Skip to main content
Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN logoLink to Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN
. 2013 Nov;38(6):366–380. doi: 10.1503/jpn.120178

COMT, neuropsychological function and brain structure in schizophrenia: a systematic review and neurobiological interpretation

Elisa Ira 1,, Martina Zanoni 1, Mirella Ruggeri 1, Paola Dazzan 1,*, Sarah Tosato 1,*
PMCID: PMC3819150  PMID: 23527885

Abstract

Background

Endophenotypes in genetic psychiatry may increase our understanding of the molecular mechanisms underlying disease risk and its manifestations. We sought to investigate the link between neuropsychological impairments and brain structural abnormalities associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia to improve understanding of the pathophysiology of this disorder.

Methods

We performed a systematic review using studies identified in PubMed and MEDLINE (from the date of the first available article to July 2012). Our review examined evidence of an association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and both neuropsychological performance and brain structure in patients with psychosis, in their relatives and in healthy individuals (step 1). The review also explored whether the neuropsychological tasks and brain structures identified in step 1 met the criteria for an endophenotype (step 2). Then we evaluated evidence that the neuropsychological endophenotypes identified in step 2 are associated with the brain structure endophenotypes identified in that step (step 3). Finally, we propose a neurobiological interpretation for this evidence.

Results

A poorer performance on the n-back task and the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) and smaller temporal and frontal brain areas were associated with the COMT Val allele in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives and met most of the criteria for an endophenotype. It is possible that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism therefore contributes to the development of these neuropsychological and brain structural endophenotypes of schizophrenia, in which the prefrontal cortex may represent the neural substrate underlying both n-back and CPT performances.

Limitations

The association between a single genetic variant and an endophenotype does not necessarily imply a causal relationship between them.

Conclusion

This evidence and the proposed interpretation contribute to explain, at least in part, the biological substrate of 4 important endophenotypes that characterize schizophrenia.

Introduction

The catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene is one of the most investigated candidate genes for schizophrenia because of its role in the degradation of dopamine (DA). This gene contains a common functional variant, the Val158Met polymorphism, which impairs the thermostability of the mature protein, altering DA levels in several brain regions and specifically in the prefrontal cortex.1,2 To date, several meta-analyses have investigated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and schizophrenia: although the evidence is controversial, the most recent meta-analysis points to a weak association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and schizophrenia (from the Schizophrenia Research Forum, www.szgene.org/meta.asp?geneID=420).36

Interestingly, the dopaminergic system is also involved in some of the neuropsychological impairments frequently described in patients with schizophrenia.7 These impairments are thought to represent the functional correlate of abnormalities in the structure of the brain areas involved in these functions (for a review, see Antonova and colleagues8). Interestingly, there is evidence that the COMT Val allele itself may be associated with abnormalities in these areas in patients with schizophrenia.911

The COMT Val158Met polymorphism may therefore contribute to the risk for schizophrenia by influencing specific neuropsychological impairments and brain structural abnormalities, which have been proposed as endophenotypes for this disorder.12,13 Endophenotypes are heritable traits that possibly represent a causal link between genes and observable phenotypes.14 They have the advantage of being measurable using strategies that involve quantitative units of analysis, and of being amenable to assessment in the laboratory.15 Furthermore, the association between a specific endophenotype and certain genetic variants could be stronger than that with the illness itself.14 According to Gottesman and Gould,14 an endophenotype should

  • be associated with the illness in the population of interest,

  • be heritable,

  • be primarily state independent,

  • cosegregate with the illness within families, and

  • be present in nonaffected family members more frequently than in the general population.

Endophenotypes in genetic psychiatry may increase our understanding of the molecular mechanisms underlying disease risk and its manifestations. Thus, investigating the link between neuropsychological impairments and brain structural abnormalities associated with COMT Val158Met in schizophrenia may provide a better understanding of the pathophysiology of this disorder.

Although many studies have investigated the relationship between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological impairments or brain structural alterations in patients with schizophrenia, in their relatives and in healthy individuals, to our knowledge, no systematic review has critically appraised this evidence and explored the potential relationship between this polymorphism and neuropsychological impairments or the brain structural alterations. We sought to fill this gap with the present systematic review.

Methods

A flow chart of the methodology is presented in Figure 1. In step 1, we searched the PubMed and MEDLINE databases, from the date of the first available article to July 2012, to systematically identify the neuropsychological tasks and brain structural variations reported, as related to the COMT Val158Met polymorphism, across disorders in the psychosis spectrum. To investigate the association between COMT and neuropsychological tasks, we used the terms “catechol-O-methyltransferase” or “COMT” AND “cognitive” or “cognition” or “neuropsychological” or “neuropsychology” or “attention” or “memory” or “executive function” AND “schizophrenia” or “psychosis” or “schizoaffective” or “schizophreniform” or “healthy.” To investigate the association between COMT and brain structure we used the terms “catechol-O-methyltransferase” or “COMT”, AND “MRI” or “magnetic resonance imaging” or “imaging” or “voxel” or “brain structures” AND “schizophrenia” or “psychosis” or “schizoaffective” or “schizophreniform” or “healthy.” We also manually searched the reference lists of the articles identified in our search. Unpublished studies, conference abstracts and poster presentations were not included. One of us (E.I.) selected the articles, which 2 of us (P.D. and S.T.) then checked against our inclusion criteria.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Systematic review methodology.

We included studies that fulfilled the following criteria. First, studies must have evaluated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological tasks in patients with diagnosed schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (i.e., schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder, schizoaffective disorder) and/or in healthy controls and/or in relatives of patients with psychosis. We included only studies conducted using a standardized cognitive measure in adult participants. Behavioural data from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies were also included when reported. Second, studies must have evaluated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and brain volumes using structural MRI in patients with diagnosed schizophrenia-spectrum disorders and/or in healthy controls and/or in relatives of patients with psychosis.

From the studies reviewed, we selected neuropsychological tasks and brain structures strongly associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism according to the following criteria. First, at least 3 studies must have evaluated the association between COMT and those neuropsychological tasks or brain structures in either patients or healthy controls. Second, at least 50% of these studies must have reported an association in patients.

In step 2, we considered each neuropsychological task and brain structure identified in step 1 to establish whether they satisfied the criteria for endophenotypes.

In step 3, we systematically searched the PubMed and MEDLINE databases, from the date of the first available article to July 2012, to verify whether the neuropsychological and the brain structural endophenotypes identified in step 2 were associated with each other. We used the name of each neuropsychological task as a search term combined with the terms “MRI” or “magnetic resonance imaging” or “imaging” or “voxel” or “brain structures” AND “schizophrenia” or “psychosis” or “schizoaffective” or “schizophreniform” or “healthy.” In addition, we manually searched the reference lists of the articles identified in our search. Unpublished studies, conference abstracts and poster presentations were not included.

Results

Step 1a: association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological tests

We identified 53 studies (Table 1) that explored the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and various neuropsychological tests. We present the neuropsychological tests that satisfied our first criterion (i.e., at least 3 studies evaluated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and those neuropsychological tests in either patients or healthy controls).

Table 1.

Studies evaluating the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological tests

Study No. patients Diagnosis No. controls (relatives) Cognitive measures Main results*
Green et al.16 160 MSIT, WASI (vocabulary, similarities, block design, matrix reasoning)
  • MSIT (accuracy and reaction time): Val/Val ↓

  • WASI: Val/Val ↓

Gong et al.17 700 Digital and spatial working memory spans No association between COMT and digital and spatial working memory spans
Blanchard et al.18 291 Spatial n-back task No association between COMT and n-back task
Greenwood et al.19 87 Schizophrenia WCST, WAIS-R (digit span)
  • WCST (category achievement): Val/Met ↓

  • No association between COMT and WCST (perseverative errors) and digit span

Wishart et al.20 95 TMT A–B, D-KEFS (trail-making subtest)
  • TMT-B: Val/Val Val/Met ↓

  • No association between COMT and TMT A

  • TMT-B: COMT x ANKK1 interaction: Val carriers + T carriers ↓

Stokes et al.21 50 Spatial n-back, go/no-go, Tower of London tasks No association between COMT and any cognitive measure
Rosa et al.22 67 Schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder 186 Modified Stroop test (manipulating level of required cognitive stability)
  • Patients and controls: task requiring cognitive stability: Val/Val ↓

  • No association between COMT and task requiring cognitive flexibility

Solís-Ortiz et al.23 74 F WCST, Stroop test, CPT (test single and AX task), verbal fluency test
  • WCST (commission errors) – Stroop test: Val/Val ↑

  • No association between COMT and CPT and verbal fluency test

Uçok et al.24 99 Schizophrenia WCST, CPT (ZA task)
  • CPT (commission errors): Val/Val Val/Met ↓

  • No association between COMT and WCST

Wilkosc et al.25 200 WCST WCST (percentage of nonperseverative errors): Val/Val Val/met ↓ (in males only)
Wirgenes et al.26 315 Schizophrenia-spectrum disorders
  • 137 schizophrenia

  • 27 schizoaffective disorder

  • 7 schizophreniform disorder

340 Bergen n-back task, digit symbol coding test, CVLT, D-KEFS (colour-word interference test, verbal fluency test)
  • Schizophrenia: n-back task: Val/Val ↓

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

  • Controls: no association between COMT and any cognitive measures

Pomarol-Clotet et al.27 42 Schizophrenia 31 N-back task (sequential letter version) No association between COMT and n-back task
Dennis et al.28 496 to 1218 CANTAB (paired associates learning, spatial working memory, verbal recognition memory—immediate recall, ID/ED, rapid visual processing, spatial span, spatial recognition memory), Green’s story recall (immediate and delayed), TMT A–B, WAIS-III (forward and backward digit span, digit symbol, symbol search), COWAT, semantic fluency, Stroop colour-word interference No association between COMT and any cognitive measures
Van den Bos et al.29 70 IGT Val/Val ↑
Yue et al.30 21 N-back task (sequential number version) Val/Met ↓, Val/Val ↑
Krug et al.31 80 Verbal fluency No association between COMT and verbal fluency
Neuhaus et al.32 111 Schizophrenia CPT-IP Val/Val ↑
Prata et al.33 42 Schizophrenia 48 Verbal fluency task
  • Schizophrenia: Val/Val Val/Met ↓

  • Controls: no association between COMT and verbal fluency task

Schmack et al.34 44 Monetary incentive delay No association between COMT and monetary incentive delay
Sheldrick et al.35 522 D2 Test of Attention, MWT-B, LNS, TMT B, verbal fluency, Wechsler Memory Scale (spatial span)
  • TMT-B: Val/Val ↓

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

Roussos et al.36 107 SoC, IGT
  • SoC: Val/Val ↓

  • IGT: Val/Val ↑

Roffman et al.37 185 Chronic schizophrenia WCST
  • No association between COMT and WCST (perseverative errors and category achievement)

  • COMT x MTHFR mutation interaction: Val/Val +T carriers ↓(perseverative errors)

Opgen-Rhein et al.38 63 Schizophrenia 40 ANT Schizophranie–controls: Val/Val ValMet ↓ reaction time and ↑ conflict effect scores
Mata et al.39 130 First-episode nonaffective psychosis
  • General verbal ability: WAIS-III (vocabulary, similarities, information, and comprehension subtests)

  • Attention and processing speed: CPT (correct trials), WAIS-III (digit symbol coding), WAIS-III (forward digits), TMT-A

  • Executive and perceptual organization function: WAIS-III (backward digits, letter fluency, category fluency), TMT-B

  • Verbal memory: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (initial learning, total learning, short-term recall, long-term recall, recognition)

  • Visual memory: Rey complex figure (immediate reproduction, delayed reproduction)

  • Motor function: Grooved Pegboard Test (dominant and nondominant hand), finger tapping test (dominant and nondominant hand)

No association between COMT and any cognitive domain
Diaz-Asper et al.40 325 Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder 330 (359) Spatial n-back task, WCST, CPT 1–9, WAIS-R (short form: arithmetic, similarities, picture completion and digit symbol), ID/ED Patients, controls, relatives:
  • N-back task: Val/Val ↓

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

Bertolino et al.41 82 Spatial n-back task, recognition memory paradigm No association between COMT and any cognitive measures
Aguilera et al.42 521 WCST, CPT-IP, WMS-R (backward visual span), WAIS III (LNS)
  • LNS: Val/Val ↓

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

Woodward et al.43 86 Schizophrenia ACTT, WCST, WISC-R (mazes), BSRT, CIGT, COWAT, WAIS-R (digit symbol)
  • ACTT and WCST (perseverative errors): Val/Val ↓

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

MacDonald et al.44 464 N-back task (sequential letter version), CPT (AX/DPX tasks)
  • CPT (DPX): Val/Val ↓

  • No association between COMT and performance on N-back

Ehlis et al.45 56 Schizophrenia-spectrum disorders Stroop test, TMT A–B, verbal fluency test
  • Stroop test: Val/Val Val/Met ↓ (trend significance)

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

Dickerson et al.46 364 Schizophrenia RBANS: immediate memory (list learning and story memory tasks), visuospatial/constructional (figure copy and line orientation tasks), language (picture naming and semantic fluency tasks), attention (digit span and coding tasks) and delayed memory (list recall, story recall, figure recall and list recognition tasks) No association between COMT and any of the RBANS index
Caldú et al.47 75 WCST, Conners’ CPT II, n-back task, WAIS (vocabulary)
  • WCST and CPT (commission errors): Val/Val Val/Met ↓

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

  • CPT (commission errors): COMT x DAT interaction: Val/Val and 10/10 ↓

Barnett et al.48 822 Schizophrenia-spectrum disorders 1088 WCST
  • Patients: no association between COMT and WCST (perseverative errors)

  • Controls: WCST (perseverative errors): Val/Val ↓

Szöke et al.49 66 Schizophrenia 50 (57 SCZ) TMT A–B, WCST Patients, controls, relatives: no association between COMT and any cognitive measure
Rybakowski et al.50 79 Schizophrenia WCST
  • WCST: Val/Val male ↑ (perseverative errors)

  • Val/Val female ↓ (nonperseverative errors)

Krabbendam et al.51 23 Schizophrenia 21 (33) CPT (Flanker version) Patients, controls, relatives: Val/Val ↓ (correct responses)
Han et al.52 132M First-episode schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder WAIS (information, digit span, vocabulary. arithmetic comprehension, similarity, picture, picture arrangement, block design, object assembly, digit symbol)
  • Digit span: Val/Val ↓

  • Similarity: Val/Val ↑

Golimbet et al.53 124 Schizophrenia 116 (79) Long-term memory, short-term memory, verbal fluency, selectivity of speech relations, calculating with switching over No association between COMT and any cognitive measure
Bertolino et al.54 27 Recognition memory paradigm Val/Val ↓ (accuracy at retrieval)
Stefanis et al.55 527 CPT-IP version Val/Val Val/Met: higher reaction time variability
Ho et al.56 159 Schizophrenia 84 WCST, digit span backward, TMT A–B No association between COMT and any cognitive measure
Galderisi et al.57 106 Schizophrenia CPT (AX task), WCST CPT (AX errors) and WSCT (perseverative errors): Val/Val ↓
Bruder et al.58 402 SDR, WSPT, N-back (sequential letter version), LNS, WCST (n = 246)
  • WCST and LNS: Val/Val ↓

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

Blasi et al.59 23 Variable attentional control task Val/Val Val/Met ↓
Rosa et al.60 89 Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, psychotic mood disorder, schizophreniform disorder, brief psychotic disorder, delusional disorder, atypical psychosis (89) WCST
  • Patients: no association between COMT and performance on WCST

  • Relatives: Val/Val ↓ (perseverative errors)

Nolan et al.61 26 Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder Competing programs task Val/Val Val/Met ↓ cognitive stability and ↑ cognitive flexibility
de Frias et al.62 286 M Episodic memory (recall test, recognition test), semantic memory (knowledge test, fluency test)
  • Recall test: Val/Val Val/Met ↓

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

Tsai et al.63 120 F WCST No association between COMT and WCST (perseverative errors)
Goldberg et al.64 74 Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder (depressed type) 68 (108) Spatial n-back task, CPT 1–9, WAIS-R (IQ similarities, arithmetic, picture completion and digit symbol)
  • Patients, controls, relatives: n-back task: Val/Val ↓

  • No association between COMT and other cognitive measures

Malhotra et al.65 73 WCST WCST (perseverative errors): Val/Val Val/Met ↓
Joober et al.66 94 Schizophrenia 31 WCST
  • Patients: WCST (perseverative errors): Val/Val Val/Met ↓

  • Controls: no association between COMT and WCST (perseverative errors)

Bilder1 58 Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder
  • General executive and perceptual organization: LNS, WCST, category fluency, letter fluency, block design, visual reproductions I and II

  • Declarative verbal learning and memory: paragraph recall I and II, word list learning I and II

  • Processing speed and attention: TMT A–B, digit symbol

Processing speed and attention domain: Val/Val ↓
Egan et al.67 175 Schizophrenia 55 (219) WCST, WAIS-R, WRAT Patients, controls, relatives: WSCT (perseverative errors): Val/Val ↓

ACTT = Auditory Consonant Trigram Test; ANT = Attention Network Test; BSRT = Buschke Selective Reminding Test; CANTAB = Cambridge Neuropsyhological Test Automated Battery; CIGT = Category Instance Generation Test; COMT = catechol-O-methyltransferase; COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; CPT = Continuous Performance Test; CPT-IP = Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs version; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; DAT = dopamine transporter; D-KEFS = Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System; F = female; DPX = Dot Pattern Expectancy task; ID/ED = intradimensional/extradimensional shift task; IGT = Iowa Gambling Task; LNS = letter and number sequencing; M = male; MSIT = Multi-Source Interference Task; MWT-B = Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest B; RBANS = Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status; SDR = Spatial Delayed Response; SoC = Stockings of Cambridge; TMT = Trail Making Test; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WAIS III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III; WAIS-R = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; WCST = Wisconsin Card sorting Test; WISC-R = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised; WMS-R = Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; WRAT = Wide Range Achievement Test; WSPT = Word Serial Position Test.

N-back task

The n-back task is conceptualized as a measure of working memory and includes executive functions, such as encoding, updating, sequence maintenance, comparing and deciding.44 Eleven studies satisfied our inclusion criteria and together suggest that while the COMT Val158Met polymorphism seems to be associated with performance on the n-back task in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder and in their siblings, this association is not present in healthy individuals. Three studies resported a significant or trend-level association between the Val/Val genotype and a worse performance (accuracy and reaction time) on the n-back task in patients with schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder and schizoaffective disorder26,40,64 and in their relatives,40,64 whereas 1 study found no association.27 Interestingly, the 3 studies that detected a significant association used a version of the n-back task that requires a higher cognitive load, suggesting that the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and performance on the n-back task in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders may become apparent when a more challenging performance is required (for details about the different versions of the tasks used, see Appendix 1, Table S1, available at cma.ca/jpn). In healthy individuals, 3 studies found a significant association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and performance on the n-back task.30,40,64 In 2 of these studies the Val/Val genotype was associated with worse performance,40,64 whereas in the third one participants with the Val/Met genotype performed worse than those with the Met/Met or Val/Val genotype.30 Eight studies did not find a COMT Val158Met genotype effect on n-back task performance in healthy individuals.18,21,26,27,41,44,47,58

Continuous Performance Test (CPT)

The CPT is conceptualized as a behavioural assessment of attentional modulation; it can detect deficits in vigilance, selective attention and sustained attention.68 Ten studies satisfied our inclusion criteria, reporting inconsistent findings on the association between the CPT and the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in both patients with schizophrenia and healthy individuals. In patients, 3 studies found a significant association between the Val/Val genotype and a poorer performance in terms of number of errors57,69 and a better performance in terms of signal discrimination.32 In contrast, 2 studies found no association either in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder or in their relatives.40,64 In healthy individuals, the Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes have been associated with worse performance in terms of commission errors,47 context processing44 and reaction time variability in some studies,55 whereas other studies reported no association.23,40,42,64 This inconsistency may be related to the use of different versions of the CPT across studies (for details about the different versions of the tasks used, see Appendix 1, Table S1). Interestingly, 1 study reported an interaction between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and the DAT 40 bp variable number tandem repeat; participants homozygous for both the COMT Val allele and the DAT 10 allele exhibited worse performance in terms of commission errors.47 This result suggests that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism may affect CPT performance by interacting with other genes, which may also explain inconsistencies in previous findings.

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)

The WCST is regarded as a neuropsychological marker of the efficiency of executive functions.70 While the COMT gene appears to have no effect on WCST performance in patients with psychosis, it seems to have an effect in healthy individuals. A meta-analysis has been conducted to assess the effect of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism on WCST perseverative errors in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders and in healthy controls;48 this included 11 studies that met our inclusion criteria.1,49,50,5658,60,63,6567 The meta-analysis revealed a small but significant association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and executive function in healthy individuals, with Met/Met carriers performing slightly better than Val/Val carriers. However, this association was not found in patients with schizophrenia.

Subsequent studies have partially confirmed these findings, reporting an association between Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes and a higher number of perseverative errors47 and nonperseverative errors25 and fewer commission errors23 in healthy individuals than in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder.24,37,40 In contrast, other studies found the Val/Val genotype to be associated with worse performance in terms of perseverative errors43 and found the Val/Met genotype to be associated with lower number of categories achieved19 in patients with schizophrenia but not in healthy individuals42 or unaffected relatives.40 Moreover, an interaction between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and the MTHFR C677T mutation on WCST performance has been found in patients with schizophrenia, with COMT Val/Val carriers who had at least 1 copy of the MTHFR T allele making more perseverative errors.37

Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B

The TMT-A requires sequencing, psychomotor speed and visuoperceptual ability; part B also requires cognitive flexibility and working memory.71,72 Performance on the TMT-A has not been found to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia45,49,56 or healthy individuals.20,28,56 Worse performance on the TMT-B has been found to be associated with Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes in healthy individuals in 1 study20 but not in others,28,56 nor in those evaluating patients with schizophrenia.45,49,56

Interestingly, an association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and worse TMT-B performance has been reported in healthy individuals carrying both the COMT Val allele and the ANKK1 (rs1800497) T allele.20

Verbal fluency

Verbal fluency is considered to be an executive function task and a semantic memory measure.53 Both Val/Val and Val/Met carriers with schizophrenia have been reported to perform worse in 1 study,33 but this association has not been confirmed by other studies involving patients with schizophrenia,26,53 their relatives53 or healthy controls.23,28,33,53,62,73

Digit span

Composed by 2 parts, forward and backward, the digit span test is considered to be a short-term memory measure, with the digit span backward also involving working memory.56 A worse performance on digit span has been found to be related to the Val/Val genotype in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder in 1 study,52 but not in another study involving patients with schizophrenia.19 Of note, the sample size of the former study52 was considerably larger than that in the latter study.19 Only the digit span backward has not been found to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia.74

According to our criteria, only the n-back task and the CPT can be considered strongly related to the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia. In healthy controls, these associations seem to be weaker or absent. Interestingly, some fMRI studies that used an n-back paradigm detected a significant association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and the activation of the frontal cortex in patients with schizophrenia26 and healthy controls21,26,41,75 regardless of n-back task performance. Thus, fMRI may be a more sensitive approach to the evaluation of the neuronal circuitry affected by variation in the COMT gene and n-back task performance and when cognitive performance is not affected.

The n-back task and the CPT assess working memory and attention modulation, respectively. Interestingly, although the evidence available is not yet sufficient to make them satisfy our criteria of performance in other working memory tasks (e.g., letter and number sequencing, Auditory Consonant Trigram Test) and in tasks of attentional modulation (e.g., Attention Network Test, Variable Attentional Control Task), these tasks seem to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia. On the other hand, verbal fluency and digit span backward have not been found to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia or in healthy controls. However, this result may reflect the lack of association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and semantic and short-term memory, respectively, which are both required for performing these tasks.

The WCST, which assesses executive function, has been related to the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in healthy controls but not in patients with psychosis.48 It is plausible that other COMT polymorphisms or other genetic variants or nongenetic factors might influence the performance on this task in individuals with psychosis.

Step 1b: Association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and brain structure

We identified 13 studies (Table 2) exploring the association between brain structure (global or regional volumes of grey and white matter) and the COMT Val158Met polymorphism. We report the regions that satisfied the first criterion (i.e., at least 3 studies evaluated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and those brain areas in either patients or healthy controls).

Table 2.

Studies evaluating the association between brain structures (global or regional grey and white matter volumes) and the COMT Val158Met polymorphism

Study No. patients Diagnosis No. controls (relatives) MRI analysis (Tesla) Regions evaluated Main results
Barnes et al.76 82 Automated (1.5 T) Total grey matter volume, DLPFC No association
Cerasa et al.77 149 Automated (3 T) Whole brain Val/Val ↓ thickness of the right inferior prefrontal sulcus and right superior temporal sulcus
Ehrlich et al.9 98 Schizophrenia 114 Automated (3 T and 1.5 T) Whole brain, frontal lobe, hippocampal and amygdala volumes Patients: Val/Val Val/Met ↓ bilateral amygdala and right hippocampal volumes; no association between COMT and whole brain and frontal regions
Controls: Val/Val Val/Met ↓ bilateral amygdalae and right hippocampal volumes; no association between COMT and whole brain and frontal regions
Honea et al.78 151 Automated VBM/ROI (1.5 T) Whole brain, DLPFC, hippocampal volumes Val/Val Val/Met ↓ grey matter volume in hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus; Val/Val ↑ (trend) grey matter volume in DLPFC; no association between COMT and whole brain
Dutt et al.79 128 Schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, schizoaffective disorder, psychotic disorder NOS 61 (194) Automated (1.5 T) Left hippocampal volume, right hippocampal volume and total lateral ventricular volume No association
Zinkstok et al.80 51 Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder Automated VBM (1.5 T) Regional grey and white matter density and total grey and white matter volume No association between COMT and grey or white matter density; COMT x PRODH rs20086720: Val allele T allele ↑ white matter density in the left inferior frontal lobe
Cerasa et al.81 57 Automated VBM (1.5 T) Hippocampus and prefrontal cortex volumes Val/Val ↓ hippocampal grey matter volumes bilaterally; Val/Val Val/Met ↑ prefrontal cortex greater grey matter volume bilaterally and ↓ tissue volume of the hippocampus bilaterally
Taylor et al.82 31 Automated (1.5 T) Whole brain, hippcampus, amygdala, caudate and temporal lobe volumes Val/Val ↓ temporal lobe (total and grey matter) and hippocampus volumes; no association between COMT and whole brain, caudate and amygdala volumes.
McIntosh et al.10 High-risk for schizophrenia 15 (78) Automated VBM (1 T) Prefrontal cortex Val/Val Val/Met ↓ anterior cingulate cortex grey matter density
Crespo-Facorro et al.83 75 Schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder, schizoaffective disorder, psychosis NOS, brief reactive psychosis Automated (1.5 T) Global and lobar volumes of grey matter and CSF Met/Met Val/Met ↑ lateral ventricles, right lateral ventricle and left lateral ventricle
Zinkstok et al.84 154 Automated VBM (1.5 T) Total and regional grey matter and white matter density No association between COMT and grey or white matter volumes and densities
Ohnishi et al.11 47 Schizophrenia 76 Automated TBM/whole brain and ROI (1.5 T) Whole brain morphology, DLPFC Controls: no association between COMT and any brain structure
Patients: Val/Val ↓ left amygdala-uncus, bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, right middle temporal gyrus, left thalamus; no association between COMT and DLPFC
Ho et al.56 100 Schizophrenia 49 Automated (1.5 T) Frontal lobe grey matter, white matter and CSF volumes Controls: no association between COMT and frontal lobe morphology Patients: no association between COMT and frontal lobe volume

COMT = catechol-O-methyltransferase; CSF = cerebrospinal fluid; DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; MRI = magnetic resonance imaging; NOS = not otherwise specified; ROI = region of interest; TBM = tensor-based morphometry; VBM = voxel-based morphometry.

Temporal regions

The most consistent finding has been that of an association with temporal areas. In patients with schizophrenia, a smaller volume of the hippocampus,9 amygdala-uncus9,11 and middle temporal gyrus11 has been associated with the Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes. Interestingly, this association has not been found in studies that included patients with any psychosis,79,80,83 indicating that the association may be specific to schizophrenia. In healthy individuals, smaller hippocampal,9,78,81,82 amygdalar,9 total and grey matter temporal lobe82 volumes as well as reduced thickness of the right superior temporal sulcus77 have been reported in Val/Val carriers. In contrast, other studies have failed to find a significant association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and temporal lobe volumes in healthy individuals or in unaffected relatives of patients with psychosis.11,79,84

Frontal areas

The Val/Val genotype has been associated with smaller grey matter density in the anterior cingulate cortex in a population at high risk for psychosis10 and with smaller grey matter volume in the left anterior cingulate cortex in patients with chronic schizophrenia.11 However, these findings have not been replicated by other studies.9,56,83 Interestingly, carriers of both the COMT Val allele and the PRODH GT or TT genotypes for rs20086720 have been found to have larger white matter volume of the inferior frontal area.80 This may suggest that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism affects frontal region morphology via an interaction with other genes, which may help explain the inconsistency found in the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and frontal volumes. In healthy individuals, both the Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes have been associated with greater grey matter volume of the prefrontal,81 and (albeit at trend level) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex78 and with a reduced thickness of the right inferior prefrontal sulcus;77 however, these findings have not been confirmed by other studies.9,11,56,76,84

Lateral ventricles

In patients with first-episode schizophrenia with nonaffective psychoses, a significant enlargement of lateral ventricles has been reported in both Met/Met and Val/Met carriers.83 This finding has not been replicated in studies involving patients with any psychosis,11,79 in unaffected relatives79 or in healthy individuals.11,79,84

Thalamus

A reduced volume of the left thalamus has been associated with the COMT Val allele in patients with chronic schizophrenia,11 but not in healthy controls.11,84 Subsequent studies have not confirmed this association in patients.80,83

According to our criteria, only temporal and frontal volumes can be considered to correlate with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in schizophrenia, with temporal volumes also associated with this genotype in healthy controls. Interestingly, the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and temporal and frontal areas has also been explored in fMRI studies. The Val allele has been reported to be associated with a reduced activation of the medial temporal lobe during an episodic memory task in healthy controls,54,85 whereas in schizophrenia the Met allele has been reported to be associated with a reduced activation in this area.86 Moreover, a recent meta-analysis87 that evaluated 20 studies involving mostly healthy individuals showed a significant association between the Val allele and increased prefrontal activation. This finding has also been reported in patients with schizophrenia and in their relatives.27,67

Step 2a: Can the neuropsychological tasks identified (n-back and CPT) be considered endophenotypes?

N-back task

Association with the illness: Several studies suggest that patients with schizophrenia have worse n-back accuracy during high working memory load conditions and longer reaction time.40,64,88

Heritability: The n-back heritability seems high, at 72.9% for accuracy and 56.4% for reaction time, as found in healthy twins.89

State independence: Impairment in the n-back task has been reported in patients with schizophrenia at various illness stages,9092 before and after antipsychotic treatment92 and independent of illness duration.91

Cosegregation with illness within family: Cosegregation is suggested by evidence that this impairment is present in relatives of patients with schizophrenia, who perform better than probands.40,64,88,93 Moreover, a significant influence of familial loading on visuospatial working memory has been detected in patients with schizophrenia but not in patients with schizoaffective disorder.94

Presence in unaffected relatives: With the exception of 2 studies,95,96 n-back performance has been found to be impaired in unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenia in comparison to healthy controls.40,64,88,93

Continuous Performance Test

Association with the illness: Schizophrenia has been consistently associated with impairment in CPT discrimination accuracy and reaction time.12,40,64,97,98

Heritability: The heritability for d′ of the CPT, Identical Pairs version (CPT-IP), has been estimated at around 39% for verbal and 49% for spatial attention.99 For the X-CPT, the heritability has been reported to vary between 48% and 62%.100,101

State independence: It has been reported that at-risk offspring in whom a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder later developed had attentional deficits as early as age 12 years, with the impairment persisting over the following 15 years.102

Cosegregation with illness within family: CPT impairment may cosegregate with psychosis, as suggested by its presence among family members of patients with schizophrenia.12 Moreover, there is evidence that nonpsychotic siblings from multiplex families exhibit worse performance on a degraded CPT version as well as less proficiency in processing the perceptual load when compared with those from simplex families.103

Presence in unaffected relatives: Unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenia show impairment in discrimination accuracy at the CPT-IP.12 However, studies that used different versions of the CPT have not reported impairment in discrimination accuracy.40,64 Relatives of patients with schizophrenia have also been found to have an impairment in hit reaction time in comparison to healthy controls.98

Both the n-back and the CPT can be considered useful endophenotypes for schizophrenia.

Step 2b: Can the previously identified brain structures (temporal and frontal areas) be considered endophenotypes?

Temporal areas

Association with the illness: Meta-analyses suggest that a reduction of total and medial temporal (amygdala–uncus–hyppocampus) volumes is present in patients with schizophrenia, particularly in the hippocampus.104110

Heritability: A moderate heritability (40%–69%) for hippocampal volume has been reported in twin studies in healthy controls.111

State independence: Hippocampus reductions seem to be state-independent and have been reported in patients at both first episode and chronic illness stages.112

Cosegregation with illness within the family: Cosegregation is suggested by the presence of hippocampal volume reduction among relatives of patients with schizophrenia,113 particularly in multiplex families.114

Presence in unaffected relatives: Results from a meta-analysis indicate that amygdalar and hippocampal volume reductions are also present in relatives of patients with schizophrenia.113

Frontal areas

Association with the illness: Various meta-analyses have consistently reported volume reductions in medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices104106,115 in patients with schizophrenia.

Heritability: The heritability of total frontal lobe volumes seems to be high (90%–95%); changes in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, superior frontal cortex and anterior cingulate volumes have also been found to be highly heritable.111 Moreover, a twin study has reported an association between smaller prefrontal grey matter volume and increasing genetic proximity to a patient with schizophrenia (monozygotic co-twins > dizygotic co-twins > control twins).116

State independence: Decreased prefrontal and cingulate grey matter volumes have been reported in high risk individuals who subsequently transition to psychosis117,118 as well as in patients with first-episode and chronic schizophrenia.104,110,115 Reductions in some regions of the frontal cortex seem to be a feature of high-risk people in whom schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder subsequently develops.119

Cosegregation with illness within family: Cosegregation of the smaller prefrontal cortex within schizophrenia families is suggested by evidence of smaller volumes in this region in family members, albeit to a lesser extent than in affected relatives.120,121 Moreover, there is evidence of a negative correlation between grey matter volume and increasing genetic risk for schizophrenia.122

Presence in unaffected relatives: Relatives of patients with schizophrenia show grey matter volume reductions in the total frontal lobe,120 prefrontal cortex123 and anterior cingulate gyrus110,115,121 in comparison to healthy individuals. However, this finding has not been replicated in other studies.124,125

Reduced grey matter volume in the medial temporal and prefrontal cortices can be considered a useful endophenotype for schizophrenia.

Step 3: Are the previously identified neuropsychological endophenotypes associated with these brain structural endophenotypes?

Six studies (Table 3) have examined the association between the n-back and CPT and the brain structural endophenotypes we identified in step 2 (grey matter volume of medial temporal and prefrontal cortices).

Table 3.

Studies evaluating the association between the n-back task and Continuous Performance Test and brain structure

Study No. patients Diagnosis No. controls (relatives) MRI analysis (Tesla) Regions evaluated Cognitive measures Main results*
Piras et al.130 181 ROI (3 T) Caudate, putamen, pallidum, thalamus, hippocampus and amygdala N-back task No association
Crespo-Facorro et al.136 142 Psychosis
  • 82 schizophrenia

  • 36 schizophreniform disorder

  • 24 patients with nonschizophrenic, nonaffective psychoses

83 Automated segmentation global volumes and ROIs (1.5 T) Volumes of whole brain, total grey matter, total white matter, cortical CSF and lateral ventricles, grey matter volumes of cortical (occipital, parietal, temporal and frontal lobes) and subcortical (caudate nucleus, thalamus and putamen) regions CPT Controls: ↑ CPT, ↓ lateral ventricle volume
Patients: no association
Laywer et al.135 71 Schizophrenia 65 Semiautomated and fully manual (1.5 T) Frontal, occipital, parietal, temporal and occipital lobes, subcortical region, CSF volume of the ventricles I, II and III, intracranial volume, corpus callosum, caudate, putamen, hippocampus, cerebellum, the posterior superior, posterior inferior and anterior vermis, and cerebellar tonsil CPT Controls/patients: association with putamen volumes and vermis regions
Salgado-Pineda et al.134 13 Antipsychotic-naive schizophrenia 13 Automated VBM and ROI (1.5 T) Whole brain grey matter, white matter and CSF, thalamus CPT Controls: no association
Patients: ↑ CPT (d′ scores), ↑ grey matter density in the left thalamic nucleus, left angular, supramarginal gyrus, left inferior frontal and postcentral gyri, grey matter density of the left and right thalamus
Antonova et al.8 45 Schizophrenia 43 Automated VBM (1.5 T) Whole brain volume and grey matter CPT Controls/patients: no association
Goldberg et al.137 14 Schizophrenia (14 co-twins) Automated (1.5 T) Hippocampus, third ventricle and a large section of the lateral ventricles CPT Patients/relatives: no association

CPT = Continuous Performance Test; CSF = cerebrospinal fluid; MRI = magnetic resonance imaging; ROI = region of interest; VBM = voxel-based morphometry.

*

↑ (neuropsychological task) = better performance.

N-back task

Although the association between n-back task and prefrontal brain function has been frequently explored with fMRI in patients with schizophrenia88,126129 and in their relatives,95 the association between this test and prefrontal (or temporal) volume in patients with schizophrenia has not been explored. In healthy controls, only 1 study has been conducted, reporting no association between n-back performance and hippocampal and amygdalar volume.130 However, reduced prefrontal activation during the n-back task has been associated with a reduction in prefrontal volume in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis, suggesting that “these 2 fundamental pathophysiological features of the disorder are interrelated.”117 Moreover, 2 studies that adopted working memory tasks similar to the n-back task (the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale arithmetic subtest and digit span backward, respectively) reported a association between worse working memory performance and reduced frontal, prefrontal and temporal lobe volumes in patients with schizophrenia.131,132 Taken together, these data suggest that worse n-back performance is associated with reduced prefrontal and temporal lobe volumes. Interestingly, an impaired performance on the n-back task has also been described in individuals with focal prefrontal damage,133 suggesting that this deficit may be shared by other disorders that primarily affect the prefrontal cortex.

Continuous Performance Test

A worse CPT performance has been associated with smaller prefrontal volume in patients with schizophrenia,134 but not in studies that included patients with any psychosis8,135,136 or healthy individuals.8,134136 Studies in patients with schizophrenia, their unaffected co-twins and healthy controls have found no association between CPT performance and hippocampal volume.8,134137

These findings suggest that worse performance on the n-back task is associated with smaller prefrontal and medial temporal volumes and that worse CPT performance is associated with smaller prefrontal volume in patients with schizophrenia. Consistent with this evidence, a meta-analysis of fMRI studies reported that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex were less activated during the n-back task in patients with schizophrenia.128 On the other hand, prefrontal cortex activation has been found to be associated with the CPT in patients with schizophrenia138140 and in their relatives.141

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review to comprehensively evaluate evidence on the link between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological performance and brain structures in patients with schizophrenia, in their relatives and in healthy controls. We found that the n-back task and the CPT (a working memory task and an attentional modulation task, respectively) are the only 2 neuropsychological tasks strongly associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia. More specifically, among patients, the Val/Val carriers showed a worse performance in both the n-back task and the CPT, with a greater number of errors, but a better performance in signal discrimination. Interestingly, evidence from fMRI studies suggests that performance on these 2 tasks is positively correlated with lower prefrontal cortex activation in patients with schizophrenia. This suggests that the cognitive domains assessed by these tasks reflect “the operation of a common underlying cognitive control mechanism supported by the prefrontal cortex.”142 Moreover, this evidence is consistent with the tonic–phasic DA hypothesis,143 which suggests that tonic DA may stabilize and maintain relevant information (“cognitive stability”) principally via D1 receptors, while phasic DA is important for updating and manipulating information (“cognitive flexibility”), principally via D2 receptors.144 In the COMT Val158Met polymorphism, while the Val allele increases phasic DA transmission and is expected to improve cognitive flexibility, the Met158 allele increases tonic DA transmission, and is therefore expected to improve working memory and executive functions that require cognitive stability.145 Furthermore, since some neuropsychological tests require both cognitive stability and cognitive flexibility,145 the DA hypothesis may help explain the inconsistency in findings regarding the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological performance.

With respect to brain structure, the evidence reviewed points to an association between smaller temporal areas (e.g., hippocampus, amygdala, middle temporal gyrus) and the Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes in patients with schizophrenia. Smaller frontal areas also seem to be associated with the Val/Val genotype in patients with schizophrenia and in people at high risk for psychosis. This is interesting since CPT performance is associated with prefrontal cortex volume in patients with schizophrenia. Although, to our knowledge, no study has been conducted to date using the n-back task and structural MRI, we speculate that this test may be associated with both prefrontal and medial temporal volumes in view of evidence of prefrontal activation during the task88,95,126129 and of an association between worse working memory performance and smaller frontal, prefrontal and temporal lobe volumes.131,132 Moreover, both neuropsychological tasks (n-back task and CPT) and brain regional volumes (medial temporal and prefrontal cortex), which have been reported to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism, met most of the criteria for an endophenotype.

Taken together, these results suggest that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism may contribute to 2 neuropsychological and 2 brain structural endophenotypes that characterize schizophrenia, with the prefrontal cortex representing a common neural substrate underlying both n-back task and CPT performance. It is interesting to note that the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and the endophenotypes detected (with the exception of the hippocampus) is more evident in patients with schizophrenia than in healthy controls. This suggests that this polymorphism alone is not sufficient to produce a variation at the neuropsychological and brain structural levels. The presence of these endophenotypes (e.g., working memory impairment, prefrontal cortex grey matter volume reduction) seems to result from an interaction between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and other genetic and nongenetic (e.g., environmental) factors (possibly present in excess in affected individuals), which together contribute to shape the neuropsychological and brain structural alterations that characterize schizophrenia.47,80,146 We speculate that alterations in the fine-tuning of cortical DA may be responsible for worse neuropsychological performance and for brain volume reductions. More specifically, the COMT Val carriers may have reduced DA levels in the prefrontal cortex, leading to a decrease in D1 receptor activation with subsequent impairment in cognitive tasks, such as working memory.147 In fact, working memory seems to be related to the activity of both D1 and D2 receptors: the D1 receptors would signal to prefrontal pyramidal neurons via N-methyl-D-aspartate postsynaptic receptors, maintaining and stabilizing task-relevant signals while reducing signal-to-noise ratio in the prefrontal cortex via GABAergic inhibitory effect on nonrelevant “noisy” signals. The D2 receptors would in turn allow the development of new informative signals.147 In addition, the presence of the COMT Val allele may indirectly influence the neurodevelopment and neuroplasticity processes, ultimately leading to changes in the volume of brain areas such as the prefrontal cortex. Since COMT is responsible for DA cortical availability,67 it may also be implicated in key neuronal processes that ultimately lead to brain morphogenesis and development. How this actually occurs remains to be established. Interestingly, a recent review has proposed that COMT influences neurodevelopment and neuroplasticity via an epistatic interaction with other genes, such as AKT1.148AKT1 and dopaminergic pathways have already been shown to be synergistically responsible for neuronal atrophy after chronic opiate exposure.148,149 Also, genetic variation in AKT1 has been linked to DA-associated prefrontal cortical structure and function in humans.148 It is possible that the combination of different risk alleles results in impaired prefrontal activity during challenging cognitive tasks and in changes in key neurodevelopmental pathways affecting neuronal morphology and development.150

Limitations

The main limitation to our interpretation of the literature is that the association between a single genetic variant and an endophenotype does not necessarily imply a causal relationship between them. In other words, we hypothesized that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism, which affects prefrontal DA levels, may contribute to both CPT and n-back impairment and reduced prefrontal and medial temporal volume. However, schizophrenia is characterized by a complex genetic architecture, with an intricate interplay among several genes. As mentioned previously, the COMT Val158Met polymorphism can interact with other genes in shaping the neuropsychological profile and the brain structural alterations that characterize schizophrenia (for example, see the studies by Caldú and colleagues,47 Zinkstok and colleagues,80 and Dickinson and colleagues146).

Moreover, variables such as sex and age need to be taken into account. For example, estrogen reduces COMT enzymatic activity, and the production of estrogen varies across age.146 Finally, the COMT Val158Met polymorphism interacts with environmental factors like stress. This has been suggested by a recent study reporting that individuals homozygous for Met performed significantly worse than those homozygous for Val on the n-back task under stress.151 This highlights the importance of considering the complex interaction between genes and environment when evaluating neurocognitive performance.151

In view of this complexity, it is possible that the endophenotypes we found to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism are also related to other polymorphisms. Furthermore, there is little evidence on the association between neuropsychological tasks and brain structure in patients with schizophrenia, and the link we suggest in this review needs further investigation. With these caveats in mind, this may explain, at least in part, the biological substrate of 4 important endophenotypes that characterize schizophrenia.

Conclusion

Our review comprehensively evaluated evidence on the link between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and both neuropsychological performance and brain structure in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives, and in healthy controls. The review also summarized this evidence within a biological framework that may be used to advance our knowledge on the molecular mechanisms that underlie the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.

Footnotes

Competing interests: None declared for M. Zanoni and P. Dazzan. As below for E. Ira, M. Ruggeri and S. Tosato.

Contributors: S. Tosato designed the review. E. Ira acquired the data and analyzed it with M. Zanoni, M. Ruggeri and P. Dazzan. E. Ira and M. Zanoni wrote the article, which M. Ruggeri, P. Dazzan and S. Tosato reviewed. All authors approved its publication.

Funding: This study was supported by Veneto Region, Italy, with the FSE grant “Acquisizione nuove tecniche automatizzate di studio di immagini di risonanza magnetica nucleare cerebrali” to E. Ira and by Fondazione Cariverona with the grant “Disabilità cognitiva e comportamentale nelle demenze e nelle psicosi. Sotto-obiettivo A.9. Basi morfofunzionali cognitive e genetiche delle psicosi maggiori: uno studio integrato longitudinale” to M. Ruggeri and S. Tosato.

References

  • 1.Bilder RM. Neurocognitive correlates of the COMT Val(158)Met polymorphism in chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:701–7. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01416-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tosato S, Bonetto C, Di Forti M, et al. Effect of COMT genotype on aggressive behaviour in a community cohort of schizophrenic patients. Neurosci Lett. 2011;495:17–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Glatt SJ, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT. Association between a functional catechol O-methyltransferase gene polymorphism and schizophrenia: meta-analysis of case-control and family-based studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:469–76. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.3.469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fan JB, Zhang CS, Gu NF, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val/Met functional polymorphism and risk of schizophrenia: a large-scale association study plus meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:139–44. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Munafò MR, Bowes L, Clark TG, et al. Lack of association of the COMT (Val158/108 Met) gene and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of case-control studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:765–70. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Allen NC, Bagade S, McQueen MB, et al. Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of genetic association studies in schizophrenia: the SzGene database. Nat Genet. 2008;40:827–34. doi: 10.1038/ng.171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Meisenzahl EM, Schmitt GJ, Scheuerecker J, et al. The role of dopamine for the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2007;19:337–45. doi: 10.1080/09540260701502468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Antonova E, Kumari V, Morris R, et al. The relationship of structural alterations to cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a voxel-based morphometry study. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:457–67. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ehrlich S, Morrow EM, Roffman JL, et al. The COMT Val108/158Met polymorphism and medial temporal lobe volumetry in patients with schizophrenia and healthy adults. Neuroimage. 2010;53:992–1000. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.McIntosh AM, Baig BJ, Hall J, et al. Relationship of catechol-O-methyltransferase variants to brain structure and function in a population at high risk of psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:1127–34. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ohnishi T, Hashimoto R, Mori T, et al. The association between the Val158Met polymorphism of the catechol-O-methyl transferase gene and morphological abnormalities of the brain in chronic schizophrenia. Brain. 2006;129:399–410. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Allen AJ. Endophenotypes in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Res. 2009;109:24–37. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.01.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Prasad KM, Keshavan MS. Structural cerebral variations as useful endophenotypes in schizophrenia: Do they help construct “extended endophenotypes”? Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:774–90. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:636–45. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mazzoncini R, Zoli M, Tosato S, et al. Can the role of genetic factors in schizophrenia be enlightened by studies of candidate gene mutant mice behaviour? World J Biol Psychiatry. 2009;10:778–97. doi: 10.1080/15622970902875152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Green AE, Kraemer DJ, Deyoung CG, et al. A gene–brain-cognition pathway: prefrontal activity mediates the effect of COMT on cognitive control and IQ. Cereb Cortex. 2012 Feb. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs035. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gong P, Zhang H, Chi W, et al. An association study on the polymorphisms of dopaminergic genes with working memory in a healthy Chinese Han population. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2012;32:1011–9. doi: 10.1007/s10571-012-9817-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Blanchard MM, Blanchard MM, Chamberlain SR, et al. Effects of two dopamine-modulating genes (DAT1 9/10 and COMT Val/Met) on n-back working memory performance in healthy volunteers. Psychol Med. 2011;41:611–8. doi: 10.1017/S003329171000098X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Greenwood K, Hung CF, Tropeano M, et al. No association between the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) val158met polymorphism and cognitive improvement following cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) in schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett. 2011;496:65–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.03.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wishart HA, Roth RM, Saykin AJ, et al. COMT Val158Met genotype and individual differences in executive function in healthy adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:174–80. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Stokes PR, Rhodes RA, Grasby PM, et al. The effects of the COMT Val108/158Met polymorphism on BOLD activation during working memory, planning, and response inhibition: A role for the posterior cingulate cortex? Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:763–71. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rosa EC, Dickinson D, Apud J, et al. COMT Val158Met polymorphism, cognitive stability and cognitive flexibility: an experimental examination. Behav Brain Funct. 2010;6:53. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Solís-Ortiz S, Perez-Luque E, Morado-Crespo L, et al. Executive functions and selective attention are favored in middle-aged healthy women carriers of the Val/Val genotype of the catechol-o-methyltransferase gene: a behavioral genetic study. Behav Brain Funct. 2010;6:67. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Uçok A, Ozturk M, Duman Z, et al. COMT Val 158 Met polymorphism is related with interpersonal problem solving in schizophrenia. Eur Psychiatry. 2010;25:320–2. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wilkosc M, Hauser J, Tomaszewska M, et al. Influence of dopaminergic and serotoninergic genes on working memory in healthy subjects. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Warsz) 2010;70:86–94. doi: 10.55782/ane-2010-1777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Wirgenes KV, Djurovic S, Sundet K, et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase variants and cognitive performance in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder versus controls. Schizophr Res. 2010;122:31–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Pomarol-Clotet E, Fatjo-Vilas M, McKenna PJ, et al. COMT Val158Met polymorphism in relation to activation and deactivation in the prefrontal cortex: A study in patients with schizophrenia and healthy subjects. Neuroimage. 2010;53:899–907. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dennis NA, Need AC, LaBar KS, et al. COMT val108/158 met genotype affects neural but not cognitive processing in healthy individuals. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:672–83. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.van den Bos R, Homberg J, Gijsbers E, et al. The effect of COMT Val158 Met genotype on decision-making and preliminary findings on its interaction with the 5-HTTLPR in healthy females. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:493–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Yue C, Wu T, Deng W, et al. Comparison of visual evoked-related potentials in healthy young adults of different catechol-O-methyl-transferase genotypes in a continuous 3-back task. Neuroreport. 2009;20:521–4. doi: 10.1097/wnr.0b013e328317f3b1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Krug A, Markov V, Sheldrick A, et al. The effect of the COMT val(158)met polymorphism on neural correlates of semantic verbal fluency. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;259:459–65. doi: 10.1007/s00406-009-0010-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Neuhaus AH, Opgen-Rhein C, Urbanek C, et al. COMT Val 158 Met polymorphism is associated with cognitive flexibility in a signal discrimination task in schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2009;42:141–4. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1112132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Prata DP, Mechelli A, Fu CH, et al. Opposite effects of catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met on cortical function in healthy subjects and patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:473–80. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Schmack K, Schlagenhauf F, Sterzer P, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met genotype influences neural processing of reward anticipation. Neuroimage. 2008;42:1631–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sheldrick AJ, Krug A, Markov V, et al. Effect of COMT val158met genotype on cognition and personality. Eur Psychiatry. 2008;23:385–9. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Pavlakis S, et al. Planning, decision-making and the COMT rs4818 polymorphism in healthy males. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:757–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Roffman JL, Weiss AP, Deckersbach T, et al. Interactive effects of COMT Val108/158Met and MTHFR C677T on executive function in schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B:990–5. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Opgen-Rhein C, Neuhaus AH, Urbanek C, et al. Executive attention in schizophrenic males and the impact of COMT Val108/158Met genotype on performance on the attention network test. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:1231–9. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mata I, Perez-Iglesias R, Pelayo-Teran JM, et al. Lack of influence of COMT Val158Met genotype on cognition in first-episode non-affective psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2008;102:206–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Diaz-Asper CM, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, et al. Genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase: effects on working memory in schizophrenic patients, their siblings, and healthy controls. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:72–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bertolino A, Di Giorgio A, Blasi G, et al. Epistasis between dopamine regulating genes identifies a nonlinear response of the human hippocampus during memory tasks. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:226–34. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Aguilera M, Barrantes-Vidal N, Arias B, et al. Putative role of the COMT gene polymorphism (Val158Met) on verbal working memory functioning in a healthy population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B:898–902. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Woodward ND, Jayathilake K, Meltzer HY. COMT val108/158met genotype, cognitive function, and cognitive improvement with clozapine in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2007;90:86–96. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.MacDonald AW, III, Carter CS, Flory JD, et al. COMT Val158Met and executive control: a test of the benefit of specific deficits to translational research. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116:306–12. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ehlis AC, Reif A, Herrmann MJ, et al. Impact of catechol-O-methyltransferase on prefrontal brain functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:162–70. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Dickerson FB, Boronow JJ, Stallings C, et al. The catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism is not associated with broad-based cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2007;96:87–92. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Caldú X, Vendrell P, Bartres-Faz D, et al. Impact of the COMT Val108/158 Met and DAT genotypes on prefrontal function in healthy subjects. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1437–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Barnett JH, Jones PB, Robbins TW, et al. Effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism on executive function: a meta-analysis of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test in schizophrenia and healthy controls. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12:502–9. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Szöke A, Schürhoff F, Méary A, et al. Lack of influence of COMT and NET genes variants on executive functions in schizophrenic and bipolar patients, their first-degree relatives and controls. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2006;141B:504–12. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Rybakowski JK, Borkowska A, Czerski PM, et al. Performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in schizophrenia and genes of dopaminergic inactivation (COMT, DAT, NET) Psychiatry Res. 2006;143:13–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Krabbendam L, Isusi P, Galdos P, et al. Associations between COMTVal158Met polymorphism and cognition: Direct or indirect effects? Eur Psychiatry. 2006;21:338–42. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Han DH, Kee BS, Min KJ, et al. Effects of catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism on the cognitive stability and aggression in the first-onset schizophrenic patients. Neuroreport. 2006;17:95–9. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000192740.38653.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Golimbet V, Gritsenko I, Alfimova M, et al. Association study of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism with auditory P300 and performance on neurocognitive tests in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2006;7:238–45. doi: 10.1080/15622970600670970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bertolino A, Rubino V, Sambataro F, et al. Prefrontal-hippocampal coupling during memory processing is modulated by COMT val158met genotype. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:1250–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Stefanis NC, van Os J, Avramopoulos D, et al. Effect of COMT Val158Met polymorphism on the Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs Version: tuning rather than improving performance. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1752–4. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ho BC, Wassink TH, O’Leary DS, et al. Catechol-O-methyl transferase Val158Met gene polymorphism in schizophrenia: working memory, frontal lobe MRI morphology and frontal cerebral blood flow. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:229, 287–98. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Galderisi S, Maj M, Kirkpatrick B, et al. COMT Val(158)Met and BDNF C(270)T polymorphisms in schizophrenia: a case-control study. Schizophr Res. 2005;73:27–30. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Bruder GE, Keilp JG, Xu H, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) genotypes and working memory: associations with differing cognitive operations. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:901–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Blasi G, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, et al. Effect of catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met genotype on attentional control. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5038–45. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0476-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Rosa A, Peralta V, Cuesta MJ, et al. New evidence of association between COMT gene and prefrontal neurocognitive function in healthy individuals from sibling pairs discordant for psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1110–2. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.1110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Nolan KA, Bilder RM, Lachman HM, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism in schizophrenia: differential effects of Val and Met alleles on cognitive stability and flexibility. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:359–61. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.de Frias CM, Annerbrink K, Westberg L, et al. COMT gene polymorphism is associated with declarative memory in adulthood and old age. Behav Genet. 2004;34:533–9. doi: 10.1023/B:BEGE.0000038491.06972.8c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Tsai SJ, Yu YW, Chen TJ, et al. Association study of a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase-gene polymorphism and cognitive function in healthy females. Neurosci Lett. 2003;338:123–6. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)01396-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Goldberg TE, Egan MF, Gscheidle T, et al. Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:889–96. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Malhotra AK, Kestler LJ, Mazzanti C, et al. A functional polymorphism in the COMT gene and performance on a test of prefrontal cognition. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:652–4. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.4.652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Joober R, Gauthier J, Lal S, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val-108/158-Met gene variants associated with performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:662–3. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.7.662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, et al. Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:6917–22. doi: 10.1073/pnas.111134598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Bellani M, Brambilla P. The use and meaning of the continuous performance test in schizophrenia. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2008;17:188–91. doi: 10.1017/s1121189x00001275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Uçok A, Ozturk M, Duman Z, et al. COMT Val 158 Met polymorphism is related with interpersonal problem solving in schizophrenia. Eur Psychiatry. 2010;25:320–2. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16:17–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Mahurin RK, Velligan DI, Hazleton B, et al. Trail making test errors and executive function in schizophrenia and depression. Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;20:271–88. doi: 10.1080/13854040590947498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Sánchez-Cubillo I, Perianez JA, Drover-Roig D, et al. Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:438–50. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Burdick KE, Funke B, Goldberg JF, et al. COMT genotype increases risk for bipolar I disorder and influences neurocognitive performance. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9:370–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00384.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.O’Hara R, Miller E, Liao CP, et al. COMT genotype, gender and cognition in community-dwelling, older adults. Neurosci Lett. 2006;409:205–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.09.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Winterer G, Musso F, Vucurevic G, et al. COMT genotype predicts BOLD signal and noise characteristics in prefrontal circuits. Neuroimage. 2006;32:1722–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Barnes A, Isohanni M, Barnett JH, et al. No association of COMT (Val158Met) genotype with brain structure differences between men and women. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e33964. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Cerasa A, Cherubini A, Quattrone A, et al. Met158 variant of the catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype is associated with thicker cortex in adult brain. Neuroscience. 2010;167:809–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.02.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Honea R, Verchinski BA, Pezawas L, et al. Impact of interacting functional variants in COMT on regional gray matter volume in human brain. Neuroimage. 2009;45:44–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Dutt A, McDonald C, Dempster E, et al. The effect of COMT, BDNF, 5-HTT, NRG1 and DTNBP1 genes on hippocampal and lateral ventricular volume in psychosis. Psychol Med. 2009;39:1783–97. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Zinkstok J, Schmitz N, van Amelsvoort T, et al. Genetic variation in COMT and PRODH is associated with brain anatomy in patients with schizophrenia. Genes Brain Behav. 2008;7:61–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00326.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Cerasa A, Gioia MC, Labate A, et al. Impact of catechol-O-methyltransferase Val(108/158) Met genotype on hippocampal and prefrontal gray matter volume. Neuroreport. 2008;19:405–8. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f5f784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Taylor WD, Zuchner S, Payne ME, et al. The COMT Val158Met polymorphism and temporal lobe morphometry in healthy adults. Psychiatry Res. 2007;155:173–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Crespo-Facorro B, Roiz-Santianez R, Pelayo-Teran JM, et al. Low-activity allele of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMTL) is associated with increased lateral ventricles in patients with first episode non-affective psychosis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2007;31:1514–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Zinkstok J, Schmitz N, van Amelsvoort T, et al. The COMT val158met polymorphism and brain morphometry in healthy young adults. Neurosci Lett. 2006;405:34–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Krach S, Jansen A, Krug A, et al. COMT genotype and its role on hippocampal-prefrontal regions in declarative memory. Neuroimage. 2010;53:978–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Di Giorgio A, Caforio G, Blasi G, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val(158)Met association with parahippocampal physiology during memory encoding in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2011;41:1721–31. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710002278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Mier D, Kirsch P, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Neural substrates of pleiotropic action of genetic variation in COMT: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:918–27. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Karch S, Leicht G, Giegling I, et al. Inefficient neural activity in patients with schizophrenia and nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients: evidence from a working memory task. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43:1185–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Blokland GA, McMahon KL, Hoffman J, et al. Quantifying the heritability of task-related brain activation and performance during the N-back working memory task: a twin fMRI study. Biol Psychol. 2008;79:70–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Daban C, Amado I, Bourdel MC, et al. Cognitive dysfunctions in medicated and unmedicated patients with recent-onset schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 2005;39:391–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Elsabagh S, Premkumar P, Anilkumar AP, et al. A longer duration of schizophrenic illness has sex-specific associations within the working memory neural network in schizophrenia. Behav Brain Res. 2009;201:41–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.01.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Krieger S, Lis S, Janik H, et al. Executive function and cognitive subprocesses in first-episode, drug-naive schizophrenia: an analysis of N-back performance. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1206–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Bove EA. Cognitive performance and basic symptoms in first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients. Compr Psychiatry. 2008;49:321–9. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Zilles D, Burke S, Schneider-Axmann T, et al. Diagnosis-specific effect of familial loading on verbal working memory in schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;259:309–15. doi: 10.1007/s00406-009-0001-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Callicott JH, Egan MF, Mattay VS, et al. Abnormal fMRI response of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in cognitively intact siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:709–19. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Scala S, Lasalvia A, Cristofalo D, et al. Neurocognitive profile and its association with psychopathology in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. A case–control study. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200:137–43. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Saoud M, d’Amato T, Gutknecht C, et al. Neuropsychological deficit in siblings discordant for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:893–902. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Wang Q, Chan R, Sun J, et al. Reaction time of the Continuous Performance Test is an endophenotypic marker for schizophrenia: a study of first-episode neuroleptic-naive schizophrenia, their non-psychotic first-degree relatives and healthy population controls. Schizophr Res. 2007;89:293–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Cornblatt BA, Risch NJ, Faris G, et al. The Continuous Performance Test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP): I. New findings about sustained attention in normal families. Psychiatry Res. 1988;26:223–38. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(88)90076-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Chen WJ, Hsiao CK, Hsiao LL, et al. Performance of the Continuous Performance Test among community samples. Schizophr Bull. 1998;24:163–74. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Cornblatt BA, Malhotra AK. Impaired attention as an endophenotype for molecular genetic studies of schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet. 2001;105:11–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Cornblatt B, Obuchowski M, Roberts S, et al. Cognitive and behavioral precursors of schizophrenia. Dev Psychopathol. 1999;11:487–508. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Tsuang HC, Lin SH, Liu SK, et al. More severe sustained attention deficits in nonpsychotic siblings of multiplex schizophrenia families than in those of simplex ones. Schizophr Res. 2006;87:172–80. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.03.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Ellison-Wright I, Glahn DC, Laird AR, et al. The anatomy of first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1015–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Fornito A, Yucel M, Patti J, et al. Mapping grey matter reductions in schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Schizophr Res. 2009;108:104–13. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Glahn DC, Laird AR, Ellison-Wright I, et al. Meta-analysis of gray matter anomalies in schizophrenia: application of anatomic likelihood estimation and network analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:774–81. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Shepherd AM, Laurens KR, Matheson SL, et al. Systematic meta-review and quality assessment of the structural brain alterations in schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:1342–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Steen RG, Mull C, McClure R, et al. Brain volume in first-episode schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;188:510–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.188.6.510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Vita A, De PL, Silenzi C, et al. Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of quantitative magnetic resonance imaging studies. Schizophr Res. 2006;82:75–88. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Wright IC, Rabe-Hesketh S, Woodruff PW, et al. Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:16–25. doi: 10.1176/ajp.157.1.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Peper JS, Brouwer RM, Boomsma DI, et al. Genetic influences on human brain structure: a review of brain imaging studies in twins. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:464–73. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Adriano F, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G. Hippocampal volume reduction in first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis. Neuroscientist. 2012;18:180–200. doi: 10.1177/1073858410395147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Boos HB, Aleman A, Cahn W, et al. Brain volumes in relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:297–304. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.3.297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Seidman LJ, Faraone SV, Goldstein JM, et al. Left hippocampal volume as a vulnerability indicator for schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging morphometric study of nonpsychotic first-degree relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:839–49. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Baiano M, David A, Versace A, et al. Anterior cingulate volumes in schizophrenia: a systematic review and a meta-analysis of MRI studies. Schizophr Res. 2007;93:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Cannon TD, Thompson PM, van Erp TG, et al. Mapping heritability and molecular genetic associations with cortical features using probabilistic brain atlases: methods and applications to schizophrenia. Neuroinformatics. 2006;4:5–19. doi: 10.1385/NI:4:1:5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Crescini A, et al. Neuroanatomy of vulnerability to psychosis: a voxel-based meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:1175–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Smieskova R, Fusar-Poli P, Allen P, et al. Neuroimaging predictors of transition to psychosis — a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:1207–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Dazzan P, Soulsby B, Mechelli A, et al. Volumetric abnormalities predating the onset of schizophrenia and affective psychoses: an MRI study in subjects at ultrahigh risk of psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:1083–91. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Ho BC. MRI brain volume abnormalities in young, nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenia probands are associated with subsequent prodromal symptoms. Schizophr Res. 2007;96:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Job DE, Whalley HC, McConnell S, et al. Voxel-based morphometry of grey matter densities in subjects at high risk of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2003;64:1–13. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(03)00158-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Cannon TD, Thompson PM, van Erp TG, et al. Cortex mapping reveals regionally specific patterns of genetic and disease-specific gray-matter deficits in twins discordant for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:3228–33. doi: 10.1073/pnas.052023499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.McIntosh AM, Job DE, Moorhead WJ, et al. Genetic liability to schizophrenia or bipolar disorder and its relationship to brain structure. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2006;141B:76–83. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Honea RA, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Hobbs KB, et al. Is gray matter volume an intermediate phenotype for schizophrenia? A voxel-based morphometry study of patients with schizophrenia and their healthy siblings. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:465–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Goldman AL, Pezawas L, Mattay VS, et al. Heritability of brain morphology related to schizophrenia: a large-scale automated magnetic resonance imaging segmentation study. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:475–83. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Mattay VS, et al. Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:1078–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.11.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Poline JB, Kohn PD, et al. Evidence for abnormal cortical functional connectivity during working memory in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1809–17. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Minzenberg MJ, Laird AR, Thelen S, et al. Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:811–22. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Nejad AB, Ebdrup BH, Siebner HR, et al. Impaired temporoparietal deactivation with working memory load in antipsychotic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2011;12:271–81. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2010.556199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Piras F, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G. Working memory performance and thalamus microstructure in healthy subjects. Neuroscience. 2010;171:496–505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Molina V, Solera S, Sanz J, et al. Association between cerebral metabolic and structural abnormalities and cognitive performance in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2009;173:88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Garlinghouse MA, Roth RM, Isquith PK, et al. Subjective rating of working memory is associated with frontal lobe volume in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2010;120:71–5. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.02.1067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Tsuchida A, Fellows LK. Lesion evidence that two distinct regions within prefrontal cortex are critical for n-back performance in humans. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:2263–75. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Salgado-Pineda P, Baeza I, Perez-Gomez M, et al. Sustained attention impairment correlates to gray matter decreases in first episode neuroleptic-naive schizophrenic patients. Neuroimage. 2003;19:365–75. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00094-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Laywer G, Nyman H, Agartz I, et al. Morphological correlates to cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia as studied with Bayesian regression. BMC Psychiatry. 2006;6:31. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-6-31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Crespo-Facorro B, Roiz-Santianez R, Perez-Iglesias R, et al. Specific brain structural abnormalities in first-episode schizophrenia. A comparative study with patients with schizophreniform disorder, non-schizophrenic non-affective psychoses and healthy volunteers. Schizophr Res. 2009;115:191–201. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Goldberg TE, Torrey EF, Berman KF, et al. Relations between neuropsychological performance and brain morphological and physiological measures in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1994;55:51–61. doi: 10.1016/0925-4927(94)90011-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Volz H, Gaser C, Hager F, et al. Decreased frontal activation in schizophrenics during stimulation with the continuous performance test — a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Eur Psychiatry. 1999;14:17–24. doi: 10.1016/s0924-9338(99)80711-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Honey GD, Pomarol-Clotet E, Corlett PR, et al. Functional dysconnectivity in schizophrenia associated with attentional modulation of motor function. Brain. 2005;128:2597–611. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Yoon JH, Minzenberg MJ, Ursu S, et al. Association of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction with disrupted coordinated brain activity in schizophrenia: relationship with impaired cognition, behavioral disorganization, and global function. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1006–14. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07060945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Delawalla Z, Csernansky JG, Barch DM. Prefrontal cortex function in nonpsychotic siblings of individuals with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:490–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Perlstein WM, Dixit NK, Carter CS, et al. Prefrontal cortex dysfunction mediates deficits in working memory and prepotent responding in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:25–38. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01675-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Grace AA. Phasic versus tonic dopamine release and the modulation of dopamine system responsivity: a hypothesis of the etiology of schizophrenia. Neuroscience. 1991;41:1–24. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90196-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Cohen JD, Braver TS, Brown JW. Computational perspectives on dopamine function in prefrontal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12:223–9. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(02)00314-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Bilder RM, Volavka J, Lachman HM, et al. The catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism: relations to the tonic-phasic dopamine hypothesis and neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1943–61. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Dickinson D, Elvevåg B. Genes, cognition and brain through a COMT lens. Neuroscience. 2009;164:72–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Tan HY, Callicott JH, Weinberger DR. Prefrontal cognitive systems in schizophrenia: towards human genetic brain mechanisms. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2009;14:277–98. doi: 10.1080/13546800903091665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Tan HY, Nicodemus KK, Chen Q, et al. Genetic variation in AKT1 is linked to dopamine-associated prefrontal cortical structure and function in humans. J Clin Invest. 2008;118:2200–8. doi: 10.1172/JCI34725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Russo SJ, Bolanos CA, Theobald DE, et al. IRS2-Akt pathway in midbrain dopamine neurons regulates behavioral and cellular responses to opiates. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:93–9. doi: 10.1038/nn1812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Tan HY, Chen AG, Kolachana B, et al. Effective connectivity of AKT1-mediated dopaminergic working memory networks and pharmacogenetics of anti-dopaminergic treatment. Brain. 2012;135:1436–45. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Buckert M, Kudielka BM, Reuter M, et al. The COMT Val158Met polymorphism modulates working memory performance under acute stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1810–21. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN are provided here courtesy of Canadian Medical Association

RESOURCES