the findings from earlier studies of normal and cognitively impaired samples [8,34,39,40]. We have not found an association between depressive symptoms and cognitive impairment, except for the association between response inhibition and depressive symptoms as measured by GDS-15 in SCI. In comparison, depressive symptoms were negatively correlated with cognitive functioning in another recent study of healthy middle-aged and older adults [63]. Taking into account that depressive symptoms in midlife or in late life are associated with an increased risk of developing dementia [35], the association between depressive symptoms and response inhibition/cognitive complaints in our study may reflect an etiologic relationship between depression and very early cognitive changes due to dementia.

CSF biomarkers were related to objective cognitive impairment. We found negative associations between memory and CSF tau measurements in the MCI subsample and positive associations between Aβ42 and memory in the cognitively healthier SCI subsample. We have previously found similar associations between tau and memory in the overlapping SCI/MCI sample, which was not divided into SCI and MCI [24,25]. To our knowledge, this is the first study of patients with SCI and MCI, giving support to the amyloid cascade hypothesis [23] by using cognitive data. Here, we find the associations between memory and CSF Aβ42 in SCI and the associations between memory and tau biomarkers in MCI, which agrees with recent findings of Buchhave et al. [22], suggesting that altered Aβ metabolism precedes tau-related pathology and neuronal degeneration in AD.

The association between memory performance and cognitive complaints in SCI is not surprising. These patients have been referred to a memory clinic which focuses on diagnosing dementia. Incipient AD is the most prevalent underlying disease and is characterized by white matter diffusivity changes encompassing the memory network [24,64]. In addition to memory performance, we have studied working memory and response inhibition. In SCI, response inhibition was significantly associated with memory/concentration complaints and CSF P-tau, while working memory was associated with memory complaints at trend level. These findings suggest that response inhibition is a sensitive measure of early cognitive decline in predementia phases. Previously, we have found that worse performance on response inhibition was associated with increased white matter diffusivity underlying the superior frontal cortex [65].

The study has some limitations. An important limitation has to do with the difficulties of detecting effect sizes in small group data sets, exposing the method to type II error. With few subjects in the SCI group, there was a high risk of neglecting consistent relationships among the studied variables. Another limitation concerns the heterogeneity inherent in the SCI and MCI concepts. Although an extensive screening procedure was used for the classification of subjects, the groups may be overlapping and may contain, among other conditions, chronic ischemic disease, normal aging, early-stage AD, and non-AD predementia. Patients with major depressive episodes have been excluded. Still, a high variance in depression scores and high relationships between depressive symptoms and cognitive complaints suggest that some of the subjects could be included due to cognitive complaints related to their depressiveness. A cross-sectional study design may not be optimal since depressive disturbances can fluctuate and may not be present at every examination. The follow-up of the populations studied will help determine the long-term clinical significance of the results.

Conclusions

This study aims to improve early diagnosis of dementia by investigating correlates of cognitive complaints and cognitive test performance in SCI and MCI. Considering the question if cognitive complaints reflect actual performance, the results from this study demonstrate that patients with SCI, not satisfying clinical criteria for MCI, assess their cognitive abilities more correctly, while patients with MCI tend to underestimate their cognitive difficulties. Further, the findings shed light on the nature of cognitive complaints and cognitive impairment by demonstrating that depressive symptoms are associated with cognitive complaints, while degenerative changes (relating to pathological levels of CSF biomarkers) are associated with an objective cognitive decline in high-risk predementia states. The results of the present manuscript are preliminary and need further confirmation on larger, prospective samples.

Acknowledgements

The study is supported by grants from South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (Helse Sør-Øst) and Akershus University Hospital. The authors thank Ivar Reinvang, Vidar Stenset, Jurate Saltyte-Benth, Anders Skinningsrud, Turid Hegde, Sissel Bardosen, Kjersti Træland Hanssen, Leif Gjerstad, Paulina Due-Tønnessen, Atle Bjørnerud, Anders Wallin and Lisbeth Johnsen for their expertise and practical contributions. We are grateful to all the participants of this study for donating their time.

References

  • 1.Reisberg B, Gauthier S. Current evidence for subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) as the pre-mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stage of subsequently manifest Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20:1–16. doi: 10.1017/S1041610207006412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Reisberg B, Shulman MB, Torossian C, Leng L, Zhu W. Outcome over seven years of healthy adults with and without subjective cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6:11–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L, Wahlund LO, Nordberg A, Backman L, Albert M, Almkvist O, Arai H, Basun H, Blennow K, de Leon M, DeCarli C, Erkinjuntti T, Giacobini E, Graff C, Hardy J, Jack C, Jorm A, Ritchie K, van Duijn C, Visser P, Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment – beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256:240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Di Carlo A, Baldereschi M, Amaducci L, Maggi S, Grigoletto F, Scarlato G, Inzitari D. Cognitive impairment without dementia in older people: prevalence, vascular risk factors, impact on disability. The Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48:775–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb04752.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jonker C, Geerlings MI, Schmand B. Are memory complaints predictive for dementia? A review of clinical and population-based studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15:983–991. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200011)15:11<983::aid-gps238>3.0.co;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lam LC, Lui VW, Tam CW, Chiu HF. Subjective memory complaints in Chinese subjects with mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;20:876–882. doi: 10.1002/gps.1370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wong CH, Lam LC, Lui VW, Chiu HF, Chan SS, Tam CW. Subjective complaints and self-evaluation of memory test performance in questionable dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;21:937–944. doi: 10.1002/gps.1586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Jonker C, Launer LJ, Hooijer C, Lindeboom J. Memory complaints and memory impairment in older individuals. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44:44–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1996.tb05636.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Jungwirth S, Fischer P, Weissgram S, Kirchmeyr W, Bauer P, Tragl KH. Subjective memory complaints and objective memory impairment in the Vienna-Transdanube aging community. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:263–268. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52066.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Tsai DH, Green RC, Benke KS, Silliman RA, Farrer LA. Predictors of subjective memory complaint in cognitively normal relatives of patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;18:384–388. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2006.18.3.384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Reid LM, Maclullich AM. Subjective memory complaints and cognitive impairment in older people. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;22:471–485. doi: 10.1159/000096295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dalla Barba G, Parlato V, Iavarone A, Boller F. Anosognosia, intrusions and ‘frontal’ functions in Alzheimer's disease and depression. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:247–259. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00091-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Graham DP, Kunik ME, Doody R, Snow AL. Self-reported awareness of performance in dementia. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25:144–152. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991;82:239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Salmon DP, Bondi MW. Neuropsychological assessment of dementia. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:257–282. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. ed 4. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hansson O, Zetterberg H, Buchhave P, Londos E, Blennow K, Minthon L. Association between CSF biomarkers and incipient Alzheimer's disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a follow-up study. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5:228–234. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70355-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hampel H, Teipel SJ, Fuchsberger T, Andreasen N, Wiltfang J, Otto M, Shen Y, Dodel R, Du Y, Farlow M, Moller HJ, Blennow K, Buerger K. Value of CSF beta-amyloid1-42 and tau as predictors of Alzheimer's disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:705–710. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Diniz BS, Pinto Junior JA, Forlenza OV. Do CSF total tau, phosphorylated tau, and beta-amyloid 42 help to predict progression of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2008;9:172–182. doi: 10.1080/15622970701535502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.de Leon MJ, Mosconi L, Blennow K, DeSanti S, Zinkowski R, Mehta PD, Pratico D, Tsui W, Saint Louis LA, Sobanska L, Brys M, Li Y, Rich K, Rinne J, Rusinek H. Imaging and CSF studies in the preclinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2007;1097:114–145. doi: 10.1196/annals.1379.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Buchhave P, Minthon L, Zetterberg H, Wallin AK, Blennow K, Hansson O. Cerebrospinal fluid levels of beta-amyloid1-42, but not of tau, are fully changed already 5 to 10 years before the onset of Alzheimer dementia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:98–106. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hardy J, Allsop D. Amyloid deposition as the central event in the aetiology of Alzheimer's disease. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 1991;12:383–388. doi: 10.1016/0165-6147(91)90609-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Grambaite R, Stenset V, Reinvang I, Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Fladby T. White matter diffusivity predicts memory in patients with subjective and mild cognitive impairment and normal CSF total tau levels. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16:58–69. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Amlien I, Bjornerud A, Reinvang I, Gjerstad L, Cappelen T, Willoch F, Due-Tonnessen P, Grambaite R, Skinningsrud A, Stenset V, Fladby T. Morphometric changes in the episodic memory network and tau pathologic features correlate with memory performance in patients with mild cognitive impairment. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2008;29:1183–1189. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Thorvaldsson V, Nordlund A, Reinvang I, Blennow K, Zetterberg H, Wallin A, Johansson B. Memory in individuals with mild cognitive impairment in relation to APOE and CSF Abeta42. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22:598–606. doi: 10.1017/S1041610210000335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Engelborghs S, Maertens K, Vloeberghs E, Aerts T, Somers N, Marien P, De Deyn PP. Neuropsychological and behavioural correlates of CSF biomarkers in dementia. Neurochem Int. 2006;48:286–295. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2005.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ivanoiu A, Sindic CJ. Cerebrospinal fluid tau protein and amyloid beta42 in mild cognitive impairment: prediction of progression to Alzheimer's disease and correlation with the neuropsychological examination. Neurocase. 2005;11:32–39. doi: 10.1080/13554790490896901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schoonenboom SN, Visser PJ, Mulder C, Lindeboom J, Van Elk EJ, Van Kamp GJ, Scheltens PH. Biomarker profiles and their relation to clinical variables in mild cognitive impairment. Neurocase. 2005;11:8–13. doi: 10.1080/13554790490896785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ausen B, Edman G, Almkvist O, Bogdanovic N. Personality features in subjective cognitive impairment and mild cognitive impairment – early indicators of dementia? Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;28:528–535. doi: 10.1159/000255104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ganguli M, Du Y, Dodge HH, Ratcliff GG, Chang CC. Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in late life: a prospective epidemiological study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:153–160. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.2.153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Wilson RS, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, Kelly JF, Tang Y, Bennett DA. Chronic psychological distress and risk of Alzheimer's disease in old age. Neuroepidemiology. 2006;27:143–153. doi: 10.1159/000095761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Barnes DE, Yaffe K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer's disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10:819–828. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70072-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Minett TS, Da Silva RV, Ortiz KZ, Bertolucci PH. Subjective memory complaints in an elderly sample: a cross-sectional study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;23:49–54. doi: 10.1002/gps.1836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Byers AL, McCormick M, Schaefer C, Whitmer RA. Midlife versus late-life depressive symptoms and risk of dementia: differential effects for Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:493–498. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Beck AT. Thinking and depression. I. Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1963;9:324–333. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1963.01720160014002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lau MA, Christensen BK, Hawley LL, Gemar MS, Segal ZV. Inhibitory deficits for negative information in persons with major depressive disorder. Psychol Med. 2007;37:1249–1259. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.APA . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. ed 4 (DSM-IV), text revision. Washington: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Jorm AF, Christensen H, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Henderson AS. Memory complaints as a precursor of memory impairment in older people: a longitudinal analysis over 7-8 years. Psychol Med. 2001;31:441–449. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Geerlings MI, Jonker C, Bouter LM, Ader HJ, Schmand B. Association between memory complaints and incident Alzheimer's disease in elderly people with normal baseline cognition. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:531–537. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.4.531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. Law Med Health Care. 1991;19:264–265. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Auer S, Reisberg B. The GDS/FAST staging system. Int Psychogeriatr. 1997;9(suppl 1):167–171. doi: 10.1017/s1041610297004869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, Crook T. Global Deterioration Scale (GDS) Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24:661–663. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Edman A, Mahnfeldt M, Wallin A. Inter-rater reliability of the step protocol. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2001;14:140–144. doi: 10.1177/089198870101400307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wallin A, Edman A, Blennow K, Gottfries CG, Karlsson I, Regland B, Sjogren M. Stepwise comparative status analysis (STEP): a tool for identification of regional brain syndromes in dementia. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 1996;9:185–199. doi: 10.1177/089198879600900406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Royall DR, Mahurin RK, Gray KF. Bedside assessment of executive cognitive impairment: the executive interview. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40:1221–1226. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb03646.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kiernan RJ, Mueller J, Langston JW, Van Dyke C. The neurobehavioral cognitive status examination: a brief but quantitative approach to cognitive assessment. Ann Intern Med. 1987;107:481–485. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-107-4-481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43:2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Grambaite R, Reinvang I, Selnes P, Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Stenset V, Fladby T. Pre-dementia memory impairment is associated with white matter tract affection. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:143–153. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Schmidt M. Rey Auditory and Verbal Learning Test. A Handbook. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Meyers JE, Meyers KR. Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial. Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Wechsler DS, Shelly CA, Petroff CA, Dang CV. Mxi1, a putative tumor suppressor gene, suppresses growth of human glioblastoma cells. Cancer Res. 1997;57:4905–4912. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS): Examiner's Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Derogatis LR. Administration, Scoring and Procedures Manual-II. Towson: Clinical Psychometric Research; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Debruyne H, Van Buggenhout M, Le Bastard N, Aries M, Audenaert K, De Deyn PP, Engelborghs S. Is the geriatric depression scale a reliable screening tool for depressive symptoms in elderly patients with cognitive impairment? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;24:556–562. doi: 10.1002/gps.2154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Burke WJ, Roccaforte WH, Wengel SP. The short form of the geriatric depression scale: a comparison with the 30-item form. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 1991;4:173–178. doi: 10.1177/089198879100400310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Sjogren M, Vanderstichele H, Agren H, Zachrisson O, Edsbagge M, Wikkelso C, Skoog I, Wallin A, Wahlund LO, Marcusson J, Nagga K, Andreasen N, Davidsson P, Vanmechelen E, Blennow K. Tau and abeta42 in cerebrospinal fluid from healthy adults 21-93 years of age: establishment of reference values. Clin Chem. 2001;47:1776–1781. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gagnon M, Dartigues JF, Mazaux JM, Dequae L, Letenneur L, Giroire JM, Barberger-Gateau P. Self-reported memory complaints and memory performance in elderly French community residents: results of the PAQUID Research Program. Neuroepidemiology. 1994;13:145–154. doi: 10.1159/000110373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kliegel M, Zimprich D, Eschen A. What do subjective cognitive complaints in persons with aging-associated cognitive decline reflect? Int Psychogeriatr. 2005;17:499–512. doi: 10.1017/s1041610205001638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Newson RS, Kemps EB. The nature of subjective cognitive complaints of older adults. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2006;63:139–151. doi: 10.2190/1EAP-FE20-PDWY-M6P1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Ausen B, Edman G, Almkvist O, Bogdanovic N. Self- and informant ratings of personality in mild cognitive impairment, reviewed. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2011;32:387–393. doi: 10.1159/000330695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Brevik EJ, Eikeland RA, Lundervold AJ. Subthreshold depressive symptoms have a negative impact on cognitive functioning in middle-aged and older males. Front Psychol. 2013;4:309. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Selnes P, Aarsland D, Bjornerud A, Gjerstad L, Wallin A, Hessen E, Reinvang I, Grambaite R, Auning E, Kjaervik VK, Due-Tonnessen P, Stenset V, Fladby T. Diffusion tensor imaging surpasses cerebrospinal fluid as predictor of cognitive decline and medial temporal lobe atrophy in subjective cognitive impairment and mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;33:723–736. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-121603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Grambaite R, Selnes P, Reinvang I, Aarsland D, Hessen E, Gjerstad L, Fladby T. Executive dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment is associated with changes in frontal and cingulate white matter tracts. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;27:453–462. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-110290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders EXTRA are provided here courtesy of Karger Publishers

RESOURCES