Abstract
Individuals diagnosed with major mental disorders such as schizophrenia are more likely to have engaged in violent behavior than mentally healthy members of the same communities. Although aggressive acts can have numerous causes, research about the underlying neurobiology of violence and aggression in schizophrenia can lead to a better understanding of the heterogeneous nature of that behavior and can assist in developing new treatment strategies. The purpose of this paper is to review the recent literature and discuss some of the neurobiological correlates of aggression and violence. The focus will be on schizophrenia, and the results of neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies that have directly investigated brain functioning and/or structure in aggressive and violent samples will be discussed as well as other domains that might predispose to aggression and violence such as deficits in responding to the emotional expressions of others, impulsivity, and psychopathological symptoms. Finally gender differences regarding aggression and violence are discussed. In this context several methodological and conceptional issues that limited the comparison of these studies will be addressed.
1. Introduction
Aggression and violence are declared as leading public health problems and therefore violence in the community has obvious social relevance for political, criminal justice, and health care systems [1]. Epidemiological studies have shown that individuals diagnosed with major mental disorders such as schizophrenia are more likely to be engaged in violent behavior than the general population [2–6]. However, violence committed by persons with schizophrenia is a heterogeneous phenomenon. It is unquestionable that societal influences, such as poverty, discrimination, exposure to violence, and physical abuse, play a key role in the genesis of violence. For example, individuals who are abused as children have increased levels of violence in adulthood [7, 8]. Furthermore, in a recent meta-analysis, Fazel et al. [3] could show that most of the risk for violence appears to be mediated by substance abuse comorbidity. Although aggressive acts can have numerous causes, research about the underlying neurobiology of violence and aggression in schizophrenia can lead to a better understanding of the heterogeneous nature of that behavior and can assist in developing new treatment strategies. In response, a large number of studies have been published to determine the roots of violence and aggression, but the underlying neurobiology is only just beginning to be understood.
The purpose of this paper is to review the recent literature and discuss some of the neurobiological correlates of aggression and violence in the literature. The focus will be on schizophrenia and the results of neuroimaging studies and neuropsychological assessments that have directly investigated brain functioning and/or structure in aggressive and violent schizophrenic samples. Additionally, other domains that might predispose to aggression and violence such as deficits in responding to the emotional expressions of others, impulsivity, and psychopathological symptoms will be discussed. In this context, the relevance of various methodological and conceptual issues on the results of these studies will be emphasized.
2. Evidence from Brain Lesions and Brain Imaging Studies
Over the last years, there has been an increasing body of data on neuropsychiatric disorders that raise the question about a possible relationship between the abnormal function of specific regions of the brain and the occurrence of violent and aggressive behavior. The regions of the brain most often linked to aggression and violence are the temporal cortex/limbic system (amygdale, hippocampus, cingulate gyrus, portions of the thalamus, and hypothalamus and their connections) and the orbitofrontal cortex (for a review, see Volavka [9]).
Since the middle of the nineteenth century, case studies of patients with neurodegenerative disorders or after traumatic brain injury have reported about violent and antisocial behavior, impulsivity, and inability to inhibit responses after damage to the orbitofrontal cortex [10–13]. For example the dramatic case of Phineas Cage, a railroad worker, who had an iron bar driven through the orbito-frontal cortex as a result of an explosion [14]. After the accident he became belligerent, socially inappropriate, unrealistic, and impersistent. The Vietnam Head Injury Study (VHIS) found that subjects with lesions limited to the frontal lobes tended to show about 10% more aggressive and violent behaviors compared with patients with nonfrontal head injury and controls without head injury [15]. Furthermore, persons with frontal network damage acquired before the age of 8 have also been reported to have adult histories of recurrent impulsive, aggressive, and antisocial behavior [16–18]. Moreover, reports have found higher rates of antisocial behavior in patients with frontotemporal dementia, even when compared with equally cognitively impaired patients with Alzheimer's disease [19, 20].
Morphometric neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent subjects have consistently found frontal lobe abnormalities (for reviews, see [21–25]). However, to date the structural MRI literature produced inconsistent results regarding the volume size abnormalities in aggressive and violent patients, with most studies finding reduced volumes of frontal structures in violent patients (see the meta-analysis of 43 structural and functional imaging studies of Yang and Raine [26]), while others reported increased volumes in these brain regions [27–31]. Furthermore abnormalities in cortical thickness in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex [32] and reduced white matter integrity in the ventral prefrontal cortex, measured with diffusion tension imaging (DTI) [33], were associated with aggressive attitudes and violence.
Only a handful of studies examined differences in grey matter volumes between violent and nonviolent schizophrenic patients. Yang et al. [34] found reduced gray matter volumes in the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus in murderers with schizophrenia, in the parahippocampal gyrus in murderers without schizophrenia, and in the prefrontal cortex in nonviolent schizophrenic patients compared to normal controls. Volume reductions in the hippocampus in violent schizophrenic patients were also seen in other studies [35, 36]. Since hippocampal/parahippocampal deficits have been linked to memory impairments and affective dysregulation, Yang et al. [34] hypothesized that volume reductions in the hippocampus may predispose individuals with schizophrenia to be less sensible to social and emotional signs, which might contribute to the generation of conflicts and the inability to recognize signals for solution, leading to conflict escalation. Less consistent patterns of gray matter volume reductions were found in other studies, comparing violent and nonviolent schizophrenic patients. These studies are reporting about reductions in mesial temporal structures [37], in the amygdale [38], in the sensorimotor cortex [35], in orbitofrontal cortex [36], in the cerebellum, and in the region around the supramarginal gyrus [39]. One study [27] found larger left orbitofrontal cortex grey matter volumes in aggressive schizophrenic patients. These inconsistent results in structural brain volumetric studies can be explained by methodological differences between studies including differences in the study samples (inclusion of multiple diagnostic categories such as schizoaffective patients or patients with comorbidity of personality disorders or psychoactive substance misuse) and treatment characteristics.
Studies focusing on the patterns of brain activation using PET, SPECT, and FMRI have documented focal decreases in frontal and temporal cortical activity associated with various neuropsychiatric disorders (for reviews, see [20–25]). They provide data on both resting brain activity and task-related activity.
Using positron emission tomography (PET), Wong et al. [40] reported reduced glucose metabolism in the temporal cortex in violent schizophrenic patients, whereas Raine et al. [41–43] found reduced glucose metabolism in the prefrontal cortex and other regions (superior parietal regions, angular gyrus and corpus callosum) in murderers using the continuous performance tasks to elicit frontal activity. Similarly, in a single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) Spaletta et al. [44] found reduced prefrontal regional cerebral blood flow in aggressive schizophrenic patients during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST). However, one has to keep in mind that the sample size in the few PET and SPECT studies showing an abnormal frontotemporal circuitry in aggressive patients is fairly small and the location of abnormal metabolism varied among patients.
Functional imaging studies (fMRI) on aggression in schizophrenia and other mental disorders produced also inconsistent results, with most studies showing an abnormal activation in the temporoventromedial cortex region, but less consistent patterns in other regions [45–52]. Again, comparison between studies is hampered by methodological differences such as different cognitive activations tasks (e.g., visual verbal working memory tasks, affective words, emotional faces), heterogeneous patient populations, and small sample sizes. Only one study examined the resting state functional connectivity in aggressive schizophrenic patients, showing a significant reduction in functional connectivity between the amygdale and ventral prefrontal cortex [53].
In summary the neuroimaging literature on aggression in schizophrenia and other mental disorders implicates dysfunctions in the frontotemporal circuitry (for reviews, see [21–25, 54]). These findings are consistent with the role of medial temporal and orbitofrontal regions in emotional processing and executive cognitive functioning, which encompasses abilities such as attention, planning, organization, abstract reasoning, self-monitoring, and the ability to use feedback to modulate behavior. This has been speculated to lead to cognitive biases that increase the chances of behaving aggressively in response to stressful and provocative situations [4]. The reported abnormalities in prefrontal size or activity may, therefore, represent a predisposition to affective states relevant to aggressive behavior, without necessarily signifying an incapacity to avoid actual violent acts, and no study has reliably demonstrated a characteristic pattern of frontal network dysfunction predictive of violent crime (for review, see Filley et al. [55]). Although the etiological implications of these prefrontal network dysfunctions are not fully understood, there is converging evidence that cognitive deficits may underlie early school failure, dropouts, alcohol and drug use, and ultimately, encounters with the legal system as violent offenders (for review, see [9, 10]).
3. Evidence from Neuropsychological Assessments
3.1. Deficits in Executive Functions
It is fairly common that the majority of schizophrenic patients have profound deficits on measures of executive functions. At the descriptive level, patients with schizophrenia seem to have difficulties selecting relevant information from their environment and attaching an appropriate meaning to that information. Green [56] provided the following definition of executive function:
“Executive functioning refers to a host of neurocognitive activities that are associated with the prefrontal cortex such as planning, problem solving, shifting cognitive set and alternating between two or more tasks.”
Therefore, executive functions allow you to set goals, make and modify mental “models” of actions, organize your activity, focus your attention selectively, and avoid impulses and distractions that could sidetrack you from accomplishing your aims.
Reviews of the literature tend to support a significant association between prefrontal executive dysfunctions measured by neuropsychological testing and increased antisocial and aggressive behavior [54, 57]. Morgan and Lilienfeld [57] found in their meta-analysis that antisocial groups performed .62 standard deviations worse on executive tests than comparison groups, but the strength of the correlation between deficits in executive functions and antisocial behavior varied according to the types of antisocial behaviors exhibited by study subjects, with criminality and delinquency showing a stronger relationship than conduct disorder, psychopathy, or clinically defined antisocial personality disorder.
Only a few studies have examined the relationship between performance on neuropsychological tests and violence among schizophrenic patients (see Table 1). However, the results of these studies have been inconsistent. Several studies [58–62] did not find a difference in neuropsychological test performance between schizophrenic patients with aggressive behavior and those without. In contrast, three studies [63–65] found superior performance on neuropsychological measures among violent schizophrenic patients compared to nonviolent patients with schizophrenia. Roy et al. [63] found, in a sample of 20 inpatients with chronic schizophrenia, that violent patients (defined on the basis of chart review and ward behavior) outperformed nonviolent patients on several subscales of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised (WAIS-R): verbal IQ, digit symbol, and block design. A study by Lapierre et al. [64] found a correlation between enhanced performance on both the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and a verbal fluency test and lifetime “number of aggression against another person” among 31 outpatients with schizophrenia. The low-violence group did not achieve significantly better scores than the violent group on any neuropsychological measure in either study. Rasmussen et al. [65] compared 13 violent schizophrenic inpatients with 13 nonaggressive schizophrenic patients and 13 healthy control subjects. Violent schizophrenic patients outperformed nonviolent schizophrenic patients on the Trail Making Test and showed faster reaction time on all reaction time tests but more failed inhibitions on the Go-NoGo test.
Table 1.
Authors | Sample characteristics | Results of relevant neuropsychological studies |
---|---|---|
Lafayette et al. [58] | 96 schizophrenic patients (34 violent arrests, 23 nonviolent arrest, 39 no arrest) | No significant differences between the groups in the WAIS, Wisconsin Card Sortig Test, Trail Making Test, Verbal fluency Test, Stroop Test, American National Adult Reading Test |
| ||
Krakowski et al. [59] | 32 transiently violent schizophrenic patients, 27 persistently violent schizophrenic patients | No difference in the average performance and verbal IQ on the WAIS-R |
| ||
Wong et al. [60] | 20 male schizophrenic patients who had committed several violent offenses, 19 male schizophrenic patients who had committed only 1 violent offense. | No difference in the WAIS performance between the groups |
| ||
Silver et al. [61] | 35 violent patients with chronic schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, 35 nonviolent schizophrenic patients, 46 healthy controls | No difference between violent and nonviolent schizophrenic patients in the cognitive test battery |
| ||
Fullam and Dolan [62] | 33 violent and 49 nonviolent male forensic inpatients with schizophrenia | No difference between violent and nonviolent schizophrenic patients in the cognitive test battery but lower IQ scores in the violent schizophrenic patients |
| ||
Roy et al. [63] | 20 chronic, treatment-resistant inpatients with schizophrenia; (11 violent and 9 low-violent men) | Violent patients outperformed nonviolent patients on the verbal IQ, digit symbol test, block design |
| ||
Lapierre et al. [64] | 31 schizophrenic men (outpatients) and 30 healthy control subjects | Higher level of violence in the more cognitively functional individuals on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and Controlled Oral Word Association test |
| ||
Rasmussen [65] | 13 inpatients aggressive schizophrenic patients from a maximum security psychiatric unit, 13 nonaggressive schizophrenic patients, 13 healthy controls | Violent schizophrenic patients outperformed nonviolent schizophrenic patients on the Trail Making Test and showed faster reaction time on all reaction time tests but more failed inhibitions on the Go-NoGo test |
| ||
Krakowski et al. [66] | 28 high, 27 low, and 34 nonviolent schizophrenic patients | High-violent patients were more impaired on the Benton Visual Retention Test, digit symbol test, block design and had lower performance IQ scores on the WAIS-R |
| ||
Adams et al. [67] | 37 male schizophrenic patients | History of violent arrests but not inpatients violence was associated with impairment on the Luria Nebraska neuropsychological battery |
| ||
Barkataki et al. [68] | 13 individuals with a history of serious violence and a diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder, 13 individuals with a history of violence and schizophrenia, 15 individuals with schizophrenia without a history of violent behaviour, 15 healthy control subjects | Violent schizophrenic patients had a higher number of errors in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test |
| ||
Foster et al. [69] | 1 year prospective study of aggression in 23 male forensic psychiatric inpatients | Scores on Stroop Color Word Tasks, Judgment of Line Orientation Test, Symbol Digit Modalities Test only significantly predicted frequency but not severity |
| ||
Krakowski et al. [70] | 33 psychiatric violent inpatients versus 69 nonviolent psychiatric inpatients | History of community violence was related to impairment on some Wisconsin Card Sorting Subtests, finger tapping, Perdue pegboard |
WAIS-R: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-revised.
Other authors showed that neuropsychological impairments were associated with violence among inpatients with schizophrenia [66–68]. Krakowski et al. [66] classified schizophrenia inpatients into high (N = 22), low (N = 27), or no violence (N = 22) groups on the basis of ward behavior. Patients in the high-violence group were significantly more impaired in the area of integrative sensory and motor function than those in the nonviolent group, as measured by their scores on the Benton Visual Retention Test and the WAIS-R performance IQ, especially on the subtests: digit symbol test and blocking test. On these subtests, violent patients received higher scores in the study by Roy et al. [63]. Adams et al. [67] found that impairment on the Lubria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery was related to history of violent arrest but not to inpatient violence in a sample of 37 incarcerated persons with schizophrenia. Barkataki et al. [68] could show that schizophrenic patients with a history of violence produced poorer performance in an executive task (WCST) than nonviolent schizophrenic patients, with the violent patients making significantly more perseverative errors.
Of the remaining studies none dealt exclusively with schizophrenia. However, in two of the studies a majority of the participants had a diagnosis of schizophrenia. One study compared 23 forensic inpatients, 16 of whom had a diagnosis of schizophrenia, who had committed a violent crime [69]. Impairment on several neuropsychological tests, the judgment of line orientation test, the symbol digit modalities test, the Stroop Interference Test, and the test of nonverbal intelligence, was correlated with the frequency and severity of violent behavior. In a larger study, Krakowski et al. [70] compared 33 inpatients with a history of community violence, as determined by self-report of arrest for violent crime and chart reviews for such arrests, with 69 inpatients who denied arrests for violent crimes. Of the 102 patients included in the study, 72 percent had a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. A history of community violence was significantly related to impairment on some WCST subtests as well as impairment in the finger-tapping test and the Perdue pegboard test (both left handed). Finally, Nestor [71] examined the neuropsychological and clinical correlates of extreme violence retrospectively in young and older inpatients of a forensic psychiatric hospital. The young group exhibited significantly higher rates of learning disabilities and history of childhood conduct disorders, whereas the older group had a significantly higher rate of psychosis.
Three studies [72–74] aimed to determine models for explaining aggressive behavior in relation to executive functions, psychopathological symptoms, and behavioral variables such as anger. In a study of Song and Min [72] the structural equation model revealed a direct, significant path of the emotion anger to aggressive behavior, whereas schizophrenic symptoms and cognitive functions were indirectly related to aggressive behavior through the relationship between the emotion of anger and aggressive behavior, suggesting that executive dysfunction may affect the stimulant of anger by weakening impulse control. Two other studies [73, 74] could show that executive dysfunction predicted aggressive and violent behavior and psychiatric symptomatology. Serper et al. [73] concluded that patients with executive dysfunctions may not possess the behavioral inhibition skills needed to cope with the presence of symptoms and other stressful events that accompany acute psychosis and hospitalization which may result, consequently, in increased manifestations of aggressive behavior. Finally, a recent study Krakowski and Czobor [75] could show that in schizophrenic patients executive function was a strong predictor of response to atypical antipsychotic medication, with clozapine exerting an antiaggressive effect even in the presence of executive dysfunction.
To summarize, a large number of studies have found that dysfunctions of the frontal cortex are associated with limitations in the executive capacity to regulate aggressive behavior (for reviews, see [4, 10, 57]). Therefore, neuropsychological deficits can reduce the number of options an individual perceives or has availablity to respond to a given situation. As a component of the human flight and fight response, aggression may serve as an adaptive mechanism. When cognitive dysfunction is present, however, aggression may dominate the behavioral response and override avoidance or withdrawal. Deficits including defective inhibitory control, impulsivity, difficulty in drawing on past learning to recognize a dangerous course of action, impaired capacity to anticipate future consequences of present behavior, and insufficient self-monitoring, may all conspire to conceal more socially desirable courses of action, which may result, consequently, in increased manifestations of aggressive behavior. Although social and environmental factors have major effects on the expression of aggression and violence, neuropsychological integrity also helps to determine the ability to behave in a social acceptable manner. However, studies investigating the relationship between violence in schizophrenic patients and neuropsychological functioning produced mixed results, with reports of better, similar, and worse performance in violent schizophrenic patients. One reason for these inconsistencies in study results may be that the standard tests of executive functions may not detect orbitofrontal or ventromedial prefrontal dysfunction relevant to aggression and criminal behavior. Furthermore, there is a substantial variation in the neuropsychological test batteries used in different studies.
3.2. Deficits in Responding to the Emotional Expressions of Others
The expression of emotions and the ability to recognize facial expressions of emotions in other people is an important component of interpersonal communication in humans [75]. Certain facial expressions like happiness, sadness, anger, fear, disgust, and surprise are universally recognized, whereas social emotions such as guilt, shame, arrogance, admiration, and flirtatiousness are particular to cultural and ethnical groups [76, 77]. Emotion is normally regulated in the human brain by a complex circuit consisting of the orbital frontal cortex, amygdala, hypothalamus, anterior cingulate cortex, and several other interconnected regions [78]. Impairments in emotion regulation, emotional inexpressiveness, and other emotion-related deficits are regarded as core features of schizophrenia (for a review, see Borod [78]). Abnormal expression of emotional states usually consists of flattening of affects and inappropriate affect and may precede the onset of psychosis by many years [79]. Abnormal experience of emotion ranges from depression to less frequently manic symptoms. Abnormal recognition is described as impaired ability to recognize facial expression of emotion.
Psychological research underscores especially the relation between deficits in regulation of negative emotions and aggressive and violent behavior [80]. Accurate interpretations of non-verbal cues such as facial expressions are important for normal social interaction [81] and deficient abilities to be appropriately guided by the social cues of others and identify one's emotional state may lead to aggressive and violent behavior [82–85]. Flattening of affect as it is seen in schizophrenia may further contribute to aggressive behavior insofar as a failure to express emotions can lead to a reliance on maladaptive ways of expressing emotions, such as through verbal and physical aggression [86].
Violent offenders show deficits in recognizing negative facial expressions of emotions [87, 88], a deficit associated with many other forms of psychopathology such as autism spectrum disorders, schizophrenia, and obsessive compulsive disorder. More specifically, previous studies could show that aggressive individuals show a negative emotional bias for ambiguous facial expressions [89]. Violent schizophrenic patients were more likely to perceive anger in emotionally neutral faces [90] and showed a poorer ability to discriminate between intensity of emotions [61]. Dodge and McNiel et al. [91, 92] predicted that a cognitive style characterized by hostile attributions increases the risk of violence because in ambiguous situations aggressive individuals who attend to fewer emotional cues are biased towards aggressive cues and display deficits in what is described as affective perspective taking. Furthermore, some authors suggest that mentalizing, which is the ability to attribute mental states to oneself and others, can be viewed as an inhibitor of violent and aggressive behavior [93]. Two studies [94, 95] could show that violent schizophrenic patients have deficient emotional but intact cognitive mentalizing abilities.
Another line of research is investigating the relationship between personality traits and aggression. The relationship between antisocial behavior or psychopathy and violence is well established (for a review, see Walsh et al. [96]), and a meta-analysis could show functional impairments in orbitofrontal, dorsolateral frontal, and anterior cingulate cortex in antisocial behavior [97]. Among schizophrenic patients antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy are associated with early onset and persistent violent offending [98, 99].
Bettencourt et al. [100] conducted a comprehensive review to understand the relationship between personality and aggressive behavior, under provoking and nonprovoking conditions. The authors could show that trait aggressiveness and trait irritability influenced aggressive behavior under both provoking and neutral conditions but that other personality variables (e.g., trait anger, type A personality, dissipation rumination) influenced aggressive behavior only under provoking conditions. Since aggression is closely linked to hypervigilance towards stimuli that could be perceived as threatening [101, 102] one other important symptom, possibly related to outbreaks of aggression and violence, particularly in adolescents and young adults, may be “gelotophobia” (from gelos, Greek for laughter), which is the fear to being laughed at [103, 104]. Similar to violent individuals persons with higher levels of gelotophobia have deficits in downregulating their negative affect, show a greater anger proneness, and have a tendency to recall interpersonal situations with a higher intensity of negative feelings [105–107]. The suspicion that gelotophobia may be related to aggression and violence had recently been fuelled by anecdotal evidence suggesting that perpetrators of violent acts such as school shootings had a horror of being mocked and may have taken revenge for having been laughed at [108, 109]. However, more research is needed to extrapolate this relationship between gelotophobia or other personality factors and aggressive behavior to schizophrenic patients.
4. Association between Impulsivity and Violence
A further factor mediating the brain-violence relationship which is also linked to the frontal lobe is impulsivity, a complex, multifaceted construct including cognitive, personality, and behavioral components (e.g., sensation seeking, risk taking, and self control) [110]. In general aggression can be distinguished into a goal-directed predatory/premeditated aggression and a reactive/impulsive aggression [111]. Premeditated aggression occurs without provocation and is defined as a planned, conscious, and controlled aggressive act that is instrumental in nature. Impulsive aggression is more reactive and typically described as an emotionally charged aggressive response either unprovoked or out of proportion to the provocation leading to agitation and a loss of behavioral control. Since impulsive individuals have difficulties inhibiting their urges this behavioral style may result in risky behavior, including drug and alcohol abuse [112]. Alcoholintoxication for example, reduces behavioral inhibition and has been linked with aggression in a number of studies [113–116]. Prior research has shown that poor impulse control is associated with aggression and violence in men but not women [117–120]. Dysfunctions of the ventromedial prefrontal region (VMPFC), including the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and medial prefrontal cortex, as well as the amygdala were associated with impulsive-aggressive behavior [33, 121–125], whereas individuals with predominantly premeditated aggression showed no prefrontal deficts [125]. Additionally, neuropsychological studies have shown a clear link between impulsive-aggressive behavior and problems in executive functioning [e.g., [126–128]]. Again, some studies could show that individuals who present with marked premediated aggression such as psychopaths do not present with poor performance on general measures of frontal-lobe functioning [129, 130]. Accumulating evidence points towards an important role of several neurotransmitters such as serotonin, dopamine, and noradrenalin in impulsive behavior, and recent findings also implicate glutamate and cannabinoid neurotransmission in impulsivity (for a review, see Pattij and Vanderschuren [131]). Therefore, impulsive behavior may be responsive to pharmacological treatment, and several studies could show that agents with serotonergic-enhancing properties such as serotonin-reuptake inhibitors [132–134] as well as antiepileptic dugs may reduce incidences of impulsive aggression, but not premediated aggression (for reviews, see Huband et al. [135] and Stanford et al. [136]).
4.1. Association between Violence and Psychopathological Symptoms
To some extent the increased risk of violence in patients with schizophrenia may be related to specific psychotic symptoms, such as delusions of thought insertion, thought control, and persecution or to command hallucinations, but the link between violence and psychotic symptoms can be minimal.
Link and Stueve [137] reported that violent behavior was associated with the presence of “threat/control-override symptoms”: delusions of domination by external force, thought insertion, and feeling that people wish to harm the patient. However, epidemiological study did not investigate whether there was a temporal linkage between delusions and violence. Junginger et al. [138] concluded that delusional motivation of violence appeared to be rare, though 40% of the violent subjects in that study reported that they had committed at least one serious act of violence that was definitely motivated by delusions. Taylor [139] interviewed 121 incarcerated forensic psychotic (mostly schizophrenic) patients about the motives for the offenses they had committed while living in the community. The author estimated that 82% of their offenses (violent and nonviolent offenses combined) were probably attributable to their illness and that psychotic symptoms probably accounted directly for most of the very violent behavior, but considerable time elapsed between the offenses and the interviews, and the respondent may have had reason to represent their criminal behavior as driven by mental illness.
Some patients may commit violent acts in response to auditory command hallucinations. In a sample of 93 psychiatric inpatients, Junginger [140] rated the level of dangerousness of the most recently reported command hallucinations and the degree to which the patient complied with the command. He concluded that psychiatric patients who experience command hallucinations are at risk for dangerous behavior. However, others have concluded that although experienced by many patients with schizophrenia and often violent in content, command hallucinations may not be important predictors of violence [141, 142]. In a comparison of violent and nonviolent patients with schizophrenia (n = 31 per group) Cheung et al. [143] reported no association between command hallucinations and violence; yet, violent patients were more likely to have had persecutory delusions and were more severely psychotic than the nonviolent patients.
In summary, there is still a controversy regarding the relationship between psychotic symptoms in schizophrenic patients and aggressive behavior with some authors reporting that positive psychotic symptoms such as delusions of thought insertion, thought control and persecution, or command hallucinations were associated with a history of assaults [144, 145], whereas others could not observe association between positive symptoms of schizophrenia and violence [62].
4.2. Gender and Aggression
In healthy people, aggression is arguably the most salient emotional behavior differentiating the sexes with men showing greater abundance [146], including arrests for homicide and violent crimes [147]. Studies of aggression in psychiatric disorders showed variable findings (for a review, see Maccoby and Jacklin [146]). Several studies have suggested that psychiatric disorders, especially schizophrenia, reduce the gender difference and in hospitalized psychiatric patients eliminate it altogether [148–151]. However, in order to understand better gender differences in violence, multiple manifestations of violence must be differentiated and the complexity of the phenomenon must be taken into consideration. For example, Hiday et al. [152] demonstrated that male subjects had a greater prevalence of more serious violence involving weapons or injury in the 4 months preceding hospitalization, but there were no gender differences when measures of violence that were more inclusive (incorporating threats as well as fights not involving weapons or injuries) were used. Additionally, Weiss et al. [153] could show that physical aggression and antisocial behavior in the community were more pronounced in schizophrenic men compared to schizophrenic women. In a study by Krakowski and Czobor [154], women with major psychiatric disorders were more verbally abusive and threatening during their hospitalization than the men. This finding is consistent with the predominance of verbal aggression reported in women showing that in the general population, women throughout life are more likely to use verbal than physical aggression [155]. A meta-analytic review of studies on gender differences in the general population [156] also indicated that the male overrepresentation in aggression is more distinct for severe physical aggression.
5. Conclusions
Violent and criminal behavior in schizophrenic patients is caused by multiple, probably interacting causal factors. Dysfunctions in the frontotemporal circuitry appear to be the most consistent feature in aggressive and violent schizophrenic patients. Limitations in the executive capacity and/or deficits in the interpretation of emotional signals such as facial affects can lead to cognitive biases that increase the chances of behaving aggressively in response to stressful and provocative situations. However, studies elucidating the relationship between neuroimaging results, neurocognitive functioning, personality traits, and antisocial and violent behavior produced inconsistent results. Most studies investigating the neurobiological correlates of aggressive and violent behavior are limited by several methodological problems. Notably, the definition and measurement of violence varied between studies. Most studies focused on community violence, while only few studies examined inpatient violence, which is not necessarily predicted by previous community violence [157]. Additionally, the number of aggressive behavior and violent incidents was assessed differently between the studies reviewed in this paper, with some authors using patients self-reports, while others used psychiatric files, reports by ward staff, or official records of arrests and convictions. Furthermore, inconsistent findings can result from sample heterogeneities such as comorbidity with antisocial personality disorder/psychopathy or substance abuse. Over and above, differences in the medication type and dosage can add to the variance in study results. Atypical antipsychotic medication has shown greater promise in treating neuropsychological deficits than conventional antipsychotic drugs [158]. In addition, atypical antipsychotic medication, particularly clozapine, has been shown to reduce aggressive and violent behavior in schizophrenic patients [75, 159–162]. The failure to take account of type and dosage of antipsychotic medication in some studies may have limited the detection of group differences in neurocognitive performance and aggressive behavior. Further methodological problems in the literature include a lack of prospective data, small samples sizes, nonrepresentative samples for the criminal population, a lack of adequate controls for known violent risk factors such as drug and alcohol misuse, physical and sexual abuse, family break down, and poverty. Future studies are necessary that examine the course of cognitive deficits, psychopathological symptoms, and aggressive behavior in schizophrenic patients at various stages of patient illness. Since several studies found an association between some genes such as the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) and aggression and violence (for review, see Soyka [54]) the combination of neuroimaging, neuropsychological, and genetic studies might broaden our understanding of the neurobiological basis that underlies aggressive and violent behavior. This interdisciplinary research may lead to an earlier identification of likely aggressors and may result in the formation of individualized treatment plans, which can reduce patients' symptoms and the propensity for criminal behavior and aggression.
References
- 1.Hodgins S, Alderton J, Cree A, Aboud A, Mak T. Aggressive behaviour, victimisation and crime among severely mentally ill patients requiring hospitalisation. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;191:343–350. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.06.029587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Swanson JW, Swartz MS, Van Dorn RA, et al. A national study of violent behavior in persons with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63(5):490–499. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fazel S, Gulati G, Linsell L, Geddes JR, Grann M. Schizophrenia and violence: systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Medicine. 2009;6(8) doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000120.e1000120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Naudts K, Hodgins S. Neurobiological correlates of violent behavior among persons with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2006;32(3):562–572. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbj036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Stueve A, Link BG. Violence and psychiatric disorders: results from an epidemiological study of young adults in Israel. Psychiatric Quarterly. 1997;68(4):327–342. doi: 10.1023/a:1025443014158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Link BG, Stueve A. New evidence on the violence risk posed by people with mental illness: on the importance of specifying the timing and the targets of violence. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1998;55(5):403–404. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.5.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Widom CS. The cycle of violence. Science. 1989;244(4901):160–166. doi: 10.1126/science.2704995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Maxfield MG, Widom CS. The cycle of violence: revisited 6 years later. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 1996;150(4):390–395. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1996.02170290056009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Volavka J. Neurobiology of Violence. 2nd edition. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Brower MC, Price BH. Neuropsychiatry of frontal lobe dysfunction in violent and criminal behaviour: a critical review. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 2001;71(6):720–726. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.71.6.720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tranel D. ‘Acquired sociopathy’: the development of sociopathic behavior following focal brain damage. Progress in Experimental Personality & Psychopathology Research. 1994:285–311. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Meyers CA, Berman SA, Scheibel RS, Hayman A. Case report: acquired antisocial personality disorder associated with unilateral left orbital frontal lobe damage. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience. 1992;17(3):121–125. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Blair RJR, Cipolotti L. Impaired social response reversal. A case of ‘acquired sociopathy’. Brain. 2000;123(6):1122–1141. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.6.1122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Harlow JM. Passage of an iron rod through the head. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1999;11(2):281–283. doi: 10.1176/jnp.11.2.281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Grafman J, Schwab K, Warden D, Pridgen A, Brown HR, Salazar AM. Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: a report of the Vietnam head injury study. Neurology. 1996;46(5):1231–1238. doi: 10.1212/wnl.46.5.1231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Price BH, Daffner KR, Stowe RM, Mesulam MM. The comportmental learning disabilities of early frontal lobe damage. Brain. 1990;113(5):1383–1393. doi: 10.1093/brain/113.5.1383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Eslinger PJ, Grattan LM, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Developmental consequences of childhood frontal lobe damage. Archives of Neurology. 1992;49(7):764–769. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530310112021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Anderson SW, Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2(11):1032–1037. doi: 10.1038/14833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stip E. Compulsive disorder and acquired antisocial behavior in frontal lobe dementia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1995;7(1):p. 116. doi: 10.1176/jnp.7.1.116b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Miller BL, Darby A, Benson DF, Cummings JL, Miller MH. Aggressive, socially disruptive and antisocial behaviour associated with fronto-temporal dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;170:150–155. doi: 10.1192/bjp.170.2.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Brower MC, Price BH. Neuropsychiatry of frontal lobe dysfunction in violent and criminal behaviour: a critical review. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 2001;71(6):720–726. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.71.6.720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bassarath L. Neuroimaging studies of antisocial behaviour. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;46(8):728–732. doi: 10.1177/070674370104600805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bufkin JL, Luttrell VR. Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent behavior: current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice. Trauma, Violence & Abuse. 2005;6(2):176–191. doi: 10.1177/1524838005275089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Palijan TZ, Radeljak S, Kovač M, Kovačević D. Relationship between comorbidity and violence risk assesment in forensic psychiatry—the implicaton of neuroimaging studies. Psychiatria Danubina. 2010;22(2):253–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hoptman MJ, Antonius D. Neuroimaging correlates of aggression in schizophrenia: an update. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2011;24(2):100–106. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328342c8e0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Yang Y, Raine A. Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research. 2009;174(2):81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hoptman MJ, Volavka J, Weiss EM, et al. Quantitative MRI measures of orbitofrontal cortex in patients with chronic schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Psychiatry Research. 2005;140(2):133–145. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rüsch N, Spoletini I, Wilke M, et al. Inferior frontal white matter volume and suicidality in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. 2008;164(3):206–214. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Glenn AL, Raine A, Yaralian PS, Yang Y. Increased volume of the striatum in psychopathic individuals. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67(1):52–58. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.De Brito SA, Mechelli A, Wilke M, et al. Size matters: increased grey matter in boys with conduct problems and callousunemotional traits. Brain. 2009;132(4):843–852. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tiihonen J, Rossi R, Laakso MP, et al. Brain anatomy of persistent violent offenders: more rather than less. Psychiatry Research. 2008;163(3):201–212. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Narayan VM, Narr KL, Kumari V, et al. Regional cortical thinning in subjects with violent antisocial personality disorder or schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164(9):1418–1427. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06101631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hoptman MJ, Volavka J, Johnson G, Weiss E, Bilder RM, Lim KO. Frontal white matter microstructure, aggression, and impulsivity in men with schizophrenia: a preliminary study. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52(1):9–14. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01311-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Yang Y, Raine A, Han CB, Schug RA, Toga AW, Narr KL. Reduced hippocampal and parahippocampal volumes in murderers with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research—Neuroimaging. 2010;182(1):9–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Barkataki I, Kumari V, Das M, Taylor P, Sharma T. Volumetric structural brain abnormalities in men with schizophrenia or antisocial personality disorder. Behavioural Brain Research. 2006;169(2):239–247. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kumari V, Barkataki I, Goswami S, Flora S, Das M, Taylor P. Dysfunctional, but not functional, impulsivity is associated with a history of seriously violent behaviour and reduced orbitofrontal and hippocampal volumes in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. 2009;173(1):39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Chesterman LP, Taylor PJ, Cox T, Hill M, Lumsden J. Multiple measures of cerebral state in dangerous mentally disordered inpatients. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health. 1994;4(3):228–239. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wong M, Fenwick P, Fenton G, Lumsden J, Maisey M, Stevens J. Repetitive and non-repetitive violent offending behaviour in male patients in a maximum security mental hospital—clinical and neuroimaging findings. Medicine, Science and the Law. 1997;37(2):150–160. doi: 10.1177/002580249703700211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Puri BK, Counsell SJ, Saeed N, Bustos MG, Treasaden IH, Bydder GM. Regional grey matter volumetric changes in forensic schizophrenia patients: an MRI study comparing the brain structure of patients who have seriously and violently offended with that of patients who have not. BMC Psychiatry. 2008;8(supplement 1, article S6) doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-S1-S6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Wong MTH, Fenwick PBC, Lumsden J, et al. Positron emission tomography in male violent offenders with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. 1997;68(2-3):111–123. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(96)02621-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Raine A, Buchsbaum MS, Stanley J, Lottenberg S, Abel L, Stoddard J. Selective reductions in prefrontal glucose metabolism in murderers. Biological Psychiatry. 1994;36(6):365–373. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)91211-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Raine A, Buchsbaum M, LaCasse L. Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by positron emission tomography. Biological Psychiatry. 1997;42(6):495–508. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00362-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Raine A, Meloy JR, Bihrle S, Stoddard J, LaCasse L, Buchsbaum MS. Reduced prefrontal and increased subcortical brain functioning assessed using positron emission tomography in predatory and affective murderers. Sciences & the Law. 1998;16:319–332. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0798(199822)16:3<319::aid-bsl311>3.0.co;2-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Spalletta G, Troisi A, Alimenti S, et al. Reduced prefrontal cognitive activation associated with aggression in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2001;50(1-2):134–135. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00164-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Dolan MC, Fullam RS. Psychopathy and functional magnetic resonance imaging blood oxygenation level-dependent responses to emotional faces in violent patients with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66(6):570–577. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Joyal CC, Putkonen A, Mancini-Marïe A, et al. Violent persons with schizophrenia and comorbid disorders: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Schizophrenia Research. 2007;91(1–3):97–102. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kumari V, Das M, Taylor PJ, et al. Neural and behavioural responses to threat in men with a history of serious violence and schizophrenia or antisocial personality disorder. Schizophrenia Research. 2009;110(1–3):47–58. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kiehl KA, Smith AM, Hare RD, et al. Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;50(9):677–684. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01222-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Raine A, Park S, Lencz T, et al. Reduced right hemisphere activation in severely abused violent offenders during a working memory task: an fMRI study. Aggressive Behavior. 2001;27(2):111–129. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Veit R, Flor H, Erb M, et al. Brain circuits involved in emotional learning in antisocial behavior and social phobia in humans. Neuroscience Letters. 2002;328(3):233–236. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)00519-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Jones AP, Laurens KR, Herba CM, Barker GJ, Viding E. Amygdala hypoactivity to fearful faces in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166(1):95–102. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07071050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Marsh AA, Finger EC, Mitchell DGV, et al. Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165(6):712–720. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hoptman MJ, D’Angelo D, Catalano D, et al. Amygdalofrontal functional disconnectivity and aggression in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2010;36(5):1020–1028. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Soyka M. Neurobiology of aggression and violence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2011;37(5):913–920. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Filley CM, Price BH, Nell V, et al. Toward an understanding of violence: neurobehavioral aspects of unwarranted physical aggression: aspen neurobehavioral conference consensus statement. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology. 2001;14(1):1–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Green MF. Schizophrenia from a Neurocognitive Perspective: Probig the Impenetrable Darkness. Boston, Mass, USA: Allyn and Bacon; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Morgan AB, Lilienfeld SO. A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review. 2000;20(1):113–136. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00096-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Lafayette JM, Frankle WG, Pollock A, Dyer K, Goff DC. Clinical characteristics, cognitive functioning, and criminal histories of outpatients with Schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services. 2003;54(12):1635–1640. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.54.12.1635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Krakowski M, Czobor P, Libiger J, et al. Violence in schizophrenic patients: the role of positive psychotic symptoms and frontal lobe impairment. American Journal of Forensic Psychiatry. 1997;18(1):39–50. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wong TH, Lumsden J, Fenton GW, Fenwick PB. Neuroimaging in mentally abnormal offenders. 1997Criminological and Legal Psychology. 27:49–58. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Silver H, Goodman C, Knoll G, Isakov V, Modai I. Schizophrenia patients with a history of severe violence differ from nonviolent schizophrenia patients in perception of emotions but not cognitive function. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2005;66(3):300–308. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v66n0305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Fullam RS, Dolan MC. Executive function and in-patient violence in forensic patients with schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;193(3):247–253. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.040345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Roy S, Herrera J, Parent M, Costa J. Violent and nonviolent schizophrenic patients clinical and developmental characteristics. Psychological Reports. 1987;61:855–861. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1987.61.3.855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Lapierre D, Braun CMJ, Hodgins S, Toupin J, Leveillee S, Constantineau C. Neuropsychological correlates of violence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1995;21(2):253–262. doi: 10.1093/schbul/21.2.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Rasmussen K, Levander S, Sletvold H. Aggressive and non-aggressive schizophrenics: symptom profile and neuropsychological differences. Psychology, Crime & Law. 1995;2:119–129. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Krakowski MI, Convit A, Jaeger J, Lin S, Volavka J. Neurological impairment in violent schizophrenic inpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1989;146(7):849–853. doi: 10.1176/ajp.146.7.849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Adams JJ, Meloy JR, Moritz MS. Neuropsychological deficits and violent behavior in incarcerated schizophrenics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1990;178(4):253–256. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199004000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Barkataki I, Kumari V, Das M, et al. A neuropsychological investigation into violence and mental illness. Schizophrenia Research. 2005;74(1):1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Foster HG, Hillbrand M, Silverstein M. Neuropsychological deficit and aggressive behavior: a prospective study. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 1993;17(6):939–946. doi: 10.1016/0278-5846(93)90021-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Krakowski M, Czobor P, Carpenter MD, et al. Community violence and inpatient assaults: neurobiological deficits. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1997;9(4):549–555. doi: 10.1176/jnp.9.4.549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Nestor PG. Neuropsychological and clinical correlates of murder and other forms of extreme violence in a forensic psychiatric population. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1992;180(7):418–423. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199207000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Song H, Min SK. Aggressive behavior model in schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Research. 2009;167(1-2):58–65. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Serper M, Beech DR, Harvey PD, Dill C. Neuropsychological and symptom predictors of aggression on the psychiatric inpatient service. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2008;30(6):700–709. doi: 10.1080/13803390701684554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Hancock M, Tapscott JL, Hoaken PNS. Role of executive dysfunction in predicting frequency and severity of violence. Aggressive Behavior. 2010;36(5):338–349. doi: 10.1002/ab.20353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Krakowski MI, Czobor P. Executive function predicts response to antiaggression treatment in schizophrenia: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2012;73(1):74–80. doi: 10.4088/JCP.11m07238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Ekman P, Friesen WV. Unmasking the Face. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA: Prentice-Hall; 1975. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Kohler CG, Turner TH, Gur RE, Gur RC. Recognition of facial emotions in neuropsychiatric disorders. CNS Spectrums. 2004;9(4):267–274. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900009202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Borod JC. The Neuropsychology of Emotion. Oxford, UK: University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 79.Walker EF, Grimes KE, Davis DM, Smith AJ. Childhood precursors of schizophrenia: facial expressions of emotion. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;150(11):1654–1660. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.11.1654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Davidson RJ, Putnam KM, Larson CL. Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation—a possible prelude to violence. Science. 2000;289(5479):591–594. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5479.591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Corden B, Critchley HD, Skuse D, Dolan RJ. Fear recognition ability predicts differences in social cognitive and neural functioning in men. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18(6):889–897. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Blair RJR. Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;182:5–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Montagne B, van Honk J, Kessels RPC, et al. Reduced efficiency in recognising fear in subjects scoring high on psychopathic personality characteristics. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;38(1):5–11. [Google Scholar]
- 84.Walker DW, Leister C. Recognition of facial affect cues by adolescents with emotional and behavioral disorders. Behavioral Disorders. 1994;19:269–276. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Grabe HJ, Spitzer C, Freyberger HJ. Alexithymia and personality in relation to dimensions of psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161(7):1299–1301. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Jakupcak M. Masculine gender role stress and Men’s fear of emotions as predictors of self-reported aggression and violence. Violence and Victims. 2003;18(5):533–541. doi: 10.1891/vivi.2003.18.5.533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Robinson L, Spencer MD, Thomson LD, et al. Facial emotion recognition in Scottish prisoners. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 2012;35(1):57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2011.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Hoaken PNS, Allaby DB, Earle J. Executive cognitive functioning and the recognition of facial expressions of emotion in incarcerated violent offenders, non-violent offenders, and controls. Aggressive Behavior. 2007;33(5):412–421. doi: 10.1002/ab.20194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Walz NC, Benson BA. Labeling and discrimination of facial expressions by aggressive and nonaggressive men with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1996;101(3):282–291. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Weiss EM, Kohler CG, Nolan KA, et al. The relationship between history of violent and criminal behavior and recognition of facial expression of emotions in men with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Aggressive Behavior. 2006;32(3):187–194. [Google Scholar]
- 91.Dodge KA. Social-cognitive mechanisms in the development of conduct disorder and depression. Annual Review of Psychology. 1993;44(1):559–584. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.003015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.McNiel DE, Eisner JP, Binder RL. The relationship between aggressive attributional style and violence by psychiatric patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71(2):399–403. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.71.2.399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Fullam R, Dolan M. Emotional information processing in violent patients with schizophrenia: association with psychopathy and symptomatology. Psychiatry Research. 2006;141(1):29–37. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Abu-Akel A, Abushua'leh K. ‘Theory of mind’ in violent and nonviolent patients with paranoid schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;69(1):45–53. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00049-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Majorek K, Wolfkühler W, Küper C, Saimeh N, Juckel G, Brüne M. ‘Theory of mind’ and executive functioning in forensic patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Forensic Sciences. 2009;54(2):469–473. doi: 10.1111/j.1556-4029.2008.00966.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Walsh Z, Swogger MT, Kosson DS. Psychopathy and instrumental violence: facet level relationships. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2009;23(4):416–424. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2009.23.4.416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Yang Y, Raine A. Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research. 2009;174(2):81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Laajasalo T, Salenius S, Lindberg N, Repo-Tiihonen E, Häkkänen-Nyholm H. Psychopathic traits in Finnish homicide offenders with schizophrenia. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 2011;34(5):324–330. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2011.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Moran P, Hodgins S. The correlates of comorbid antisocial personality disorder in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2004;30(4):791–802. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Bettencourt BA, Talley A, Benjamin AJ, Valentine J. Personality and aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132(5):751–777. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Dodge KA. The structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression. In: Pepler DJ, Rubin KH, editors. The Development and Treatment of Childhood Aggression. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1991. pp. 201–218. [Google Scholar]
- 102.Berkowitz L. Aggression: A Social Psychological Analysis. New York, NY, USA: McGraw-Hill; 1962. [Google Scholar]
- 103.Ruch W, Proyer RT. The fear of being laughed at: individual and group differences in Gelotophobia. Humor. 2008;21(1):47–67. [Google Scholar]
- 104.Titze M. Gelotophobia: the fear of being laughed at. Humor. 2009;22(1-2):27–48. [Google Scholar]
- 105.Papousek I, Ruch W, Freudenthaler HH, Kogler E, Lang B, Schulter G. Gelotophobia, emotion-related skills and responses to the affective states of others. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;47(1):58–63. [Google Scholar]
- 106.Edwards KR, Martin RA. The fear of being laughed at, social anxiety, and memories of being teased during childhood. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling. 2010;52:94–107. [Google Scholar]
- 107.Weiss EM, Schulter G, Freudenthaler HH, Hofer E, Pichler N, Papousek I. Potential markers of aggressive behavior: the fear of other persons’ laughter and its overlaps with mental disorders. PLoS ONE. 2012;(5) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038088.e38088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Holden C. No laughing matter. Science. 2009;324:p. 699. [Google Scholar]
- 109.Ruch W. Fearing humor? Gelotophobia: the fear of being laughed at Introduction and overview. Humor. 2009;22(1-2):1–25. [Google Scholar]
- 110.Nigg JT. On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126(2):220–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Barratt ES, Felthous AR. Impulsive versus premeditated aggression: implications for mens rea decisions. Behavioral Sciences and the Law. 2003;21(5):619–630. doi: 10.1002/bsl.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Tarter RE, Kirisci L, Mezzich A, et al. Neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160(6):1078–1085. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Bushman BJ. Effects of alcohol on human aggression. Validity of proposed explanations. Recent Developments in Alcoholism. 1997;13:227–243. doi: 10.1007/0-306-47141-8_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Cherek DR, Steinberg JL, Vines RV. Low doses of alcohol affect human aggressive responses. Biological Psychiatry. 1984;19(2):263–267. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Pihl RO, Peterson JB, Lau MA. A biosocial model of the alcohol-aggression relationship. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1993;54(11):128–139. doi: 10.15288/jsas.1993.s11.128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Lejuez CW, Magidson JF, Mitchell SH, Sinha R, Stevens MC, De Wit H. Behavioral and biological indicators of impulsivity in the development of alcohol use, problems, and disorders. Alcoholism. 2010;34(8):1334–1345. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01217.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Cherek DR, Lane SD. Laboratory and psychometric measurements of impulsivity among violent and nonviolent female parolees. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;46(2):273–280. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00309-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Cherek DR, Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Rhoades H. Studies of violent and nonviolent male parolees: II. Laboratory and psychometric measurements of impulsivity. Biological Psychiatry. 1997;41(5):523–529. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00426-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.James M, Seager JA. Impulsivity and schemas for a hostile world: postdictors of violent behaviour. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology. 2006;50(1):47–56. doi: 10.1177/0306624X05279804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Kumari V, Barkataki I, Goswami S, Flora S, Das M, Taylor P. Dysfunctional, but not functional, impulsivity is associated with a history of seriously violent behaviour and reduced orbitofrontal and hippocampal volumes in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. 2009;173(1):39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Gansler DA, Lee AKW, Emerton BC, et al. Prefrontal regional correlates of self-control in male psychiatric patients: impulsivity facets and aggression. Psychiatry Research. 2011;191(1):16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Lee AKW, Jerram M, Fulwiler C, Gansler DA. Neural correlates of impulsivity factors in psychiatric patients and healthy volunteers: a voxel-based morphometry study. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2011;5(1):52–64. doi: 10.1007/s11682-010-9112-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Matsuo K, Nicoletti M, Nemoto K, et al. A voxel-based morphometry study of frontal gray matter correlates of impulsivity. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30(4):1188–1195. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Antonucci AS, Gansler DA, Tan S, Bhadelia R, Patz S, Fulwiler C. Orbitofrontal correlates of aggression and impulsivity in psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Research. 2006;147(2-3):213–220. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Coccaro EF, Sripada CS, Yanowitch RN, Phan KL. Corticolimbic function in impulsive aggressive behavior. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;69(12):1153–1159. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Dolan M, Deakin WJF, Roberts N, Anderson I. Serotonergic and cognitive impairment in impulsive aggressive personality disordered offenders: are there implications for treatment? Psychological Medicine. 2002;32(1):105–117. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701004688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Hoaken PNS, Shaughnessy VK, Pihl RO. Executive cognitive functioning and aggression: is it an issue of impulsivity? Aggressive Behavior. 2003;29(1):15–30. [Google Scholar]
- 128.Giancola PR, Moss HB, Martin CS, Kirisci L, Tarter RE. Executive cognitive functioning predicts reactive aggression in boys at high risk for substance abuse: a prospective study. Alcoholism. 1996;20(4):740–744. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01680.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Lapierre D, Braun CMJ, Hodgins S. Ventral frontal deficits in psychopathy: neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33(2):139–151. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00110-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Mitchell DGV, Colledge E, Leonard A, Blair RJR. Risky decisions and response reversal: is there evidence of orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in psychopathic individuals? Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(12):2013–2022. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00056-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Pattij T, Vanderschuren LJMJ. The neuropharmacology of impulsive behaviour. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences. 2008;29(4):192–199. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2008.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Butler T, Schofield PW, Greenberg D, et al. Reducing impulsivity in repeat violent offenders: an open label trial of a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;44(12):1137–1143. doi: 10.3109/00048674.2010.525216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Kavoussi RJ, Liu J, Coccaro EF. An open trial of sertraline in personality disordered patients with impulsive aggression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1994;55(4):137–141. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Coccaro EF, Kavoussi RJ. Fluoxetine and impulsive aggressive behavior in personality-disordered subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1997;54(12):1081–1088. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830240035005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Huband N, Ferriter M, Nathan R, Jones H. Antiepileptics for aggression and associated impulsivity. Cochrane Database of Aystematic Reviews. 2010;2 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003499.pub3.CD003499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Stanford MS, Anderson NE, Lake SL, Baldridge RM. Pharmacologic treatment of impulsive aggression with antiepileptic drugs. Current Treatment Options in Neurology. 2009;11(5):383–390. doi: 10.1007/s11940-009-0043-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Link BG, Stueve A. Psychotic symptoms and the violent/illegal behavior of mental patients compared to community controls. In: Monahan J, Steadman HJ, editors. Violence and Mental Disorder. Developments in Risk Assessment. Chicago, Ill, USA: The University of Chicago Press; 1994. pp. 137–159. [Google Scholar]
- 138.Junginger J, Parks-Levy J, McGuire L. Delusions and symptom-consistent violence. Psychiatric Services. 1998;49(2):218–220. doi: 10.1176/ps.49.2.218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Taylor PJ. Motives for offending among violent and psychotic men. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1985;147:491–498. doi: 10.1192/bjp.147.5.491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Junginger J. Command hallucinations and the prediction of dangerousness. Psychiatric Services. 1995;46(9):911–914. doi: 10.1176/ps.46.9.911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.McNiel DE, Binder RL. The relationship between acute psychiatric symptoms, diagnosis, and short- term risk of violence. Hospital and Community Psychiatry. 1994;45(2):133–137. doi: 10.1176/ps.45.2.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Zisook S, Byrd D, Kuck J, Jeste DV. Command hallucinations in outpatients with schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1995;56(10):462–465. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Cheung P, Schweitzer I, Crowley K, Tuckwell V. Violence in schizophrenia: role of hallucinations and delusions. Schizophrenia Research. 1997;26(2-3):181–190. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(97)00049-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Nolan KA, Volavka J, Czobor P, et al. Aggression and psychopathology in treatment-resistant inpatients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2005;39(1):109–115. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Fresán A, Apiquian R, De La Fuente-Sandoval C, et al. Violent behavior in schizophrenic patients: relationship with clinical symptoms. Aggressive Behavior. 2005;31(6):511–520. [Google Scholar]
- 146.Maccoby EE, Jacklin CN. The Psychology of Sex Differences. Stanford, Calif, USA: Stanford University Press; 1974. [Google Scholar]
- 147.Pastore AL. Sourcebook of Criminal Justice Statistics, 1999.
- 148.Hodgins S. Mental disorder, intellectual deficiency, and crime: evidence from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1992;49(6):476–483. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820060056009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Swanson JW, Holzer CE, Ganju VK, Jono RT. Violence and psychiatric disorder in the community: evidence from the epidemiologic catchment area surveys. Hospital and Community Psychiatry. 1990;41(7):761–770. doi: 10.1176/ps.41.7.761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Lam JN, McNiel DE, Binder RL. The relationship between patients’ gender and violence leading to staff injuries. Psychiatric Services. 2000;51(9):1167–1170. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.51.9.1167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Binder RL, McNiel DE. The relationship of gender to violent behavior in acutely disturbed psychiatric patients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1990;51(3):110–114. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Hiday VA, Swartz MS, Swanson JW, Borum R, Wagner HR. Male-female differences in the setting and construction of violence among people with severe mental illness. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 1998;33(1):S68–S74. doi: 10.1007/s001270050212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Weiss EM, Marksteiner J, Hinterhuber H, Nolan KA. Gender differences in aggression in schizophrenia patients. Neuropsychiatrie. 2006;20(3):186–191. [Google Scholar]
- 154.Krakowski M, Czobor P. Gender differences in violent behaviors: relationship to clinical symptoms and psychosocial factors. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161(3):459–465. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.3.459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Crick NR, Grotpeter JK. Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development. 1995;66(3):710–722. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00900.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Eagly AH, Steffen VJ. Gender and aggressive behavior. A meta-analytic review of the Social Psychological Literature. Psychological Bulletin. 1986;100(3):309–330. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Dinakar HS, Sobel RN. Violence in the community as a predictor of violence in the hospital. Psychiatric Services. 2001;52(2):240–241. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.52.2.240-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Weiss EM, Bilder RM, Fleischhacker WW. The effects of second-generation antipsychotics on cognitive functioning and psychosocial outcome in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology. 2002;162(1):11–17. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1053-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Swanson JW, Swartz MS, Elbogen EB. Effectiveness of atypical antipsychotic medications in reducing violent behavior among persons with Schizophrenia in community-based treatment. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2004;30(1):3–20. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Swanson JW, Swartz MS, Elbogen EB, Van Dorn RA. Reducing violence risk in persons with schizophrenia: olanzapine versus risperidone. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2004;65(12):1666–1673. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v65n1212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Krakowski MI, Czobor P, Citrome L, Bark N, Cooper TB. Atypical antipsychotic agents in the treatment of violent patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63(6):622–629. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.6.622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Krakowski MI, Czobor P, Nolan KA. Atypical antipsychotics, neurocognitive deficits, and aggression in schizophrenic patients. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2008;28(5):485–493. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e3181855cd6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]