Abstract
Background
In order to assess the importance of environmental and genetic risk on transition from health to psychotic disorder, a prospective study of individuals at average (n = 462) and high genetic risk (n = 810) was conducted.
Method
A three-year cohort study examined the rate of transition to psychotic disorder. Binary measures indexing environmental exposure (combining urban birth, cannabis use, ethnicity and childhood trauma) and proxy genetic risk (high-risk sibling status) were used to model transition.
Results
The majority of high-risk siblings (68%) and healthy comparison subjects (60%) had been exposed to one or more environmental risks. The risk of transition in siblings (n = 9, 1.1%) was higher than the risk in healthy comparison subjects (n = 2, 0.4%; ORadj = 2.2,95%CI:5–10.3). All transitions (100%) were associated with environmental exposure, compared to 65% of non-transitions (p = 0.014), with the greatest effects for childhood trauma (ORadj = 34.4,95%CI:4.4–267.4), cannabis use (OR = 4.1,95%CI:1.1, 15.4), minority ethnic group (OR = 3.8,95%CI:1.2,12.8) and urban birth (OR = 3.7,95%CI:0.9,15.4). The proportion of transitions in the population attributable to environmental and genetic risk ranged from 28% for minority ethnic group, 45% for urban birth, 57% for cannabis use, 86% for childhood trauma, and 50% for high-risk sibling status. Nine out of 11 transitions (82%) were exposed to both genetic and environmental risk, compared to only 43% of non-transitions (p = 0.03).
Conclusion
Environmental risk associated with transition to psychotic disorder is semi-ubiquitous regardless of genetic high risk status. Careful prospective documentation suggests most transitions can be attributed to powerful environmental effects that become detectable when analysed against elevated background genetic risk, indicating gene-environment interaction.
Introduction
Several environmental factors have been implicated in the aetiology of psychotic illness including urban birth and upbringing [1], minority position [2], childhood trauma [3] and cannabis use [4]. The impact of some environmental risk factors may be greater in those at increased genetic risk suggesting possible interaction between genetic and environmental risks [5].
Associations between environment and mental illness typically are assessed in case-control studies that are easy to conduct but prone to bias and confounding. Cohort studies following individuals from health to illness transition are preferable but expensive and impractical given long incubation periods between exposure and outcome. In addition, given that the prevalence of psychotic disorder in the general population is low [6], cohort studies require large samples that, in order to reduce costs, are subjected to inaccurate non-clinical diagnostic assessments. A high-risk cohort study, following individuals with higher than average genetic risk for psychotic disorder has advantages, given that transition rates will be higher, thus reducing required length of follow-up. In addition, by combining individuals of high average risk in the cohort, a proxy variable indexing genetic risk is created allowing for additional examination of genetic effects, as well as gene-environment interaction [7]. There is also research on transition in individuals described at ‘Ultra-High Risk’, however transition in this population does not refer to transition from health to psychotic disorder, as UHR samples in fact already are help-seeking patients with mental disorder diagnoses [8] who present for treatment at mental health services [9]. In the current article, a detailed and careful follow-up was conducted of healthy siblings (of patients with non-affective psychotic disorder) and healthy comparison subjects sampled in the context of the GROUP study [10], in order to determine true transitions from (non-psychotic) health to psychotic disorder on the basis of clinical interview, and determine the contribution of genetic and environmental factors, taking into account the range of postnatal risk factors for which meta-analytic evidence exists [5]. Given these findings, we expect that urban birth, minority position, childhood trauma and cannabis use all contribute to an increased probability of transition to psychotic disorder, and that this impact may be increased in individuals at higher than average genetic risk.
Methods
Subjects
Full details of the GROUP study have been presented elsewhere [10], [11]. In representative geographical areas in the Netherlands and Belgium, patients were identified through clinicians working in regional psychotic disorder services, whose caseload was screened for inclusion criteria. Subsequently, a group of patients presenting at these services either as out-patients or in-patients were recruited for the study. Healthy comparison subjects were selected through random mailings to addresses in the catchment areas of the cases. The GROUP study was not conducted in a geographically well- defined small area, as it in fact included the majority of mental health services in the Netherlands, and a substantial part of mental health services in Dutch-speaking Belgium. Healthy comparison subjects could not be representative in all aspects, as an exclusion criterion was absence of a family history of psychotic disorder. The goal was to collect a control group that (i) was collected from the same geographical area as the case in the relevant mental health service, (ii) was sufficiently large to allow for chance variation and (iii) was frequency-matched in age- and sex distribution to the siblings and (iv) had absence of family history of psychotic disorder. Table 1 shows that healthy comparison subjects and siblings had similar sex distribution and also did not have large differences in age.
Table 1. Demographics of participants in the GROUP study.
Variable | Siblings (n = 1057) | Healthy comparison subjects (n = 589) | |
Mean (standard deviation) | Mean (standard deviation) | t value (p) | |
Age at T0 | 27.8 (8.3) | 30.4 (10.6) | 5.53 (<0.001) |
Gender, male (%) | 45.6 | 45.7 | −0.03 (0.511) |
Education, Verhagea | 5.1 (2.1) | 5.4 (1.8) | 3.26 (0,001) |
WAIS-III Estimated IQ | 103.0 (15.3) | 109.9 (14.8) | 8.73 (<0.001) |
Ethnicity, Caucasian (%) | 83.2 | 92.0 | 4.98 (<0.001) |
Urbanicity at birthb | 2.7 (1.7) | 2.6 (1.7) | −0.27 (0.790) |
Education (Verhage): range 0 (no education), 3–5 (school diploma) to 8 (university degree).
Urbanicity: 1 = <500/km2; 2 = 500–1000/km2; 3 = 1000–1500/km2; 4 = 1500–2500/km2; 5 = 2500+/km2.
The full GROUP sample at baseline consisted of 1119 patients with non-affective psychotic disorder, 1057 siblings of these patients, 919 parents of the patients and 589 unrelated healthy comparison subjects. Inclusion criteria were: (i) age range 16 to 50 years and (ii) good command of Dutch language. For patients, an additional inclusion criterion was the presence of a clinical diagnosis of non-affective psychotic disorder. Healthy comparison subjects status was confirmed by using the Family Interview for Genetic studies [12] with the control as informant, to establish absence of first degree relatives with a psychotic disorder. Diagnosis was based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder-IV (DSM-IV) criteria [13], assessed with the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History (CASH) interview [14] or Schedules for Clinical Assessment for Neuropsychiatry (SCAN 2.1) [15]. The majority of patients had a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia (DSM-IV 295.x; n = 940, 84%). In the sibling and control groups, there were respectively 151 (14%) and 60 participants (10%) with a history of a common mental disorder at baseline, the majority of whom had a mood disorder (DSM-IV 296.x). For the purpose of the current analysis, the siblings and healthy comparison subjects groups were included.
The study was approved by the standing ethics committee (Medisch Ethische Toetsingscommissie, UMC Utrecht), and all the subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the committee's guidelines. This committee waived the need for additional informed consent of parents or supervisors for underaged participants ages 16 and older, given the non-experimental/medical nature of this study.
Substance use
Substance use was assessed using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) [16] and through urinalysis. Two different measures of cannabis exposure, assessed both at baseline and follow-up were used to construct incident exposure to cannabis over the follow-up period: (i) CIDI lifetime cannabis use (hereafter: interview cannabis use): none (0), versus any use (1) and (ii) current cannabis use assessed by urinalysis (hereafter: urinalysis cannabis use): none (0) and present (1). Urinalysis was carried out as a screen for the presence of cannabis at the national Alcohol- and Drug use ‘Jellinek’ Laboratory. The method used was immunoassays with a cut-off of 50 ng/ml. In addition, as an integrity parameter, the creatinine level of every sample was measured. Cannabis urine screening has a detection window up to 30 days, but the detection time has been documented in literature to be even longer (up to three months), depending on level of cannabis use [17]. Given the relatively high cut-off level of 50 ng/ml, a conservative detection window of one month can be inferred. A dichotomous measure was created reflecting first exposure to cannabis over the follow-up period, defined as any instance of positive interview cannabis use or positive urinalysis cannabis use in those without interview cannabis use and without urinalysis cannabis use at baseline.
Childhood trauma
Childhood trauma was assessed with the Dutch version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) 25 item Short Form [18], consisting of 25 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never to 5 = very often). Emotional, physical and general abuse, and emotional and physical neglect were assessed, five items covering each trauma type [18]. Total trauma represents the mean score of all 25 items (range in siblings and healthy comparison subjects: 1–4.3). Conform previous analyses in this sample [19], trauma scores were dichotomized a priori into high trauma and low trauma, the cut-off being defined as the 80th percentile of scores for the healthy comparison subjects.
Urban birth
Subjects were asked where they were born. To describe urbanicity, a historical population density record was generated for each municipality from 1930 onwards using the database of the Central Bureau of Statistics (Netherlands) and the HISSTAT database (University of Gent, Department Modern History, Belgium). When data was not available, linear extrapolations were computed. When historical names of municipalities disappeared from historical records (e.g. due to city mergers) available date from the agglomerate city were used. For each location, population density (by square kilometre, excluding water) at the municipality for that year was computed, on the basis of which the urbanicity code (1 = <500/km2; 2 = 500–1000/km2; 3 = 1000–1500/km2; 4 = 1500–2500/km2; 5 = 2500+/km2) was calculated. In accordance with research using the five-level exposure, a binary urban birth exposure was calculated combining categories 1 to 3 (“0”), reflecting low urbanicity, and 4 and 5 (“1”), reflecting high urbanicity [20], [21].
Psychosis measures
The Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE; www.cape42.homestead.com) was developed in order to rate self-reports of lifetime psychotic experiences. Items are modelled on patient experiences as contained in the PSE-9 [22] and the schedules assessing negative symptoms such as the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) [23] and the Subjective Experience of Negative Symptoms (SENS) [24]. Items are scored on a 4-point scale. In the current analyses, CAPE dimensions of frequency of positive experiences (20 items) and negative experiences (14 items) were included (measured at baseline and 3-year follow-up), representing the person's perceived psychosis load over the lifetime (at baseline) or in the past three years (follow-up). A total score representing the mean of all items was calculated for each dimension (CAPE positive: range in siblings and comparison subjects: 0–2.5; CAPE negative: range 0–2.4).
Other measures
At baseline and at follow-up, the short form of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) – III was assessed for an indication of intellectual functioning, and included the following tests: ‘Block Design’, ‘Digit Symbol’, ‘Arithmetic’ and ‘Information’ [25], [26]. The WHOQOL-BREF [27] was used at baseline and at follow-up to assess four domains of quality of life (1) physical health, (2) mental health, (3) social relationships and (4) environment. At baseline, the Premorbid Adjustment Scale (PAS) [28] was administered. The PAS is a rating scale that includes measures of social isolation, peer relationships, functioning outside of the family, and school functioning at 3 age periods (up to age 12 [4 items], 12–15 [5 items], 16–18 years [10 items]). Validity, interrater reliability and internal consistency have been found to be high [29], [30]. An overall score based on the three age periods was created with a sample range from 0 (healthiest adjustment) to 5 (lowest adjustment).
Follow-up
Healthy comparison subjects and siblings were eligible for follow-up. Of these, 78% (n = 1272) were assessed at 3-year follow-up (healthy comparison subjects: 78%, n = 462; siblings: 77%, n = 810). Measures of cannabis use at follow-up reflected use over the interval between baseline and follow-up. Ratings of CASH, SCAN, SIS-R and CAPE at follow-up reflected the period between baseline and follow-up. Mean follow-up was 3.3 years (SD = 0.5).
Transition
Transition from health to psychotic disorder was defined as (i) onset of non-affective psychotic disorder in individuals without psychotic disorder (DSMIV 295, 297, 298) and without psychotic affective disorder at baseline [n = 11; 7 with 295, 3 with 298, 1 with documented psychotic illness who refused follow-up], (ii) onset of affective disorder or other non-psychotic disorder with evidence of psychotic symptoms rated of at least “considerable” or “severe” quality (or equivalent) on the CASH, PANSS or SCAN in individuals without baseline affective disorder or other non-psychotic disorder and without evidence of psychotic symptoms rated of at least “considerable” or “severe” quality at baseline (n = 0). Individuals who refused to be seen at follow-up were queried about mental health and contacts with mental health services. Participating relatives of refusing participants also provided information.
Analysis
Analyses were conducted using Stata, version 12 [31]. Analyses focused on the siblings (n = 1057 at baseline and n = 810 at follow-up) and healthy comparison subjects (n = 589 at baseline, n = 462 at follow-up). The dependent variable in the analyses was transition to psychotic disorder. Standard errors were corrected for hierarchical clustering of the data at the level of the family (clustering of siblings in the same family) or, when applicable, for clustering at the two levels occasioned by clustering of individuals in the same family and of repeated measures within the same person, using the Stata routines of cluster, xtreg or xtmixed, as appropriate.
Associations were expressed as the odds ratio from the logistic regression model (dichotomous transition outcome) or the regression coefficient (B) from multilevel random regression models (continuous variables). All analyses were a priori adjusted for age and sex. Comparisons between transition and non-transition status were additionally adjusted for sibling high risk status, in order to verify whether transition and non-transition differed in key variables independent of sibling high risk status.
In order to validate transitions, a comparison was made between transition and non-transition status with respect to key baseline variables as well as with respect to changes from baseline to follow-up. We thus expected that those who would make a transition to psychotic disorder would display more developmental impairment and higher levels of psychometric risk indicators at baseline (as measured with the CAPE, WHOQOL, PAS and WAIS). Differences in change from baseline to follow-up were examined in an xtmixed model of a repeated measure, whilst fitting an interaction between measurement occasion and transition status. Stratified associations were derived by linear combination from the model containing the interaction using the Stata margin command. The population attributable fraction associated with proxy environmental and genetic exposures was calculated using the cc command in Stata, and defined as the reduction in incidence that would be observed if the population were entirely unexposed, compared with its current exposure pattern.
Results
Sample and attrition
At baseline, the risk set consisted of 589 healthy comparison subjects and 1057 siblings. Baseline demographic characteristics are shown in table 1. Of these, respectively 462 (men: 44%, mean age: 34.2 years, sd = 10.6) and 810 (men: 44%, mean age: 30.5 years, sd = 7.9) were seen at follow-up. Attrition was associated with male sex, urban environment and ethnic minority status, as well as with lower IQ and small differences in premorbid adjustment (Table 2). Attrition was not associated with age at baseline, CAPE positive or negative symptoms, cannabis use, childhood trauma and WHOQOL-BREF domains (small or non-significant differences; Table 2).
Table 2. Differences at baseline as a function of follow-up attrition.
Mean or % | SD | n | F or χ2 | p | ||
Age at baseline | No follow-up | 28.1 | 9.6 | 374 | 2.4 | 0.126 |
Follow-up | 28.9 | 9.2 | 1,272 | |||
Male sex | No follow-up | 51% | 374 | 5.2 | 0.022 | |
Follow-up | 44% | 1,272 | ||||
Minority ethnic group | No follow-up | 24% | 374 | 37.5 | <0.001 | |
Follow-up | 12% | 1,272 | ||||
Urban birth | No follow-up | 41% | 326 | 8.5 | 0.004 | |
Follow-up | 32% | 1,193 | ||||
Cannabis use | No follow-up | 39% | 369 | 0.4 | 0.547 | |
Follow-up | 37% | 1,272 | ||||
Early Trauma | No follow-up | 50% | 137 | 0.2 | 0.654 | |
Follow-up | 52% | 1,177 | ||||
CAPE positive | No follow-up | 0.21 | 0.21 | 325 | 1.6 | 0.21 |
Follow-up | 0.20 | 0.18 | 1,156 | |||
CAPE negative | No follow-up | 0.50 | 0.37 | 325 | 3.1 | 0.077 |
Follow-up | 0.54 | 0.36 | 1,156 | |||
WHOQOL physical | No follow-up | 4.05 | 0.57 | 310 | 8.0 | 0.005 |
Follow-up | 4.15 | 0.53 | 1,174 | |||
WHOQOL mental | No follow-up | 3.85 | 0.55 | 309 | 1.0 | 0.326 |
Follow-up | 3.88 | 0.52 | 1,174 | |||
WHOQOL social | No follow-up | 3.86 | 0.76 | 309 | 1.2 | 0.282 |
Follow-up | 3.91 | 0.65 | 1,174 | |||
WHOQOL environmental | No follow-up | 3.89 | 0.57 | 309 | 26.7 | <0.001 |
Follow-up | 4.06 | 0.47 | 1,174 | |||
IQ | No follow-up | 101.0 | 14.1 | 342 | 37.6 | <0.001 |
Follow-up | 106.7 | 15.7 | 1,236 | |||
PAS premorbid adjustment | No follow-up | 1.20 | 0.63 | 347 | 4.9 | 0.027 |
Follow-up | 1.11 | 0.63 | 1,187 |
Transition
Those who made a transition to psychotic disorder were younger at baseline (transition: 22.9 years, sd = 4.8; non-transition: 29.0 years, sd = 9.2 years; F = 4.82, p = 0.028). Transition was not associated with sex (OR = 1.4, 95% CI: 0.4–4.7). The 11 transitions were characterized by higher baseline psychopathology (CAPE positive and negative domains), poorer WHO-QOL scores, lower IQ and poorer premorbid adjustment (Table 3). In addition, transitions also displayed greater increases in psychopathology from baseline to follow-up (CAPE positive and CAPE negative) and greater decreases in quality of life, with the exception of the environmental domain. Transition was not associated with changes in IQ (Table 3). One of the 11 individuals had a non-psychotic DSM-IV diagnosis at baseline (300.3 obsessive-compulsive disorder), in the absence of significant psychotic symptoms at interview.
Table 3. Key variable baseline values and change over time (baseline to follow-up) as a function of transition status.
BASELINE | FOLLOW-UP | |||||||||||||||
Baseline transition vs. non-transition | Change transition vs. non-transition | |||||||||||||||
time x transition interaction# | Stratified change values* | |||||||||||||||
mean | SD | n | B | p | mean | SD | n | p | B | p | ||||||
CAPE positiveA | Non-Transition | 0.2 | 0.2 | 1,147 | 0.34 | <0.001 | 0.1 | 0.1 | 1,213 | 0.44 | <0.001 | −0.09 | <0.001 | |||
Transition | 0.5 | 0.3 | 10 | 0.9 | 0.7 | 8 | 0.35 | <0.001 | ||||||||
CAPE negativeB | Non-Transition | 0.5 | 0.4 | 1,147 | 0.31 | 0.005 | 0.4 | 0.4 | 1,213 | 0.75 | <0.001 | −0.10 | <0.001 | |||
Transition | 0.8 | 0.3 | 10 | 1.5 | 0.5 | 8 | 0.65 | <0.001 | ||||||||
WHOQOL physicalC | Non-Transition | 4.2 | 0.5 | 1,165 | −0.69 | <0.001 | 4.2 | 0.5 | 1,217 | −0.68 | <0.001 | 0.08 | <0.001 | |||
Transition | 3.5 | 0.7 | 10 | 2.9 | 0.9 | 8 | −0.60 | <0.001 | ||||||||
WHOQOL mentalD | Non-Transition | 3.9 | 0.5 | 1,165 | −0.48 | 0.003 | 3.9 | 0.5 | 1,217 | −0.70 | <0.001 | 0.06 | <0.001 | |||
Transition | 3.4 | 0.6 | 10 | 2.7 | 0.9 | 8 | −0.65 | <0.001 | ||||||||
WHOQOL socialE | Non-Transition | 3.9 | 0.6 | 1,165 | −0.37 | 0.076 | 3.9 | 0.7 | 1,216 | −1.10 | <0.001 | 0.04 | 0.062 | |||
Transition | 3.6 | 0.8 | 10 | 2.4 | 1.2 | 8 | −1.06 | <0.001 | ||||||||
WHOQOL environmentalF | Non-Transition | 4.1 | 0.5 | 1,165 | −0.75 | <0.001 | 4.2 | 0.5 | 1,217 | −0.23 | 0.159 | 0.12 | <0.001 | |||
Transition | 3.3 | 0.4 | 10 | 3.2 | 0.6 | 8 | −0.10 | 0.527 | ||||||||
IQ | Non-Transition | 106.8 | 15.6 | 1,227 | −11.70 | 0.015 | 110.0 | 16.7 | 1,211 | 0.59 | 0.839 | 3.20 | <0.001 | |||
Transition | 93.8 | 16.6 | 10 | 101.1 | 19.4 | 8 | 3.80 | 0.198 | ||||||||
PAS premorbid | Non-Transition | 1.1 | 0.6 | 1,177 | 0.62 | 0.001 | − | − | ||||||||
adjustmentG | Transition | 1.7 | 0.9 | 11 | − | − |
All associations adjusted for age, sex and high-risk sibling status.
# Tests whether change over time is significantly different for transition versus non-transition participants.
Change over time calculated separately for transition and non-transition participant.
CAPE positive symptoms, higher scores indicating increased frequency of symptoms.
CAPE negative symptoms, higher scores indicating increased frequency of symptoms.
Quality of life − physical health, higher scores indicating better quality.
Quality of life − mental health, higher scores indicating better quality.
Quality of life − social relationships, higher scores indicating better quality.
Quality of life − environment, higher scores indicating better quality.
Premorbid adjustment, higher scores indicating more social isolation, fewer peer relationships, worse functioning outside family and worse school functioning.
Environmental and genetic prediction of transition
The majority of high-risk siblings (68%) and healthy comparison subjects (60%) had been exposed to one or more environmental risks. The risk of transition in siblings (n = 9 out of 810, 1.1%) was higher than the risk in healthy comparison subjects (n = 2 out of 462, 0.4%; OR adjusted for age and sex = 2.2, 95% CI: 0.5−10.3; Table 4). All transitions were associated with environmental exposure, compared to 65% of non-transitions (p = 0.014), with the greatest effects for childhood trauma (OR adjusted for age, sex and sibling status = 34.4, 95% CI: 4.4−267.4), cannabis use (OR = 4.1, 95% CI: 1.1, 15.4), minority ethnic group (OR = 3.8, 95% CI: 1.2, 12.8) and urban birth (OR = 3.7, 95% CI: 0.9, 15.4) (Table 3). The proportion of transitions in the population attributable to environmental risk (PAF), assuming causality, ranged from 28% for minority ethnic group, 45% for urban birth, 57% for cannabis use, 86% for childhood trauma, and 50% for high-risk sibling status (Table 4). Nine out of 11 transitions (82%) were exposed to both proxy genetic and environmental risk, compared to only 43% of non-transitions (p = 0.03; Table 5).
Table 4. Transition as a function of proxy environmental and genetic exposures.
Non-transition | Transition | Odds ratioadj * | 95% CI | PAF # | ||||
n | % | n | % | |||||
Minority position | Majority | 1,117 | 88.5 | 7 | 63.6 | 3.8 | 1.2−12.8 | 28% |
Minority | 145 | 11.5 | 4 | 36.4 | ||||
Urban birth | Non-urban | 807 | 68.0 | 3 | 32.0 | 3.7 | 0.9−15.4 | 45% |
Urban | 379 | 37.5 | 5 | 62.5 | ||||
Cannabis use | No use | 798 | 63.2 | 3 | 27.3 | 4.1 | 1.1−15.4 | 57% |
Use | 464 | 36.8 | 8 | 72.7 | ||||
Early trauma | No | 921 | 78.9 | 1 | 11.1 | 34.4 | 4.4−267.4 | 86% |
Yes | 247 | 21.2 | 8 | 88.9 | ||||
Any exposure | No | 447 | 35.4 | 0 | 0.0 | ∞ | ||
Yes | 815 | 64.6 | 11 | 100.0 | ||||
High risk group | Comparison subject | 460 | 99.6 | 2 | 0.4 | 2.2 | 0.5−10.3 | 50% |
Sibling | 802 | 98.9 | 9 | 1.1 |
Odd ratio's adjusted for age sex and high-risk sibling status.
# PAF = population attributable fraction, or the reduction in incidence that would be observed if the population were entirely.
unexposed, compared with its current exposure pattern.
∞ = OR is infinity due to zero denominator.
Table 5. Transition status as a function of exposure to proxy environmental (E) and/or genetic (G) exposures.
Neither G nor E | G or E | G and E | ||
Non-transition | n | 184 | 539 | 539 |
% | 14.6 | 42.7 | 42.7 | |
Transition | n | 0 | 2 | 9 |
% | 0.0 | 18.2 | 81.8 |
Pearson chi2 (2) = 7.0 Pr = 0.030.
Discussion
In order to assess the importance of environmental and genetic risk on transition from health to psychotic disorder, a prospective study of a cohort of individuals with average and high genetic risk was carried out. The findings suggest that the rate of exposure to any environmental risk in the population is very high, or semi-ubiquitous, and that transition from health to psychotic disorder is strongly dependent on such exposure. Thus, all environmental risk factors were associated with transition to psychotic disorder, with the greatest effect, in terms of both relative and attributable risk, for childhood trauma. Exposure to environmental risk did not vary as a function of genetic high risk status, suggesting absence of genetic control of environmental exposure, or gene-environment correlation. In those who made the transition to psychotic disorder, 82% were exposed to both proxy genetic and environmental risk, compared to only 43% of those who did not transition. This finding suggests that exposure to both genetic and environmental risk factors is necessary for transition, which is compatible with underlying gene-environment interaction. Careful prospective documentation therefore suggests most transitions can be attributed to powerful environmental effects operating against elevated background genetic risk.
Incidence of transition
Johnstone and colleagues followed a cohort of 163 young adults at average and high genetic risk, of which about 12% made a transition to psychotic disorder within 2.5 years [7], representing a yearly transition rate of 4.9% [7]. The yearly transition rate in the current study was 0.34% for siblings (1.1%/3.3 years of follow-up), and 0.13% for healthy comparison subjects (0.4%/3.3 years of follow-up). Differences with the Edinburgh high risk study of Johnstone and colleagues [32] may be related to the fact that their “high-risk” denoted more familial loading (2 affected relatives), and that the mean age was younger (21 years). The incidence in the healthy comparison subjects of the current study (0.13%) appears high compared to the classic incidence estimate of schizophrenia (0.02%). However, a direct comparison is not valid as our outcome included all affective and non-affective psychosis, was restricted to a young age group and case identification did not depend on use of health care. Previous work has shown that the rate of psychosis, thus defined, is up to six times higher than typical estimates [33].
The binary concept of transition may be difficult to define [9]. However, in the context of the current study, transition was from health to psychotic disorder, a clear and valid qualitative contrast that can be assessed reliably in the context of a clinical follow-up. The comparisons between transition and non-transition add to the notion of a valid contrast, given pre-transition differences in premorbid adjustment and cognition, that did not further decline after onset, conform expectation [34], [35]. However, the finding that cognition does not decline after onset has not been undisputed [36]. In addition, individuals developing psychotic disorder over the follow-up period displayed higher non-specific indices of psychometric risk and maladjustment as measured with the CAPE and WHOQOL at baseline, conform the model of clinical staging [37]. Furthermore, transition resulted in substantial increases for these variables, indicating true clinical change.
Comparison with previous work
[LOOSSER]The results are in line with previous research showing associations between several environmental risk factors and development of psychotic symptoms or psychotic disorder [1]–[4], particularly in those at high risk [5]. As “high-risk” in the current study was defined on the basis of higher than average genetic risk, rather that psychometric risk as observed in UHR samples [38] or samples with attenuated psychotic symptoms in the general population [39], comparison with previous work is limited. Both Habets and colleagues [40], as well as Welch and colleagues [41] showed that cannabis use was associated with differential impact on brain structures in individuals at familial high risk for schizophrenia, which Habets and colleagues furthermore did not observe in controls. Similarly, epidemiological studies have demonstrated that the impact of urbanicity on schizophrenia risk is greater in those with additional evidence of elevated genetic risk [21], [42], [43].
Genetic risk and environment risk: ubiquitous?
Studies focussing on the nature and extent of molecular genetic risk for schizophrenia have provided “molecular genetic evidence for a substantial polygenic component to the risk of schizophrenia involving thousands of common alleles of very small effect” [44]. In other words, molecular genetic variation contributing to risk for schizophrenia can be considered ubiquitous and distributed. Interestingly, the current study, being one of the first to examine multiple environmental risks together, suggests that conceptually the situation with regard to environmental risks may be similar. Thus, most individuals in the population were exposed to one or more of the environmental risks included in this study, and most of the transitions were attributable to environmental risk factors, against a background of genetic risk (most of the transitions being siblings of higher than average genetic risk). Methodologically this is an important issue, as the impact of a risk factor on a disease outcome cannot be detected if the entire population is exposed, unless the population can be separated into those who are differentially susceptible. Given the very high rate of exposure to environmental risks, the results suggest that careful follow-up of samples of differential genetic risk for psychotic disorder may be necessary to examine the true impact of environmental risk factors.
The nature of the impact of the environmental risks examined in the current study requires further clarification. First, the sample was too small to examine to what degree the environmental risks acted additively or more-than-additively. Previous work in general populations samples suggest that relationships may be both additive [45] and more-than-additive [46], [47]. In addition, the focus was on postnatal risk factors, although pre-natal risks may also play an important role [48]–[51].
Second, the data are not informative as to when and how the environmental factors examined impact on development to increase risk, and whether environmental risks gave rise to enduring liability early in life, or acted as precipitants in individuals at higher than average genetic risk. The temporal focus of the current investigation was on transition from health to illness, and retrospective examination of environmental impact is methodologically challenging.
A remarkable finding was the very high relative and attributable risk associated with childhood trauma. Given the prospective nature of the investigation, bias associated with a “search for meaning” cannot explain the results, in agreement with a growing number of prospective analyses testing the relationship between childhood adversity on the one hand, and psychosis on the other [52]–[56]. The results confirm the need to urgently identify the nature and the mechanism of risk associated with early adversity, as well the clinical implications thereof [57].
Methodological issues
Strengths of the study include careful prospective assessment and confirmation of control status by excluding those with a positive family history. Because of the relatively short follow-up period, the number of individuals in this study who transitioned to psychotic disorder was relatively small. Although some results were statistically conclusive, other analyses, for example risk associated with sibling status, were underpowered. As the sample will be seen again at six-year follow-up, amplification of the sample and more robust results will be possible, as well as more fine-grained testing of relationships between genetic and non-genetic risks. Another issue is selection, as the focus was on transition in siblings and healthy comparison subjects who had lived through a substantial period of risk. It cannot be excluded that the mix of risk factors impacting on transition varies as a function of age-at-onset, thus the results cannot necessarily be generalised to transitions from health to illness at all ages.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful for the generosity of time and effort by the families who make the GROUP project possible.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the Geestkracht program of the Dutch Health Research Council (ZON-MW, grant number 10-000-1002) and matching funds from participating universities and mental health care organizations (Site Amsterdam: Academic Psychiatric Centre AMC, Ingeest, Arkin, Dijk en Duin, Rivierduinen, Erasmus MC, GGZ Noord Holland Noord; Site Utrecht: University Medical Centre Utrecht, Altrecht, Symfora, Meerkanten, Riagg Amersfoort, Delta; Site Groningen: University Medical Centre Groningen, Lentis, GGZ Friesland, GGZ Drenthe, Dimence, Mediant, GGZ De Grote Rivieren and Parnassia psycho-medical centre; Site Maastricht: Maastricht University Medical Centre, GGZ Eindhoven, GGZ Midden-Brabant, GGZ Oost-Brabant, GGZ Noord- Midden Limburg, Mondriaan Zorggroep, Prins Clauscentrum Sittard, RIAGG Roermond, Universitair Centrum Sint-Jozef Kortenberg, CAPRI University of Antwerp, PC Ziekeren Sint-Truiden, PZ Sancta Maria Sint-Truiden, GGZ Overpelt, OPZ Rekem). The analyses were supported by unrestricted grants from Jansen-Cilag, Eli Lilly and Company, Astra-Zeneca and Lundbeck. The research leading to these results has received funding from the European Community's Seventh Framework Program under grant agreement No. HEALTH-F2-2009-241909 (Project EU-GEI). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Vassos E, Pedersen CB, Murray R, Collier DA, Lewis CM (2012) Meta-analysis of the association of urbanicity with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bull 38(6): 1118–1123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Cantor-Graae E, Selten JP (2005) Schizophrenia and migration: a meta-analysis and review. Am J Psychiat 162: 12–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, Lieverse R, Lataster T, et al. (2012) Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophrenia Bull;38 (4): 661–671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Matheson SL, Shepherd AM, Laurens KR, Carr VJ (2011) A systematic meta-review grading the evidence for non-genetic risk factors and putative antecedents of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res;133 (1–3): 133–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP (2010) The environment and schizophrenia. Nature;468 (7321): 203–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. van Nierop M, Van Os J, Gunther N, Myin-Germeys I, De Graaf R, et al. (2012) Phenotypically continuous with clinical psychosis, discontinuous in need for care: Evidence for an extended psychosis phenotype. Schizophrenia Bull;38 (2): 231–238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Johnstone EC, Ebmeier KP, Miller P, Owens DG, Lawrie SM (2005) Predicting schizophrenia: findings from the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. Br J Psychiatry 186: 18–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fusar-Poli P, Nelson B, Valmaggia L, Yung AR, McGuire PK (2012) Comorbid Depressive and Anxiety Disorders in 509 Individuals With an At-Risk Mental State: Impact on Psychopathology and Transition to Psychosis. Schizophrenia Bull; doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 9. Fusar-Poli P, van Os J (2013) Lost in transition: setting the psychosis threshold in prodromal research. Acta Psychiat Scand;127 (3): 248–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. GROUP (2011) Evidence that familial liability for psychosis is expressed as differential sensitivity to cannabis: an analysis of patient-sibling and sibling-control pairs. Arch Gen Psychiat;68 (2): 138–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Korver N, Quee PJ, Boos HB, Simons C, de Haan L (2012) Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP), a multi site longitudinal cohort study focused on gene-environment interaction: objectives, sample characteristics, recruitment and assessment methods. Int J Methods Psychiat Res;21 (3): 205–221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.NIMH Genetics Initiative (1992) Family Interview for Genetic Studies (FIGS). Rockville, Md: National Institute of Mental Health.
- 13.American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed., text rev. ed: Washington, DC.
- 14. Andreasen NC, Flaum M, Arndt S (1992) The Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History (CASH). An instrument for assessing diagnosis and psychopathology. Arch Gen Psychiatry;49 (8): 615–623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Wing JK, Babor T, Brugha T, Burke J, Cooper JE, et al. (1990) SCAN. Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry. Arch Gen Psychiatry;47 (6): 589–593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.World Health Organisation (1990) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) Version 1.0. Geneva: World Health Organisation.
- 17. Musshoff F, Madea B (2006) Review of biologic matrices (urine, blood, hair) as indicators of recent or ongoing cannabis use. Ther Drug Monit;28 (2): 155–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Bernstein DP, Ahluvalia T, Pogge D, Handelsman L (1997) Validity of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire in an adolescent psychiatric population. J Am Acad Child Psy;36 (3): 340–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Heins M, Simons C, Lataster T, Pfeifer S, Versmissen D, et al. (2011) Childhood trauma and psychosis: A case-control and case-sibling comparison across different levels of genetic liability, psychopathology, and type of trauma. Am J Psychiat;168 (12): 1286–1294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Marcelis M, Takei N, Van Os J (1999) Urbanization and risk for schizophrenia: does the effect operate before or around the time of illness onset? Psychol Med;29 (5): 1197–1203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Van Os J, Hanssen M, Bak M, Bijl RV, Vollebergh W (2003) Do urbanicity and familial liability coparticipate in causing psychosis? Am J Psychiatry;160 (3): 477–482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Wing JK, Cooper JE, Sartorius N (1974) The measurement and classification of psychiatric symptoms. London: Cambridge University Press.
- 23. Andreasen NC (1982) Negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Definition and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry;39 (7): 784–788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Selten JP, Sijben NE, van den Bosch RJ, Omloo Visser J, Warmerdam H (1993) The subjective experience of negative symptoms: a self-rating scale. Compr Psychiatry;34 (3): 192–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Blyler CR, Gold JM, Iannone VN, Buchanan RW (2000) Short form of the WAIS-III for use with patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research;46 (2−3): 209–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Velthorst E, Levine SZ, Henquet C, de Haan L, van Os J, et al. (2012) To cut a short test even shorter: Reliability and validity of a brief assessment of intellectual ability in Schizophrenia-a control-case family study. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 27. The WHOQOL Group (1998) Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. Psychol Med 28: 551–558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Cannon Spoor HE, Potkin SG, Wyatt RJ (1982) Measurement of premorbid adjustment in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull;8 (3): 470–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Brill N, Reichenberg A, Weiser M, Rabinowitz J (2008) Validity of the premorbid adjustment scale. Schizophrenia Bull;34 (5): 981–983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Small NE, Mohs RC, Halperin R, Rosen WG, Masterson C, et al. (1984) A study of the reliability of reported premorbid adjustment in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry;19 (2): 203–211. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.StataCorp (2011) STATA Statistical Software: Release 12. Texas: College Station.
- 32. Johnstone EC, Ebmeier KP, Miller P, Owens DGC, Lawrie SM (2005) Predicting schizophrenia: findings from the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. Brit J Psychiat 186: 18–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Perala J, Suvisaari J, Saarni SI, Kuoppasalmi K, Isometsa E, et al. (2007) Lifetime prevalence of psychotic and bipolar I disorders in a general population. Arch Gen Psychiatry;64 (1): 19–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Heaton RK, Gladsjo JA, Palmer BW, Kuck J, Marcotte TD, et al. (2001) Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry;58 (1): 24–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Russell AJ, Munro JC, Jones PB, Hemsley DR, Murray RM (1997) Schizophrenia and the myth of intellectual decline. Am J Psychiatry;154 (5): 635–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kahn RS, Keefe RSE (in press) Schizophrenia is a cognitive illness; time for a change in focus. JAMA. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 37. McGorry P, Van Os J (2013) Redeeming Diagnosis in Psychiatry: Timing versus Specificity Lancet. 381: 343–345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Fusar-Poli P, Bonoldi I, Yung A, Borgwardt S, Kempton MJ, et al. (2012) Predicting psychosis: meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Arch Gen Psychiat;69 (3): 220–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Kaymaz N, Drukker M, Lieb R, Wittchen H, Werbeloff N, et al. (2012) Do subthreshold psychotic experiences predict clinical outcomes in unselected non-help-seeking population-based samples? A systematic review and meta-analysis, enriched with new results. Psychol Med; DOI:10.1017/S0033291711002911. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 40. Habets P, Marcelis M, E G, Drukker M, van Os J (2011) Reduced cortical thickness as an outcome of differential sensitivity to environmental risks in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry;69 (5): 487–494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Welch KA, Stanfield AC, McIntosh AM, Whalley HC, Job DE, et al. (2011) Impact of cannabis use on thalamic volume in people at familial high risk of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry;199 (5): 386–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. van Os J, Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB (2004) Confirmation of synergy between urbanicity and familial liability in the causation of psychosis. Am J Psychiatry;161 (12): 2312–2314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Weiser M, van Os J, Reichenberg A, Rabinowitz J, Nahon D, et al. (2007) Social and cognitive functioning, urbanicity and risk for schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 191: 320–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, Visscher PM, O'Donovan MC, et al. (2009) Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 45. Cougnard A, Marcelis M, Myin-Germeys I, De Graaf R, Vollebergh W, et al. (2007) Does normal developmental expression of psychosis combine with environmental risk to cause persistence of psychosis? A psychosis proneness-persistence model. Psychol Med;37 (4): 513–527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Harley M, Kelleher I, Clarke M, Lynch F, Arseneault L, et al. (2010) Cannabis use and childhood trauma interact additively to increase the risk of psychotic symptoms in adolescence. Psychological Medicine;40 (10): 1627–1634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Clarke MC, Tanskanen A, Huttunen M, Whittaker JC, Cannon M (2009) Evidence for an interaction between familial liability and prenatal exposure to infection in the causation of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry;166 (9): 1025–1030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. McGrath J, Burne TH, Feron F, Mackay-Sim A, Eyles DW (2010) Developmental vitamin D deficiency and risk of schizophrenia: a 10-year update. Schizophrenia Bull;36 (6): 1073–1078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Cannon M, Jones PB, Murray RM (2002) Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: historical and meta-analytic review. Am J Psychiat;159 (7): 1080–1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Brown AS (2012) Epidemiologic studies of exposure to prenatal infection and risk of schizophrenia and autism. Dev Neurobiol;72 (10): 1272–1276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Kirkbride JB, Susser E, Kandakovic M, Kresovich JK, Davey Smith G, et al. (2012) Prenatal nutrition, epigenetics and schizophrenia risk: can we test causal effects? Epigenomics;4 (3): 303–315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Wicks S, Hjern A, Gunnell D, Lewis G, Dalman C (2005) Social adversity in childhood and the risk of developing psychosis: a national cohort study. Am J Psychiat 162: 1652–1657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Janssen I, Krabbendam L, Bak M, Hanssen M, Vollebergh W, et al. (2004) Childhood abuse as a risk factor for psychotic experiences. Acta Psychiat Scand 109: 38–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Elklit A, Shevlin M (2011) Female Sexual Victimization Predicts Psychosis: A Case-Control Study Based on the Danish Registry System. Schizophrenia Bull;37 (6): 1305–1310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Schreier A, Wolke D, Thomas K, Horwood J, Hollis C, et al. (2009) Prospective study of peer victimization in childhood and psychotic symptoms in a nonclinical population at age 12 years. Arch Gen Psychiat;66 (5): 527–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Cutajar MC, Mullen PE, Ogloff JRP, Thomas SD, Wells DL, et al. (2010) Schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders in a cohort of sexually abused children. Arch Gen Psychiat;67 (11): 1114–1119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Read J, Bentall RP (2012) Negative childhood experiences and mental health: theoretical, clinical and primary prevention implications. Br J Psychiatry;200 (2): 89–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]