Abstract
A classical view of neural coding relies on temporal firing synchrony among functional groups of neurons, however, the underlying mechanism remains an enigma. Here we experimentally demonstrate a mechanism where time-lags among neuronal spiking leap from several tens of milliseconds to nearly zero-lag synchrony. It also allows sudden leaps out of synchrony, hence forming short epochs of synchrony. Our results are based on an experimental procedure where conditioned stimulations were enforced on circuits of neurons embedded within a large-scale network of cortical cells in vitro and are corroborated by simulations of neuronal populations. The underlying biological mechanisms are the unavoidable increase of the neuronal response latency to ongoing stimulations and temporal or spatial summation required to generate evoked spikes. These sudden leaps in and out of synchrony may be accompanied by multiplications of the neuronal firing frequency, hence offering reliable information-bearing indicators which may bridge between the two principal neuronal coding paradigms.
Keywords: network, topology, firing synchrony, in vitro modular networks, neuronal circuit
Introduction
One of the major challenges of modern neuroscience is to elucidate the brain mechanisms that underlie firing synchrony among neurons. Such spike correlations with differing degrees of temporal precision have been observed in various sensory cortical areas, in particular in the visual (Eckhorn et al., 1988; Gray et al., 1989), auditory (Ahissar et al., 1992; Nicolelis et al., 1995), somatosensory (Nicolelis et al., 1995), and frontal (Vaadia et al., 1995) areas. Several mechanisms have been suggested, including the slow and limited increase in neuronal response latency per evoked spike (Vardi et al., 2013b). On a neuronal circuit level its accumulative effect serves as a non-uniform gradual stretching of the effective neuronal circuit delay loops. Consequently, small mismatches of only a few milliseconds among firing times of neurons can vanish in a very slow gradual process consisting of hundreds of evoked spikes per neuron.
The phenomenon of sudden leaps from firing mismatches of several tens of milliseconds to nearly zero-lag synchronization, below a millisecond, is counterintuitive. Since the dynamical variations in neuronal features, e.g., the increase in neuronal response latencies per evoked spike, are extremely small, one might expect only very slow variations in firing timings. Moreover, relative changes among firing times of neurons require dynamic relaxation of the entire neuronal circuit to achieve synchronization. Hence, sudden leaps, in and out of synchrony, seem unexpected.
In the present study, we propose a new experimentally corroborated mechanism allowing leaps in and out of synchrony. The procedure is based on conditioned stimulations enforced on neuronal circuits embedded within a large-scale network of cortical cells in vitro (Marom and Shahaf, 2002; Morin et al., 2005; Wagenaar et al., 2006; Vardi et al., 2012). These stimulations varied in strength, so that the evoked spikes of selected neurons required temporal summation. We demonstrate that the underlying biological mechanism to sudden leaps in and out of synchrony is the unavoidable increase of the neuronal response latency (Aston-Jones et al., 1980; De Col et al., 2008; Ballo and Bucher, 2009; Gal et al., 2010) to ongoing stimulations, which imposes a non-uniform stretching of the neuronal circuit delay loops.
Materials and methods
Culture preparation
Cortical neurons were obtained from newborn rats (Sprague–Dawley) within 48 h after birth using mechanical and enzymatic procedures (Marom and Shahaf, 2002; Vardi et al., 2012, 2013b). All procedures were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and Bar-Ilan University Guidelines for the Use and Care of Laboratory Animals in Research and were approved and supervised by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
The cortex tissue was digested enzymatically with 0.05% trypsin solution in phosphate-buffered saline (Dulbecco's PBS) free of calcium and magnesium, supplemented with 20 mM glucose, at 37°C. Enzyme treatment was terminated using heat-inactivated horse serum, and cells were then mechanically dissociated. The neurons were plated directly onto substrate-integrated multi-electrode arrays (MEAs) and allowed to develop functionally and structurally mature networks over a time period of 2–3 weeks in vitro, prior to the experiments. Variability in the number of cultured days in this range had no effect on the observed results. The number of plated neurons in a typical network is in the order of 1,300,000, covering an area of about 380 mm2. The preparations were bathed in minimal essential medium (MEM-Earle, Earle's Salt Base without L-Glutamine) supplemented with heat-inactivated horse serum (5%), glutamine (0.5 mM), glucose (20 mM), and gentamicin (10 g/ml), and maintained in an atmosphere of 37°C, 5% CO2 and 95% air in an incubator as well as during the electrophysiological measurements. All experiments were conducted on cultured cortical neurons that were functionally isolated from their network by a pharmacological block of glutamatergic and GABAergic synapses. For each plate, 12–20 μl of a cocktail of synaptic blockers was used, consisting of 10 μM CNQX (6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione), 80 μM APV (amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid), and 5 μM Bicuculline. This cocktail did not block the spontaneous network activity completely, but rather made it sparse. At least 1 h was allowed for stabilization of the effect.
Measurements and stimulation
An array of 60 Ti/Au/TiN extracellular electrodes, 30 μm in diameter and spaced either 200 or 500 μm from each other (Multi-Channel Systems, Reutlingen, Germany) was used. The insulation layer (silicon nitride) was pre-treated with polyethyleneimine (Sigma, 0.01% in 0.1 M Borate buffer solution). A commercial setup (MEA2100-2x60-headstage, MEA2100-interface board, MCS, Reutlingen, Germany) for recording and analyzing data from two 60-electrode MEAs was used, with integrated data acquisition from 120 MEA electrodes and 8 additional analog channels, integrated filter amplifier and 6-channel current or voltage stimulus generator (for both MEAs). Mono-phasic square voltage pulses (−900 to −100 mV, 100–500 μs) were applied through extracellular electrodes. Each channel was sampled at a frequency of 50 k sample/s. Action potentials were detected on-line by threshold crossing. For each of the recording channels a threshold for spike detection was defined separately, prior to the beginning of the experiment.
Cell selection
Each circuit node was represented by a stimulation source (source electrode) and a target for the stimulation—the recording electrode (target electrode). These electrodes (source and target) were selected as the ones that evoked well-isolated, well-formed spikes and reliable responses with high signal-to-noise ratio. This examination was done with stimulus intensity of −800 mV using 30 repetitions at a rate of 5 Hz followed by 1200 repetitions at a rate of 10 Hz.
Stimulation control
A node response was defined as a spike occurring within a typical time window of 2–10 ms following the electrical stimulation. The activity of all source and target electrodes was collected, and entailed stimuli were delivered in accordance to the circuit connectivity.
Circuit connectivity, τ
Conditioned stimulations were enforced on the circuit neurons embedded within a large-scale network of cortical cells in vitro, according to the circuit connectivity. Initially, each delay was defined as the expected time between the evoked spikes of two linked neurons; e.g., conditioned to a spike recorded in the target electrode assigned to neuron A, a spike will be detected in the target electrode of neuron B after τAB ms. For this end, conditioned to a spike recorded in the target electrode of neuron A, a stimulus will be applied after τAB-LB(0) ms to the source electrode of neuron B, where LB(0) is the initial latency of neuron B.
In cases where missed evoked spikes caused a termination of the neuronal circuit activity, stimulation was given to neuron A after a period of 100 ms, to restart the circuit's activity.
All neurons were stimulated at a rate of 10 Hz (Figures 1, 3) or 8 Hz (Figure 2), before the leap to synchronization.
Strong stimulations, (−800 mV, 200 μs), resulting in a reliable neural response, were given to all circuit neurons excluding neuron C (Figures 1, 2) and E (Figure 3). Weak stimulations (Figure 1: −450 mV, 40 μs. Figure 2: −600 mV, 60 μs. Figure 3: −700 mV, 60 μs) were given to neuron C (Figures 1, 2) or E (Figure 3), so that an evoked spike is expected only if the time-lag between two consecutive weak stimulations is short enough. In cases where the time-lag between two consecutive stimulations was shorter than 20 μs (from the end of the first stimulation to the beginning of the consecutive one), a unified strong stimulation was applied, to overcome technical limitations. The weak stimulations were defined for each neuron separately, due to differences in their threshold.
TTS (TS stands for temporal summation) is the maximal time-lag between two weak stimulations which typically results in an evoked spike. This quantity was empirically estimated by gradually changing the time-lag between two weak stimulations, and found to differ between neurons.
Data analysis
Analyses were performed in a Matlab environment (MathWorks, Natwick, MA, USA). Action potentials were detected by threshold crossing. In the context of this study, no significant difference was observed in the results under threshold crossing or voltage minima for spike detection. Reported results were confirmed based on at least ten experiments each, using different sets of neurons and several tissue cultures.
Results
Leap to synchrony accompanied by a doubled firing frequency
Experimental results
We first demonstrate leaps to synchrony using a neuronal circuit consisting of four neurons and conditioned stimulations split into weak/strong stimulations (Figure 1A). A strong stimulation consists of a relatively high amplitude and/or relatively long pulse duration such that an evoked spike is generated reliably, whereas a weak stimulation consists of a lower amplitude and/or pulse duration, such that an evoked spike is expected only if the time-lag between two consecutive weak stimulations is short enough. All delays (denoted on connecting lines between neurons in Figure 1A) were selected to initially include the response latency of the target neuron, e.g., the time-lag from neuron A to B, τAB, was initially set to τ−LB(0) where LB(0) stands for the initial response latency of neuron B. For τ = 50 ms, neurons A and B initially fire alternately, in and out of phase, at a frequency of ~10 Hz (Figure 1B). Neuron D fires ~τ/2 ms laggard to neuron A (Figure 1C) and the time-gap between two weak stimulations arriving at neuron C, Δ(StimC), is initially ε (Figures 1A,B). The experimentally estimated maximal time-gap between stimulations of neuron C which generates an evoked spike (temporal summation) is denoted by TTS, thus for Δ(StimC) > TTS ≈ 0.24 ms neuron C typically does not fire. As a result of the increase in the response latency of neuron D, Δ(StimC) is reduced (green line in Figure 1B) sufficiently so that neuron C starts firing [Δ(StimC) ≤ TTS] (Figure 1C). The circuit now consists of two delay loops, ~2τ (A-B-A) and ~3τ (A-C-B-A). Since the greatest common divisor (GCD) of the circuit delay loops is GCD(2,3) = 1, conditioned to the firing of neuron C, zero-lag synchronization between neurons A and B is theoretically expected (Kanter et al., 2011) after a very short transient, τ (Figure 1C). This phenomenon is quantitatively measured by the time-lag between spikes of neurons A and B, SyncAB. The emergence of zero-lag synchrony is clearly demonstrated by the leap to SyncAB ≈ 0 ms (blue line in Figure 1B), which is accompanied by a sudden frequency multiplication from ~10 to ~20 Hz (Figure 1C). Note that the sudden multiplication in frequency, by itself, shortens SyncAB from 100 to 50 ms, however, it cannot lead to zero-lag synchrony. The sudden emergence of SyncAB ≈ 0 ms requires only a single firing of neuron C, and is then maintained by the mutual firing of neurons A and B, independently of the firing of neuron C (Figure 1C). For a given TTS, the number of evoked spikes of neuron D until the leap to synchrony, n, increases with ε (Figure 1D). Quantitatively, using the experimental response latency profile of neuron D, LD, one can find n fulfilling the equality:
(1) |
where ΔLD(n) stands for the increase in response latency of neuron D after n evoked spikes (Figure 1E). Note that neuron D is laggard to neuron A, thus the number of evoked spikes of neuron A until the leap to synchrony increases with ε as well, in accordance with Equation (1) (Figure 1D). Since TTS varies between neurons and even within the same neuron over different trials, deviations from this equation are expected (e.g., ΔLD for ε = 0.8 ms and ε = 1 ms are almost the same, Figures 1D,E). A slow gradual increase in SyncAB after a leap to synchrony (Figure 1D) is theoretically attributed to the difference in the increase of neuronal response latencies |ΔLA(n)-ΔLB(n)| and the leap out of synchrony (Figure 1D) is a consequence of a response failure of neurons A and/or B (see Section “Slow Divergence out of Synchrony” in Appendix). Similar results were obtained and exemplified for spatial summation (not shown), where weak stimulations were given to a neuron through two different source electrodes. An evoked spike is expected only if the time-lag between two consecutive weak stimulations, controlled by the relative stimulation timings of the source electrodes, is short enough.
Simulations of population dynamics
The sudden leap to synchrony was experimentally verified under the limitation where each circuit node is represented by a single neuron, and is demonstrated to be robust under simulations of population dynamics (Figures 1F,G). Each one of the four nodes (Figure 1A) now represents a population comprised of 40 Hodgkin-Huxley sparsely connected neurons (for simulation details, see Vardi et al., 2013a). For the parameters used, TTS ≈ 1.3 ms, ε = 2 ms and 0.2 ms variance for the Gaussian distribution of the delays, a leap to synchrony is expected following Equation (1) after ~20 spikes of cluster A (Figure 1F). The simulated SyncAB is defined as the absolute difference between the average spiking times of the neurons comprising clusters A and B, where at least 50% of the neurons in a cluster fired (Figure 1G). Initially, several neurons in cluster C fire as a result of relatively close stimulations from either cluster A or D. This sporadic firing is a consequence of the Gaussian distribution of the delays between populations, however, their impact on the firing activity of cluster B is negligible. As neurons of cluster D fire repeatedly, Δ(StimC) decreases and more neurons from cluster C fire. Consequently, the activity of cluster C is enhanced such that a leap to synchrony is observed, accompanied by frequency doubling from ~10 to ~20 Hz (Figures 1F,G). A leap out of synchrony was not observed in the simulations, since population dynamics are more robust to a single neuron's response failure in comparison to a neuronal circuit where each node is represented by a single neuron (Figures 1A,D). Low connectivity, as well as a wider Gaussian distribution of delays between populations are expected to enhance fluctuations and response failures, and will eventually lead to a leap out of synchrony.
Population dynamics exhibit consistency with most of the experimental results, hence minimizing the possibility of these results as being only an artifact of the tissue culture. Nevertheless, the verification of our results in more realistic scenarios is required, including shorter delays and their interplay with the neuronal refractory period, the morphology of the neurons instead of considering neurons as points (Doiron et al., 2006), as well as possible adaptation mechanisms in the form of short and long term synaptic plasticity (Abbott and Regehr, 2004; Izhikevich, 2006). In addition, more accurate and systematic statistical measures of synchrony (Kreuz et al., 2009; Shimokawa and Shinomoto, 2009) can be adopted to describe the transition to synchrony in the case of population dynamics.
Leap to synchrony accompanied by tripled firing frequency
More general features of a sudden leap to synchrony are exemplified by increasing the delay from neuron B to A, τBA, from τ (Figure 1A) to 2τ (Figure 2A). The circuit now consists of two delay loops, ~3τ (A-B-A) and ~4τ (A-C-B-A) (Figure 2A). Since GCD(4,3) = 1, zero-lag synchronization is theoretically expected, conditioned to the firing of neuron C. Initially, neurons A and B fire at a frequency of ~8 Hz (3τ = 125 ms) (Figure 2C) and SyncAB ≈ τ (Figure 2B). Neuron C starts to fire as Δ(StimC) ≤ TTS ≈ 0.2 ms, resulting in SyncAB ≈ 0 which is accompanied by tripled firing frequency (Figure 2C). The number of evoked spikes by neuron D (or its leader neuron A) to the leap increases with ε in a non-linear manner following ΔLD(n), in accordance with Equation (1) (Figures 2D,E).
Typically, several leaps in and out of synchrony between neurons A and B occur before arriving at a stable nearly zero-lag synchronization (Figure 2D). These oscillations are attributed to unreliable responses of neuron C, and increase the duration of the relaxation to synchrony (Figure 2D). Similar oscillations on the way out of synchrony (Figure 2D) are attributed to the first response failure of either neuron A or B. Consequently, neurons A and B fire alternately in time-lags τ and 2τ. The final exit out of synchrony occurs in the second response failure of neurons A or B.
Simulation results (Figures 2F,G) confirmed the robustness of the experimentally observed leap to synchrony in population dynamics. The oscillations in the relaxation to synchrony are attributed to response failures of cluster C. These failures are a consequence of fluctuations in the firing timings of clusters A and D and the Gaussian distribution of their delays to cluster C.
Epochs of synchrony not accompanied by a change in frequency
A mechanism to leap out of synchrony as well as the interrelation between the sudden leap to synchrony and the firing frequency are at the center of the next examined neuronal circuit (Figure 3A). This circuit consists solely of a 2τ-delay loop, hence neurons A and F fire alternately in ~τ ms time-lags. Nevertheless, neuron A affects neuron E by weak stimulations arriving from two comparable initial delay routes; ~2τ ms (A-F-E) and ~2τ −ε ms (A-B-C-D-E) (Figure 3A). Initially, neuron E does not fire since ε ≈ 1.7 ms > TTS ≈ 0.5 ms. Since the overall increase in the neuronal response latency of a chain is accumulative, proportional to the number of neurons it comprises, Δ(StimE) gradually decreases below TTS (Figure 3B) and neuron E suddenly starts to fire. Consequently, since neuron A fires every ~2τ ms and neuron E fires ~2τ ms laggard to A, SyncAE ≈ 0 (Figures 3B,C). As Δ(StimE) decreases, the response of neuron E becomes more reliable (Figures 3B,C) and a leap out of synchrony is observed when Δ(StimE) again exceeds ~TTS (Figure 3B). Since neuron E's firing does not close a new neuronal loop, the leaps in and out of synchrony do not affect the firing frequency of the neuronal circuit (Figure 3C). The number of spikes to synchrony increases with ε as well as the time-gap between neurons during synchronization, SyncAE (Figures 3D,E). Simulation results (not shown) confirmed the robustness of the experimentally observed leap in and out of synchrony without a frequency change in population dynamics.
Discussion
Understanding the brain mechanisms that underlie firing synchrony is one of the great challenges of neuroscience. There are many variants of population codes, where a set of neurons in a population acts together to perform a specific computational task (Palm, 1990; Eichenbaum, 1993; Ainsworth et al., 2012). There is much discussion over whether rate coding or temporal coding is used to represent perceptual entities in populations of neurons in the cortex. A number of reports suggest that almost all the information in a stimulus is embedded in the rate code of active neurons (Aggelopoulos et al., 2005), while others suggest that synchrony among spiking of neuronal populations carry the information (deCharms and Merzenich, 1996). Experimental support for changes solely in firing rate when the perceptual task is modified (e.g., Lamme and Spekreijse, 1998; Roelfsema et al., 2004) is as compelling as those works that show changes in synchrony in the absence of firing rate changes (e.g., Womelsdorf et al., 2005), whereas in other experiments changes in both rate and spike correlations are observed concurrently (e.g., Biederlack et al., 2006). In any case, the usefulness of rate coding and temporal coding as information carriers of brain activity is a function of the decoding complexity, which is tightly correlated with their accuracy.
Rate and temporal coding are typically inaccurate in brain activities, although there are several well-known exceptions where neurons fire with high temporal accuracy (Bullock, 1970; Bullock et al., 1972; Moortgat et al., 2000). Rate precision, measured by inter-spike interval (ISI) distributions, typically follows a broad distribution, deviating from a Poissonian one (Amarasingham et al., 2006). In addition, relative spike timings between coactive neurons are usually within the precision of several milliseconds (Kayser et al., 2010; Wang, 2010). In the case of a broad distribution of ISIs, the mission to grasp gradual changes in temporal and/or rate coding (e.g., changes from an average firing rate of 5–6 Hz), on a timescale of a few ISIs, is a heavy computational mission which might not be satisfactorily resolved. The underlying cause of this computational difficulty is the broad distribution of the ISIs which is overlapped between gradually changed temporal codes or gradually changed rate codes.
To overcome this difficulty we proposed a mechanism which enables the emergence of a sudden leap to synchrony together with or independent of a leap in the firing frequency. This mechanism results in leaps from firing mismatches of several dozens of milliseconds to nearly zero-lag synchronization, and can be accompanied by a sudden frequency multiplication of the neuronal firing rate. These sudden changes occur on a time scale of extremely few ISIs, and are easily detectable as the distributions of the ISIs before and after the leaps are non-overlapping. Hence, one ISI is sufficient to detect the transition without accumulatively estimating the ISI distribution. These fast and robust indicators might be used as reliable information carriers of time-dependent brain activity.
The proposed mechanism also allows for the simultaneous emergence of sudden leaps in rate and temporal synchrony, hence bridging between these two major schools of thought in neuroscience (Eckhorn et al., 1988; Gray et al., 1989; Ahissar et al., 1992; Nicolelis et al., 1995). This mechanism requires recurrent neuronal circuits, and synchrony appears even among neurons which do not share a common drive. Sub-threshold stimulations (e.g., the stimulations to neuron C in Figures 1, 2 and to neuron E in Figure 3) serve as a switch that momentarily closes or opens a loop in the neuronal circuit. The state of the switch changes a global quantity of the network, the GCD of the entire circuit's loops, which determines the state of synchrony (e.g., zero-lag synchrony, cluster synchrony, shifted zero-lag synchrony) (Kanter et al., 2011; Nixon et al., 2012). These demonstrated prototypical examples call for a theoretical examination of more structured scenarios, including multiple leaps in and out of synchrony. In addition, a more realistic biological environment has to be examined containing synaptic noise and adaptation.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Moshe Abeles and Evi Kopelowitz for stimulating discussions. Fruitful computational assistance by Yair Sahar and technical assistance by Hana Arnon are acknowledged. This research was supported by the Ministry of Science and Technology, Israel.
Appendix
Slow divergence out of synchrony
The slow increase in SyncAB (Figure 1D) is analytically examined below for a case of two phase-to-phase neurons, A and B, as depicted in Figure A1. The derivation below is in the spirit of Ermentrout's analysis of coupled type I membranes (Ermentrout, 1996). We first define the following quantities and assumptions:
ti(q) ≡ the timing of the qth spike of neuron i, e.g., tA(0) is the timing of the first spike of neuron A, where the count starts at 0.
Li(q) ≡ neuronal latency of neuron i at its qth spike.
The initial time delays are τAB = τBA ≡ τ.
Assuming initial conditions, t = 0, where both neurons fire simultaneously, i.e., tA(0) ≡ 0, tB(0) = 0.
The spiking times of neurons A and B are given by
Substituting (ii) into (i) and vice versa:
one can find that the solution of these coupled recursive equations is given by:
Consequently, the firing time-gap between the two neurons is given by
Under the assumption of continuous increase in latency and large q
(A1) |
Note that these calculations refer to even values of q. Similar equations can be obtained for odd values of q (not shown). In addition, fluctuations in the latencies may also enhance the deviation from synchronization.
References
- Abbott L. F., Regehr W. G. (2004). Synaptic computation. Nature 431, 796–803 10.1038/nature03010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggelopoulos N. C., Franco L., Rolls E. T. (2005). Object perception in natural scenes: encoding by inferior temporal cortex simultaneously recorded neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 1342–1357 10.1152/jn.00553.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahissar M., Ahissar E., Bergman H., Vaadia E. (1992). Encoding of sound-source location and movement—activity of single neurons and interactions between adjacent neurons in the monkey auditory-cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 67, 203–215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ainsworth M., Lee S., Cunningham M. O., Traub R. D., Kopell N. J., Whittington M. A. (2012). Rates and rhythms: a synergistic view of frequency and temporal coding in neuronal networks. Neuron 75, 572–583 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amarasingham A., Chen T.-L., Geman S., Harrison M. T., Sheinberg D. L. (2006). Spike count reliability and the Poisson hypothesis. J. Neurosci. 26, 801–809 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2948-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aston-Jones G., Segal M., Bloom F. E. (1980). Brain aminergic axons exhibit marked variability in conduction velocity. Brain Res. 195, 215–222 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90880-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballo A. W., Bucher D. (2009). Complex intrinsic membrane properties and dopamine shape spiking activity in a motor axon. J. Neurosci. 29, 5062–5074 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0716-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biederlack J., Castelo-Branco M., Neuenschwander S., Wheeler D. W., Singer W., Nikoliæ D. (2006). Brightness induction: rate enhancement and neuronal synchronization as complementary codes. Neuron 52, 1073–1083 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullock T., Hamstra R., Jr., Scheich H. (1972). The jamming avoidance response of high frequency electric fish. J. Comp. Physiol. 77, 1–22 10.1007/BF00696517 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bullock T. H. (1970). The reliability of neurons. J. Gen. Physiol. 55, 565 10.1085/jgp.55.5.565 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- deCharms C. R., Merzenich M. M. (1996). Primary cortical representation of sounds by the coordination of action-potential timing. Nature 381, 13 10.1038/381610a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Col R., Messlinger K., Carr R. W. (2008). Conduction velocity is regulated by sodium channel inactivation in unmyelinated axons innervating the rat cranial meninges. J. Physiology 586, 1089–1103 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.145383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doiron B., Rinzel J., Reyes A. (2006). Stochastic synchronization in finite size spiking networks. Phys. Rev. E 74, 030903 10.1103/PhysRevE.74.030903 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eckhorn R., Bauer R., Jordan W., Brosch M., Kruse W., Munk M., et al. (1988). Coherent oscillations—a mechanism of feature linking in the visual-cortex—multiple electrode and correlation analyses in the cat. Biol. Cybern. 60, 121–130 10.1007/BF00202899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H. (1993). Thinking about brain cell assemblies. Science 261, 993–993 10.1126/science.8351525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ermentrout B. (1996). Type I membranes, phase resetting curves, and synchrony. Neural Comput. 8, 979–1001 10.1162/neco.1996.8.5.979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gal A., Eytan D., Wallach A., Sandler M., Schiller J., Marom S. (2010). Dynamics of excitability over extended timescales in cultured cortical neurons. J. Neurosci. 30, 16332–16342 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4859-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray C. M., Konig P., Engel A. K., Singer W. (1989). Oscillatory responses in cat visual-cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature 338, 334–337 10.1038/338334a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izhikevich E. M. (2006). Polychronization: computation with spikes. Neural Comput. 18, 245–282 10.1162/089976606775093882 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanter I., Kopelowitz E., Vardi R., Zigzag M., Kinzel W., Abeles M., et al. (2011). Nonlocal mechanism for cluster synchronization in neural circuits. EPL 93 10.1209/0295-5075/93/66001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser C., Logothetis N. K., Panzeri S. (2010). Millisecond encoding precision of auditory cortex neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 16976–16981 10.1073/pnas.1012656107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreuz T., Chicharro D., Andrzejak R. G., Haas J. S., Abarbanel H. D. I. (2009). Measuring multiple spike train synchrony. J. Neurosci. Methods 183, 287–299 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.06.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamme V. A., Spekreijse H. (1998). Neuronal synchrony does not represent texture segregation. Nature 396, 362–366 10.1038/24608 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marom S., Shahaf G. (2002). Development, learning and memory in large random networks of cortical neurons: lessons beyond anatomy. Q. Rev. Biophys. 35, 63–87 10.1017/S0033583501003742 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moortgat K. T., Bullock T. H., Sejnowski T. J. (2000). Precision of the pacemaker nucleus in a weakly electric fish: network versus cellular influences. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 971–983 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morin F. O., Takamura Y., Tamiya E. (2005). Investigating neuronal activity with planar microelectrode arrays: achievements and new perspectives. J. Biosci. Bioeng. 100, 131–143 10.1263/jbb.100.131 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolelis M. A. L., Baccala L. A., Lin R. C. S., Chapin J. K. (1995). Sensorimotor encoding by synchronous neural ensemble activity at multiple levels of the somatosensory system. Science 268, 1353–1358 10.1126/science.7761855 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nixon M., Fridman M., Ronen E., Friesem A. A., Davidson N., Kanter I. (2012). Controlling synchronization in large laser networks. Phys. Rev. Lett. 108, 214101 10.1103/PhysRevLett.108.214101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palm G. (1990). Cell assemblies as a guideline for brain research. Concepts Neurosci. 1, 133–147 [Google Scholar]
- Roelfsema P. R., Lamme V. A., Spekreijse H. (2004). Synchrony and covariation of firing rates in the primary visual cortex during contour grouping. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 982–991 10.1038/nn1304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimokawa T., Shinomoto S. (2009). Estimating instantaneous irregularity of neuronal firing. Neural Comput. 21, 1931–1951 10.1162/neco.2009.08-08-841 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaadia E., Haalman I., Abeles M., Bergman H., Prut Y., Slovin H., et al. (1995). Dynamics of neuronal interactions in monkey cortex in relation to behavioral events. Nature 373, 515–518 10.1038/373515a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vardi R., Guberman S., Goldental A., Kanter I. (2013a). An experimental evidence-based computational paradigm for new logic-gates in neuronal activity. EPL 103:66001 10.1209/0295-5075/103/66001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vardi R., Timor R., Marom S., Abeles M., Kanter I. (2013b). Synchronization by elastic neuronal latencies. Phys. Rev. E 87:012724 10.1103/PhysRevE.87.012724 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vardi R., Wallach A., Kopelowitz E., Abeles M., Marom S., Kanter I. (2012). Synthetic reverberating activity patterns embedded in networks of cortical neurons. EPL 97:66002 10.1209/0295-5075/97/66002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar D. A., Pine J., Potter S. M. (2006). An extremely rich repertoire of bursting patterns during the development of cortical cultures. BMC Neurosci. 7:11 10.1186/1471-2202-7-11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X.-J. (2010). Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol. Rev. 90, 1195–1268 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womelsdorf T., Fries P., Mitra P. P., Desimone R. (2005). Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection. Nature 439, 733–736 10.1038/nature04258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]