Abstract
The ability of animals to remember the what, where and when of a unique past event is used as an animal equivalent to human episodic memory. We currently view episodic memory as reconstructive, with an event being remembered in the context in which it took place. Importantly, this means that the components of a what, where, when memory task should be dissociable (e.g. what would be remembered to a different degree than when). We tested this hypothesis by training hummingbirds to a memory task, where the location of a reward was specified according to colour (what), location (where), and order and time of day (when). Although hummingbirds remembered these three pieces of information together more often than expected, there was a hierarchy as to how they were remembered. When seemed to be the hardest to remember, while errors relating to what were more easily corrected. Furthermore, when appears to have been encoded as a combination of time of day and sequence information. As hummingbirds solved this task using reconstruction of different memory components (what, where and when), we suggest that similar deconstructive approaches may offer a useful way to compare episodic and episodic-like memories.
Keywords: cognition, hummingbird, what–where–when, memory reconstruction, episodic-like memory
1. Introduction
Episodic memory is the system by which humans recall their past experiences and it is this experience that distinguishes it from semantic memory. While semantic memory covers ‘known’ memories, such as remembering that the Battle of Hastings took place in 1066, an episodic memory is one for which the individual has a sense of the event having occurred within a personal past [1], for example reminiscing about sitting in a classroom looking out of the window while learning about the Battle of Hastings. Given that the focus of this definition is on the individual's subjective experience, it has been suggested that episodic memory relies on faculties such as autonoetic consciousness (i.e. an ability to imagine ourselves in the past) and a sense of subjective time [2]. This renders episodic memory either a uniquely human ability or at least only (currently) accessible to study in humans.
Given this difficulty in testing episodic memory in animals, researchers have redefined the problem under the banner of ‘episodic-like’ memory [3]. This has been defined as the ability to integrate the what, the where and the when aspects of a unique past event and to use that memory flexibly to guide behaviour [4]. As a result of using this definition, the ability to act on these three components of a past event simultaneously has now been demonstrated in a range of species (including scrub jays, Aphelocoma coerulescens: [3], chickadees, Poecile atricapillus: [5], meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus: [6] and rats Rattus norvegicus: [7]).
Although there is considerable debate regarding the extent to which episodic-like memory resembles episodic memory [8–10], the central tenet of episodic memory, the experience of the individual, is currently untestable and seems likely to remain so in the foreseeable future. There are, however, other features of episodic memory that are amenable to investigation within an episodic-like framework. For example, episodic memory in humans is regarded as reconstructive [11–13]: that is events are not stored as a whole in memory as by a video recorder, but rather via a system in which the elements of a memory are stored separately and recombined to create the event as it is recollected. This leads to various predictable errors when memory fails [14–16]. By contrast, episodic-like memory investigations typically require an animal to demonstrate that it can remember the three memory components together. If the same mechanisms underpin both episodic-like memory and episodic memory, we suggest that the components of memory of the former should also be dissociable and recombined at the point of recall. This reconstruction may on occasion be imperfect or incomplete, as is often the case for human memory, and thus the errors made in episodic-like memory tasks may be as informative as animals' successes. If animals make errors in episodic-like memory tasks that are comparable to the errors humans make when using episodic memory, perhaps these two systems are similar in form as well as function.
Hummingbirds provide a useful model system for studying the interplay of different sorts of information in memory, as they can remember numerous aspects of the flowers from which they feed, including their colour [17,18], location [19–22] and when they were last visited [23,24]: what, where and when. Here, we investigated whether in a system where the what, where and when components of memories can be experimentally manipulated, whether hummingbirds remembered all three pieces of information together as a whole or whether errors tended to reflect failure to recall one aspect of what, where and when (Experiment 1). If hummingbirds do not make errors at random with respect to what, where and when, then perhaps these pieces of information are stored separately in memory, in a manner consistent with the reconstructive model of episodic memory. In particular, we predicted that when would be the piece of information causing the most frequent errors, as this is frequently the most difficult aspect of episodic-like memory to demonstrate [25,26]. In our first experiment, birds might remember the temporal (when) component either by the sequence of rewarded flowers or by the time of day. In our second experiment, we tested whether the birds use time of day, sequence or a combination of the two.
2. Material and methods
(a). Subjects
The subjects in these experiments were 18 free-living male rufous hummingbirds defending feeding territories along the Westcastle Valley, in the Eastern range of the Rocky Mountains (49°21′ N, 114°25′ W), Alberta, Canada (12 in Experiment 1; six in Experiment 2). Each territory was centred on a single hummingbird feeder, containing 14% sucrose solution. Birds were marked on their breast feathers with a small amount of non-toxic ink, to allow individuals to be identified. Observations were conducted between 07.30 and 19.30 (Mountain Standard Time). Experiment 1 was conducted in June–July 2005 and June 2006, and Experiment 2 from June to July 2008. All work was carried out under permit from Environment Canada and Alberta Sustainable Resource Development with the ethical approval of the University of Lethbridge Animal Welfare Committee.
(b). Experiment 1
(i). Training
There were 2 days of training prior to the experimental procedure. On the first of these days, we presented each bird with an array of four flowers, coloured blue, red, pink or purple and arranged in a rough 60 × 60 cm square. One flower was filled with sucrose and the remaining three were filled with water, which the birds find unpalatable. The bird was allowed to visit this array until he had fed from the sucrose-filled flower six times, after which time the flowers were removed and the bird's feeder was replaced. This completed the training for the ‘Morning’ session. The ‘Afternoon’ training began at least 4 hours later. The bird was presented with the second array of four flowers of the same colours in the same relative positions but in a new location at least 10 m from the location of the morning array. For the afternoon array, the rewarded flower was not of the same colour as that rewarded in the morning array and the remaining three flowers contained water. Again, the bird was allowed to feed from the rewarded flower six times.
(ii). Experimental procedure
Following training, in the morning (at any time between 07.30 and 11.00, with the time broadly consistent across days for each bird), we removed a bird's feeder and presented him with both the morning and afternoon arrays of flowers simultaneously, arranged as they had been the previous day. The only flower of the eight to contain sucrose was the one that had been rewarded in the previous morning's training. All the remaining seven flowers contained water. The bird was allowed to visit any of the flowers until he had made six visits to the sucrose-filled flower, which was refilled after each visit, at which point both arrays were removed and the feeder was replaced. Four hours after both arrays were removed, they were returned but with the afternoon flower being the only flower containing reward. Again, the bird was allowed to visit flowers until he had visited the sucrose-filled flower six times. His feeder was then returned for the remainder of the day.
All visits to all flowers were recorded at both morning and afternoon sessions. The number of sessions (morning and afternoon included) experienced by each male varied from 5 to 17 (median = 12).
In this design, there were three pieces of information that a bird needed to use in order to locate the rewarded flower: What: the flower's colour; Where: the array in which the flower is located and When: whether it is the morning or the afternoon. We could, therefore, look at whether these components of memory are separable by looking at the errors birds made, as they can be classified according to which of these pieces of information is missing from a bird's choice (figure 1). What errors were those where the bird chose the correct array at the correct time of day but a flower of the wrong colour (chance = 0.375); where errors were those where the bird chose a flower of the correct colour at the correct time but in the wrong array (chance = 0.125); when errors were those where the bird chose the flower of the correct colour and in the correct array, but that was the flower that was rewarded at the alternative time (chance = 0.125); all-wrong errors were those where the bird chose a flower of the wrong colour, in the wrong place and at the wrong time (chance = 0.250). Correct choices were those in which the bird chose the rewarded flower (chance = 0.125).
There are two different aspects of birds' errors that we can examine in this experiment: the kinds of error made and how the birds went about correcting them. To determine whether some errors were easier to correct than the others, we looked at birds' subsequent choices after making an error, either up to the point where they located the rewarded flower or to their fourth choice within a trial (birds failed to find the rewarded flower within the first four visits on only 20 of a total of 134 trials). We then constructed a partition tree (R package rpart) modelling whether or not an error was corrected on a subsequent choice as a function of trial number, visit number within a trial, the type of error made on the last visit and the individual.
(c). Experiment 2
In Experiment 1, when is defined both by time of day and sequence (i.e. the afternoon comes after the morning). To assess how birds were using these two components to estimate when, we conducted the second experiment in which we presented the birds with the same arrays as used in Experiment 1 in morning and afternoon sessions for 5 days. For Experiment 2, however, we included a test phase whereby birds were given an Early test: 2 hours before a morning session; a Midday test: in-between the morning and afternoon sessions; and a Late test: 2 hours after the afternoon session was completed.
Birds were allowed to make one visit to the arrays during each of these early/midday/late tests, after which all flowers were removed and the feeder was replaced. All flowers were empty during these tests. To reiterate, during testing birds continued to experience morning and afternoon trainings at the usual times and birds only experienced one test session per day. The order of the tests was randomized between birds and each bird completed each test only once.
Depending on whether birds used sequential or time of day information, or some combination of the two, birds' choices at these tests should differ and the explanations for how birds might choose a flower are as follows:
(i) Birds always chose randomly between the morning and afternoon flower, irrespective of time (chance). Prediction: birds would choose the morning and afternoon flowers equally across all three tests.
(ii) Birds always chose the morning flower, irrespective of time (all AM). Prediction: birds would choose the morning flower across all three tests.
(iii) Birds always chose the afternoon rewarded flower, irrespective of time (all PM). Prediction: birds would choose the afternoon flower across all three tests.
(iv) Birds always chose the flower corresponding to the time of day nearest to that to which they were trained (time of day). Prediction: birds would choose the morning flower in the early test, show no preference in the midday test and would choose the afternoon flower in the late test.
(v) Birds always chose the next flower due to be rewarded in the sequence (sequence: avoid previous). Prediction: birds would choose the morning flower in the early test, the afternoon flower in the midday test and the morning flower in the late test.
(vi) Birds always avoided the next flower due to be rewarded in the sequence (sequence: avoid next). Prediction: birds would choose the afternoon flower in the early test (as the next reward would be the morning flower), the morning flower in the midday test (as the next reward would be the afternoon flower) and the afternoon flower in the late test (as the next reward would be the morning flower the following day).
(vii) Birds combined sequential and time of day information to make their flower choices (mixed). Prediction: birds would choose the afternoon flower in the early test (as the morning flower would be rewarded at the time experienced during morning training and the rewarded flower alternates), equally between the morning and afternoon flowers in the midday test (as this test is midway between two known but different rewards) and the morning flower in the late test (as the last reward experienced was that of the flower from the afternoon and the reward alternates).
As the aim of these tests was to compare which of the two flowers (morning or afternoon) birds expected to contain the reward at untrained times of day, we modelled this test as a binary choice, where birds could either choose to visit the morning or the afternoon flower. This meant that we had to exclude cases in which birds made their first choice in a test to a flower that was never rewarded, which accounted for only one of 18 test trials. We used a likelihood approach to compare the likelihood of each hypothesis given the data [27]. We calculated the probability of observing the birds' choices under each of these scenarios and compared the negative log-likelihoods (−LL), with the smallest −LL denoting the most likely of the competing hypotheses [27].
3. Results
(a). Experiment 1
Male rufous hummingbirds were able to learn a what, where, when task, making their first choice to the correct flower significantly more often than expected by chance (t11 = 6.16, p < 0.001). Correspondingly, birds made all-wrong errors significantly less often than expected by chance (t11 = 9.32, p < 0.001). Critically, the prevalence of what, where and when errors differed. As predicted, when errors occurred more often than expected by chance (t11 = 3.09, p = 0.010). Furthermore, what errors occurred less often than chance and where errors occurred at chance levels: (what: t11 = 2.41, p = 0.035, where: t11 = 0.71, p = 0.494; figure 2).
Not only were errors non-random, when males made a what error but also they were much more likely to visit the correct flower on their next choice (86% of choices following a what error) than when they had made a where, when or all-wrong error (25% of these errors; figure 3).
(b). Experiment 2
In early tests, the birds' chose the flower rewarded in the afternoon significantly more often than expected by chance: all six birds chose the afternoon rewarded flower (two-tailed test binomial test: N = 6, test-proportion = 0.5, exact p = 0.031; figure 4). In the midday tests, the birds' performance did not differ significantly from chance: four birds chose the flower rewarded in the morning and two chose the flower rewarded in the afternoon (two-tailed binomial test: N = 6, test-proportion = 0.5, exact p = 0.688). In the late tests, birds tended to choose the flower rewarded in the morning (two-tailed test binomial test: N = 5, test-proportion = 0.5, exact p = 0.063): five birds chose the flower rewarded in the morning while the sixth bird chose a flower that was never rewarded (we excluded this choice from the analysis). When we compared the birds' performance across the early, midday and late tests with our hypotheses, their choices were most consistent with those predicted by the mixed hypothesis (−LL = 0.678; table 1).
Table 1.
hypothesis | expected probability of choosing the morning flower |
negative log-likelihood (−LL) |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
early | midday | late | sum | |
(1) chance | 0.50 | 0.50 | 0.50 | 3.94 |
(2) all AM | 0.99 | 0.99 | 0.99 | 14.90 |
(3) all PM | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 16.90 |
(4) time of day | 0.99 | 0.50 | 0.01 | 22.60 |
(5) sequence (avoid previous) | 0.99 | 0.01 | 0.99 | 18.90 |
(6) sequence (avoid next) | 0.01 | 0.99 | 0.01 | 12.90 |
(7) mixed | 0.01 | 0.50 | 0.99 | 0.68 |
4. Discussion
Rufous hummingbirds' errors on this what, where, when task were not random. Errors very rarely represented a failure to remember any aspect of the rewarded flower (i.e. all-wrong errors), rather their errors generally represented the failure to remember one aspect of the what, where or when. In particular, birds were most likely to make errors regarding when a flower should be rewarded. This is consistent with other studies, where the time component appears to be the most difficult for many animals to learn [25,26]. Furthermore, birds made what errors less frequently than expected by chance. It therefore appears that these birds store what, where and when as separate pieces of information, as is thought to be the case for human episodic memory [13].
Not only were birds less likely to make what errors, when these errors did occur they were more likely to be followed by a visit to the correct flower. Here, what is signalled by flower colour, and previous research has suggested that while hummingbirds can use colour to direct foraging when no other cue is available [17,18], colour information tends to be overshadowed by spatial information [22,24,28]. The ready correction of what errors would suggest that birds knew the correct flower colour but preferred to use other information first. It is worth noting that not all what errors in this experiment were equivalent: birds either chose the flower colour rewarded at the alternative time (one of three flowers) or a flower colour that was never rewarded (two of three flowers). It seems that there is a difference in how readily birds made these two types of errors: most of the birds' what errors in training were to the colour rewarded at the other time (33 of 37 initial what errors), rather than to one of the two never rewarded colours. Given that most of these what errors were to a specific type of flower, the proportion of what errors relative to chance was actually very high (figures 2 and 3). It seems likely that birds may have learned which flower colours were rewarded and which were not, but their errors stemmed from failing to integrate this information with where and when. We cannot, however, yet exclude the possibility that owing to the distance (10 m) that separated the two arrays correcting what errors was less costly than was correcting where or when errors. Testing this possibility (and indeed exploring the optimal use of multi-component memory more generally) requires further experimentation.
Based on the difficulty that animals often have had in remembering the when component in episodic-like experiments [25,26], we predicted that the birds would find the when component of the task the most difficult to remember, which was indeed the case. However, this component of the task was distinct from the where or what components in that there was more than one kind of information about when that the birds could have used, as the rewarded flower could have been predicted by both the time of day and by the sequence in which arrays were rewarded across the day. Experiment 2 suggests that the hummingbirds used both time of day and sequence information in combination: they learned a sequence (i.e. that the reward in the two flowers alternated) but they also learned that the sequence was anchored to specific times of day. This explanation is consistent with the finding that rats can combine different types of information to guide their behaviour [29].
Under its original formulation, animals must remember all three of what, where and when simultaneously in order to show episodic-like memory [4]. However, this all-or-nothing approach to memory is not in close accord with human episodic memories, which are often incomplete and thought to be recalled by using reconstructive processes, where an episode is reassembled as it is recalled [12]. While our experiment does not directly test episodic-like memory, as animals received repeated trials on the same task, our data suggest that the what, where and when components of a memory may likewise be separable in animals. The separability of these components of a memory is supported by the variation in memory impairments in mice trained to a what, where and when task [30]: hippocampal-lesioned animals were impaired on the overall task, while mice with lesions to the prefrontal cortex were specifically impaired on the where element of the task.
If what, where and when are indeed remembered in a reconstructive way by hummingbirds and other animals, there may be a number of ways to investigate the similarities (or not) between human episodic memory and animal episodic-like memory. In humans, the integrative nature of episodic memory leads to a variety of memory failures that may be amenable to testing in animals. These include generalization, where subjects incorrectly remember aspects of an event that did not occur owing to their close relation to an event's actual context. For example, subjects will frequently ‘remember’ words they have not seen if a list of words contains other thematically related words, such as remembering hospital having seen the words ambulance, doctor, operation, X-ray, ward, etc. [14,15]. Another type of reconstructive memory error is blending, where two memories which share many features can be confused with each other and combined to make a novel (and inaccurate) memory [16]. Given that our experiment suggests that information about what, where and when are also separable in animal memory, it seems to us that it would be useful to look for these kinds of reconstruction errors in episodic-like tasks, to see whether animal memory resembles human memory in form as well as content.
Acknowledgements
We thank Kate Morgan, Michelle Arnold, James Ainge and Madeline Eacott for helpful discussion, and two anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. The data will be made available on request from the corresponding author.
Funding statement
This work was supported by the Natural Environment Research Council (DTG studentship to R.E.S.M.) and NSERC (T.A.H.).
References
- 1.Tulving E. 1993. What is episodic memory? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2, 67–70 (doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770899) [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tulving E. 2002. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 1–25 (doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Clayton NS, Dickinson A. 1998. Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395, 272–274 (doi:10.1038/26216) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Clayton NS, Bussey TJ, Dickinson A. 2003. Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 685–691 (doi:10.1038/nrn1180) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Feeney M, Roberts W, Sherry D. 2009. Memory for what, where, and when in the black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). Anim. Cogn. 12, 767–777 (doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0236-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ferkin MH, Combs A, delBarco-Trillo J, Pierce AA, Franklin S. 2008. Meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus, have the capacity to recall the ‘what’, ‘where’, and ‘when’ of a single past event. Anim. Cogn. 11, 147–159 (doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0101-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Babb SJ, Crystal JD. 2006. Episodic-like memory in the rat. Curr. Biol. 16, 1317–1321 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.025) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Suddendorf T, Busby J. 2003. Mental time travel in animals? Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 391–396 (doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00187-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Clayton NS, Bussey TJ, Emery NJ, Dickinson A. 2003. Prometheus to Proust: the case for behavioural criteria for ‘mental time travel’. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 436–437 (doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Suddendorf T, Busby J. 2003. Like it or not? The mental time travel debate: reply to Clayton et al. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 437–438 (doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW. 2000. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol. Rev. 107, 261–288 (doi:10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.261) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Schacter DL, Addis DR. 2007. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 773–786 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2087) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Schacter DL, Norman KA, Koutstaal W. 1998. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49, 289–318 (doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.289) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Read J. 1996. From a passing thought to a false memory in 2 minutes: confusing real and illusory events. Psychol. Bull. Rev. 3, 105–111 (doi:10.3758/bf03210749) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Roediger HL, McDermott KB. 1995. Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 21, 803–814 (doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803) [Google Scholar]
- 16.Nystrom LE, McClelland JL. 1992. Trace synthesis is cued-recall. J. Mem. Lang. 31, 591–614 (doi:10.1016/0749-596x(92)90030-2) [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bateson M, Healy SD, Hurly TA. 2002. Irrational choices in hummingbird foraging behaviour. Anim. Behav. 63, 587–596 (doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1925) [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bateson M, Healy SD, Hurly TA. 2003. Context-dependent foraging decisions in rufous hummingbirds. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, 1271–1276 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2365) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hurly TA. 1996. Spatial memory in rufous hummingbirds: memory for rewarded and non-rewarded sites. Anim. Behav. 51, 177–183 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0015) [Google Scholar]
- 20.Healy SD, Hurly TA. 1998. Rufous hummingbirds’ (Selasphorus rufus) memory for flowers: patterns or actual spatial locations? J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 24, 396–404 (doi:10.1037/0097-7403.24.4.396) [Google Scholar]
- 21.Healy SD, Hurly TA. 1995. Spatial memory in rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus): a field test. Anim. Learn. Behav. 23, 63–68 (doi:10.3758/BF03198016) [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hurly TA, Healy SD. 1996. Memory for flowers in rufous hummingbirds: location or local visual cues? Anim. Behav. 51, 1149–1157 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0116) [Google Scholar]
- 23.Henderson J, Hurly TA, Bateson M, Healy SD. 2006. Timing in free-living rufous hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus. Curr. Biol. 16, 512–515 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.01.054) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Marshall RES, Hurly TA, Healy SD. 2012. Do a flower's features help hummingbirds to learn its contents and refill rate? Anim. Behav. 83, 1163–1169 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.02.003) [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hampton RR, Hampstead BM, Murray EA. 2005. Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) demonstrate robust memory for what and where, but not when, in an open-field test of memory. Learn. Motiv. 36, 245–259 (doi:10.1016/j.lmot.2005.02.004) [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bird LR, Roberts WA, Abroms B, Kit KA, Crupi C. 2003. Spatial memory for food hidden by rats (Rattus norvegicus) on the radial maze: studies of memory for where, what, and when. J. Comp. Psychol. 117, 176–187 (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.117.2.176) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hilborn R, Mangel M. 1997. The ecological detective: confronting models with data, 315 p Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hurly TA, Healy SD. 2002. Cue learning by rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus). J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 28, 209–223 (doi:10.1037/0097-7403.28.2.209) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pizzo M, Crystal J. 2002. Representation of time in time–place learning. Learn. Behav. 30, 387–393 (doi:10.3758/bf03195963) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.DeVito LM, Eichenbaum H. 2010. Distinct contributions of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to the ‘what–where–when’ components of episodic-like memory in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 215, 318–325 (doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.014) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]