Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Oct 30.
Published in final edited form as: Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011 Jul 1;165(7):603–609.

Physical Activity Awareness of British Adolescents

Kirsten Corder 1, Esther MF van Sluijs 1, Ian Goodyer 2, Charlotte L Ridgway 1, Rebekah M Steele 1, Diane Bamber 2, Valerie Dunn 2, Simon J Griffin 1, Ulf Ekelund 1
PMCID: PMC3812705  EMSID: EMS52679  PMID: 24187480

Abstract

Objective

To assess adolescent PA awareness and investigate associations with biological and psychosocial factors.

Design

Cross-sectional from November 2005 to July 2007 (ROOTS study).

Setting

Population-based sample recruited via Cambridgeshire and Suffolk schools (UK).

Participants

N=799 (44% male, 14.5±0.5 years).

Main Exposures

Self-rated PA perception, self-reported psychosocial factors, measured anthropometry.

Outcome Measure

PA measured using accelerometry over five days. ‘Inactive’ defined as accelerometry-measured <60 min/day of at least moderate PA (MVPA). Associations between awareness (agreement between self-rated and accelerometry-measured active/inactive) and potential correlates investigated using multinomial logistic regression.

Results

70% of adolescents were inactive (81% of girls, 56% of boys, OR(95% CI) 3.41(2.41, 4.82)). 53% of all girls (63% of inactive girls) and 34% of all boys (60% of inactive boys) inaccurately rated themselves as active (over-estimators). Compared to girls accurately describing themselves as inactive (29%), girl over-estimators had lower fat mass (OR(95% CI) 0.84(0.70, 0.99)), higher SES (high vs. low 2.4(1.07, 5.32)), reported more parent-support (1.57(1.12, 2.22)) and better family relationships (0.25(0.09, 0.67)). Amongst boys accurately describing themselves as inactive (22%), over-estimators had lower fat mass (0.86(0.77, 0.96)) reported more peer-support (1.75(1.32, 2.30)) and less teasing (0.75(0.61, 0.92)).

Conclusions

A substantial number of adolescents believe themselves to be more physically active than they really are. They maybe unaware of potential health risks, and may be unlikely to participate in PA promotion programs. Increasing information of PA health benefits beyond weight control might help encourage behavior change.

Keywords: Physical activity, perception, awareness, correlates, psychosocial

Introduction

Insufficient physical activity (PA) is a risk factor for obesity and related metabolic disorders in youth 1 and is positively associated with mental health 2 and bone development 3. Adolescence is an important period for promoting PA and preventing an age-related PA decline 4-7. Recent reviews highlight the limited success of PA promotion in youth 8-10; the reasons for this are largely unknown.

One hypothesis for the limited effectiveness of PA promotion is a lack of awareness of health behavior, such as a belief of being more healthy than reality 11. Previous studies have shown that this misperception is relatively common for PA 9,12,13; possibly because PA is a complex behavior where the threshold between healthy and unhealthy may be unclear 12. People may not be aware of what healthy levels are, and changing health recommendations may play a role. Even if someone is aware of what contributes ‘sufficient’ PA, in order to accurately estimate their PA level they need to determine their activity intensity during different behaviors, quantify their activity of this intensity, and sum it over time.

Increased awareness may be a proximal effect of behavior change 14, but those who over-estimate their PA may see no need to change their behavior 12 and not be susceptible to PA promotion. Improving PA awareness may therefore be a crucial initial component of promotion campaigns, although few interventions consider this 9,12,15.

Associations between psycho-social, personal and behavioral factors and PA awareness have been examined in adults 12,16-18 and children, 13 but not adolescents. Most previous studies of PA awareness have focused on adults, and most have used questionnaires to assess ‘true’ PA levels 12,16-18. These studies show that between 46% and 61% of inactive adults over-estimate their PA, and these over-estimators also tended to have more favorable anthropometric characteristics than those realistic about their inactivity 16-18. A recent study of PA awareness using objectively-measured PA in children indicated that 80% of parents of inactive children (those engaged in <60 mins/day of moderate and vigorous PA (MVPA)) wrongly thought that their child was sufficiently active and 40% of inactive children over-estimated themselves 13. Compared to parents who accurately described their children as inactive, over-estimators were more likely to have girls, a child with a lower fat mass or their children reported more parent- and peer-support 13. Child over-estimators reported receiving more peer-support 13.

We aimed to examine adolescents’ PA awareness using objectively-assessed PA, and to investigate associated biological and psychosocial factors.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 799 adolescents, recruited to the ROOTS study, described previously 19; a longitudinal cohort study examining psychopathology during adolescence. Adolescents were assessed at baseline (14 years-old) (T0), and at 18 (T2) and 36 (T3) months post-baseline with repeat psychosocial measures. PA and body composition were assessed six months after baseline (T1), between November 2005 and July 2007.

At T0, 27 secondary schools from Cambridgeshire and Suffolk, UK were approached and 18 agreed to participate. Study information, invitation letters and parent and student consent forms were sent to parents via schools.

Of the 1185 baseline participants, 998 (84%) adolescents and parents completed postal informed consent for the T1 PA assessment and 931 (93% of those consenting) attended a testing session at school. All procedures were explained and participants could choose to decline all or any part of the study. ROOTS was approved by the Cambridge research ethics committee.

Anthropometry

At T1 height (Leicester height measures, Chasmors Ltd., Leicester, UK) weight and body composition (Tanita, type TBF-300A. Tokyo, Japan) were measured at school testing sessions. Validated and published equations were used to calculate fat mass (FM) from impedance 20 and expressed as fat mass index (FMI) (fat mass (kg)/height (m2)) 21. Age and gender were self-reported. Ethnicity was not presented due to homogeneity (92% white).

Objective PA measurement

PA was assessed using the validated Actiheart heart rate and movement sensor (Cambridge Neurotechnology, Papworth, UK) 22-24. The Actiheart was attached to the torso and recorded movement data in 30-s epochs. Volunteers were instructed to wear the monitor continuously, including during sleep and water-based activities for the remainder of the testing day and then four consecutive days, including two weekend days. Participants returned the monitors to school for collection.

To find a MVPA cut-point comparable with the widely used Actigraph accelerometer, a comparison between the Actiheart and Actigraph 7164 was done in a separate group of adolescents who wore both a 7164 Actigraph and an Actiheart while walking and running on a treadmill under controlled conditions 23. The laboratory study suggested a conversion factor of 5 (Actigraph counts = Actiheart counts x 5), which was confirmed in a free-living study of 254 12-17 year-olds 25.

After conversion a custom program removed: data recorded after 11pm and before 6am; periods of ≥60 minutes with continuous zero activity counts and days with <600 minutes of recording (the cut-off for a valid day). Participants with less than three valid days were excluded 26. Time (min/d) spent in MVPA was derived using 2000 (Actigraph) counts/min as the lower threshold 27,28. participants were then classified as ‘active’ or ‘inactive’ using a threshold of an average of 60 min/d MVPA, according to British PA recommendations 29,30.

Self-rated PA

Self-rated PA was assessed at the beginning of the PA measurement session before any assessment of PA or body composition as follows; ‘How physically active would you say you have been during this school term: very inactive, fairly inactive, not inactive/not active, fairly active or very active’. Four PA awareness groups were derived using objective- and self-rated PA (Figure 1 and 2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Classification of 348 adolescent boys into four physical activity awareness groups by adolescent perception

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Classification of 451 adolescent girls into four physical activity awareness groups by adolescent perception.

Socio-economic status

The ACORN index (A Classification Of Residential Neighborhoods) was used as a proxy for socio-economic status (SES). The ACORN index is a postcode (zip code) based system which categorizes UK postcodes into five categories using 125 demographic and 287 lifestyle variables 31. Where SES data was unavailable, the category closest to the mean score for the school was used. Categories were combined to represent low, middle and high SES.

Parent- and peer-support

Parent- and peer-support for PA were assessed by questionnaire32 administered at T1. Responses were on a 5-point scale from ‘never’ to ‘every day’. Composite peer- and parent-support scores were derived from four and five questions respectively. Confirmatory principal-component factor analysis was used to determine internal consistency; Cronbach’s alpha was used to confirm suitability of the groupings (Parent: alpha=0.80; Peer: alpha=0.83). Peer teasing for not being good at PA and sports was assessed using one question, used in the original format.

Moods and Feelings

The Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ) 33, a 33-item self-report measure of depressed mood was administered at T0 and has validity as a screen for adolescents with unipolar depression 33. Respondents rate their symptoms over the last 2-weeks and items are scored on a 4-point Scale (never/mostly/sometimes/never). A summed score was used; higher MFQ scores indicate increased risk for depression 34-36. The internal consistency in this sample was high (Cronbach’s alpha=0.96).

Friendship

This friendship-questionnaire completed at T0 assesses eight items on current friendship satisfaction such as happiness with number of friends 36. A higher value represents a more positive score. Confirmatory factor analysis confirmed the suitability of a single-factor model and Cronbach’s alpha was used to confirm the suitability of the grouping (alpha=0.74).

Family function

The short family assessment device (FAD) 37 is an adolescent-reported 12-item questionnaire measuring general family function, administered at T0. Items are scored on a 4-point scale (as above) and assess family wellbeing such as feeling accepted. A higher value represents a negative score. Confirmatory factor analysis confirmed the suitability of a single-factor model and Cronbach’s alpha was used to confirm the suitability of the grouping (alpha=0.88).

Statistical analysis

Analyses were done using STATA/SE 11 (Statacorp, College Station, TX). Characteristics of those with missing data and between boys and girls were assessed using t-tests or chi-squared tests. Analyses were carried out separately for boys and girls due to sex differences in PA levels and psychosocial variables. All psychosocial variables were checked for collinearity, VIF statistics were <2.0.

Multinomial logistic regression was used to examine differences between inactive adolescents (over-estimators versus realistically inactive) and between active adolescents (under-estimators versus realistically active) for all potential correlates separately.

Multiple multinomial logistic regression was then used to assess the extent to which multiple factors influenced an adolescent’s PA awareness, with ‘Realistic Inactive’ as the reference category, this analysis was also done with ‘Realistic Active’ as the reference category. Variables significantly different in the simple models (including interaction terms) were included in adjusted models and subsequently removed if they did not reach p=0.05. If more than one variable met these criteria, variables were removed one at a time, starting with that with the highest p-value. Analyses were adjusted for school-level clustering.

The main emphasis of this study is on differences between inactive adolescents (over-estimators and realistic inactive) as they are most likely to be targeted in PA promotion, although all groups are included in the models.

Results

Of those attending a testing session, 17 volunteers were not fully tested due to medical reasons or refusal to participate. Due to the late introduction of the perception questionnaire, 799 adolescents (86% tested at T1) are included. No significant differences were found by sex (p=0.69), weight (p=0.08), FMI (p=0.85) or MVPA (p=0.27) between those with complete T1 data and the 133 with missing data, however, those of lower SES (p=0.001) had more missing data.

Figures 1 and 2 show the classification of boys and girls into four PA awareness groups by objectively-measured and self-rated PA. 57% of boys accurately reported their PA (22% realistically inactive; 35% realistically active). Of the 56% of boys who were inactive, 60% over-estimated their PA (34% of all boys) (Figure 1). 42% of girls accurately reported their PA level (29% realistically inactive; 13% realistically active). Of the 81% of girls who were inactive, 65% over-estimated their PA (53% of all girls) (Figure 2). Compared to girls, boys were more likely to be realistically active (OR (95% CI) 3.50 (2.26, 5.42)) and less likely to be realistically inactive (0.29 (0.18, 0.44)) or to over-estimate their PA (0.23 (0.16, 0.33)).

Compared to realistically active boys, those under-estimating their PA had higher BMI and FMI and poorer mood (Table 1). When examining inactive boys, those over-estimating their PA did more MVPA, had higher BMI and FMI, reported higher peer- and parent-support, and lower teasing than those realistic about their inactivity. Of the active girls, those underestimating their PA were older, had higher FMI and reported more teasing and lower peer- and parent-support than those who were realistically active (Table 2). Inactive girls over-estimating their PA level had higher MVPA, SES, peer- and parent-support, friendship and better mood and family scores, but lower FMI and teasing than those realistically inactive. For girls there were significant interactions between SES and peer-support, FAD, friendship and mood (interactions for the simple models shown as eTable 1).

Table 1.

Characteristics of boys (n=348) by physical activity awareness group. Values are mean (SD) unless otherwise stated.

Realistic Active
(RA)
N=123
Mean(SD)
Under-estimator
(UE)
N=31
Mean(SD)
UE vs. RA*
OR (95% CI)
Over-estimator
(OE)
N=117
Mean(SD)
Realistic Inactive
(RI)
N=77
Mean(SD)
OE vs. RI*
OR (95% CI)
Age (years) 14.5 (0.5) 14.5 (0.5) 0.66 (0.28,
1.59)
14.5 (0.5) 14.5 (0.5) 1.37 (0.26,
7.11)
MVPA (min·d−1) 89.3 (20.0) 82.3 (17.5) 0.98 (0.96,
1.01)
41.1 (12.0) 36.4 (1.8) 1.03 (1.00,
1.06)
BMI (kg·m−2) 20.1 (2.8) 21.6 (3.8) 1.13 (1.04,
1.24)
20.1 (3.1) 20.8 (4.0) 0.92 (0.84,
0.99)
Fat mass index
(kg·m−2)
2.4 (1.6) 3.3 (2.2) 1.25 (1.07,
1.45)
2.5 (2.1) 3.2 (2.7) 0.83 (0.75,
0.92)
SES (%)
High 79.8 20.2 57.9 42.1
Middle 74.4 25.6 3.44 (0.61,
19.45)
62.2 37.8 0.68 (0.30,
1.50)
Low 90.9 9.1 2.54 (0.72,
8.99)
70.8 29.2 0.56 (0.21,
1.50)
Peer support 0.73 (0.94) 0.40 (0.92) 0.66 (0.43,
1.02)
0.22 (0.95) −0.35 (0.98) 1.77 (1.33,
2.35)
Peer teasing −0.14 (0.85) 0.06 (0.99) 1.33 (0.98,
1.81)
0.09 (1.04) 0.48 (1.17) 0.73 (0.60,
0.91)
Parent support 0.53 (0.99) 0.33 (1.04) 0.82 (0.55,
1.21)
0.10 (1.03) −0.37 (0.94) 1.58 (1.02,
2.45)
FAD 0.017 (0.75) 0.04 (0.83) 1.03 (0.72,
1.45)
−0.08 (0.99) 0.15 (0.91) 0.75 (0.51,
1.11)
Friendship 0.03 (0.77) 0.17 (0.69) 1.31 (0.75,
2.28)
−0.08 (0.75) −0.2 (0.73) 1.28 (0.79,
2.06)
Mood 12.0 (7.2) 15.9 (10.1) 1.05 (1.02,
1.08)
12.3 (8.2) 13.5 (8.9) 0.98 (0.94,
1.03)
*

reference category. MVPA moderate and vigorous physical activity. SES, socio-economic status derived from home post-code (ACORN category) scored 1 lowest SES to 3 highest. Analyses clustered by school. FAD Family Assessment Device. Significant interactions present for girls between SES and peer support, FAD, friendship, mood. Significant interactions between SES and peer support, FAD, friendship, mood.

Table 2.

Characteristics of girls (n=451) by physical activity awareness group. Values are mean (SD) unless otherwise stated.

Realistic Active
(RA)
N=58
Mean(SD)
Under-estimator
(UE)
N=27
Mean(SD)
UE vs. RA*
OR (95% CI)
Over-estimator
(OE)
N=237
Mean(SD)
Realistic Inactive
(RI)
N=129
Mean(SD)
OE vs. RI*
OR (95% CI)
Age (years) 14.4 (0.50) 14.6 (0.10) 4.03 (1.19,
13.68)
14.6 (0.52) 14.6 (0.82) 0.92 (0.42,
2.00)
MVPA (min·d−1) 78.1 (16.0) 78.2 (15.8) 1.00 (0.98,
1.03)
36.6 (12.6) 31.9 (13.0) 1.03 (1.01,
1.05)
BMI (kg·m−2) 20.7 (2.7) 21.3 (2.3) 1.06 (0.99,
1.14)
20.7 (3.4) 21.6 (3.8) 0.93 (0.85,
1.02)
Fat mass index
(kg·m−2)
5.0 (2.1) 6.0 (1.9) 1.18 (1.03,
1.34)
5.2 (2.4) 6.1 (3.0) 0.88 (0.79,
0.99)
SES (%)
High 70.5 29.6 68.7 31.3
Middle 65.0 35.0 1.08 (0.23,
4.95)
64.6 35.4 2.90 (1.63,
5.14)
Low 66.7 33.3 0.84 (0.30,
2.34)
39.5 60.5 3.39 (2.12,
5.40)
Peer support 0.07 (0.85) −0.6 (0.85) 0.40 (0.20,
0.76)
−0.04 (0.84) −0.60 (0.89) 2.08 (1.64,
2.63)
Peer teasing −0.32 (0.57) 0.19 (1.31) 2.00 (1.13,
3.54)
−0.12 (0.88) 0.09 (1.12) 0.82 (0.66,
1.03)
Parent support 0.16 (0.80) −0.69 (0.92) 0.35 (0.18,
0.67)
0.08 (0.88) −0.55 (0.93) 2.21 (1.72,
2.83)
FAD −0.07 (0.87) 0.24 (0.90) 1.42 (0.83,
2.43)
−0.20 (0.92) 0.15 (1.01) 0.68 (0.55,
0.83)
Friendship 0.09 (0.79) −0.14 (0.72) 0.66 (0.42,
1.05)
0.04 (0.73) −0.18 (0.77) 1.48 (1.04,
2.12)
Mood 17.0 (12.3) 19.0 (13.0) 1.02 (0.96,
1.07)
14.8 (9.5) 17.9 (10.5) 0.97 (0.95,
0.99)
*

reference category. MVPA moderate and vigorous physical activity. SES, socio-economic status derived from home post-code (ACORN category) scored 1 lowest SES to 3 highest. Analyses clustered by school. FAD Family Assessment Device. Significant interactions present for girls between SES and peer support, FAD, friendship, mood. Significant interactions between SES and peer support, FAD, friendship, mood.

Table 3 shows results of the final models of multiple correlates on PA awareness. For inactive boys, these results were similar to the simple models. Compared to girls who were realistic about being inactive, those over-estimating their PA were more likely to be of higher SES, to report higher parent-support and to have a lower FMI and FAD; significant interactions between SES/peer-support and SES/FAD were also included in the final model for girls.

Table 3.

Results of Multinomial Logistic Regression of multiple factors on adolescent physical activity awareness for boys and girls.

OE (reference RI) OE (reference RA)

OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Boys
Fat mass index 0.86 0.77, 0.96 0.97 0.85, 1.11
Peer support 1.75 1.32, 2.30 0.55 0.40, 0.76
Teasing 0.75 0.61, 0.92 1.31 0.97, 1.76
N 117 117
Girls
SES Low#
Middle 2.9 0.95, 8.74 3.8 1.51, 9.40
High 2.4 1.07, 5.32 5.2 2.18, 12.2
Fat mass index 0.84 0.70, 0.99 1.03 0.89, 1.21
Parent support 1.57 1.12, 2.22 0.86 0.59, 1.26
Peer support 1.14 0.61, 2.14 1.04 0.43, 2.50
FAD 0.25 0.09, 0.67 1.15 0.59, 2.24
SES*Peer support Low#
Middle 2.50 1.02, 6.05 0.88 0.21, 3.68
High 1.41 0.63, 3.15 0.84 0.28, 0.25
SES*FAD Low#
Middle 3.67 0.90, 15.0 0.82 0.32, 2.10
High 4.04 1.45, 11.25 0.63 0.23, 1.68
N 237 237

Reference category for analysis is Realistic Inactive (RI), Over-estimators (OE), Boys N=77, Girls N=129, or Realistic Active (RA) Boys N=123, Girls N=58, OR; Odds Ratio, 95% CI; 95% confidence interval. SES, socio-economic status derived from home post-code (ACORN category) scored 1 lowest SES to 3 highest

#

reference category is low SES. FAD Family Assessment Device. Analysis clustered by school. Results from multinomial logistic regression across all four groups of physical activity awareness including N=348 adolescent boys and N=451 adolescent girls.

Discussion

Approximately 70% of these adolescents (81% of girls and 56% of boys) were not meeting PA guidelines of 60 min/day MVPA 29,30. 65% of inactive girls and 60% of inactive boys wrongly thought that they were ‘active’.

PA levels were low, only 44% boys and 19% of girls were meeting recommendations. This is less than the 81% and 60% of 15 year-old boys and girls respectively who were shown to be meeting these recommendations in a recent European study 5, yet these British adolescents appear to be more active than the 11% and 3% of US boys and girls aged 12 to 15 shown to be meeting these guidelines 38. Although this may indicate true differences, the results are not directly comparable due to different PA data preparation methods. The number of adolescents meeting PA guidelines depends on the cut-points used to define MVPA.

This is the second study investigating PA awareness in youth and the adolescents in this study appeared to be less aware than 9-10 year old children in the previous study, where 40% of inactive children perceived themselves as active 13. This could be due to social desirability bias, being unaware of what constitutes ‘physical activity’ or that children in the previous study were more active. However, over-estimation in these adolescents seems to be similar to the 46% to 61% of inactive adults over-estimating their PA in previous studies 12,16-18.

Girls were more likely to over-estimate their PA than boys, possibly because social desirability bias may be more prevalent for girls as seen with dietary reports 39. Compared to adolescents who were realistic about their inactivity, those over-estimating their PA level had a lower FMI. This result supports previous evidence suggesting that over-estimators may assume that they are sufficiently active because of their favorable body composition 16,17,40.

Adolescents who over-estimated their PA level reported higher peer- and parent-support than those realistic about their inactivity and boys overestimating their PA reported less teasing than those realistic about their inactivity. Although these adolescents appear to be receiving support for PA, they have still been classified as ‘inactive’ so this does not necessarily mean that this activity is at recommended levels. Although more support appears to be associated with a lack of awareness, this support could confer other benefits, such as reduced risk of depressive symptoms 41. For inactive girls, FAD and parent/peer-support were positively associated with over-estimation, this may be imitation of the familial and/or peer group pattern amongst relatively satisfied and well adjusted teenagers. Although parent- and peer-support are positive correlates of PA42,43 our findings suggest that they may be associated with a lack of PA awareness. Therefore, intervention components targeting PA awareness may need to use different strategies than those directly targeting PA.

Our proxy SES measure appears to be important in relation to awareness among girls, but not boys. Significant interactions between SES/peer-support and SES/FAD were included in the final model for girls, supporting previous research that girls and those of lower SES tend to have ‘higher’ scores on measures of psychosocial difficulties. Within the realistic inactive group, peer-support/FAD scores were more positive for lower SES girls. Conversely for over-estimators, peer-support/FAD scores were more negative for girls of lower SES. Although unavailable, inclusion of individual SES indicators such as parental education would be useful to further explore this association between SES and PA awareness. Peer- and parent-support appear important in relation to awareness and as these associations appear to differ by SES, interventions aiming to target PA awareness via support should perhaps tailor strategies by SES.

Education regarding benefits of PA other than for weight control may play an important role in PA promotion among adolescents as there are additional health outcomes including features of the metabolic syndrome 1 and bone health 3 which are associated with youth PA and too much focus may have been given to the importance of PA for weight control relative to other benefits.

The majority of inactive adolescents over-estimate their PA level and may be less likely to want to increase their PA. Improving awareness could be an important component of adolescent PA promotion. Self-monitoring and feedback using pedometers may be effective to promote PA awareness 44,45. To determine the most effective types of feedback and their effect on awareness and PA, further research studying changes in objectively measured PA behavior is needed 9,15. More work is also required studying how improving awareness helps to increase PA, and whether it may improve intervention effectiveness.

Study strengths and limitations

We are unaware of any other studies assessing PA awareness in adolescents. This is especially important considering the PA decline observed throughout adolescence 6 suggesting this as a valuable time for PA promotion 9. It is unknown how to best assess PA awareness, and the measure used here is the only one previously used in youth 13 and leaves interpretation up to the participant. Although responses may be influenced by awareness and knowledge of PA guidelines, we consider this measure appropriate for this population. Our results are somewhat dependent on the dichotomization of self-rated PA, specifically the classification of those answering ‘not active/not inactive’ as ‘inactive’ (12% boys, 19% girls). These adolescents were hypothesized to be ambivalent about their PA level and due to reporting bias, more likely to be inactive than active in reality. Further, due to the average objectively measured PA level in this group being ’inactive’, participants identifying as similar to their peers were also most likely inactive. Study strengths include objective PA measurement to classify awareness. Objective data should more accurately represent ‘true’ PA than a questionnaire and should overcome correlated error from two self-reported datasets 46. Our results rely on the thresholds used to determine active/inactive, however, results were similar when using 30 min/day MVPA to define active/inactive (eTable 2).

This is a cross-sectional analysis and we are unable to determine the direction of association and cannot infer causality. We must acknowledge the limitation that mood, FAD and friendship were measured six months before PA and they may have changed. There was some differential drop out, exacerbated by late introduction of the perception questionnaire with lower SES adolescents having more missing data, limiting generalisability. Cambridgeshire and Suffolk may not be representative of the whole UK and this group had slightly lower levels of overweight than adolescents from the East of England 47.

Conclusion

Most inactive adolescents wrongly consider themselves as sufficiently active and adolescents with a lower FMI inaccurately describe themselves as ‘active’. PA awareness may be especially important when targeting PA promotion interventions for adolescent girls. Increasing awareness of PA health benefits beyond weight control, might reverse misperceptions and encourage behavior change.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary tables

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge Debra Russo, Kate Westgate, Alice Tompson, Lisa Purslow, and Cheryl Kidney for help with data collection and Stephen Sharp for statistical support. We also acknowledge Jeannette Brodbeck for assistance preparing the psychosocial measures for analysis. We are also grateful to all schools and volunteers participating in the ROOTS study. ROOTS data collection was supported by the Wellcome Trust (grant no: 074296/Z/04/Z), Medical Research Council Epidemiology Unit and Medical Research Council Human Nutrition Research. This research was completed within the NIHR Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care for Cambridgeshire and Peterborough.

KC did the data analysis and drafted the manuscript. IG, DB and VJD were responsible for the baseline ROOTS study. KC, DB, RS, CR and UE organized and carried out the PA data collection. EVS and SG assisted with formulation of the research question. All authors contributed to, and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Footnotes

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

References

  • 1.Steele R, Brage S, Corder K, Wareham N, Ekelund U. Physical activity, cardiorespiratory fitness, and the metabolic syndrome in youth. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2008;105(1):342–351. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00072.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mutrie N, Parfitt G. Physical activity and its link with mental, social and moral health in young people. In: Biddle S, Sallis J, Cavill N, editors. Young and Active? Young people and health-enhancing physical activity - evidence and implications. Health Education Authority; London: 1998. pp. 49–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Janz K. Physical activity and bone development during childhood and adolescence. Implications for the prevention of osteoporosis. Minerva Pediatr. 2002 Apr;54(2):93–104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Janz K, Burns T, Levy S. Tracking of Activity and Sedentary Behaviors in Childhood. The Iowa Bone Development Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2005;29(3):171–178. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2005.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Riddoch CJ, Bo Andersen L, Wedderkopp N, et al. Physical activity levels and patterns of 9- and 15-yr-old European children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2004 Jan;36(1):86–92. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000106174.43932.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Nader PR, Bradley RH, Houts RM, McRitchie SL, O’Brien M. Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity from ages 9 to 15 years. Jama. 2008 Jul 16;300(3):295–305. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.3.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Leslie E, Owen N, Salmon J, Bauman A, Sallis J, Kai Lo S. Insufficiently Active Australian College Students: Perceived Personal, Social, and Environmental Influences. Preventive medicine. 1999;28:20–27. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1998.0375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Salmon J, Booth ML, Phongsavan P, Murphy N, Timperio A. Promoting physical activity participation among children and adolescents. Epidemiol Rev. 2007;29:144–159. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxm010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.van Sluijs E, McMinn A, Griffin S. Effectiveness of interventions to promote physical activity in children and adolescents: systematic review of controlled trials. British Medical Journal. 2007;6(335) doi: 10.1136/bmj.39320.843947.BE. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dobbins M, De Corby K, Robeson P, Husson H, Tirilis D. School-based physical activity programs for promoting physical activity and fitness in children and adolescents aged 6-18. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2009 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD007651. Art. No.: CD007651. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD007651(1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Oenema A, Brug J. Feedback strategies to raise awareness of personal dietary intake: results of a randomised controlled trial. Preventive medicine. 2003;36(4):429–439. doi: 10.1016/s0091-7435(02)00043-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ronda G, van Assema P, Brug J. Stages of change, psychological factors and awareness of physical activity levels in the Netherlands. Health Promotion International. 2001;16(4):305–314. doi: 10.1093/heapro/16.4.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Corder K, van Sluijs EM, McMinn AM, Ekelund U, Cassidy A, Griffin SJ. Perception versus reality awareness of physical activity levels of British children. Am J Prev Med. 2010 Jan;38(1):1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bauman A, Bowles HR, Huhman M, et al. Testing a hierarchy-of-effects model: pathways from awareness to outcomes in the VERB campaign 2002-2003. Am J Prev Med. 2008 Jun;34(6 Suppl):S249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Watkinson C, van Sluijs EM, Sutton S, Marteau T, Griffin SJ. Randomised controlled trial of the effects of physical activity feedback on awareness and behaviour in UK adults: the FAB study protocol [ISRCTN92551397] BMC Public Health. 10(1):144. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lechner L, Bolman C, van Dijke M. Factors related to misperception of physical activity in The Netherlands and implications for health promotion programmes. Health Promotion International. 2006;21(2):104–112. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dal011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.van Sluijs E, Griffin S, van Poppel M. A cross-sectional study of awareness of physical activity: associations with personal, behavioural and psychosocial factors. International Journal of Behavioural Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2007;8(4) doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-4-53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Watkinson C, van Sluijs EM, Sutton S, Hardeman W, Corder K, Griffin SJ. Overestimation of physical activity level is associated with lower BMI: a cross-sectional analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7:68. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Goodyer IM, Croudace T, Dunn V, Herbert J, Jones PB. Cohort profile: risk patterns and processes for psychopathology emerging during adolescence: the ROOTS project. Int J Epidemiol. 2010 Apr;39(2):361–369. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyp173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Tyrrell VJ, Richards G, Hofman P, Gillies GF, Robinson E, Cutfield WS. Foot-to-foot bioelectrical impedance analysis: a valuable tool for the measurement of body composition in children. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2001 Feb;25(2):273–278. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wells JC, Cole TJ. Adjustment of fat-free mass and fat mass for height in children aged 8 y. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2002 Jul;26(7):947–952. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Corder K, Brage S, Mattocks C, et al. Comparison of two methods to assess PAEE during six activities in children. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2007;39(12):2180–2188. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e318150dff8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Corder K, Brage S, Wareham NJ, Ekelund U. Comparison of PAEE from Combined and Separate Heart Rate and Movement Models in Children. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2005;37(10):1761–1767. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000176466.78408.cc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Brage S, Brage N, Franks P, Ekelund U, Wareham NJ. Reliability and validity of the combined heart rate and movement sensor Actiheart. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2005;59:561–570. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ridgway CL, Brage S, Sharp S, et al. Does birth weight influence physical activity in youth? A combined analysis of four studies using objectively measured physical activity. In submission. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 26.Mattocks C, Ness A, Leary S, et al. Use of Accelerometers in a Large Field-Based Study of Childen: Protocols, Design Issues, and Effects on Precision. Journal of Physical Activity and Health. 2008;5(S1):S98–S111. doi: 10.1123/jpah.5.s1.s98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Brage S, Brage N, Wedderkopp N, Froberg K. Reliability and validity of the Computer Science and Applications Accelerometer in a Mechanical Setting. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science. 2003;7(2):101–119. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Trost S, Ward D, Moorehead S, Watson P, Riner W, Burke J. Validity of the computer and science and applications (CSA) activity monitor in children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1998 Apr;30(4):629–633. doi: 10.1097/00005768-199804000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Biddle S, Gorely T, Stensel D. Health-enhancing physical activity and sedentary behaviour in children and adolescents. Journal of Sports Sciences. 2004;22:679–701. doi: 10.1080/02640410410001712412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Strong W, Malina R, Blimkie C, et al. Evidence based physical activity for school-age youth. Journal of Pediatrics. 2005 Jun;146:732–737. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2005.01.055. 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.ACORN www.caci.co.uk/acorn.
  • 32.Prochaska JJ, Rodgers MW, Sallis JF. Association of parent and peer support with adolescent physical activity. Research quarterly for exercise and sport. 2002;73(2):206–210. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2002.10609010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Daviss W, Birmaher B, Melhem N, Axelson D, Michaels S, Brent D. Criterion validity of the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire for depressive episodes in clinic and non-clinic subjects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(9):927–934. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01646.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Goodyer I, Croudace T, Ban M, Herbert J. The serotonin transporter (5HTTLPR), morning cortisol and subsequent depression in adolescents. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;1(195):39–45. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.054775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Goodyer I, Herbert J, Tamplin A, Altham P. Recent life events, cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone and the onset of major depression in high-risk adolescents. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:499–504. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.6.499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Goodyer IM, Herbert J, Tamplin A, Secher SM, Pearson J. Short-term outcome of major depression: II. Life events, family dysfunction, and friendship difficulties as predictors of persistent disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997 Apr;36(4):474–480. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Tamplin A, Goodyer IM. Family functioning in adolescents at high and low risk for major depressive disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;10(3):170–179. doi: 10.1007/s007870170023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Masse LC, Tilert T, McDowell M. Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008 Jan;40(1):181–188. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31815a51b3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hebert JR, Ma Y, Clemow L, et al. Gender differences in social desirability and social approval bias in dietary self-report. Am J Epidemiol. 1997 Dec 15;146(12):1046–1055. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Corder K, Brage S, Wright A, et al. Physical Activity Energy Expenditure of Adolescents in India. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2010 Feb 4; doi: 10.1038/oby.2010.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Barrera M, Jr., Garrison-Jones C. Family and peer social support as specific correlates of adolescent depressive symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1992 Feb;20(1):1–16. doi: 10.1007/BF00927113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sallis JF, Prochaska JJ, Taylor WC. A review of correlates of physical activity of children and adolescents. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2000 May;32(5):963–975. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200005000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.van der Horst K, Chin A, Paw M, Twisk J, van Mechelen W. A Brief Review on Correlates of Physical Activity and Sedentariness in Youth. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2007;39(8):1241–1250. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e318059bf35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lubans DR, Morgan PJ, Tudor-Locke C. A systematic review of studies using pedometers to promote physical activity among youth. Preventive medicine. 2009 Apr;48(4):307–315. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, Lane DJ, Mahler HI, Kulik JA. Using UV photography to reduce use of tanning booths: a test of cognitive mediation. Health Psychol. 2005 Jul;24(4):358–363. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.4.358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Corder K, Ekelund U. Physical Activity. In: Armstrong N, van Mechelen W, editors. Paediatric Exercise Science and Medicine. 2nd ed Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.The Information Centre . Statistics on Obesity, Physical Activity and Diet: England, 2006. Government Statistical Service; [Accessed 21st November 2007, 2007 Online]. 2006. Report. Available at. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary tables

RESOURCES