Abstract
Learning and maintaining the sounds we use in vocal communication require accurate perception of the sounds we hear performed by others and feedback-dependent imitation of those sounds to produce our own vocalizations. Understanding how the central nervous system integrates auditory and vocal-motor information to enable communication is a fundamental goal of systems neuroscience, and insights into the mechanisms of those processes will profoundly enhance clinical therapies for communication disorders. Gaining the high-resolution insight necessary to define the circuits and cellular mechanisms underlying human vocal communication is presently impractical. Songbirds are the best animal model of human speech, and this review highlights recent insights into the neural basis of auditory perception and feedback-dependent imitation in those animals. Neural correlates of song perception are present in auditory areas, and those correlates are preserved in the auditory responses of downstream neurons that are also active when the bird sings. Initial tests indicate that singing-related activity in those downstream neurons is associated with vocal-motor performance as opposed to the bird simply hearing itself sing. Therefore, action potentials related to auditory perception and action potentials related to vocal performance are co-localized in individual neurons. Conceptual models of song learning involve comparison of vocal commands and the associated auditory feedback to compute an error signal that is used to guide refinement of subsequent song performances, yet the sites of that comparison remain unknown. Convergence of sensory and motor activity onto individual neurons points to a possible mechanism through which auditory and vocal-motor signals may be linked to enable learning and maintenance of the sounds used in vocal communication.
Keywords: Songbird, perception, vocalization, corollary discharge, sensorimotor, comparator
1 Central Importance of Vocal Signals in Human Communication
Accurate perception and imitation of the sounds we hear performed by others are fundamental to human communication through spoken language, and the neural basis of those abilities has fascinated researchers for centuries. At a basic level, the neural mechanisms of vocal communication must accomplish two tasks. First, a continuous stream of acoustic signals must be broken into behaviorally relevant segments, and each of those segments must be accurately perceived. This process is especially challenging because acoustic signals cannot be resampled as they can in the case of fixed stimuli such as this text. Vocal communication is a temporally dynamic process, and information will quite literally pass the listener by unless robust mechanisms are in place to facilitate rapid and reliable perception. Second, auditory perception must be linked to motor performance to enable imitation of the sounds that compose our spoken language. The sounds we use in speech are initially learned as sensory signals performed by others, but over the course of development we use sensory feedback to refine our performance of those sounds. The quality of imitation that we typically achieve is extraordinary, as evident in the preservation of specific features that define regional dialects.
In considering how auditory input is used to shape motor output, some researchers have speculated that the sensory framework in which speech is processed as an auditory signal and the motor framework in which speech is processed as a vocal signal must be similar, and in order for communication to occur then at some point in neural processing those sensory and motor representations must be the same (Liberman et al., 1967; Rizzolatti and Arbib, 1998). An attractive idea is that individual neurons are active in association with specific vocal signals both when they are heard and when they are spoken, and it is through that sensorimotor correspondence that auditory perception may be translated into vocal motor performance (Arbib, 2005; Ferrari et al., 2003; Gallese et al., 1996; Iacoboni et al., 1999; Iacoboni et al., 2005; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Rizzolatti et al., 2001). Given these challenges faced by the brain in perception and imitation, it is somewhat astounding that communication typically proceeds as fluidly as it does. Fluency of vocal communication is a testimony to the robust effectiveness of the underlying brain mechanisms, and this commentary will highlight recent insights into a possible basis of perception and imitation of the sounds used in vocal communication.
Specialized regions of the human cortex have been implicated in perception and performance of the sounds used in speech. These regions, such as Wernicke's and Broca's areas, are well known, however they are by no means alone in their contribution to the behavioral complexity of vocal communication. Other sites, including primary and secondary cortical regions associated with sensory and motor processing, as well as subcortical regions through cortical-striatal-thalamocortical loops have been identified as important in speech (Alm, 2004; Doupe and Kuhl, 1999; Fox et al., 1996). A challenge to understanding the neural basis of human vocal communication is that discerning the contributions of those sites is complicated by the necessity of high-resolution information in both the temporal (millisecond) and spatial (microns) domain and difficulty disambiguating speech-related activity from other information processed by mammalian corticostriatal projections. Simply put, gaining the high-resolution insight necessary to define the cellular and circuit mechanisms underlying human vocal communication is presently impractical. In precious few cases in which human neurons can be sampled during hearing and speaking, neurons have been found to be active in each state, but it is not clear the degree to which that activity is sensory, motor-related or some combination (e.g., Creutzfeldt et al., 1989). Therefore, it has been a topic of considerable debate whether individual neurons in the human brain express a precise sensorimotor correspondence and whether such cells may play a role in linking the perception and execution of vocal signals (Brass et al., 2007; Hari et al., 1998; Mukamel et al., 2010). It has been a priority for many years to study a diverse array of songbird species so that we can discover ideal model species in which to investigate the possible existence of such neurons and to consider how they may contribute to the neural basis of learned vocal communication.
2 Songbirds as a Model System to Understand the Neural Basis of Vocal Communication
Songbirds provide an excellent animal model of human speech. In a form of learning that is rare across animals, songbirds acquire their songs by imitating other members of their species, just as humans imitate others to learn the sounds we use in speech (Doupe and Kuhl, 1999; Jarvis, 2004). Songbirds and humans are both critically dependent on auditory input to learn and maintain their vocalizations, and the pattern of song development is strikingly similar to babbling and subsequent vocal refinement in humans (Bolhuis and Gahr, 2006; Doupe and Kuhl, 1999; Mooney, 2009). Briefly, sensory representations of the signals that will eventually be imitated are memorized during a juvenile sensitive period (Funabiki and Konishi, 2003; Marler and Peters, 1981; Marler and Peters, 1982; Marler and Peters, 1987; Peters et al., 1992). Later in juvenile development, vocalizations begin and auditory feedback is used to refine those vocalizations to achieve accurate imitation and maintain those signals in adulthood (e.g., Marler and Sherman, 1983; Woolley and Rubel, 1997). Parallelism in the learning, performance and communicative significance of speech and birdsong make songbirds a uniquely advantageous animal model to understand how vocal signals are perceived and transformed into a motor framework to enable communication.
In seeking to understand the neural basis of vocal communication, songbirds provide two additional important advantages. First, song can be easily quantified and manipulated to facilitate experimentation and assessment of the relation between behavior and the associated neural activity. Second, and most importantly, song behavior is associated with dedicated regions in the songbird brain. This interconnected network, collectively called the “song system”, comprises nuclei of the forebrain, striatum, thalamus and brainstem, and the presence of a song system distinguishes songbirds from avian species that do not learn their vocalizations (Kroodsma and Konishi, 1991; Mooney et al., 2008; Wild, 2004). The song system contains two major pathways, the song motor pathway (SMP) and an anterior forebrain pathway (AFP), each of which emerges from the sensorimotor nucleus HVC (Figures 1 and 2). An intact SMP is necessary for song production, with lesions resulting in severe degradation or elimination of song (Nottebohm et al., 1976; Simpson and Vicario, 1990). In contrast, the AFP is not necessary for adult song (Nottebohm et al., 1976), but it is necessary for juvenile song learning and adult song plasticity (Andalman and Fee, 2009; Bottjer et al., 1984; Brainard and Doupe, 2001; Olveczky et al., 2005; Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991; Sohrabji et al., 1990; Williams and Mehta, 1999). Furthermore, activity in HVC and the AFP has been implicated in song perception, as lesions to either HVC or its synaptic target in the AFP (Area X) disrupt perception of songs performed by self or others (Gentner et al., 2000; Scharff et al., 1998). Each of these pathways is active during singing (Hahnloser et al., 2002; Hessler and Doupe, 1999; Kao and Brainard, 2006; Kozhevnikov and Fee, 2007; McCasland and Konishi, 1981; Olveczky et al., 2005; Olveczky et al., 2011; Prather et al., 2008; Sakata and Brainard, 2008; Schmidt, 2003; Troyer and Doupe, 2000; Yu and Margoliash, 1996), and cells in each pathway receive auditory input (Katz and Gurney, 1981; Margoliash, 1986; Mooney, 2000).
Research into the functional significance of the song system has been wide ranging, but many studies have focused on two broad themes. First, how is auditory perception encoded in the brain, and how is that perception stored and recalled as a model of imitative behavior? Second, how are auditory perception and vocal motor commands compared to enable feedback-dependent learning? Early investigations of those themes revealed the importance of auditory experience in juvenile learning and adult maintenance of song. Peter Marler and colleagues showed that sparrows raised in social isolation and therefore deprived of a song model developed songs lacking the acoustic complexity of song-tutored birds (e.g., Marler and Tamura, 1964). These results revealed that adult song is shaped by a tutor song that is experienced and memorized in early life (Funabiki and Konishi, 2003; Marler and Peters, 1981; Marler and Peters, 1982; Marler and Peters, 1987; Marler and Peters, 1988). Around that same time, Mark Konishi (1965) performed another classic experiment in which he deafened juvenile sparrows after exposure to a tutor song but before they began to sing. He found that birds were unable to reproduce the song that they had memorized, revealing that both early auditory experience of a model and ongoing auditory feedback throughout development are necessary for vocal development. In those experiments, Konishi also deafened adult sparrows and saw little or no change even long after deafening, leading to the idea that feedback was of relatively less importance in adult song maintenance than in juvenile learning. Years later, additional experiments revealed that this was not the case in all species. Adult zebra finches that are deafened (Lombardino and Nottebohm, 2000; Nordeen and Nordeen, 1992) or exposed to chronic alteration of auditory feedback (Leonardo and Konishi, 1999) express deteriorated songs in days or weeks, and vocal deterioration in Bengalese finches can occur even more rapidly (Okanoya and Yamaguchi, 1997; Woolley and Rubel, 1997). This dependence on auditory feedback for learning and maintenance is a hallmark of vocal learning, as preservation of human speech is similarly dependent on auditory feedback (Cowie and Douglas-Cowie, 1992). Together with the discovery of a song system specialized for song perception, performance and plasticity, these data reveal that comparative studies of species that express different degrees of sensitivity to altered auditory feedback (e.g., white crowned sparrows and Bengalese finches) afford an excellent opportunity to understand not only the circuit and cellular basis of feedback-dependent learning but also the mechanisms through which we acquire and preserve the signals we use to communicate through speech.
In the years since the discovery of the song system, additional forebrain regions have also been identified as important contributors in auditory processing. Those regions, including the caudal mesopallium (CM) and the caudomedial nidopallium (NCM), are not involved in song production and are present in both male and female birds (Figure 2) (Bauer et al., 2008; Jarvis and Nottebohm, 1997). Therefore, they are not considered part of the canonical song system, however they are active in association with auditory processing of features far beyond the acoustic song structure, such as the familiarity of a song or its role as the model from which the bird learned its song during development (Bolhuis and Eda-Fujiwara, 2003; Bolhuis et al., 2000; Chew et al., 1996; Chew et al., 1995; Phan et al., 2006; Terpstra et al., 2004). CM and NCM (collectively called the auditory lobule) are well positioned to serve such a role, as they receive input directly and indirectly from the avian primary auditory cortex (Field L) and project to nucleus HVC and to other sites in the forebrain (Bauer et al., 2008; Vates et al., 1996). Thus, activity of cells in the auditory lobule and the sensorimotor nucleus HVC to which they project have been implicated in auditory perception. These findings recommend CM, NCM and HVC as excellent sites in which to investigate the neural basis through which auditory perception shapes vocal performance.
3 Correlates of Auditory Perception in Secondary Auditory Cortical Areas
Neural correlates of auditory cognition have been detected in a variety of sites in the songbird brain. Patterns of gene expression and results from electrophysiological recordings have revealed song-evoked activation of neurons in the avian analog of secondary auditory cortex (CM and NCM) (Jarvis, 2004; Karten and Shimizu, 1989; Vates et al., 1996). Initial studies reported gene expression in CM and NCM that was selective for song of their own species, drawing attention to those areas as potentially important in processing complex auditory stimuli (Mello et al., 1992). Subsequent studies using electrophysiological methods have sought to define the behaviorally relevant song features that are encoded in the activity of those neurons. The earliest recordings from neurons in CM and NCM confirmed that those cells are indeed selective in their auditory responses (Chew et al., 1996; Chew et al., 1995; but see Stripling et al., 1997). Specifically, auditory responses of neurons in CM and NCM are selective not only for song, but also for songs of the bird's own species versus songs of other species (Mello et al., 1992; Phan et al., 2006; Stripling et al., 2001; Terleph et al., 2007). In contrast, primary auditory cortical neurons (Field L) are very broadly responsive to not only song but also to many other stimuli such as calls, pure tones or white noise (Amin et al., 2004; Grace et al., 2003; Lewicki and Arthur, 1996; Margoliash, 1986; Meliza and Margoliash, 2012). In further support of the idea that activity in the auditory lobule represents more complex features than the primary auditory cortex, CM and NCM neurons in different species are tuned to species-typical song features, but that degree of selectivity is not evident in the activity of thalamorecipient cells in the primary auditory cortex (Fields L1 and L2) (Meliza and Margoliash, 2012; Terleph et al., 2007). This selectivity for species-typical song features among cells in the auditory lobule may facilitate integration of song elements in service of recognition of song and singer identity.
Cells in the auditory lobule also express another intriguing aspect of auditory selectivity in that the responses of those cells to a given song are dependent on the context in which that song is presented. Auditory responses become progressively weaker with repeated presentation of the same song, but even in that habituated state, presentation of new songs is capable of driving a strong response (Chew et al., 1996; Chew et al., 1995; Stripling et al., 1997). Habituation to a specific song can persist even without ongoing presentation of that song or even if other songs are played in the interim (Chew et al., 1996; Chew et al., 1995; Stripling et al., 1997). The context-dependent auditory responses of cells in the auditory lobule have been interpreted as a means of representing familiarity with a specific song stimulus. Together, these complex patterns of auditory response among cells in the auditory lobule led to the idea that those neurons are active in association with perception of song features beyond the physical structure of the stimulus.
The speculation that cells in the auditory lobule contribute to song perception has been further bolstered by results from the past decade showing that activity of those cells can encode behaviorally relevant categories of song stimuli. In starlings trained to recognize different groups of songs, activity of CM neurons recorded under anesthesia revealed that individual CM neurons are selectively active in association with songs belonging to one or the other of those groups, and cells can extend their categorization to novel songs that conform to the specifications that define the training group for which those cells are selectively active (Gentner and Margoliash, 2003). Thus, information about learned categories of song perception is encoded in the activity of cells in the auditory lobule, and recent results have identified a subregion of the auditory lobule (CMM, the medial portion of CM) in which cells are extremely selective for specific song components and information about learned categories is especially well-represented in the activity of individual cells (Jeanne et al., 2011; Meliza et al., 2010). These data implicate neurons in the auditory lobule, especially those in CM, as part of a network that can be profoundly shaped by experience and that likely plays an important role in the emergence of song perception.
Recent results also indicate that cells in CM and NCM may contribute to not only song perception but also to memorization of those songs in service of vocal learning. The earliest such hints came from studies of ZENK gene expression in which male zebra finches were played the songs that served as the tutor song for their own song learning. Gene expression in NCM is greatest in birds that copy their tutor song most accurately, suggesting a possible link between NCM activity and the precision of tutor song representation (Bolhuis et al., 2000; Terpstra et al., 2004). Later experiments extended those findings by recording auditory responses in NCM of awake, restrained zebra finches and finding that those cells are selectively responsive to the song of a tutor heard during early development, and properties of that auditory response were correlated with the fidelity of the bird's imitation of that model (Phan et al., 2006). Those tantalizing results led to the speculation that the auditory lobule could play an important role in the memorization of the tutor song, and that was tested in an elegant experiment by Sarah London and David Clayton (2008). In a series of behavioral experiments coupled with studies of gene expression in CM and NCM, cells in those sites were reversibly inactivated only during tutor exposure but not during alternate days when the pupil was allowed to rehearse in the absence of tutor instruction. In those birds, the pupil's imitation of the tutor song was compromised. In contrast, inactivation of CM and NCM during rehearsal but not during tutor song exposure had little or no effect on the quality of imitation. Together, these results make a strong case that cells in the auditory lobule play central role in auditory processing and the formation of a tutor memory that guides imitative learning (Gobes and Bolhuis, 2007). In another set of electrophysiological recordings in very young zebra finches that had been exposed to tutor song but had not yet begun to rehearse, Adret and colleagues (2012) found neurons in the auditory lobule that were selectively active in association with hearing the tutor song. Although those cells were not especially common, they nonetheless demonstrate the presence of cells in songbird auditory cortical areas that are modified by perceptual and/or social experience during vocal learning and development. Studies of female birds have also suggested a role for auditory lobule neurons in song memorization even without any attempt at vocal imitation. Those cells are active in association with presentation of the familiar song of a female bird's father (Terpstra et al., 2006), or the song or the call of a female bird's mate (Menardy et al., 2012; Vignal et al., 2008; Woolley and Doupe, 2008). Together with results from male birds, these data support the idea that cells in CM and NCM play important roles in perceptual grouping and memorization of the sounds used in vocal communication.
4 Correlates of Auditory Perception in Sensorimotor Cortical Areas
In light of the present consideration of perception and its relation to imitation of the signals used in vocal communication, an important question is whether neural correlates of perception are preserved or perhaps even refined at the level of sensorimotor structures. To test that possibility, Rich Mooney, Steve Nowicki, Susan Peters, Rindy Anderson and I performed a series of studies using swamp sparrows (Melospiza georgiana). Just as humans perceive categorical differences among specific sounds used in speech, swamp sparrows perceive categorical differences between song notes of different duration (Diehl et al., 2004; Nelson and Marler, 1989). Below a categorical boundary, notes are perceived as short regardless of their actual duration; above that boundary, notes are perceived as having long duration. We investigated whether categorical perception of song notes was evident in the activity of individual neurons in the sensorimotor nucleus HVC (Prather et al., 2009). The most informative approach in seeking to understand the neural basis of auditory perception is to investigate the activity of neurons in awake and freely behaving birds as they are engaged in song perception. To achieve that in our experiments, we used a miniature, motorized recording device (Fee and Leonardo, 2001) to sample the activity of individual HVC neurons that project into the forebrain pathway implicated in song perception and plasticity (HVCX cells, Figure 3). In swamp sparrows, individual HVCX neurons express categorical auditory responses to songs containing notes of different duration, and the categorical boundary evident in the activity of individual neurons predicts the boundary evident in song perception (Figure 4) (Prather et al., 2009). Importantly, song stimuli can evoke robust activity in HVCX cells regardless of whether the bird produces the song or the same song is produced by another bird (Figure 5) (Prather et al., 2008). Auditory responses of swamp sparrow HVCX cells are selective for specific song features, and they are responsive to those features regardless of whether they are part of the bird's own repertoire or part of the repertoire of a nearby conspecific. Therefore the selective auditory responses of these cells are not simply a self-tuning mechanism. Instead, they provide a substrate for perception of signals used in communication between individuals. Additional studies have also provided evidence that activity of individual HVC neurons reflects complex features of auditory perception. Specifically, HVC neurons of very young birds recorded during song learning are selectively responsive to the tutor song that the bird is engaged in imitating (Nick and Konishi, 2005; Volman, 1996). Furthermore, HVC auditory responses to songs heard or rehearsed only during juvenile development can persist into adulthood, and auditory responses to those developmentally relevant songs are commonly as strong as or stronger than responses evoked by anything in the adult repertoire (Prather et al., 2010). Because neurons in CM and NCM have also been implicated in song perception and those sites project directly and indirectly to HVC (Bauer et al., 2008; Pinaud et al., 2008; Vates et al., 1996) including important connections through the nucleus interface of the nidopallium (NIf, Figure 2) (Bauer et al., 2008; Cardin and Schmidt, 2004; Coleman and Mooney, 2004; Hosino and Okanoya, 2000; Roberts et al., 2012) and the nucleus Avalanche (Av, Figure 2) as the portion of CM that projects monosynaptically to HVC (Akutagawa and Konishi, 2010), it remains unknown whether neural correlates of perception emerge in HVC or whether HVC activity reflects information present in synaptic inputs from the auditory lobule. Nonetheless, auditory perception is reflected in the activity of individual neurons in HVC of awake and freely behaving birds, providing a locus where perception and motor performance may be linked. Important in our consideration of possible translation of perception into vocal performance, all of the HVCX neurons that express categorical auditory responses are also active when the bird sings (Figure 6). This colocalization of activity related to perception and vocalization in one and the same neuron establishes HVCX cells as very attractive candidates for understanding how auditory perception may influence vocal performance.
5 Translating Sensory Perception into Vocal Performance
Consistent with a role in linking auditory perception and vocal motor performance, individual HVCX neurons are active in association with a specific song both when it is heard and when it is sung. Using each swamp sparrow's adult song repertoire as a means of exploring auditory response selectivity, we found that individual neurons are selective for one song type in the bird's repertoire. The song type that is represented in action potential responses varies among cells, but each HVCX neuron is selective in a nearly all-or-none fashion for what we called its “primary song type” (Prather et al., 2008). Selectivity was also observed when the bird sang. Invariably, the primary song type as defined in the auditory domain was also the primary song type as defined by activity of that cell when the bird sang the elements of its repertoire (Figure 6B). Importantly, tests using transient distortion of auditory feedback indicated that this activity during singing was a motor-related signal as opposed to the bird simply hearing itself sing (Prather et al., 2008). Therefore, individual HVCX neurons in the swamp sparrow brain express precise representations of both auditory perception and song performance. Notably, a correspondence like that found in swamp sparrows is also evident in Bengalese finches (Lonchura striata domestica) (Fujimoto et al., 2011; Prather et al., 2008). These species are of distant phylogenetic relation, and the swamp sparrow song system grows and regresses across seasons but the Bengalese finch song system does not (Tramontin and Brenowitz, 2000). The presence of a temporally precise sensorimotor correspondence and selective representation of individual vocal elements in HVCX neurons of such distantly related species with such different patterns of brain plasticity suggests that sensorimotor correspondence is a fundamental feature of perception and performance of learned vocal signals. It remains to be seen through what network and over what time scales song perception may influence performance of the signals used in vocal communication, but this finding of neural activity related to auditory perception and vocal motor performance co-localized in one and the same neuron provides an especially advantageous animal model in which to investigate how those signals can collectively guide imitative vocal learning.
The presence of auditory and singing-related activity in individual HVCX neurons provides a mechanism that could facilitate song learning and maintenance. Conceptual models of song learning typically involve the comparison of vocal motor commands against the associated auditory feedback to compute an error signal that is used to guide refinement of subsequent song performances (Keller and Hahnloser, 2009; Leonardo, 2004; Mooney, 2009; Solis et al., 2000; Troyer and Doupe, 2000). Therefore, sites that receive both motor-related and auditory input and thus could be sites of sensorimotor comparison have been a topic of great interest in our field. But simply receiving input from both motor-related and auditory sources is not sufficient to perform this comparison because of the potential difference in the timing of activity from each of those sources regarding one and the same vocal signal. It is not clear how the initial motor command from HVC into the song motor pathway, which lasts only milliseconds and precedes the vocalization (Hahnloser et al., 2002), could be preserved for a sufficient duration to enable it to be compared against auditory feedback arriving tens of milliseconds later. One mechanism through which the initial motor command could be preserved and delayed in its recurrence onto sensory-recipient cells is called a corollary discharge. A corollary discharge is essentially a copy of the initial motor command that is diverted along a pathway that does not result in motor activation (Bell, 1989; Crapse and Sommer, 2008a; Crapse and Sommer, 2008b; Sommer and Wurtz, 2008). If the arrival of a corollary discharge onto its target cells occurred at that same time as the arrival of the associated auditory feedback, then such an arrangement could enable direct comparison of neural representations of the motor command and the sensory feedback (Mooney, 2009). Intriguingly, in the species in which this sensorimotor correspondence has been studied to date, the singing-related activity in HVCX neurons is delayed such that it occurs in exact temporal register with the latency of auditory responses in the same neuron (Figure 6B) (Prather et al., 2008). These data support the idea that HVCX singing-related activity may provide a prediction of the expected auditory feedback, and that prediction may be compared to the actual auditory feedback to compute an error signal. The sensorimotor correspondence that we and others have described in HVCX neurons provides a locus where such a comparison may occur in service of learning and maintaining the signals used in vocal communication.
Cells in which activity changes in response to a mismatch between the bird's vocalization and the associated auditory feedback have been described in two sites in the songbird brain. Specifically, cells in the primary auditory cortex of zebra finches (Field L) (Keller and Hahnloser, 2009) and some HVC interneurons in Bengalese finches (Sakata and Brainard, 2008) are selectively active in association with such mismatches. These data confirm that auditory-vocal comparisons occur in the songbird brain, and their conservation across species lends additional support to the idea that such comparisons are a fundamental feature of vocal learners. Michael Brainard and colleagues have demonstrated a functional role for feedback in shaping real-time vocal plasticity (Sakata and Brainard, 2006; Sakata and Brainard, 2008; Sakata and Brainard, 2009; Sober and Brainard, 2009; Sober and Brainard, 2012; Tumer and Brainard, 2007) and the activity of specific neurons (Sakata and Brainard, 2006; Sober et al., 2008), however it remains unknown how neural correlates of distorted auditory feedback are manifest as changes in vocal behavior. Interestingly, not all cases of distorted feedback induce changes in vocal output (e.g., trials recorded soon after the onset of distortion in Leonardo and Konishi 1999 and cases of transient distortion in Prather et al. 2008), and in cases where changes are evident, many of those changes are very subtle (Kozhevnikov and Fee, 2007; Sakata and Brainard, 2006; Sakata and Brainard, 2008). This general resistance to changes in song note sequence and spectral properties in the face of transient mismatches between vocal behavior and auditory feedback is advantageous because it preserves learned behavior in the presence of ambient sounds or songs of other birds nearby. In the case of manipulations that are sufficient to induce changes in note sequence and spectral properties, such as deafening or prolonged exposure to distorted feedback (Leonardo and Konishi, 1999; Nordeen and Nordeen, 1992), one idea is that prolonged error results in the sustained alteration of activity in real-time error detectors such as neurons in Field L and HVC interneurons. Such a paradigm would suggest that chronic alteration of activity of those cells eventually induces changes in the activity of another set of neurons, and it is those cells that are the agents of vocal change. Those putative integrators of prolonged error have not yet been identified, but HVCX neurons are attractive candidates because they receive monosynaptic input (HVC interneurons, Figure 1) and polysynaptic input (Field L, Figure 2) from cells implicated in real-time detection of sensorimotor mismatches and they are the origin of an anterior forebrain pathway implicated in vocal plasticity (Figure 2).
Presently, the possible role of HVCX neurons as sensorimotor comparators remains unknown. On the one hand, the sensorimotor correspondence expressed by HVCX neurons makes them ideal candidates to act as sensorimotor comparators, yet on the other hand our earlier tests indicate that the singing-related activity of HVCX neurons is not affected by acute distortions of auditory feedback. One possible explanation for our earlier results is that the singing-related activity of HVCX neurons is a corollary discharge of song performance, such that comparison of that signal against sensory feedback occurs at some downstream location, presumably in the AFP. This seems unlikely in light of data showing that auditory activity in the AFP is silenced when HVC is inactivated (Roy and Mooney, 2009). Those data suggest that there is little or no additional auditory input beyond that point, however those data are from anesthetized birds and auditory responses can be quite different in the awake bird (e.g., HVCRA cells express strong auditory responses in the anesthetized bird but little or no response in the awake bird) (Cardin and Schmidt, 2003; Dave et al., 1998; Mooney, 2000; Nick and Konishi, 2001; Raksin et al., 2012; Schmidt and Konishi, 1998) (personal observations in awake zebra finches). Intriguingly, preliminary findings reveal auditory responses in VTA neurons that project to Area X of awake zebra finches (Las and Fee, 2008). Those data indicate that the latency of those responses is too short for activity to have passed through the AFP to Area X and then to VTA (Gale et al., 2008; Las and Fee, 2008), suggesting an alternative pathway through which auditory information may enter the AFP to be compared with singing-related activity that enters via HVCX neurons.
An alternative explanation for our previous finding that HVCX neurons are unaffected by short-term changes in auditory feedback is that the strength of corollary discharge that HVCX cells receive from recurrent motor pathways may be much greater than the strength of auditory feedback, such that effects of distorted feedback would be very difficult to detect using extracellular recordings and difficult to detect even with intracellular recordings. The manipulation of auditory input that we used (overlaying a second copy of the primary song type at a random phase delay) was quite effective at eliminating auditory action potentials in those cells, yet there was no detectable effect on HVCX singing-related activity or the bird's song performance, even when the distortion signal was played quite loudly to compete with bone conduction of the self-generated vocalization (Prather et al., 2008). Our manipulation was not sufficient to evoke changes in song behavior, but song changes are commonly observed with other, more prolonged changes in auditory feedback (Andalman and Fee, 2009; Charlesworth et al., 2011; Kozhevnikov and Fee, 2007; Leonardo and Konishi, 1999; Lombardino and Nottebohm, 2000; Sober and Brainard, 2009; Tumer and Brainard, 2007). This difference leaves open the possibility that any possible comparison occurring in HVCX cells is specific to other forms of distorted auditory feedback or may require distortion over a much longer duration in order to affect song behavior. Some cells in HVC, which are unidentified but are thought to be interneurons, are sensitive to acute alterations of auditory feedback, but feedback-induced changes in the activity of those cells are relatively subtle (Sakata and Brainard, 2008). Fundamental challenges in gaining further insight will be the need to record intrasomatically from individual neurons over many song performances (Long et al., 2010), the need to record from identified populations of neurons over durations sufficient for behavioral changes to emerge, and the need to selectively manipulate neural activity to ask whether altered activity of HVCX neurons plays a causal role in altering auditory perception or vocal performance (Roberts et al., 2010; Scharff et al., 2000; Tschida and Mooney, 2012). Attention should be given to the possibility that not only real-time error detection but also offline changes such as those that occur during sleep may play important roles in adaptive vocal plasticity (Dave and Margoliash, 2000; Deregnaucourt et al., 2005; Hahnloser and Fee, 2007; Hahnloser et al., 2006; Margoliash and Schmidt, 2010; Peigneux et al., 2004; Rasch et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2010; Shank and Margoliash, 2009). Continued refinement of our approaches to achieve those goals will be invaluable in our quest to understand the information encoded in the activity of HVCX neurons and their role as possible sensorimotor comparators.
6 Central Questions and Important Future Directions
The findings reviewed here highlight several questions that are of central importance in our field. First, where in the brain does auditory perception emerge, and how is that perception stored and recalled as a template to guide vocal learning? As noted in section 2, CM and NCM represent features of song perception far beyond the acoustic song structure, and activity in those reciprocally interconnected sites is essential in the formation of a tutor song memory that guides juvenile learning. Therefore, it seems likely that activity in the auditory lobule is an important component of song perception, but additional studies using fluorescent imaging to examine the effect of tutoring on neurons in the song system have also revealed effects of tutor experience on sensorimotor cells in HVC. Specifically, dendritic spines on HVCX neurons are rapidly stabilized within 24 hours following a young male bird's first exposure to tutor song (Roberts et al., 2010), and focal disruption of activity in HVC also impairs song copying (Roberts et al., 2012). As noted above, cells in the auditory lobule have also been implicated in song perception and memory, and those cells receive synaptic input from HVC neurons (Akutagawa and Konishi, 2010). Together, those results indicate that a process as complex as perception, memorization and recall of a song memory in service of imitation is distributed across several brain sites. Future investigations will focus on CM, NCM and HVC as important contributors to the emergence of song perception and the utilization of song memories.
A second set of centrally important questions is: through what networks are sensory and motor signals compared, and how does activity emerging from that comparison modify subsequent motor performances to guide juvenile learning and adult maintenance of vocal signals? Initial investigations indicate that when the bird is singing, HVCX activity is not affected by disruption of auditory feedback (Kozhevnikov and Fee, 2007; Prather et al., 2008). As noted in section 4, that does not rule out the possibility that a much longer-duration mismatch between the motor command signal and the associated auditory feedback could result in the emergence of feedback-sensitivity in those cells. Such conservative responses to brief sensorimotor errors would be very beneficial to prevent plasticity in the face of transient mismatches induced by environmental noise. Sites downstream of HVCX cells have been implicated in song learning and behavioral plasticity, supporting a possible functional role for sensorimotor comparison in HVCX cells or at some downstream location (Bottjer et al., 1984; Olveczky et al., 2005; Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991; Sohrabji et al., 1990). If subsequent tests reveal that HVCX neurons are insensitive to auditory feedback under any condition, then the presence of a motor-related signal in the AFP would raise the possibility that sites in that pathway may also be part an auditory-vocal comparator circuit. Identifying the circuit(s) through which motor commands and sensory feedback are compared will also open the door to exploring how the output of sensorimotor comparator neurons is harnessed to refine juvenile motor performance as it becomes progressively more similar to a memorized song model. Presently it remains unknown which brain regions participate in sensorimotor comparison and whether the output of that hypothetical comparator may simply free the system to take on a new state, or whether it may instruct the system in a directed transition to minimize the difference between performance and model (Solis et al., 2000). Those topics will remain an important focus in our field, and songbirds will continue to be an excellent animal model to understand the circuitry and cellular mechanisms that underlie perception and performance of the sounds used in vocal communication. Because of the growing body of evidence that structures in the songbird brain are analogous, and in some cases homologous, to structures in the human brain, defining the neural basis of vocal communication in songbirds holds the promise of profoundly improving clinical therapies for human communication disorders.
HIGHLIGHTS
Songbirds are an excellent model for studying the neural basis of vocal communication
Insight into a basis of auditory perception and sensorimotor integration is reviewed
Individual neurons are activated during both song perception and song performance
Comparison of performance vs. feedback guides vocal learning and maintenance
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I am very grateful to Rich Mooney, Steve Nowicki, Susan Peters and Rindy Anderson for our collaboration in the laboratory experiments and field work using swamp sparrows. S. Prather, K. Murphy and J. Dunning provided comments on the manuscript. A portion of the research reported in this publication was supported by an Institutional Development Award (IDeA) from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under grant number P30 GM103398.
ABBREVIATIONS
A discussion of many of these brain sites and their analogous or homologous relation to sites in the mammalian nervous system has been published in papers published by a consortium of researchers led by Anton Reiner (Reiner et al., 2004) and Erich Jarvis (Jarvis et al., 2005).
- Av
Nucleus avalanche
- Area X
Area X (specialized region of the avian striatum)
- CM
Caudal mesopallium
- DLM
Nucleus dorsolateralis anterior, pars medialis
- DMP
Nucleus dorsomedialis posterior thalami
- Field L
Field L (avian primary auditory cortex)
- HVC
HVC (abbreviation used as a proper noun)
- LLv
Ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus
- LMAN
Lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium
- MLd
Nucleus mesencephalicus lateralis, pars dorsalis
- MMAN
Medial magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium
- NCM
Caudomedial nidopallium
- NIf
Nucleus interface of the nidopallium
- nXIIts
Hypoglossal nucleus, tracheosyringeal nerve (12th cranial nerve nucleus)
- Ov
Nucleus ovoidalis
- RA
Robust nucleus of the arcopallium
- UVA
Nucleus uvaeformis
- VP
Ventral pallidum
- VTA
Ventral tegmental area
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
REFERENCES
- Adret P, Meliza CD, Margoliash D. Song tutoring in presinging zebra finch juveniles biases a small population of higher-order song-selective neurons toward the tutor song. J Neurophysiol. 2012;108:1977–87. doi: 10.1152/jn.00905.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akutagawa E, Konishi M. New brain pathways found in the vocal control system of a songbird. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518:3086–100. doi: 10.1002/cne.22383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J Commun Disord. 2004;37:325–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amador A, Perl YS, Mindlin GB, Margoliash D. Elemental gesture dynamics are encoded by song premotor cortical neurons. Nature. 2013;495:59–64. doi: 10.1038/nature11967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amin N, Grace JA, Theunissen FE. Neural response to bird's own song and tutor song in the zebra finch field L and caudal mesopallium. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2004;190:469–89. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0511-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andalman AS, Fee MS. A basal ganglia-forebrain circuit in the songbird biases motor output to avoid vocal errors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:12518–23. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903214106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arbib MA. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behav Brain Sci. 2005;28:105–24. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x05000038. discussion 125–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aronov D, Andalman AS, Fee MS. A specialized forebrain circuit for vocal babbling in the juvenile songbird. Science. 2008;320:630–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1155140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashmore RC, Wild JM, Schmidt MF. Brainstem and forebrain contributions to the generation of learned motor behaviors for song. J Neurosci. 2005;25:8543–54. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1668-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer EE, Coleman MJ, Roberts TF, Roy A, Prather JF, Mooney R. A synaptic basis for auditory-vocal integration in the songbird. J Neurosci. 2008;28:1509–22. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3838-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell CC. Sensory coding and corollary discharge effects in mormyrid electric fish. J Exp Biol. 1989;146:229–253. doi: 10.1242/jeb.146.1.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolhuis JJ, Eda-Fujiwara H. Bird brains and songs: neural mechanisms of birdsong perception and memory. Anim Biol. 2003;53:129–145. [Google Scholar]
- Bolhuis JJ, Gahr M. Neural mechanisms of birdsong memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:347–57. doi: 10.1038/nrn1904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolhuis JJ, Zijlstra GG, den Boer-Visser AM, Van Der Zee EA. Localized neuronal activation in the zebra finch brain is related to the strength of song learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97:2282–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.030539097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottjer SW, Miesner EA, Arnold AP. Forebrain lesions disrupt development but not maintenance of song in passerine birds. Science. 1984;224:901–3. doi: 10.1126/science.6719123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard MS, Doupe AJ. Postlearning consolidation of birdsong: stabilizing effects of age and anterior forebrain lesions. J Neurosci. 2001;21:2501–17. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-07-02501.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brass M, Schmitt RM, Spengler S, Gergely G. Investigating action understanding: inferential processes versus action simulation. Curr Biol. 2007;17:2117–21. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brenowitz EA, Beecher MD. Song learning in birds: diversity and plasticity, opportunities and challenges. Trends in Neurosciences. 2005;28:127–132. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Schmidt MF. Song system auditory responses are stable and highly tuned during sedation, rapidly modulated and unselective during wakefulness, and suppressed by arousal. J Neurophys. 2003;90:2884–99. doi: 10.1152/jn.00391.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Schmidt MF. Auditory responses in multiple sensorimotor song system nuclei are co-modulated by behavioral state. J Neurophys. 2004;91:2148–63. doi: 10.1152/jn.00918.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charlesworth JD, Tumer EC, Warren TL, Brainard MS. Learning the microstructure of successful behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:373–80. doi: 10.1038/nn.2748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chew SJ, Vicario DS, Nottebohm F. A large-capacity memory system that recognizes the calls and songs of individual birds. PNAS. 1996;93:1950–1955. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.5.1950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chew SJ, Mello C, Nottebohm F, Jarvis E, Vicario DS. Decrements in Auditory Responses to a Repeated Conspecific Song Are Long-Lasting and Require 2 Periods of Protein-Synthesis in the Songbird Forebrain. PNAS. 1995;92:3406–3410. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.8.3406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman MJ, Mooney R. Synaptic transformations underlying highly selective auditory representations of learned birdsong. J Neurosci. 2004;24:7251–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0947-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman MJ, Roy A, Wild JM, Mooney R. Thalamic gating of auditory responses in telencephalic song control nuclei. J Neurosci. 2007;27:10024–36. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2215-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowie R, Douglas-Cowie E. Postlingually Acquired Deafness: Speech Deterioration and the Wider Consequences Mouton de Gruyter. New York: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Crapse TB, Sommer MA. Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008a;9:587–600. doi: 10.1038/nrn2457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crapse TB, Sommer MA. Corollary discharge circuits in the primate brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008b;18:552–557. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creutzfeldt O, Ojemann G, Lettich E. Neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal lobe. II. Responses to the subjects own voice. Exp Brain Res. 1989;77:476–89. doi: 10.1007/BF00249601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dave AS, Margoliash D. Song replay during sleep and computational rules for sensorimotor vocal learning. Science. 2000;290:812–6. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5492.812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dave AS, Yu AC, Margoliash D. Behavioral state modulation of auditory activity in a vocal motor system. Science. 1998;282:2250–4. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5397.2250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deregnaucourt S, Mitra PP, Feher O, Pytte C, Tchernichovski O. How sleep affects the developmental learning of bird song. Nature. 2005;433:710–6. doi: 10.1038/nature03275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diehl RL, Lotto AJ, Holt LL. Speech perception. Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:149–79. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doupe AJ, Kuhl PK. Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1999;22:567–631. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DuBois AL, Nowicki S, Searcy WA. Discrimination of vocal performance by male swamp sparrows. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65:717–726. [Google Scholar]
- Fee MS, Leonardo A. Miniature motorized microdrive and commutator system for chronic neural recording in small animals. J Neurosci Methods. 2001;112:83–94. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(01)00426-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrari PF, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L. Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:1703–14. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02601.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox PT, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Hirsch TB, Downs JH, Martin C, Jerabek P, Glass T, Lancaster JL. A PET study of the neural systems of stuttering. Nature. 1996;382:158–61. doi: 10.1038/382158a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujimoto H, Hasegawa T, Watanabe D. Neural Coding of Syntactic Structure in Learned Vocalizations in the Songbird. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:10023–10033. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1606-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Funabiki Y, Konishi M. Long memory in song learning by zebra finches. J Neurosci. 2003;23:6928–6935. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-17-06928.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gale SD, Person AL, Perkel DJ. A novel basal ganglia pathway forms a loop linking a vocal learning circuit with its dopaminergic input. J Comp Neurol. 2008;508:824–39. doi: 10.1002/cne.21700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain. 1996;119(Pt 2):593–609. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gentner TQ, Margoliash D. Neuronal populations and single cells representing learned auditory objects. Nature. 2003;424:669–674. doi: 10.1038/nature01731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gentner TQ, Hulse SH, Bentley GE, Ball GF. Individual vocal recognition and the effect of partial lesions to HVc on discrimination, learning, and categorization of conspecific song in adult songbirds. J Neurobiol. 2000;42:117–33. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(200001)42:1<117::aid-neu11>3.0.co;2-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gobes SMH, Bolhuis JJ. Birdsong memory: A neural dissociation between song recognition and production. Curr Biol. 2007;17:789–793. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grace JA, Amin N, Singh NC, Theunissen FE. Selectivity for conspecific song in the zebra finch auditory forebrain. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89:472–87. doi: 10.1152/jn.00088.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Fee MS. Sleep-related spike bursts in HVC are driven by the nucleus interface of the nidopallium. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:423–35. doi: 10.1152/jn.00547.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird. Nature. 2002;419:65–70. doi: 10.1038/nature00974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. Sleep-related neural activity in a premotor and a basal-ganglia pathway of the songbird. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:794–812. doi: 10.1152/jn.01064.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hari R, Forss N, Avikainen S, Kirveskari E, Salenius S, Rizzolatti G. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:15061–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hessler NA, Doupe AJ. Singing-related neural activity in a dorsal forebrain-basal ganglia circuit of adult zebra finches. J Neurosci. 1999;19:10461–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-23-10461.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hosino T, Okanoya K. Lesion of a higher-order song nucleus disrupts phrase level complexity in Bengalese finches. Neuroreport. 2000;11:2091–5. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200007140-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science. 1999;286:2526–8. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5449.2526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G. Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e79. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis E, Gunturkun O, Bruce L, Csillag A, Karten H, Kuenzel W, Medina L, Paxinos G, Perkel DJ, Shimizu T, Striedter G, Wild JM, Ball GF, Dugas-Ford J, Durand SE, Hough GE, Husband S, Kubikova L, Lee DW, Mello CV, Powers A, Siang C, Smulders TV, Wada K, White SA, Yamamoto K, Yu J, Reiner A, Butler AB, Consorti ABN. Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:151–159. doi: 10.1038/nrn1606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis ED. Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Ann Ny Acad Sci. 2004;1016:749–777. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis ED, Nottebohm F. Motor-driven gene expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94:4097–102. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.8.4097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeanne JM, Thompson JV, Sharpee TO, Gentner TQ. Emergence of learned categorical representations within an auditory forebrain circuit. J Neurosci. 2011;31:2595–606. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3930-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jurgens U. Neural pathways underlying vocal control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26:235–58. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00068-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kao MH, Brainard MS. Lesions of an avian basal ganglia circuit prevent context-dependent changes to song variability. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:1441–55. doi: 10.1152/jn.01138.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karten H, Shimizu T. The Origins of Neocortex: Connections and Lamination as Distinct Events in Evolution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1989;1:291–301. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.4.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz LC, Gurney ME. Auditory responses in the zebra finch's motor system for song. Brain Res. 1981;221:192–7. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)91073-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller GB, Hahnloser RH. Neural processing of auditory feedback during vocal practice in a songbird. Nature. 2009;457:187–90. doi: 10.1038/nature07467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konishi M, Emlen ST, Ricklefs RE, Wingfield JC. Contributions of bird studies to biology. Science. 1989;246:465–72. doi: 10.1126/science.2683069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. Singing-related activity of identified HVC neurons in the zebra finch. J Neurophys. 2007;97:4271–4283. doi: 10.1152/jn.00952.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroodsma DE, Konishi M. A Suboscine Bird (Eastern Phoebe, Sayornis-Phoebe) Develops Normal Song without Auditory-Feedback. Anim Behav. 1991;42:477–487. [Google Scholar]
- Las L, Fee M. Recordings of striatal-projecting neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) of the juvenile zebra finch during song learning. Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting Abstracts.2008. [Google Scholar]
- Leonardo A. Experimental test of the birdsong error-correction model. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:16935–40. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407870101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonardo A, Konishi M. Decrystallization of adult birdsong by perturbation of auditory feedback. Nature. 1999;399:466–70. doi: 10.1038/20933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonardo A, Fee MS. Ensemble coding of vocal control in birdsong. J Neurosci. 2005;25:652–61. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3036-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewicki MS, Arthur BJ. Hierarchical organization of auditory temporal context sensitivity. J Neurosci. 1996;16:6987–98. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-21-06987.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liberman AM, Cooper FS, Shankweiler DP, Studdert-Kennedy M. Perception of the speech code. Psychol Rev. 1967;74:431–61. doi: 10.1037/h0020279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lombardino AJ, Nottebohm F. Age at deafening affects the stability of learned song in adult male zebra finches. J Neurosci. 2000;20:5054–64. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-13-05054.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- London SE, Clayton DF. Functional identification of sensory mechanisms required for developmental song learning. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:579–86. doi: 10.1038/nn.2103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long MA, Jin DZ, Fee MS. Support for a synaptic chain model of neuronal sequence generation. Nature. 2010;468:394–9. doi: 10.1038/nature09514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margoliash D. Preference for autogenous song by auditory neurons in a song system nucleus of the white-crowned sparrow. J Neurosci. 1986;6:1643–61. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-06-01643.1986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margoliash D, Schmidt MF. Sleep, off-line processing, and vocal learning. Brain Lang. 2010;115:45–58. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marler P, Tamura M. Culturally Transmitted Patterns of Vocal Behavior in Sparrows. Science. 1964;146:1483–&. doi: 10.1126/science.146.3650.1483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marler P, Peters S. Sparrows Learn Adult Song and More from Memory. Science. 1981;213:780–782. doi: 10.1126/science.213.4509.780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marler P, Peters S. Long-Term Storage of Learned Birdsongs Prior to Production. Anim Behav. 1982;30:479–482. [Google Scholar]
- Marler P, Sherman V. Song Structure without Auditory-Feedback - Emendations of the Auditory Template Hypothesis. J Neurosci. 1983;3:517–531. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-03-00517.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marler P, Peters S. A Sensitive Period for Song Acquisition in the Song Sparrow, Melospiza-Melodia - a Case of Age-Limited Learning. Ethology. 1987;76:89–100. [Google Scholar]
- Marler P, Peters S. Sensitive Periods for Song Acquisition from Tape Recordings and Live Tutors in the Swamp Sparrow, Melospiza-Georgiana. Ethology. 1988;77:76–84. [Google Scholar]
- McCasland JS, Konishi M. Interaction between auditory and motor activities in an avian song control nucleus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1981;78:7815–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.78.12.7815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meliza CD, Margoliash D. Emergence of selectivity and tolerance in the avian auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2012;32:15158–68. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0845-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meliza CD, Chi Z, Margoliash D. Representations of conspecific song by starling secondary forebrain auditory neurons: toward a hierarchical framework. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103:1195–208. doi: 10.1152/jn.00464.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mello CV, Vicario DS, Clayton DF. Song Presentation Induces Gene-Expression in the Songbird Forebrain. PNAS. 1992;89:6818–6822. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.15.6818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menardy F, Touiki K, Dutrieux G, Bozon B, Vignal C, Mathevon N, Del Negro C. Social experience affects neuronal responses to male calls in adult female zebra finches. Eur J Neurosci. 2012;35:1322–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08047.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney R. Different subthreshold mechanisms underlie song selectivity in identified HVc neurons of the zebra finch. J Neurosci. 2000;20:5420–36. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-14-05420.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney R. Neural mechanisms for learned birdsong. Learn Mem. 2009;16:655–69. doi: 10.1101/lm.1065209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney R, Prather JF. The HVC microcircuit: the synaptic basis for interactions between song motor and vocal plasticity pathways. J Neurosci. 2005;25:1952–64. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3726-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney R, Prather JF, Roberts T. Neurophysiology of Birdsong Learning. In: Eichenbaum H, editor. Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference, Vol. Vol. 3 Memory Systems. Elsevier; Oxford: 2008. pp. 441–474. [Google Scholar]
- Mukamel R, Ekstrom AD, Kaplan J, Iacoboni M, Fried I. Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Curr Biol. 2010;20:750–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson DA, Marler P. Categorical perception of a natural stimulus continuum: birdsong. Science. 1989;244:976–8. doi: 10.1126/science.2727689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nick TA, Konishi M. Dynamic control of auditory activity during sleep: correlation between song response and EEG. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:14012–14016. doi: 10.1073/pnas.251525298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nick TA, Konishi M. Neural song preference during vocal learning in the zebra finch depends on age and state. J Neurobiol. 2005;62:231–42. doi: 10.1002/neu.20087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nixdorf-Bergweiler B, Bischof H. A Stereotaxic Atlas Of The Brain Of The Zebra Finch, Taeniopygia Guttata, With Special Emphasis On Telencephalic Visual And Song System Nuclei in Transverse and Sagittal Sections. National Center for Biotechnology Information (US); Bethesda, MD: 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Nordeen KW, Nordeen EJ. Auditory feedback is necessary for the maintenance of stereotyped song in adult zebra finches. Behav Neural Biol. 1992;57:58–66. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(92)90757-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm F, Stokes TM, Leonard CM. Central control of song in the canary, Serinus canarius. J Comp Neurol. 1976;165:457–86. doi: 10.1002/cne.901650405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okanoya K, Yamaguchi A. Adult bengalese fiches (lonchura striata var. domestica) require real-time auditory feedback to produce normal song syntax. J Neurobiol. 1997;33:343–356. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olveczky BP, Andalman AS, Fee MS. Vocal experimentation in the juvenile songbird requires a basal ganglia circuit. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olveczky BP, Otchy TM, Goldberg JH, Aronov D, Fee MS. Changes in the neural control of a complex motor sequence during learning. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:386–97. doi: 10.1152/jn.00018.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peigneux P, Laureys S, Fuchs S, Collette F, Perrin F, Reggers J, Phillips C, Degueldre C, Del Fiore G, Aerts J, Luxen A, Maquet P. Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep? Neuron. 2004;44:535–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters S, Marler P, Nowicki S. Song Sparrows Learn from Limited Exposure to Song Models. Condor. 1992;94:1016–1019. [Google Scholar]
- Phan ML, Pytte CL, Vicario DS. Early auditory experience generates long-lasting memories that may subserve vocal learning in songbirds. PNAS. 2006;103:1088–1093. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510136103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinaud R, Terleph TA, Tremere LA, Phan ML, Dagostin AA, Leao RM, Mello CV, Vicario DS. Inhibitory network interactions shape the auditory processing of natural communication signals in the songbird auditory forebrain. J Neurophys. 2008;100:441–55. doi: 10.1152/jn.01239.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prather JF, Peters S, Nowicki S, Mooney R. Precise auditory-vocal mirroring in neurons for learned vocal communication. Nature. 2008;451:305–10. doi: 10.1038/nature06492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prather JF, Peters S, Nowicki S, Mooney R. Persistent representation of juvenile experience in the adult songbird brain. J Neurosci. 2010;30:10586–10598. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6042-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prather JF, Nowicki S, Anderson RC, Peters S, Mooney R. Neural correlates of categorical perception in learned vocal communication. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:221–8. doi: 10.1038/nn.2246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raksin JN, Glaze CM, Smith S, Schmidt MF. Linear and nonlinear auditory response properties of interneurons in a high-order avian vocal motor nucleus during wakefulness. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107:2185–2201. doi: 10.1152/jn.01003.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasch B, Buchel C, Gais S, Born J. Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science. 2007;315:1426–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1138581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiner A, Perkel DJ, Bruce LL, Butler AB, Csillag A, Kuenzel W, Medina L, Paxinos G, Shimizu T, Striedter G, Wild M, Ball GF, Durand S, Gunturkun O, Lee DW, Mello CV, Powers A, White SA, Hough G, Kubikova L, Smulders TV, Wada K, Dugas-Ford J, Husband S, Yamamoto K, Yu J, Siang C, Jarvis ED. Revised nomenclature for avian telencephalon and some related brainstem nuclei. J Comp Neurol. 2004;473:377–414. doi: 10.1002/cne.20118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G, Arbib MA. Language within our grasp. Trends Neurosci. 1998;21:188–94. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(98)01260-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:169–92. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:661–70. doi: 10.1038/35090060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts TF, Tschida KA, Klein ME, Mooney R. Rapid spine stabilization and synaptic enhancement at the onset of behavioural learning. Nature. 2010;463:948–52. doi: 10.1038/nature08759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts TF, Klein ME, Kubke MF, Wild JM, Mooney R. Telencephalic neurons monosynaptically link brainstem and forebrain premotor networks necessary for song. J Neurosci. 2008;28:3479–89. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0177-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts TF, Gobes SM, Murugan M, Olveczky BP, Mooney R. Motor circuits are required to encode a sensory model for imitative learning. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:1454–9. doi: 10.1038/nn.3206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosen MJ, Mooney R. Inhibitory and excitatory mechanisms underlying auditory responses to learned vocalizations in the songbird nucleus HVC. Neuron. 2003;39:177–94. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00357-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosen MJ, Mooney R. Synaptic interactions underlying song-selectivity in the avian nucleus HVC revealed by dual intracellular recordings. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:1158–75. doi: 10.1152/jn.00100.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roy A, Mooney R. Song decrystallization in adult zebra finches does not require the song nucleus NIf. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102:979–91. doi: 10.1152/jn.00293.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakata JT, Brainard MS. Real-time contributions of auditory feedback to avian vocal motor control. J Neurosci. 2006;26:9619–28. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2027-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakata JT, Brainard MS. Online contributions of auditory feedback to neural activity in avian song control circuitry. J Neurosci. 2008;28:11378–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3254-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakata JT, Brainard MS. Social context rapidly modulates the influence of auditory feedback on avian vocal motor control. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102:2485–97. doi: 10.1152/jn.00340.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scharff C, Nottebohm F. A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning. J Neurosci. 1991;11:2896–913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-09-02896.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scharff C, Nottebohm F, Cynx J. Conspecific and heterospecific song discrimination in male zebra finches with lesions in the anterior forebrain pathway. J Neurobiol. 1998;36:81–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scharff C, Kirn JR, Grossman M, Macklis JD, Nottebohm F. Targeted neuronal death affects neuronal replacement and vocal behavior in adult songbirds. Neuron. 2000;25:481–92. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80910-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MF. Pattern of interhemispheric synchronization in HVc during singing correlates with key transitions in the song pattern. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:3931–49. doi: 10.1152/jn.00003.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MF. Introduction: Contributions of bird studies to behavioral and neurobiological research. ILAR J. 2010;51:305–9. doi: 10.1093/ilar.51.4.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MF, Konishi M. Gating of auditory responses in the vocal control system of awake songbirds. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:513–518. doi: 10.1038/2232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Searcy WA, Mcarthur PD, Peters SS, Marler P. Response of Male Song and Swamp Sparrows to Neighbor, Stranger, and Self Songs. Behaviour. 1981;77:152–163. [Google Scholar]
- Shank SS, Margoliash D. Sleep and sensorimotor integration during early vocal learning in a songbird. Nature. 2009;458:73–7. doi: 10.1038/nature07615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simpson HB, Vicario DS. Brain pathways for learned and unlearned vocalizations differ in zebra finches. J Neurosci. 1990;10:1541–56. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-05-01541.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sober SJ, Brainard MS. Adult birdsong is actively maintained by error correction. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:927–31. doi: 10.1038/nn.2336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sober SJ, Brainard MS. Vocal learning is constrained by the statistics of sensorimotor experience. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:21099–21103. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1213622109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sober SJ, Wohlgemuth MJ, Brainard MS. Central contributions to acoustic variation in birdsong. J Neurosci. 2008;28:10370–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2448-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sohrabji F, Nordeen EJ, Nordeen KW. Selective impairment of song learning following lesions of a forebrain nucleus in the juvenile zebra finch. Behav Neural Biol. 1990;53:51–63. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(90)90797-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solis MM, Brainard MS, Hessler NA, Doupe AJ. Song selectivity and sensorimotor signals in vocal learning and production. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97:11836–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.22.11836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2008;31:317–38. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Striedter GF, Vu ET. Bilateral feedback projections to the forebrain in the premotor network for singing in zebra finches. J Neurobiol. 1998;34:27–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stripling R, Volman SF, Clayton DF. Response modulation in the zebra finch neostriatum: Relationship to nuclear gene regulation. J Neurosci. 1997;17:3883–3893. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-10-03883.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stripling R, Kruse AA, Clayton DF. Development of song responses in the zebra finch caudomedial neostriatum: Role of genomic and electrophysiological activities. J Neurobiol. 2001;48:163–180. doi: 10.1002/neu.1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terleph TA, Mello CV, Vicario DS. Species differences in auditory processing dynamics in songbird auditory telencephalon. Dev Neurobiol. 2007;67:1498–510. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terpstra NJ, Bolhuis JJ, den Boer-Visser AM. An analysis of the neural representation of birdsong memory. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4971–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0570-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terpstra NJ, Bolhuis JJ, Riebel K, van der Burg JM, den Boer-Visser AM. Localized brain activation specific to auditory memory in a female songbird. J Comp Neurol. 2006;494:784–91. doi: 10.1002/cne.20831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tramontin AD, Brenowitz EA. Seasonal plasticity in the adult brain. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:251–8. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01558-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Troyer TW, Doupe AJ. An associational model of birdsong sensorimotor learning I. Efference copy and the learning of song syllables. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:1204–23. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.3.1204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tschida KA, Mooney R. Deafening drives cell-type-specific changes to dendritic spines in a sensorimotor nucleus important to learned vocalizations. Neuron. 2012;73:1028–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tumer EC, Brainard MS. Performance variability enables adaptive plasticity of `crystallized' adult birdsong. Nature. 2007;450:1240–1244. doi: 10.1038/nature06390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vates GE, Broome BM, Mello CV, Nottebohm F. Auditory pathways of caudal telencephalon and their relation to the song system of adult male zebra finches. J Comp Neurol. 1996;366:613–42. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960318)366:4<613::AID-CNE5>3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vicario DS. Organization of the zebra finch song control system: II. Functional organization of outputs from nucleus Robustus archistriatalis. J Comp Neurol. 1991;309:486–494. doi: 10.1002/cne.903090405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vignal C, Bouchut C, Mathevon N. Sound-induced brain activity depends on stimulus subjective salience in female zebra finches. C R Biol. 2008;331:347–56. doi: 10.1016/j.crvi.2008.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volman SF. Quantitative assessment of song-selectivity in the zebra finch “high vocal center”. J Comp Physiol A. 1996;178:849–62. doi: 10.1007/BF00225832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wild JM. The avian nucleus retroambigualis: a nucleus for breathing, singing and calling. Brain Res. 1993;606:319–324. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)91001-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wild JM. Functional neuroanatomy of the sensorimotor control of singing. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1016:438–62. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams H, Mehta N. Changes in adult zebra finch song require a forebrain nucleus that is not necessary for song production. J Neurobiol. 1999;39:14–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolley SC, Doupe AJ. Social context-induced song variation affects female behavior and gene expression. PLoS Biol. 2008;6:e62. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolley SM, Rubel EW. Bengalese finches Lonchura Striata domestica depend upon auditory feedback for the maintenance of adult song. J Neurosci. 1997;17:6380–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-16-06380.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu AC, Margoliash D. Temporal hierarchical control of singing in birds. Science. 1996;273:1871–5. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5283.1871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]