Summary
Background and objectives
Diabetes mellitus is associated with increased risk of cognitive impairment. This study examines whether microvascular disease, as measured by albuminuria and decline in estimated GFR (eGFR), is associated with cognitive decline during 3.3 years of follow-up in individuals with diabetes with a normal baseline eGFR (approximately 90 ml/min per 1.73 m2).
Design, setting, participants, & measurements
Participants were from the Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes study (N=2977; mean age 62.5±5.8 years; recruitment from August 2003 to December 2005, followed through June 2009), which examined the association of intensive versus standard glucose control on cognitive function. Participants underwent three neuropsychologic tests at baseline, 20 months, and 40 months. Tests included information processing speed, verbal memory, and executive function. Mixed-effects models were used to assess the association of albuminuria and eGFR on the percentage decline in each test.
Results
Participants with albuminuria at baseline and follow-up (persistent albuminuria) (−5.8% [95% confidence interval (CI), −7.3 to −4.2]) and participants with albuminuria at follow-up but none at baseline (progressive albuminuria) (−4.1% [95% CI, −5.6 to −2.7]) had greater percentage declines on information processing speed than participants without albuminuria at baseline and at follow-up (no albuminuria) (−2.6% [95% CI, −3.4 to −1.9]) (P=0.001 and P=0.10, respectively). There were borderline percentage changes in the test of verbal memory (4.8% [95% CI, 2.4 to 7.1] and 4.7% [95% CI, 2.5 to 7.0] versus 7.1% [95% CI, 6.0 to 8.3]; P=0.11 and P=0.08, respectively). On logistic regression analysis, persistent albuminuria (odds ratio, 1.37 [95% CI, 1.09 to 1.72]) and progressive albuminuria (odds ratio, 1.25 [95% CI, 1.02 to 1.56]) were associated with a ≥5% decline in information processing speed scores but not with verbal memory or executive function performance. A 1 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year eGFR decline had a borderline association with decline in tests of cognitive function.
Conclusions
Persistent albuminuria and progressive albuminuria are associated with a decline in cognitive function in relatively young individuals with diabetes with unimpaired eGFR. These findings do not rule out the possibility of other processes causing cognitive decline.
Introduction
Individuals with type 2 diabetes (DM) are at a 50%–60% increased risk of cognitive impairment compared with people without DM (1). They also develop cognitive decline more rapidly and at an earlier age (2). Identifying markers for very early cognitive decline in people with DM before the onset of evident impairment is therefore of interest and importance.
Moderate CKD (estimated GFR [eGFR] <60 ml/min per 1.73 m2) and end stage renal failure are associated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment (3,4). Davey et al. (5) recently examined cognitive function in people with mild CKD (eGFR of approximately 78 ml/min per 1.73 m2). Those with a rapid decline in eGFR of ≥3 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year over 5 years had a greater risk of cognitive impairment than those without such a decline. This finding not only expanded the range of renal function associated with cognitive impairment, but also suggested that dynamic markers of renal function, such as a decline in eGFR, and not only levels of attained eGFR, are important in determining cognitive decline.
Albuminuria is another marker of renal microvascular disease. Its prevalence has increased over the past 2 decades, owing to the increasing prevalence of hypertension, diabetes, and obesity (6). In people with DM, its prevalence reaches 37.6% by age 60–69 years (7). We have shown that albuminuria, based on a single measurement, is associated with an increased prevalence and risk of cognitive impairment in people with and without DM (8–10).
Recently, our understanding of the natural history of albuminuria has changed. Unlike the paradigm of the 1980s, which posited that albuminuria was a first step in a committed process that inexorably leads to renal failure, it is now known that albuminuria can be dynamic. It can be stable, intermittent, progressive, or remitting (hereafter called albuminuria status) (11,12). How albuminuria status is associated with cognitive change has not been studied.
In this study, we examine the association between albuminuria status with performance on three tests of cognitive function in a cohort of adults with DM whose baseline eGFR level was approximately 90 ml/min per 1.73 m2. We further test whether decline in eGFR during follow-up was an independent risk factor for cognitive impairment. Participants for this analysis were from the Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes (ACCORD MIND) study. It examined whether intensive lowering of hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) levels prevents decline in cognitive function compared with standard treatment of blood glucose levels. The study reported no differences in cognitive outcomes between the two groups (13).
Materials and Methods
ACCORD was a randomized, multicenter, 2×2 factorial design trial of 10,251 participants with DM at risk for cardiovascular disease (CVD) events (14). All participants were enrolled in the glycemia trial, which compared a therapeutic strategy targeted to a glycated HbA1c level of <6.0% with a strategy that targeted an HbA1c level of 7.0%–7.9%. The primary study outcome was the time to the first occurrence of a major cardiovascular event, the composite of nonfatal myocardial infarction, nonfatal stroke, or cardiovascular death. The lipid trial (54% of the cohort) compared the masked administration of placebo or fenofibrate to persons taking simvastatin, whose LDL was <100 mg/dl. This substudy tested the hypothesis that lowering triglycerides and raising HDL cholesterol on the background of the LDL level <100 mg/dl lowers CVD outcomes. The BP trial included the other 46% of participants and compared a therapeutic strategy targeted to a systolic BP (SBP) of <120 mmHg to one targeting <140 mmHg for CVD outcomes. The glycemia and lipid studies reported no benefit for intensive interventions (13,15). The BP study reported no benefit either, except for the prevention of stroke (16). All participants signed informed consent upon entry into the main ACCORD trial and all study sites obtained institutional review board permission for study participation. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (ClinicalTrials.gov NCT00182910).
ACCORD MIND was an ancillary study to ACCORD (17). It consisted of 2977 ACCORD participants, recruited from August 2003 through December 2005 and followed through June 2009. Its primary objective was to determine whether intensive versus standard glucose therapy slowed deterioration of cognitive function (specifically the Digit Symbol Substitution Test [DSST]) over 40 months of follow-up. Secondary outcomes were to determine whether intensive BP control and lowering of triglycerides and raising HDL cholesterol slowed the rate of DSST decline compared with standard treatment. To achieve these aims, there were at least 350 participants in each of the eight cells of the ACCORD randomization scheme (glucose intensive versus standard subgrouped by BP and lipid assignment groups). To account for a 15% loss to follow-up, >350 participants were recruited for each cell. Power calculations for arriving at these estimates were previously published (17,18).
Cognition was assessed at baseline (within 45 days of randomization), 20 months, and 40 months after randomization using a 30-minute battery that included standardized tests sensitive to changes in memory, information processing speed, and executive function, and that had been validated in older persons and was available in a Spanish version. The test selection was based on tests used previously as part of a large multicenter magnetic resonance imaging study of vascular determinants of brain lesions (19). Quality of testing was ensured through training, certification, and monitoring. Inclusion criteria were willingness to participate in a 5-year study, age ≥55 years, and English or Spanish as a primary language. Exclusion criteria were preexisting conditions that could interfere with participation (e.g., dementia, substance abuse, recent stroke) or nonskin cancer within the past 5 years. Participants had to have a creatinine level <1.5 mg/dl at entry into the ACCORD study and the MIND substudy.
The DSST is a measure of psychomotor speed and performance (20,21). It is calculated as the total number of test items correctly coded in 90 seconds, with a maximum possible score of 90. Other cognitive outcomes were verbal memory and executive function. Verbal memory was measured with the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (22), and is reported as the average number of words recalled (0–15) over the immediate, short, and delayed recall trials. Executive function was measured with the modified Stroop test (23) and is reported as the interference score; a higher score is indicative of worse function. A baseline DSST was obtained for 2957 participants (99%); 94% had at least one (20- or 40-month) follow-up and were included in the final analysis. Completion rates for the RAVLT (baseline of 2977; 2744 with at least one follow-up test [93.2%]) and Stroop (baseline of 2941; 2732 with at least one follow-up examination [92.9%]) tests were similar. The Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (24), a screening test for dementia, was also administered to compare our cohort with other cohorts in terms of baseline cognition. It was not considered a study outcome.
Albuminuria was defined as ≥30 mg albumin per gram creatinine in a spot urine sample. Owing to the small number of participants with macroalbuminuria, albuminuria was not dichotomized into microalbuminuria and macroalbuminuria. Participants were categorized by the presence or absence of albuminuria at baseline and on at least one test at follow-up. Participants with albuminuria at baseline and on all three follow-up urine tests had persistent albuminuria. Those with no albuminuria at baseline and on the three follow-up urine tests had no albuminuria. Participants with no albuminuria at baseline who had albuminuria on at least one follow-up test were progressors. Those with albuminuria at baseline but none on follow-up were remitters. The eGFR was calculated using the Chronic Kidney Disease Epidemiology Collaboration equation (25).
Baseline covariates were age, race/ethnicity, sex, level of highest attained education, body mass index (BMI), duration of DM, smoking status, number of alcoholic drinks consumed per day, lipid levels, BP, and prior CVD events. Depression was assessed using the Patient Health Questionnaire. A value of ≥10 is consistent with depression. The number of severe hypoglycemic episodes (in which the participant reported receiving medical care or assistance from another individual and either a documented blood glucose <50 mg/dl [2.8 mmol/L] or recovery with carbohydrate treatment) was recorded.
Participant characteristics are summarized as means ± SDs and percentages. To test the association of the albuminuria status with cognitive function, mixed-effects models were used that incorporated information from baseline and from the 20-month and 40-month outcome measures (26). This approach uses all available outcome data to adjust for possible bias resulting from missing data at the 40-month examination in the estimation of the 40-month difference. Contrasts were used to test the hypothesis of no difference between albuminuria status groups. Model 1 adjusts for baseline cognitive measure, a visit effect, albuminuria status, and an albuminuria status by visit interaction. Model 2 further adjusts for age, sex, race, and education. Model 3 adds secondary prevention status, hypertension status, systolic BP, smoking status, BMI, eGFR, and a term for the randomized intervention groups as covariates. Mean baseline values of covariates were used. Model 4 adds baseline eGFR and change in eGFR over 40 months.
Logistic regression models were done to examine the associations of albuminuria status and change in eGFR with tests of cognition. Modeling was done in the following steps: model 1, unadjusted; model 2, age, race, sex, alcohol, education; model 3, variables in model 2 plus BMI, baseline SBP, and LDL cholesterol and history of CVD; and model 4, variables in model 3 plus baseline eGFR and change in eGFR over 40 months.
In sensitivity analyses, an indicator variable for having had a hypoglycemic event requiring assistance during the trial period was added to model 4 to see to what extent a hypoglycemic event affected the relationship between albuminuria status and change in DSST or RAVLT scores.
Analyses were conducted with SAS 9.2 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).
Results
Baseline and Follow-Up Characteristics
Compared with participants with no albuminuria (57.1% of the cohort), those with persistent albuminuria (15.9%) were older, were more likely to be male and African American, and were more likely to have higher SBP, BMI, longer duration of DM, and more CVD (Table 1). Characteristics of the remitters (12.5%) and progressors (14.5%) varied somewhat from those with persistent albuminuria. Remitters were more likely than the other three albuminuria group to be in the intensive BP arm of the ACCORD Blood Pressure Study. All four groups had on average preserved renal function (eGFR approximately 90 ml/min per 1.73 m2). After adjustment for age, depression, and education status, participants with persistent albuminuria had the poorest performance on tests of cognition at baseline, whereas those with no albuminuria had the best levels.
Table 1.
Characteristic | No Albuminuria | Albuminuria | Remitters | Progressors | Overall | P Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Participants | 1689 (57.1) | 469 (15.9) | 370 (12.5) | 429 (14.5) | 2957 | |
Age (yr) | 62.1±5.8 | 63.2±6.0 | 62.6±5.8 | 63.0±5.8 | 62.5±5.8 | 0.002 |
Female sex | 851 (50.4) | 170 (36.2) | 156 (42.2) | 200 (46.6) | 1377 (46.6) | <0.001 |
Education | 0.12 | |||||
Less than high school graduate | 200 (11.8) | 77 (16.4) | 55 (14.9) | 54 (12.6) | 386 (13.1) | |
High school graduate/GED | 428 (25.3) | 118 (25.2) | 93 (25.1) | 126 (29.4) | 765 (25.9) | |
Some college/technical training | 590 (34.9) | 167 (35.6) | 123 (33.2) | 144 (33.6) | 1024 (34.6) | |
College graduate or more | 471 (27.9) | 107 (22.8) | 99 (26.8) | 105 (24.5) | 782 (26.4) | |
Race/ethnicity | 0.02 | |||||
African American | 247 (14.6) | 92 (19.6) | 76 (20.5) | 64 (14.9) | 479 (16.2) | |
White | 1207 (71.5) | 304 (64.8) | 252 (68.1) | 299 (69.7) | 2062 (69.7) | |
Hispanic | 108 (6.4) | 41 (8.7) | 24 (6.5) | 36 (8.4) | 209 (7.1) | |
Other | 127 (7.5) | 32 (6.8) | 18 (4.9) | 30 (7.0) | 207 (7.0) | |
Alcohol consumption (drinks per week) | 0.71 | |||||
0 | 78.0 | 77.0 | 74.6 | 79.7 | 77.7 | |
1–2 | 12.7 | 13.4 | 14.3 | 12.4 | 13.0 | |
3–7 | 6.6 | 5.8 | 6.8 | 5.4 | 6.3 | |
≥8 | 2.8 | 3.8 | 4.3 | 2.6 | 3.1 | |
Systolic BP (mmHg) | 133.0±16.5 | 141.9±18.9 | 141.0±18.6 | 133.6±17.7 | 135.5±17.8 | <0.001 |
Duration of diabetes (yr) | 9.5±7.1 | 12.5±7.5 | 11.8±8.1 | 10.3±6.9 | 10.4±7.4 | <0.001 |
Total cholesterol (mg/dl) | 183.3±40.7 | 185.4±45.5 | 184.4±44.2 | 181.1±40.2 | 183.4±41.9 | 0.59 |
LDL (mg/dl) | 104.1±33.5 | 103.1±34.7 | 102.8±33.2 | 102.4±33.2 | 103.5±33.6 | 0.31 |
HDL, women (mg/dl) | 47.5±12.1 | 46.3±11.8 | 46.6±11.8 | 46.2±12.8 | 47.1±12.1 | 0.11 |
HDL, men (mg/dl) | 38.9±9.0 | 38.4±10.8 | 38.5±9.4 | 37.7±8.4 | 38.6±9.3 | 0.08 |
eGFR (MDRD formula) | 91.2 (86.3–96.2) | 88.6 (83.0–94.4) | 90.0 (84.0–95.2) | 88.9 (83.7–94.2) | 90.4 (85.0–95.6) | <0.001 |
BMI (kg/m2) | 32.8±5.3 | 33.0±5.4 | 33.8±5.5 | 33.1±5.3 | 33.0±5.4 | 0.03 |
History of CVD | 404 (23.9) | 173 (36.9) | 135 (36.5) | 151 (35.2) | 863 (29.2) | <0.001 |
Intensive lipid participant (%) | 51.6 | 48.0 | 58.0 | 46.2 | 50.9 | 0.09 |
Intensive BP participant (%) | 53.2 | 45.1 | 57.7 | 46.6 | 51.8 | 0.02 |
Intensive glucose participant (%) | 50.4 | 45.8 | 53.2 | 47.1 | 49.5 | 0.11 |
Depression (PHQ >10) | 245 (14.5) | 73 (15.6) | 61 (16.5) | 60 (14.0) | 439 (14.8) | 0.71 |
Baseline cognition test levels (age, education, and depression adjusted) | ||||||
DSST | 54.3±15.6 | 48.1±15.9 | 50.6±15.6 | 52.4±15.8 | 52.6±15.9 | <0.001 |
Stroop test | 30.9±15.5 | 33.8±17.5 | 33.3±19.1 | 33.2±17.7 | 32.0±16.7 | 0.001 |
RAVLT | 7.7±2.5 | 7.0±2.5 | 7.2±2.6 | 7.6±2.5 | 7.5±2.5 | 0.04 |
MMSE | 27.5±2.5 | 27.2±2.6 | 27.3±2.5 | 27.4±2.5 | 27.4±2.5 | 0.33 |
Follow-up renal function | ||||||
ΔeGFR per year | −0.60 (−0.77, −0.01) | −0.58 (−0.72, 0.25) | −0.54 (−0.75, 0.21) | −0.59 (−0.76, 0.14) | −0.59 (−0.76, 0.05) | 0.003 |
ΔeGFR per year | −0.38±0.93 | −0.20±1.19 | −0.27±1.13 | −0.28±1.15 | −0.32±1.04 | <0.001 |
Hypoglycemic episodes requiring any assistance during the trial period | 98 (6.6) | 40 (10.7) | 33 (9.5) | 41 (9.9) | 212 (8.1) | 0.01 |
Data are presented as n (%), mean ± SD, or median (interquartile range), unless otherwise specified. GED, General Education Development, equivalent of high school diploma; eGFR, estimated GFR; MDRD, Modification of Diet in Renal Disease study equation; BMI, body mass index; CVD, cardiovascular disease; PHQ, Patient Health Questionnaire; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination.
Participants without albuminuria had greater decreases in mean and median eGFR over follow-up compared with the other albuminuria groups. The mean eGFR decline was <1 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year in all albuminuria groups. Participants with no albuminuria had fewer hypoglycemic events than the other three albuminuria groups.
Longitudinal Change in Cognition by Albuminuria Status
Participants with persistent albuminuria had a statistically significant percentage decline in DSST scores over 40 months compared with those with no albuminuria (−5.8% [95% confidence interval (CI), −7.3 to −4.2] versus −2.6% [95% CI, −3.4 to −1.9]; P=0.001) (Table 2 and Supplemental Table 1). Participants with progressive albuminuria had a decline in DSST score (−4.1% [95% CI, −5.6 to −2.7]) approaching statistical significance compared with those without any albuminuria (P=0.10). Both of these albuminuria groups had trends toward a decline in RAVLT scores (P=0.11 and P=0.08, respectively), but little change in Stroop scores. Remitters had cognition scores similar to participants without albuminuria.
Table 2.
Model | No Albuminuria | Albuminuria | Remitters | Progressors |
---|---|---|---|---|
Participants (n) | 1689 | 469 | 370 | 429 |
DSST | −2.6 (−3.4, −1.9) | −5.8 (−7.3, −4.2) | −2.8 (−4.3, −1.2) | −4.1 (−5.6, −2.7) |
Stroop | −1.3 (−3.4, 0.8) | 1.4 (−2.9, 5.6) | −1.0 (−5.4, 3.3) | −0.7 (−4.7, 3.2) |
RAVLT | 7.1 (6.0, 8.3) | 4.8 (2.4, 7.1) | 6.7 (4.3, 9.1) | 4.7 (2.5, 7.0) |
MMSE | −1.2 (−1.6, −0.08) | −1.6 (−2.4, −0.08) | −0.05 (−1.4, 0.03) | −1.2 (−2.0, −0.05) |
See the Supplemental Material for raw data. P values are as follows: No albuminuria versus albuminuria: 0.001 for DSST, 0.28 for Stroop, 0.11 for RAVLT, and 0.46 for MMSE; no albuminuria versus remitters: 0.82 for DSST, 0.75 for Stroop, 0.80 for RAVLT, and 0.19 for MMSE; and no albuminuria versus progressors: 0.10 for DSST, 0.88 for Stroop, 0.08 for RAVLT, and 0.86 for MMSE. DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination.
Factors Independently Associated with ≥5% Decline in DSST Score
On unadjusted analysis, persistent albuminuria and progressive albuminuria were each significantly associated with a ≥5% decline in DSST score, with odds ratios (ORs) of 1.47 (95% CI, 1.20 to 1.86) and 1.31 (95% CI, 1.05 to 1.62), respectively (Table 3; full model in Supplemental Table 2). Successive adjustment for demographic, cardiovascular, and renal factors mildly attenuated these estimates (OR, 1.37 [95% CI, 1.09 to 1.72]; OR, 1.26 [95% CI, 1.002 to 1.56], respectively) but they remained statistically significant. A 1 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year decline in eGFR (approximately twice the median annual decrease during the trial) had a marginal association with DSST decline (OR, 1.02 [95% CI, 0.99 to 1.04]). The association of persistent albuminuria on DSST decline relative to 1 year of aging was approximately 7.2 years (ratio of log [OR persistent albuminuria] to log [OR per year of age]), i.e., persistent albuminuria was equivalent to 7.2 years of aging in terms of DSST decline. The association of progressive albuminuria was equivalent to approximately 3.2 years.
Table 3.
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
No albuminuria | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 |
Persistent albuminuria | 1.47 (1.20 to 1.86)a | 1.44 (1.56 to 1.80)a | 1.41 (1.12 to 1.76)a | 1.37 (1.09 to 1.72)a |
Remitters | 1.15 (0.92 to 1.45) | 1.12 (0.89 to 1.42) | 1.08 (0.85 to 1.37) | 1.07 (0.84 to 1.36) |
Progressors | 1.31 (1.05 to 1.62)a | 1.26 (1.02 to 1.57)a | 1.26 (1.02 to 1.57)a | 1.25 (1.002 to 1.56)a |
Baseline eGFR (ml/min per 1.73 m2) | 0.99 (0.98 to 1.01) | |||
ΔeGFR (1 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year) | 1.02 (0.995 to 1.04) | |||
Age (per year) | 1.02 (1.00 to 1.03)a | 1.02 (1.01 to 1.03)a | 1.02 (1.00 to 1.03)a |
The full model appears in the Supplemental Material. Model 1, unadjusted; model 2, adjusted for age, sex, race (white, black, Hispanic, other), education (less than high school, high school, some college, college or higher), and alcohol consumption (1–2, 3–7, ≥8 drinks per week); model 3, variables in model 2 plus body mass index, systolic BP, secondary cardiovascular disease prevention (yes versus no), and LDL; model 4, variables in model 3 plus baseline eGFR and 1 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year decrease in eGFR. DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; ACCORD MIND, Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes; eGFR, estimated GFR.
Statistically significant.
When a hypoglycemic event requiring assistance was added to model 4 in a sensitivity analysis, ORs and 95% CIs were similar (persistent albuminuria versus no albuminuria: OR, 1.48 [95% CI, 1.17 to 1.89]; remitters versus no albuminuria: OR, 1.01 [95% CI, 0.84 to 1.36]; and progressors versus no albuminuria: OR, 1.27 [95% CI, 1.01 to 1.58]).
Factors Independently Associated with ≥5% Decline in RAVLT Score
None of the albuminuria categories was significantly related to a >5% decline in verbal memory (Supplemental Table 3). A 1 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year decline in eGFR was also not significantly related to RAVLT decline (OR, 1.02 [95% CI, 0.99 to 1.04]; P=0.18). A sensitivity analysis showed that adding hypoglycemic episodes requiring assistance to model 4 did not change the nonsignificant associations of albuminuria status with a change in RAVLT score.
Discussion
In this study of middle-aged adults with diabetes with preserved baseline eGFR (approximately 90 ml/min per 1.73 m2), the DSST declined in participants with persistent and progressive albuminuria compared with participants without albuminuria. Decline was greater in participants with persistent albuminuria compared with those with progressive albuminuria, suggestive of a dose effect. The findings were independent of baseline eGFR and declining eGFR, which were only mildly diminished (0.54–0.60 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year) at the end of follow-up, leaving most participants with normal to near normal renal function. These findings extend prior studies of the association of renal disease and cognitive decline to an even earlier stage of renal disease than previously reported.
Diabetes and albuminuria are vascular risk factors. The cognitive findings described here may therefore be vascular in origin. Several mechanisms by which albuminuria and cognitive impairment are related can be offered. One possibility is that the two microcirculatory systems share common pathomechanisms: both are low resistance beds exposed to high circulatory flow (27). Impaired autoregulation from small vessel endothelial dysfunction can increase pressure in both circulatory systems, leading to organ damage. For example, with impaired renal function, nitric oxide inhibitor levels increase. Nitric oxide regulates the microcirculation and the blood–brain barrier, which are implicated in brain white matter disease (28). Alternatively, complications associated with albuminuria, such as anemia, acidosis, hyperparathyroidism, and hypertension, can affect cognitive function (29).
Our findings should be put into context. Participants had on average normal baseline cognitive function (MMSE score >26), and were generally well educated, a factor that protects against cognitive decline (30). The changes in the DSST were modest and on an individual level would not likely be clinically significant. However, the average age of the cohort was 10–15 years younger than the age at which cognitive impairment is usually recognized (31). If the rate of information processing speed decline persisted, evident cognitive impairment would likely develop with aging. Participants with persistent and progressive albuminuria had the equivalent of 7.2 and 3.2 years of cognitive aging relative to 1 year of calendar aging, respectively. On a population level, these findings could have large effects.
Our findings are consistent with studies that have shown that the DSST detects small cognitive changes in people with high levels of cognition (32) and in people with DM (1,33). The Stroop test, which measures executive function, did not show change in association with albuminuria status. However, previous studies using other measures of executive function have found (34–36) associations of albuminuria with decline in executive function.
In our study, albuminuria progressors were more likely to experience DSST decline. This is similar to a study in which progressors had increased odds of cognitive decline as measured by the MMSE (8). In that study, like ours, albuminuria remitters were similar in terms of cognitive change to participants without any albuminuria. Another study with longitudinal data reported poorer cognitive function in participants with progressive albuminuria compared with those with stable albuminuria (37). These findings emphasize that dynamic characteristics of albuminuria are important determinants of cognitive decline.
Several studies have shown that moderate CKD can be associated with impaired cognition (38–41). There are few studies, however, that have longitudinally examined the effect of eGFR decline on cognition. In the Cardiovascular Health Study (3), an increase in serum creatinine from 1.0 to 2.0 mg/dl was associated with a 26% increased risk (95% CI, 1.02 to 1.60) of dementia. That study did not have measures of eGFR. In our study, there was a trend for a 1 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year eGFR decline (approximately double the median annual eGFR decline) to be associated with lower DSST scores in the cohort. Two population-based studies with unimpaired renal function and a low prevalence of diabetes reported that only an eGFR decline of >3–4 ml/min per 1.73 m2 per year was significantly associated with cognitive decline (5,42).
Our results suggest that in an exclusively diabetic population, small declines in eGFR may have a stronger negative association with cognitive decline than in populations without DM.
Our findings are characterized by prospective data collection, repeated measures of cognitive testing, near complete data capture, characterization of albuminuria over time, and a large number of participants. Risk factors for cognitive decline were captured, permitting for adjustment. We recognize that the follow-up was short and may not be predictive of subsequent cognitive decline. A follow-up study, entitled ACCORDION, will add 3–5 years of follow-up.
In summary, we identify adults with diabetes, whose eGFR at baseline was approximately 90 ml/min per 1.73 m2, with persistent or progressive albuminuria as a group at risk for cognitive decline. Such observations offer a possible new avenue for understanding subsequent cognitive impairment in persons with diabetes and offer an opportunity for intervention.
Disclosures
None.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The ACCORD MIND study was funded through an intra-agency agreement between the National Institute on Aging and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (grant AG-0002) and the National Institute on Aging Intramural Research Program. The ACCORD study was funded by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (grants N01-HC-95178, N01-HC-95179, N01-HC-95180, N01-HC-95181, N01-HC-95182, N01-HC-95183, and N01-HC-95184).
Footnotes
Published online ahead of print. Publication date available at www.cjasn.org.
This article contains supplemental material online at http://cjasn.asnjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.2215/CJN.11321112/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Cukierman T, Gerstein HC, Williamson JD: Cognitive decline and dementia in diabetes—systematic overview of prospective observational studies. Diabetologia 48: 2460–2469, 2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Yaffe K, Falvey C, Hamilton N, Schwartz AV, Simonsick EM, Satterfield S, Cauley JA, Rosano C, Launer LJ, Strotmeyer ES, Harris TB: Diabetes, glucose control, and 9-year cognitive decline among older adults without dementia. Arch Neurol 69: 1170–1175, 2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Seliger SL, Siscovick DS, Stehman-Breen CO, Gillen DL, Fitzpatrick A, Bleyer A, Kuller LH: Moderate renal impairment and risk of dementia among older adults: The Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. J Am Soc Nephrol 15: 1904–1911, 2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Murray AM, Knopman DS: Cognitive impairment in CKD: no longer an occult burden. Am J Kidney Dis 56: 615–618, 2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Davey A, Elias MF, Robbins MA, Seliger SL, Dore GA: Decline in renal functioning is associated with longitudinal decline in global cognitive functioning, abstract reasoning and verbal memory [published online ahead of print November 18, 2012]. Nephrol Dial Transplant10.1093/ndt/gfs470 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Coresh J, Selvin E, Stevens LA, Manzi J, Kusek JW, Eggers P, Van Lente F, Levey AS: Prevalence of chronic kidney disease in the United States. JAMA 298: 2038–2047, 2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Garg AX, Kiberd BA, Clark WF, Haynes RB, Clase CM: Albuminuria and renal insufficiency prevalence guides population screening: Results from the NHANES III. Kidney Int 61: 2165–2175, 2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Barzilay JI, Gao P, O’Donnell M, Mann JF, Anderson C, Fagard R, Probstfield J, Dagenais GR, Teo K, Yusuf S, ONTARGET and TRANSCEND Investigators : Albuminuria and decline in cognitive function: The ONTARGET/TRANSCEND studies. Arch Intern Med 171: 142–150, 2011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Murray AM, Barzilay JI, Lovato JF, Williamson JD, Miller ME, Marcovina S, Launer LJ, Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes (ACCORD-MIND) Substudy Investigators : Biomarkers of renal function and cognitive impairment in patients with diabetes. Diabetes Care 34: 1827–1832, 2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Barzilay JI, Fitzpatrick AL, Luchsinger J, Yasar S, Bernick C, Jenny NS, Kuller LH: Albuminuria and dementia in the elderly: A community study. Am J Kidney Dis 52: 216–226, 2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ismail-Beigi F, Craven TE, O’Connor PJ, Karl D, Calles-Escandon J, Hramiak I, Genuth S, Cushman WC, Gerstein HC, Probstfield JL, Katz L, Schubart U, ACCORD Study Group : Combined intensive blood pressure and glycemic control does not produce an additive benefit on microvascular outcomes in type 2 diabetic patients. Kidney Int 81: 586–594, 2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Perkins BA, Krolewski AS: Early nephropathy in type 1 diabetes: The importance of early renal function decline. Curr Opin Nephrol Hypertens 18: 233–240, 2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gerstein HC, Miller ME, Byington RP, Goff DC, Jr, Bigger JT, Buse JB, Cushman WC, Genuth S, Ismail-Beigi F, Grimm RH, Jr, Probstfield JL, Simons-Morton DG, Friedewald WT, Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Study Group : Effects of intensive glucose lowering in type 2 diabetes. N Engl J Med 358: 2545–2559, 2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Buse JB, Bigger JT, Byington RP, Cooper LS, Cushman WC, Friedewald WT, Genuth S, Gerstein HC, Ginsberg HN, Goff DC, Jr, Grimm RH, Jr, Margolis KL, Probstfield JL, Simons-Morton DG, Sullivan MD, ACCORD Study Group : Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes (ACCORD) trial: Design and methods. Am J Cardiol 99[suppl]: 21i–33i, 2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ginsberg HN, Elam MB, Lovato LC, Crouse JR, 3rd, Leiter LA, Linz P, Friedewald WT, Buse JB, Gerstein HC, Probstfield J, Grimm RH, Ismail-Beigi F, Bigger JT, Goff DC, Jr, Cushman WC, Simons-Morton DG, Byington RP, ACCORD Study Group : Effects of combination lipid therapy in type 2 diabetes mellitus. N Engl J Med 362: 1563–1574, 2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cushman WC, Evans GW, Byington RP, Goff DC, Jr, Grimm RH, Jr, Cutler JA, Simons-Morton DG, Basile JN, Corson MA, Probstfield JL, Katz L, Peterson KA, Friedewald WT, Buse JB, Bigger JT, Gerstein HC, Ismail-Beigi F, ACCORD Study Group : Effects of intensive blood-pressure control in type 2 diabetes mellitus. N Engl J Med 362: 1575–1585, 2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Williamson JD, Miller ME, Bryan RN, Lazar RM, Coker LH, Johnson J, Cukierman T, Horowitz KR, Murray A, Launer LJ, ACCORD Study Group : The Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes Study (ACCORD-MIND): Rationale, design, and methods. Am J Cardiol 99:112i-122i, 2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Launer LJ, Miller ME, Williamson JD, Lazar RM, Gerstein HC, Murray AM, Sullivan M, Horowitz KR, Ding J, Marcovina S, Lovato LC, Lovato J, Margolis KL, O’Connor P, Lipkin EW, Hirsch J, Coker L, Maldjian J, Sunshine JL, Truwit C, Davatzikos C, Bryan RN, ACCORD MIND investigators : Effects of intensive glucose lowering on brain structure and function in people with type 2 diabetes (ACCORD MIND): A randomised open-label substudy. Lancet Neurol 10: 969–977, 2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Launer LJ, Oudkerk M, Nilsson LG, Alperovitch A, Berger K, Breteler MM, Fuhrer R, Giampaoli S, Nissinen A, Pajak A, Sans S, Schmidt R, Hofman A: CASCADE: A European collaborative study on vascular determinants of brain lesions. Study design and objectives. Neuroepidemiology 19: 113–120, 2000 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test – Revised, New York, Psychological Corporation, 1988 [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hoyer WJ, Stawski RS, Wasylyshyn C, Verhaeghen P: Adult age and digit symbol substitution performance: A meta-analysis. Psychol Aging 19: 211–214, 2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd Ed., New York, Oxford University Press, 1995 [Google Scholar]
- 23.Houx PJ, Jolles J, Vreeling FW: Stroop interference: Aging effects assessed with the Stroop Color-Word Test. Exp Aging Res 19: 209–224, 1993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, Folstein MF: Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. JAMA 269: 2386–2391, 1993 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Matsushita K, Mahmoodi BK, Woodward M, Emberson JR, Jafar TH, Jee SH, Polkinghorne KR, Shankar A, Smith DH, Tonelli M, Warnock DG, Wen CP, Coresh J, Gansevoort RT, Hemmelgarn BR, Levey AS, Chronic Kidney Disease Prognosis Consortium : Comparison of risk prediction using the CKD-EPI equation and the MDRD study equation for estimated glomerular filtration rate. JAMA 307: 1941–1951, 2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Molenberghs G, Kenward M: Missing Data in Clinical Studies, Chichester, England, John Wiley and Sons Ltd, 2007 [Google Scholar]
- 27.McCarthy I: The physiology of bone blood flow: A review. J Bone Joint Surg Am 88[Suppl 3]: 4–9, 2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hoth KF, Tate DF, Poppas A, Forman DE, Gunstad J, Moser DJ, Paul RH, Jefferson AL, Haley AP, Cohen RA: Endothelial function and white matter hyperintensities in older adults with cardiovascular disease. Stroke 38: 308–312, 2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Inker LA, Coresh J, Levey AS, Tonelli M, Muntner P: Estimated GFR, albuminuria, and complications of chronic kidney disease. J Am Soc Nephrol 22: 2322–2331, 2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Sattler C, Toro P, Schönknecht P, Schröder J: Cognitive activity, education and socioeconomic status as preventive factors for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res 196: 90–95, 2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Petersen RC, Stevens JC, Ganguli M, Tangalos EG, Cummings JL, DeKosky ST, Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology : Practice parameter: early detection of dementia: Mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review). Neurology 56: 1133–1142, 2001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Proust-Lima C, Amieva H, Dartigues J-F, Jacqmin-Gadda H: Sensitivity of four psychometric tests to measure cognitive changes in brain aging-population-based studies. Am J Epidemiol 165: 344–350, 2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Spauwen PJJ, Köhler S, Verhey FRJ, Stehouwer CDA, van Boxtel MPJ: Effects of type 2 diabetes on 12-year cognitive change: Results from the Maastricht aging study. Diabetes Care 36: 1554–1561, 2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Heringa SM, van den Berg E, Dekker JM, Nijpels G, Kessels RPC, Kappelle LJ, Stehouwer CDA, Biessels GJ: Albuminuria and cognitive functioning in an older population: The Hoorn study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 32: 182–187, 2011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jassal SK, Kritz-Silverstein D, Barrett-Connor E: A prospective study of albuminuria and cognitive function in older adults: The Rancho Bernardo study. Am J Epidemiol 171: 277–286, 2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Weiner DE, Bartolomei K, Scott T, Price LL, Griffith JL, Rosenberg I, Levey AS, Folstein MF, Sarnak MJ: Albuminuria, cognitive functioning, and white matter hyperintensities in homebound elders. Am J Kidney Dis 53: 438–447, 2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Joosten H, Izaks GJ, Slaets JPJ, de Jong PE, Visser ST, Bilo HJG, Gansevoort RT: Association of cognitive function with albuminuria and eGFR in the general population. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol 6: 1400–1409, 2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kurella M, Chertow GM, Fried LF, Cummings SR, Harris T, Simonsick E, Satterfield S, Ayonayon H, Yaffe K: Chronic kidney disease and cognitive impairment in the elderly: The health, aging, and body composition study. J Am Soc Nephrol 16: 2127–2133, 2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hailpern SM, Melamed ML, Cohen HW, Hostetter TH: Moderate chronic kidney disease and cognitive function in adults 20 to 59 years of age: Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III). J Am Soc Nephrol 18: 2205–2213, 2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Buchman AS, Tanne D, Boyle PA, Shah RC, Leurgans SE, Bennett DA: Kidney function is associated with the rate of cognitive decline in the elderly. Neurology 73: 920–927, 2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Etgen T, Sander D, Chonchol M, Briesenick C, Poppert H, Förstl H, Bickel H: Chronic kidney disease is associated with incident cognitive impairment in the elderly: The INVADE study. Nephrol Dial Transplant 24: 3144–3150, 2009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Helmer C, Stengel B, Metzger M, Froissart M, Massy ZA, Tzourio C, Berr C, Dartigues JF: Chronic kidney disease, cognitive decline, and incident dementia: The 3C Study. Neurology 77: 2043–2051, 2011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.