Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Nov 12.
Published in final edited form as: J Fam Psychol. 2012 Aug;26(4):10.1037/a0029164. doi: 10.1037/a0029164

Marital Hostility and Child Sleep Problems: Direct and Indirect Associations via Hostile Parenting

Kimberly A Rhoades 1, Leslie D Leve 2, Gordon T Harold 3, Anne M Mannering 4, Jenae M Neiderhiser 5, Daniel S Shaw 6, Misaki N Natsuaki 7, David Reiss 8
PMCID: PMC3824960  NIHMSID: NIHMS492642  PMID: 22888782

Abstract

The current study examined two family process predictors of parent-reported child sleep problems at 4.5 years in an adoption sample: marital hostility and hostile parenting. Participants were 361 linked triads of birth parents, adoptive parents, and adopted children. We examined direct and indirect pathways from marital hostility to child sleep problems via hostile parenting. Mothers’ marital hostility at 9 months was associated with child sleep problems at 4.5 years. Fathers’ marital hostility at 9 months evidenced an indirect effect on child sleep problems at 4.5 years via fathers’ hostile parenting at 27 months. Findings were significant even after controlling for genetic influences on child sleep (i.e., birth parent internalizing disorders). The findings suggest targets for prevention and intervention programs that are potentially modifiable (e.g., hostile parenting, marital hostility), and inform theory by demonstrating that relations among marital hostility, hostile parenting, and child sleep problems are significant after accounting for genetic influences.

Keywords: sleep problems, marital hostility, parenting, early childhood, adoption


Sleep problems during infancy and early childhood, including difficulties initiating and maintaining sleep, are relatively common, with prevalence rates between 34% and 45% (McGreavey, Donnan, Pagliani, & Sullivan, 2005; Simola et al., 2010). Furthermore, childhood sleep problems are associated with later behavioral, affective, academic, cognitive, and neurological functioning (El-Sheikh, Buckhalt, Keller, Cummings, & Acebo, 2007b; Gregory & O’Connor, 2002; Sadeh, Gruber, & Raviv, 2002). Because child sleep problems are a precursor to subsequent adjustment problems, it is important to more fully understand the processes associated with the exacerbation or amelioration of sleep problems during early childhood. However, studies examining relations among family processes and child sleep in early childhood are relatively rare and none have jointly considered genetic and environmental influences in explicating the effects of the family environment on sleep during early childhood.

Family Environmental Influences on Child Sleep Problems

Researchers have theorized that to achieve deep sleep, an individual must experience his/her sleeping environment as reasonably predictable, controllable, and free of potential threats (Dahl & El-Sheikh, 2007). Exposure to stressful family interactions during the day, including marital hostility and hostile parenting, could interfere with children feeling safe at bedtime and throughout the night. Marital hostility may be perceived by the child as a potential source of threat to the family system (Davies & Cummings, 1994). This threat, however, could also function indirectly via its impact on parenting (Harold, Shelton, Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2004; Kaczynski, Lindahl, Malik, & Laurenceau, 2006; Rhoades et al., 2011; Schoppe-Sullivan, Schermerhorn, & Cummings, 2007). The spillover hypothesis (Erel & Burman, 1995) suggests that parents’ negative thoughts and emotions during marital conflict affect subsequent parent-child interactions. Thus, an association between marital hostility and child sleep problems could be either direct, as a function of the threatening nature of marital hostility to the child’s well-being in the family system, and/or indirect, via more proximal parenting behaviors.

Marital conflict has been shown to predict concurrent and subsequent child sleep problems (El-Sheikh, Buckhalt, Cummings, & Keller, 2007a; Kelly & El-Sheikh, 2011). More specifically, Kelly and El-Sheikh (2011) reported that marital conflict predicts increases in child sleep disruptions over a 2-year period during middle childhood. Associations have also been found between maternal sensitivity and independence-encouraging behaviors and child sleep (Priddis, 2009; Sadeh, Mindell, Luedtke, & Wiegard, 2009). For example, one study found that infants whose mothers demonstrated more observed emotional availability at bedtime woke up fewer times, required less maternal care during the night, and had fewer sleep difficulties (Teti, Kim, Mayer, & Countermine, 2010).

The majority of parenting research has focused on mothers. Some recent work, however, has indicted that fathers’ parenting behaviors are significantly influenced by marital relations, with effects either being equivalent for mothers and fathers or stronger for fathers than for mothers (Kerig, Cowan, & Cowan, 1993; Schofield et al., 2009). Our inclusion of both mothers and fathers in the current study enabled us to further investigate similarities and/or differences in associations among marital and parenting hostility and child sleep problems for mothers versus fathers. Additionally, the majority of the research in this area has been conducted during either infancy or middle childhood. Because child sleep problems are associated with difficulties that likely make the school transition more challenging, including ADHD (Cortese, Faraone, Konofal, & Lecendreux, 2009), neurobehavioral task deficits (Sadeh et al., 2002), and daytime sleepiness (Fallone, Acebo, Seifer, & Carskadon, 2005), it is important to examine predictors of child sleep problems prior to school entry, as early school success is crucial in predicting subsequent educational outcomes (Gutman, Sameroff, & Cole, 2003). The current study addresses this gap in the literature by examining predictors of child sleep problems from age 9 months to 4.5 years.

The Adoption Design

Research on family influences on child sleep has typically been conducted with biologically-related families. In studies of children and their biological rearing parents, the fact that children share half of their genes with each parent introduces genetic confounds that may, in part, account for associations between child sleep and family factors. Specifically, in biological families, the same genes that influence parents’ behaviors (e.g., their marital hostility and/or parenting behaviors) might also affect child sleep problems. Thus, the association between marital hostility or hostile parenting and child sleep problems may be due to genetic influences shared between parent and child (i.e., passive gene-environment correlation; Horwitz & Neiderhiser, 2011), rather than from specific family environmental influences. The present study, an adoption design where children were placed at birth with genetically-unrelated parents allowed for the examination of associations between family processes and child sleep problems without the potential influence of shared genes between parent and child.

An adoption design that links adoptive families with the child’s birth parents provides an added advantage, because it can provide a control for potential genetic influences on child sleep by including phenotypes in birth parents that have known genetic overlap with sleep problems (depression/anxiety). We include birth parent internalizing disorders as a genetic control variable in the present study for two primary reasons: (1) children of mothers with internalizing disorders demonstrate more sleep problems than children whose mothers do not have these disorders (Field, 1995; Gregory et al., 2005; Stoléru, Nottelmann, Belmont, & Ronsaville, 1997), suggesting that maternal internalizing disorders may serve as a potential familial/genetic risk for child sleep problems; and (2) there is substantial genetic overlap between depression/anxiety and sleep problems (Gregory et al., 2011; Gregory, Rijsdijk, Lau, Dahl, & Eley, 2009), suggesting a common genetic factor that partially accounts for both depression/anxiety disorders and sleep problems. Because the children in the present study are reared by genetically-unrelated parents, associations between birth parent internalizing diagnoses and child sleep problems are most reasonably inferred to be due to genetic factors. We include a measure of prenatal exposure in the present study to further control for contributions from the birth parents.

Study Aims and Hypotheses

A previous study using this sample found that marital instability when children were 9 months old was associated with child sleep problems at 18 months, even after accounting for child sleep problems at 9 months (Mannering et al., 2011). The current study extends the findings of Mannering et al. (2011) by (a) examining both direct and indirect associations between observed marital hostility and parent-reported child sleep via hostile parenting; and (b) investigating child sleep from 18 months to 4.5 years of age, encompassing a developmental period when establishing a healthy sleep routine is critical for a successful transition to school. By including information on both mothers and fathers, we can also test similarities/differences in the effects of marital hostility and hostile parenting on child sleep problems for mothers versus fathers. Using a full adoption design with data from birth parents, we also accounted for genetic influences (i.e., birth parent internalizing disorder) and prenatal influences on child sleep problems.

We tested the following three hypotheses using a prospective adoption study, with assessments at 9, 18, and 27 months, and 4.5 years. First, we hypothesized that mothers’ and fathers’ observed marital hostility during infancy (i.e., 9 months) would predict parents’ reports of children’s difficulties initiating and maintaining sleep (hereafter referred to as “child sleep problems”) during the preschool period (i.e., 4.5 years). Second, we hypothesized that both mothers’ and fathers’ hostile parenting during toddlerhood (i.e., 27 months) would predict child sleep problems during the preschool period. Third, consistent with the spillover hypothesis, we hypothesized a significant indirect effect of marital hostility on child sleep problems via hostile parenting for both mothers and fathers.

Method

Participants and Procedures

Participants were 361 adoption-linked sets of adopted children, their adoptive mothers and fathers, and their birth mothers and a subset of birth fathers (n = 121) who participated in Cohort I of the Early Growth and Development Study (EGDS) (Cohort II was recruited subsequently and contains an additional 200 families; however, age 4.5 data have not been collected for Cohort II). Eligibility criteria included: (1) domestic adoption placement, (2) placement occurred within 3 months postpartum, (3) non-relative placement, (4) no known major medical conditions, and (5) birth and adoptive parents were able to understand English at the eighth-grade level. Informed consent was appropriately obtained prior to assessment. In the full EGDS study, the children were approximately 9 months old during the first child assessment (M = 9.2, SD = 0.96), 18 months old during the second assessment (M = 17.95, SD = 0.96), 27 months old during the third child assessment (M = 27.6, SD = 1.56), and 4.5 years old during the fourth child assessment (M = 4.62, SD = 0.16). Forty-three percent of the children were female. The median child age at adoption placement was 2 days. The adoptive parents were typically college educated, middle- to upper-class families. The adoptive mother and adoptive father mean ages at the child’s birth were 37.75 (SD = 5.46) and 38.39 years (SD = 5.82), respectively. The ethnicity of adoptive mothers and fathers was: 91.7% and 90.7% Caucasian, 3.6% and 5.1% African American, 2.2% and 1.7% Hispanic or Latino, 1.1% and 1.1% multiracial, 0.6% and 0.6% Asian, 0.3% and 0% American Indian or Alaskan Native, 0% and 0.3% Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander, and 0.6% and 0.6% unknown or unreported. Birth mothers and birth fathers typically had less than a college education and had household annual incomes less than $25,000. Birth mother and birth father mean age at the child’s birth was 24.12 years (SD = 5.89) and 25.45 years (SD = 7.20), respectively. The ethnicity of birth mothers and fathers was: 71.7% and 72.4% Caucasian, 11.4% and 8.7% African American, 6.7% and 8.7% Hispanic or Latino, 4.4% and 4.7% multiracial, 2.8% and .08% American Indian or Alaskan Native, 1.9% and 0% Asian, 0.3% and 0% Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander, and 0.8% and 4.7% unknown or unreported. For full demographic information refer to Leve, Neiderhiser, Scaramella, & Reiss (2010). The sample included 20 same-sex adoptive couples. There were no significant differences in any of the study variables for same-sex versus male-female couples. We ran all models first including and then excluding same-sex couples; the results were unchanged. Results presented here do not include the same-sex couples, due to our focus on mother- and father-specific influences. Results from the analyses that included the same-sex couples are available from the first author upon request.

Retention rates remained high throughout the course of the study (87–92% for birth mothers; 83–91% for birth fathers, and 84–98%, for adoptive families across the study assessments). There were three significant differences in demographic characteristics at the first assessment between families who had complete data and those who were missing data at a later assessment. Families with missing data reported slightly lower household incomes, slightly longer marriages, and slightly older adoptive father age at child birth (ps < .05).

Measures

Marital hostility

As part of the 9-month in-home assessment, parents participated in a 20-min videotaped marital interaction task. Parents were instructed to discuss 19 topics about their relationship. Topics were designed to elicit both positive and negative emotions, for example: how they met, what they enjoy doing with each other, and what they find the most frustrating about each other and their relationship. Trained observational coders who were blind to all study hypotheses coded this task by using the Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales – Marital Interaction Code (Dogan et al., 2004; Melby et al., 1990). We included the following variables in our marital hostility construct: antisocial behavior, hostility, and negative mood.

The antisocial code includes instances in which a mother/father resists, defies, or is inconsiderate of their partner by being noncompliant, insensitive, or obnoxious, as well as when he/she is uncooperative or withdraws from social interaction. The hostility code includes hostile, angry, critical, disapproving and/or rejecting behavior toward the partner’s behaviors, appearance, or state. The negative mood code captures how much the mother/father appears unhappy, dissatisfied, sad, pessimistic, angry, and/or expresses negative sentiments toward self, others outside the interaction task, or things in general. The codes obtained from the 20-minute marital interaction task have previously demonstrated satisfactory reliability and have been significantly associated with self and spouse ratings of marital hostility and marital quality (Melby, Conger, Ge, & Warner, 1995).

All codes were rated on a 1–7 scale (1 = not at all characteristic; 7 = mainly characteristic). Approximately 30% of the interactions were coded by two observers. ICC’s for these three codes ranged from .63 to .66, with a mean ICC of .64. The three codes were significantly intercorrelated for both mothers and fathers (rs = .45–.49 for mothers and .43–.46 for fathers) and were used as indicators of two latent marital hostility constructs: mother marital hostility and father marital hostility.

Child sleep problems

At 4.5 years, child sleep problems were measured using the Tayside Children’s Sleep Questionnaire (McGreavey et al., 2005). This questionnaire contains 10 items that assess disorders of initiating and maintaining sleep in children 1 to 5 years old. Items are rated on a 6-point scale from 0 (never) to 5 (every night), with the exception of the question about length of time to fall asleep, which is rated from 0 (less than 15 minutes) to 5 (more than 60 minutes). The ten items are: “how long after going to bed does your child fall asleep”; “child goes to bed reluctantly”; “child has difficulty getting to sleep at night”; “child does not fall asleep in his or her own bed”; “child wakes up two or more times in the night”; “after waking up, the child has difficulty falling asleep again by himself or herself”; “the child sleeps in parents bed at some time during the night”; “if the child wakes up, he or she uses a comforter (e.g. pacifier or blanket)”; “child wants a drink during the night”; and “do you think your child has sleeping difficulties.” Mothers and fathers independently completed this scale (mother α = .75; father α = .71). These two measures (mother-report and father-report) were used as indicators of a latent child sleep problems variable at 4.5 years. This parent-report sleep measure has demonstrated reliability, face, content, and discriminant validity (McGreavey et al., 2005), and was independently evaluated as adhering to most psychometric tool development requirements (Spruyt & Gozal, 2011), although it has yet to be standardized or subjected to confirmatory factor analyses.

Hostile parenting

Mother and father hostility toward the child was assessed at 27 months using the 18-item Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales (Melby et al., 1995). The hostility subscale (mother α = .77; father α = .70) consists of 5 items rated on a 1 to 7 scale: 1 (never occurs) to 7(always occurs). Items include: “How often did you”… “get angry with him or her”; “shout or yell at him or her because you were angry with him or her”; “criticize him/her and his/her ideas”; “argue with him/her whenever you disagreed about something”; and “hit, push, grab, or shove him or her.” Mothers and fathers completed the scale and independently rated their own behavior toward their child.

Covariates/Control variables

Birth mother and birth father internalizing disorders

We assessed lifetime internalizing disorders in birth mothers and birth fathers using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI; Kessler & Üstün, 2004) at child age 18 and 48 months. The CIDI was obtained twice to more fully capture potential genetic risk, because birth parent internalizing disorder that presents at any point in the child’s life may impart genetic risk to the child, and because birth parent internalizing disorder onset might have occurred after the first CIDI interview. The CIDI is a comprehensive, standardized interview that assesses 17 major diagnostic areas according to the definitions and criteria of the tenth revision of the International Classification of Diseases and the fourth edition of the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Moderate to good concordance was found for lifetime disorder prevalence assessed via the CIDI versus the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). The CIDI underestimates lifetime prevalence compared to the SCID (Haro et al., 2006). Birth parents were classified as having a clinically significant internalizing disorder if one or both birth parents met DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for lifetime Major Depressive Disorder, Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Panic Disorder, Social Phobia, or Specific Phobia at either assessment period. If birth mothers or fathers did not meet criteria for any lifetime disorder at the first assessment and were missing the second assessment, they were classified as missing because they did not have an opportunity to endorse symptoms at the most recent assessment. Sixty-two percent of the birth mothers and 17% of the birth fathers met criteria for one or more disorder. As is typically found (e.g., Eaton et al., 2012), more women than men endorsed a lifetime internalizing disorder, although the percentage of birth parents, especially birth mothers, reporting lifetime internalizing disorder is considerably higher than what is typically seen in nationally representative samples (Eaton et al., 2012). This is likely due to differences between women who choose adoption and those who either do not have children or who choose to parent. To create a composite birth parent variable, we coded positive lifetime history for any dyad where the birth mother and/or birth father met criteria for one or more internalizing disorder(s). Sixty-eight percent of the dyads met criteria for a positive lifetime history of internalizing disorder (n meeting criteria = 246).

Child sleep problems at 18 months

At 18 months, mothers completed the Sleep Habits Questionnaire (SHQ; Goodlin-Jones, Sitnick, Tang, Liu, & Anders, 2008) and the sleep problems subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000). To create a measure that most closely mirrored the outcome measure of child sleep problems at 4.5 years, 2 items were selected from the SHQ and 3 items were selected from the CBCL that closely matched the items on the age 4.5 measure of child sleep. The items from the SHQ include: “falls asleep within 20 minutes (reverse coded)” and “how often has your child wakened you at night.” The items from the CBCL include: “trouble getting to sleep”, “sleeps less than most children”, and “wakes up often at night.” Parents rated the frequency of each behavior on a 3-point scale: rarely, sometimes, or usually. A mean score was computed for mothers and fathers; higher scores indicated greater sleep problems (α = .72 for mothers and .72 for fathers). Mother and father ratings were highly correlated (r = .74); thus, a mean mother/father rating was used in all analyses. Because this measure was constructed for this study and has not been validated, we also conducted all analyses controlling for the bedtime resistance subscale of the SHQ (see measure description in the “Measures used in supplementary analyses” section, below).

Overreactive parenting

At 18 months, mothers and fathers completed the Parenting Scale (Arnold, O’Leary, Wolf, & Acker, 1993). The Parenting Scale is a 30-item measure designed to assess parental discipline strategies. Parents rated their likelihood of using specific discipline strategies in response to child misbehaviors on 7-point scales anchored by one effective and one ineffective discipline strategy. A score of 1 indicated effective discipline and a score of 7 indicated dysfunctional discipline. We computed the Overreactive subscale based on confirmatory factor analyses of the Parenting Scale (Rhoades & O’Leary, 2007; mother α = .69; father α = .62). Thus, the Overreactive subscale contains five items (e.g., “When I’m upset and under stress…I’m picky and on my child’s back/I am no more picky than normal”; “When my child misbehaves…I raise my voice or yell/I speak to my child calmly). Although the internal consistencies of these subscales are moderately low, they are reasonable given the small number of items (5) included in each subscale and similar to those found in other studies using the same factors (e.g., Rhoades & O’Leary, 2007). The Parenting Scale has demonstrated validity; mothers of clinic children reported more overreactive discipline than mothers of non-clinic children and mother-reported overreactivity on the Parenting Scale is significantly correlated with observed overreactivity and observed and mother-reported child behavior in children as young as 18 months (Arnold et al., 1993; Jouriles et al., 1991).

Adoption openness

To control for similarities between birth and adoptive parents that might result from contact between parents, we included the level of openness in the adoption (e.g., contact and knowledge about the other party) as a covariate in all analyses. Openness in the adoption was measured using a composite of birth mother, adoptive mother, and adoptive father ratings of perceived adoption openness completed at the first assessment (Ge et al., 2008). Inter-rater agreement was high (r range = .72–.85, p values all < .001).

Obstetric complications

Perinatal obstetric complications can confound genetic influence estimates (see Pemberton et al., 2010 and Natsuaki, et al., 2010 for examples of associations among perinatal complications and genetic influences) and this variable was therefore included as a covariate. Obstetric complications were assessed at 4 months using birth mother report of her: (1) Maternal/Pregnancy Complications (e.g., illness, exposure to drugs); (2) Labor and Delivery Complications (e.g., prolonged labor, cord complications); and (3) Neonatal Complications (e.g., prematurity, low birth weight) using a pregnancy screener and a pregnancy calendar method. Scoring was derived from the McNeil-Sjostrom Scale for Obstetric Complications (McNeil & Sjostrom, 1995), with item scores ranging from 1 (not harmful or relevant) to 6 (very great harm to or deviation in offspring). The obstetric complications total was created by calculating the frequency of scores greater than or equal to 3, indicating risk that is at least “potentially, but not clearly, harmful or relevant.”

Child gender

Because child gender may be associated with family processes (Davies & Lindsay, 2004; Shek, 2008; Stroud, Durbin, Wilson, & Mendelsohn, 2001) and child sleep problems (Simola et al., 2010), child gender was included as a covariate (coded 1 for boys and 2 for girls) in all analyses.

Family income

Because child sleep problems may be associated with SES (Kelly & El-Sheikh, 2011), we included family income at 9 months as a covariate. We asked mothers and fathers to report their individual income and we then combined those reports to obtain an estimate of family income.

Measures used in supplementary analyses

Child bedtime resistance

At 9 months, mothers and fathers completed the Sleep Habits Questionnaire (SHQ; Goodlin-Jones et al., 2008). We used the 6-item Bedtime Resistance subscale from this questionnaire, which assesses difficulty initiating or maintaining sleep (e.g., “child needs parent in room to fall asleep”, “child struggles at bedtime”). Parents rated the frequency of each behavior during the past week on a 3-point scale: rarely (0–1), sometimes (2–4), or usually (5 or more). A mean score was computed for each parent; higher scores indicated greater bedtime resistance (mother α = .74, father α = .71). Mother- and father-reported bedtime resistance scores were correlated (r = .80) and were averaged to create a composite parent measure of child sleep problems.

Total Behavior Problems

At 4.5 years, mothers and fathers completed the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000). The Total Problems scale was used (α = .95 for mothers and .96 for fathers). Mother and father reports were significantly correlated (r = .43, p < .001) and were averaged.

Data Analytic Plan

Hypothesis testing proceeded in three steps: testing direct and indirect effects of marital hostility and hostile parenting on child sleep problems, evaluation of mother vs. father differences, and supplementary analyses. First, we evaluated the fit of the model to the data using Mplus 6.11 (Muthén & Muthén, 2007), which uses full information maximum likelihood (FIML) to estimate parameters when data are missing. FIML produces unbiased estimates when data are missing at random (MAR). For all variables included in the current study there was less than 15% missing. The Little’s test of missing data indicated that the data were missing completely at random (MCAR); Little’s MCAR χ2 (456) = 481.33, p = ns. The model was deemed to have adequate fit if the chi-square was nonsignificant, the comparative fit index (CFI) was > .95, and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was < .06 (Hu & Bentler, 1999). All indirect effects were estimated with bias-corrected bootstrapping and were considered statistically significant if the corresponding 95% confidence interval did not include zero (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002; Preacher & Hayes, 2008). In addition to the hypothesized paths, the initial model accounted for the influence of birth parent internalizing disorders, adoption openness, obstetric complications, child gender, and family income (not shown in Figure 1 for clarity).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Structural equation model predicting child sleep problems from marital hostility and hostile parenting.

Note: χ2 (103) = 124.54, p = ns; CFI = .98; RMSEA = .03; NM = negative mood, ANT = antisocial, HOS = hostility, AM = adoptive mother, AF = adoptive father; all path estimates are standardized; control paths are not shown for clarity.

*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001

Second, we tested for mother vs. father differences. We first ran the full model with all mother-specific and father-specific paths freely estimated. We then ran three models in which the paths from (1) mother marital hostility and father marital hostility to child sleep problems, (2) from mother hostile parenting and father hostile parenting to child sleep problems, and (3) from mother marital hostility to mother hostile parenting and from father marital hostility to father hostile parenting were constrained to be equal. To evaluate whether these paths were significantly different, χ2 difference tests were conducted. As an additional test, we ran two additional models: one that included only mothers’ marital and parental hostility and one that included only fathers’ marital and parental hostility.

Third, we ran supplementary analyses to test for potential measurement artifacts. To test whether sleep problems are simply a proxy for overall child behavior problems, we re-analyzed the model controlling for total CBCL score at 4.5 years. To test whether the results were influenced by our use of a measure of sleep items at 18 months that was created by combining items across two instruments specifically for this study, we re-analyzed the model using a standard assessment: the Bedtime Resistance subscale of the Sleep Habits Questionnaire at 9 months.

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Correlations

Bivariate correlations, means, standard deviations, and sample sizes for all study variables are provided in Table 1. Mothers’ and fathers’ marital hostility were positively associated with fathers’ hostile parenting. Mothers’ observed negative mood and hostility and fathers’ negative mood toward their partners were associated with child sleep at 4.5 years. Fathers’ hostile parenting at 27 months was associated with child sleep problems at 4.5 years.

Table 1.

Bivariate Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18.
1. AM Marital
 Negative Mood --
2. AM Marital .45***
 Hostility --
3. AM Marital .49*** .83***
 Antisocial --
4. AF Marital .23*** .21*** .19***
 Negative Mood --
5. AF Marital .20*** .34*** .33*** .43***
 Hostility --
6. AF Marital .17** .34*** .31*** .46*** .72***
 Antisocial --
7. AM .14 .05 .03 .06 .07 .07
 Overreactive Parenting --
8. AF .09 .07 .08 .14* .13* .12* .23***
 Overreactive Parenting --
9. AP 18 month .04 .08 .03 .17** .03 .08 −.07 .02
 Child Sleep --
10. AM Hostile −.02 −.05 −.01 −.01 .03 .01 .46*** .11 −.04
 Parenting --
11. AF Hostile .12* .11 .10 .11 .15** .18** .21*** .48*** .02 .25***
 Parenting --
12. AM 4.5 year .08 .14* .11 .1 .03 .11 −.01 .05 .38*** .02 .11
 Child Sleep --
13. AF 4.5 year .14* .09 .10 .16* −.06 .04 .05 .05 .38*** .05 .17* .67***
 Child Sleep --
14. BP −.04 .02 .01 .06 .09 .05 −.09 −.04 −.01 −.06 .07 .01 .01
 Internalizing Diagnosis --
15. Adoption −.06 .02 −.01 .06 −.07 −.04 −.09 −.05 .05 −.05 −.08 .06 .03 .15**
 Openness --
16. Obstetric .03 .01 .01 −.07 −.03 −.01 −.10 −.07 −.03 −.06 .02 −.08 −.07 .05 .02
 Comp. --
17. AP Family −.09 .03 .06 −.04 .04 .00 −.01 −.15** .02 −.06 −.06 .11 .12 .05- .01 −.04
 Income --
18. Child −.02 .03 .05 .01 .02 .04 −.01 −.02 −.08 .05 −.14* −.16** −.08 −.01 −.08 −.02 .03
 Gender --
Mean 2.21 1.57 1.68 2.02 1.36 1.66 2.14 2.12 1.31 9.05 8.90 9.20 8.98 na .03 2.21 7.07 na
Standard Deviation 1.32 1.13 1.16 1.15 .79 1.15 .77 .73 .36 2.59 2.47 5.74 5.25 na .93 1.28 1.93 na

Note. AP = adoptive parents; AM = adoptive mother; AF = adoptive father; BP = birth parent.

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

Models Predicting Child Sleep Problems at 4.5 Years

We evaluated the full hypothesized model which provided a good fit to the data (see Figure 1), χ2 (103) = 124.54, p = ns; CFI = .98; RMSEA = .03. This model accounted for birth parent internalizing diagnosis, adoption openness, obstetric complications, child gender, and family income. There were significant associations between child gender and father hostile parenting (β = −.15, p < .05), with fathers of girls reporting less hostile parenting than fathers of boys, and between income and sleep problems (β = .18, p < .05). No other control variables were significantly associated with marital hostility, hostile parenting, or child sleep. Fathers’ marital hostility significantly predicted fathers’ hostile parenting at 27 months (β = .14, p < .05). Fathers’ hostile parenting at 27 months significantly predicted parent-reported child sleep problems at 4.5 years after controlling for child sleep problems at 18 months (β = .24, p < .001). Mothers’ marital hostility predicted parent-reported child sleep problems at 4.5 years (β = .14, p < .05). In addition, the indirect path from fathers’ marital hostility to parent-reported child sleep problems at 4.5 years via fathers’ hostile parenting at 27 months was significant (unstandardized 95% bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence interval = .05–.73). The model accounted for 28% of the variance in child sleep problems at 4.5 years.

Mother/Father Differences

To test for differences between mothers and fathers in predicting child sleep problems, we estimated models in which the paths from mother/father marital hostility and parenting to child sleep problems were constrained and compared the resulting model fit to the model in which those paths were free to vary. Constraining the paths from mother hostile parenting and father hostile parenting to child sleep to be equal resulted in significantly worse model fit (Δ χ2 (1) = 6.98, p < .01); model fit was marginally worse (Δ χ2 (1) = 3.39, p < .07) when paths from mother and father marital hostility to child sleep were constrained to be equal. Constraining the paths from marital hostility to hostile parenting to be equal for mothers and fathers also resulted in marginally worse model fit (Δ χ2 (1) = 3.29, p < .07). These results indicate that the effects of hostile parenting on child sleep are significantly greater for fathers than for mothers, while the effects of marital hostility on child sleep are marginally greater for mothers than for fathers. Also, marital hostility is marginally more strongly associated with hostile parenting for fathers than for mothers. To examine whether the results were influenced by the inclusion of both mothers and fathers in the same model, we ran two separate additional models, one including father marital hostility and father hostile parenting, the other including mother marital hostility and mother hostile parenting. The significance of the path estimates obtained from the mother-only and father-only models were identical to those obtained in the full model.

Supplementary Analyses

To test whether our results were influenced by children’s behavior problems and/or by our choice of sleep variable, we re-analyzed the full model controlling for total CBCL score at 4.5 years and controlling for bedtime resistance at 9 months (instead of our newly-constructed measure of parent-reported sleep problems at 18 months). In both instances, all significant results were retained.

Discussion

Previous results from this sample indicated that marital instability is longitudinally associated with change in child sleep problems from 9 to 18 months (Mannering et al., 2011). The current findings demonstrate that the predictive association between marital hostility and child sleep problems, when examined later in development, is direct for mothers and indirect for fathers via fathers’ hostile parenting. Importantly, the direct effect from mothers’ marital hostility and the indirect effect from fathers’ marital hostility to child sleep problems at 4.5 years via fathers’ hostile parenting at age 27 months were significant even after accounting for child sleep problems at 18 months and mother and father overreactive discipline at 18 months. These findings suggest that interventions designed to reduce marital hostility and hostile parenting could result in sleep problem reductions in their children.

Our findings suggest that family processes indicative of low stability or security in the family system are associated with child sleep problems. This is consistent with the emotional security theory (Davies & Cummings, 1994), which posits that children’s reactions to marital relations are a function of their perceived implications on the well-being of the family, and that family processes indicative of insecurity in the family subsystem are likely to be associated with child adjustment problems. Theoretically, disturbances in child sleep patterns are a marker of the impact of family stress on neurobiological functioning (El-Sheikh et al., 2007a; Sadeh et al., 2002). As such, the current results suggest that two indicators of family stress, specifically marital hostility and hostile parenting, may impact child sleep through their influence on children’s felt security in the family system, even after controlling for the effects of shared genes and earlier child sleep problems. The current study did not include measures of emotional security and thus, could not directly examine this possibility. However, research in biologically-related families has demonstrated that marital conflict is associated with child sleep via child emotional security in school-age children (El-Sheikh et al., 2007a).

The results for fathers are also consistent with the spillover hypothesis (Erel & Burman, 1995). The significant association between marital hostility and hostile parenting for fathers, but not mothers, is consistent with prior research demonstrating that fathers are more likely to show evidence of spillover of negativity from the marital relationship to the parent-child relationship than mothers (Cummings, Goeke-Morey, & Raymond, 2004; Kerig et al., 1993; Schofield et al., 2009). Other findings using this sample have also found evidence of more robust father effects than are often reported in the early childhood literature regarding biological fathers. For example, Rhoades et al. (2011) found similar effects of mother and father overreactive parenting on child anger during toddlerhood. This may be due to adoptive mothers and fathers being more similar in the amount of time they spend interacting with their infants than biological parents, suggesting that adoptive fathers might be more involved in interacting with their young children than biological fathers (Holditch-Davis, Sandelowski, & Harris, 1999). Because most research on marital hostility and conflict uses composite measures of mother and father marital processes, our finding of a direct effect of marital hostility on child sleep problems for only mothers’ marital hostility is rather novel and should be replicated before drawing firm conclusions about the differential contribution of mother versus father hostility on child sleep problems. It is possible that because mothers’ parenting is less influenced by marital hostility (Cummings et al., 2004) than fathers’ parenting, the influence of marital hostility remains a direct effect in these models for mothers; whereas for fathers, this association is accounted for by hostile parenting.

A key strength of this study is our ability to account for measured genetic influences (i.e., birth parent internalizing disorders). Because there are significant genetic contributions to child sleep and common genetic factors underlie internalizing problems and sleep problems (Gregory, Eley, O’Connor, Rijsdijk, & Plomin, 2005; Gregory et al., 2009; Gregory et al., 2011), our findings that child sleep problems are associated with marital hostility and hostile parenting even after accounting for both birth parent internalizing disorder and passive gene-environment correlation further strengthen the conclusion that these relations are not solely due to genetic factors. Rather, the results suggest that there are identifiable and modifiable family environmental variables that are prospectively associated with child sleep problems and that could be targeted in prevention or intervention programs designed to reduce or prevent sleep problems in early childhood. Evidence-based treatments designed to reduce parental hostility (e.g., The Incredible Years (Webster-Stratton & Reid, 2010), Triple P (Sanders, Prinz, & Shapiro, 2012)) and marital hostility (e.g., Couple Care for Parents (Halford, Petch, & Creedy, 2010), marital-focused couples’ groups (Cowan, Cowan, & Barry, 2011)) in parents of young children could thus potentially also reduce child sleep problems in these same populations. In light of the physical health, mental health, and academic consequences of sleep problems in early childhood, these programs could potentially meaningfully impact children’s development; additional research should be conducted to directly test the effectiveness and efficacy of these empirically supported programs in reducing and/or preventing child sleep problems. Although the effectiveness of these evidence-based programs in reducing or preventing child sleep problems has not been empirically evaluated to our knowledge, there is some preliminary evidence suggesting that behavioral family interventions and parenting services can reduce sleep problems in very early childhood (Martin, Barajas, Brooks-Gunn, & Hale, 2011; Selim, France, Blampied, & Liberty, 2006).

There are four limitations of this study worth noting. First, the measures of hostile parenting and child sleep were parent report. Objective and subjective measures of child sleep both contribute important information about overall sleep quantity and quality (Sadeh, 1994, 2004); objective measures, such as actigraphy, convey information about sleep and waking during the night that parents might not detect/notice, whereas subjective measures can assess daytime sleepiness and specific behaviors immediately prior to bed and through the night that cannot be assessed via objective measures. Thus, the generalizability of our results and conclusions could be increased if similar results were found using objective and subjective measures of child sleep. Unfortunately, we do not have objective measures of child sleep for this sample. We did, however, use both mother and father reports of child sleep problems to reduce single rater bias. In addition, we used an observational measure of marital hostility to further reduce rater bias. Thus, there are no paths in the model (other than control paths) in which two associated measures were obtained from the same single individual’s report. Taking these factors into consideration, our conclusions would be stronger if similar results were found using observed hostile parenting. Second, we did not have the same measures of child sleep or parenting at the 9-, 18-, and 27- month and the 4.5 year assessment periods, mainly due to developmental differences at the various assessment periods. We attempted to use control measures as similar to the later assessment instruments as possible. Thus, although the constructs are similar, paths from overreactive parenting at 18 months to hostile parenting at 27 months and from child sleep problems at 18 months to child sleep problems at 4.5 years should be regarded as controls and not as true auto-regressive paths. Third, our sample, particularly the sample of adoptive parents, has limited ethnic and sociodemographic diversity. In addition, this sample showed fairly normative and low-moderate amounts of marital hostility and hostile parenting. Although the full range of scores was observed for each construct, this sample was not recruited for and is not characterized by high levels of either marital hostility or hostile parenting (see means in Table 1). Caution should be used in generalizing these results to high risk populations and populations demonstrating high levels of hostility. Fourth, we did not have a reliable and valid measure of birth parent sleep problems and our genetic control measure (i.e., birth parent internalizing disorders) was obtained in adulthood while our measures of child sleep problems were obtained during childhood. These two factors may have attenuated any potential genetic effects on child sleep problems.

In conclusion, our results suggest that fathers’ observed marital hostility during infancy was indirectly associated with parent-reported child sleep problems at 4.5 years of age via fathers’ hostile parenting during toddlerhood, whereas mothers’ observed marital hostility during infancy had direct effects on child sleep problems at 4.5 years. Our findings identify targets for sleep problem prevention and intervention programs that are potentially modifiable, such as hostile parenting practices and marital hostility, and inform theory by demonstrating that relations among mothers’ marital hostility, fathers’ marital hostility, fathers’ hostile parenting, and parent-reported child sleep problems are significant even after controlling for the effects of shared genes among family members.

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by the following grant: R01 HD042608; NICHD, NIDA, and the Office of the Director; NIH; U.S. PHS (PI Years 1–5: David Reiss, MD; PI Years 6–10: Leslie Leve, PhD). The writing of this manuscript was partially supported by the following grant: P30 DA023920, NIDA, NIH, U.S. PHS. Gordon Harold was supported by a project grant awarded by The Nuffield Foundation. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development or the National Institutes of Health.

We thank the adoptive families, birth parents, and adoption agency staff members who participated in this study, Michelle Baumann and Sally Guyer for technical assistance, and Rand Conger, John Reid, Laura Scaramella, and Xiaojia Ge for scientific contributions.

Footnotes

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

Contributor Information

Kimberly A. Rhoades, Oregon Social Learning Center

Leslie D. Leve, Oregon Social Learning Center

Gordon T. Harold, University of Leicester

Anne M. Mannering, Oregon State University

Jenae M. Neiderhiser, The Pennsylvania State University

Daniel S. Shaw, University of Pittsburg

Misaki N. Natsuaki, University of California, Riverside

David Reiss, Yale University.

References

  1. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnold DS, O’Leary SG, Wolf LS, Acker MM. The Parenting Scale: A measure of dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations. Psychological Assessment. 1993;9:137–144. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.5.2.137. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cortese S, Faraone SV, Konofal E, Lecendreux M. Sleep in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Meta-analysis of subjective and objective studies. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;48:894–908. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ae09c9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cowan CP, Cowan PA, Barry J. Couples’ groups for parents of preschoolers: Ten-year outcomes of a randomized trial. Journal of Family Psychology. 2011;25:240–250. doi: 10.1037/a0023003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cummings EM, Goeke-Morey MC, Raymond . Fathers in family context: Effects of marital quality and marital conflict. In: Lamb ME, editor. The role of father in child development. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2004. pp. 196–221. [Google Scholar]
  6. Dahl RE, El-Sheikh M. Considering sleep in a family context: Introduction to the special issue. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007;21:1–3. doi: 10.1037/0893.3200.21.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Davies PT, Cummings EM. Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;116:387–411. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Davies PT, Lindsay LL. Interparental Conflict and Adolescent Adjustment: Why Does Gender Moderate Early Adolescent Vulnerability? Journal of Family Psychology. 2004;18:160–170. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dogan S, Lei A, Milne-Kahn J, Pong H, Wu E, Conger R. Family interaction behavioral codes for the Early Growth and Development Study. 2004 Unpublished coding manual. [Google Scholar]
  10. Eaton NR, Keyes KM, Krueger RF, Balsis S, Skodol AE, Markon KE, Hasin DS. An invariant dimensional liability model of gender differences in mental disorder prevalence: Evidence from a national sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012;121:282–288. doi: 10.1037/a0024780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. El-Sheikh M, Buckhalt JA, Cummings EM, Keller PS. Sleep disruptions and emotional insecurity are pathways of risk for children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007a;48:88–96. doi: 10.111/j.1469-7610.2006.01604.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. El-Sheikh M, Buckhalt JA, Keller PS, Cummings EM, Acebo C. Child emotional security and academic achievement: The role of sleep disruptions. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007b;21:29–38. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Erel O, Burman B. Interrelatedness of marital relations and parent-child relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118:108–132. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fallone G, Acebo C, Seifer R, Carskadon MA. Experimental restriction of sleep opportunity in children: Effects on teacher ratings. Sleep. 2005;28:1561–1567. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.12.1561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Field T. Infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development. 1995;18:1–13. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(95)90003-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ge X, Natsuaki M, Martin DM, Leve LD, Neiderhiser JM, Shaw DS, Reiss D. Bridging the divide: Openness in adoption and postadoption psychosocial adjustment among birth and adoptive parents. Journal of Family Psychology. 2008;22:529–540. doi: 10.1037/a0012817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Goodlin-Jones BL, Sitnick SL, Tang K, Liu J, Anders TF. The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire in toddlers and preschool children. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2008;29:82–88. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e318163c39a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gregory AM, Buysse DJ, Willis TA, Rijsdijk FV, Maughan B, Rowe R, Eley TC. Associations between sleep quality and anxiety and depression symptoms in a sample of young adult twins and siblings. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2011;71:250–255. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gregory AM, O’Connor TG. Sleep problems in childhood: A longitudinal study of developmental change and association with behavioral problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:964–971. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200208000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gregory AM, Eley TC, O’Connor T, Rijsdijk F, Plomin R. Family influences on the association between sleep problems and anxiety in a large sample of pre-school aged twins. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;39:1337–1348. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gregory AM, Rijsdijk F, Lau JYF, Dahl RE, Eley TC. The direction of longitudinal associations between sleep problems and depression symptoms: A study of twins aged 8 and 10 years. Sleep: Journal of Sleep and Sleep Disorders Research. 2009;32:189–199. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.2.189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gutman LM, Sameroff AJ, Cole R. Academic trajectories from first to twelfth grades: Growth curves according to multiple risk and early child factors. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:777–790. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.4.777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Halford WK, Petch J, Creedy DK. Promoting a positive transition to parenthood: A randomized clinical trial of couple relationship education. Prevention Science. 2010;11:89–100. doi: 10.1007/s11121-009-0152-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Haro JM, Arbabzadeh-Bouchez S, Brugha TS, De Girolamo G, Guyer ME, Jin R, Kessler RC. Concordance of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview Version 3.0 (CIDI 3.0) with standardized clinical assessments in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2006;15:167–180. doi: 10.1002/mpr.196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Harold GT, Shelton KH, Goeke-Morey MC, Cummings EM. Marital conflict, child emotional security about family relationships and child adjustment. Social Development. 2004;13:350–376. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00272.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Holditch-Davis D, Sandelowski M, Harris BG. Effect of infertility on mothers’ and fathers’ interactions with young infants. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. 1999;17:159–173. doi: 10.1080/02646839908409095. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Horwitz BN, Neiderhiser JM. Gene-environment interplay, family relationships, and child adjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2011;73:804–816. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2011.00846.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kaczynski KJ, Lindahl KM, Malik NM, Laurenceau J. Marital conflict, maternal and paternal parenting, and child adjustment: A test of mediation and moderation. Journal of Family Psychology. 2006;20:199–208. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kelly R, El-Sheikh M. Marital conflict and children’s sleep: Reciprocal relations and socioeconomic effects. Journal of Family Psychology. 2011;25:412–422. doi: 10.1037/a0023789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kerig PK, Cowan PA, Cowan CP. Marital quality and gender differences in parent-child interaction. Developmental Psychology. 1993;29:931–939. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.29.6.931. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kessler RC, Üstün TB. The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) The International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2004:93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Leve LD, Neiderhiser JM, Scaramella LV, Reiss D. The Early Growth and Development Study: Using the prospective adoption design to examine genotype–environment interplay. Behavior Genetics. 2010;40:306–314. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2008.01106. (Reprinted from 2008 Acta Psychologica Sinica 40, 1106 1115) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mannering AM, Harold GT, Leve LD, Shelton KH, Shaw DS, Conger RD, Reiss D. Longitudinal associations between marital instability and child sleep problems across infancy and toddlerhood in adoptive families. Child Development. 2011;82:1252–1266. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01594.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Martin A, Barajas RG, Brooks-Gunn J, Hale L. Parenting services may be an opportunity for improving bedtime routines among at-risk preschoolers. Behavioral Sleep Medicine. 2011;9:237–242. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2011.606771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. McGreavey JA, Donnan PT, Pagliari HC, Sullivan FM. The Tayside children’s sleep questionnaire: A simple tool to evaluate sleep problems in young children. Child: Care, Health, and Development. 2005;31:539–655. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2005.00548.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. McNeil T, Sjostrom K. McNeil-Sjostrom Scale for Obstetric Complications. Lund University; Sweden: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  39. Melby J, Conger R, Book R, Rueter M, Lucy L, Repinski D, Ahrens K, Black D, Brown D, Huck S, Mutchler L, Rogers S, Ross J, Stavros T. The Iowa family interaction rating scales. 2. Iowa State University, Center for Family Research in Rural Mental Health; Ames: 1990. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
  40. Melby J, Conger RD, Ge X, Warner TD. The use of structural equation modeling in assessing the quality of marital observations. Journal of Family Psychology. 1995;9:280–293. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.9.3.280. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Muthén L, Muthén B. Mplus User’s Guide. 5. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  42. Natsuaki MN, Ge X, Leve LD, Neiderhiser JM, Shaw DS, Conger RD, Reiss D. Genetic liability, environment, and the development of fussiness in toddlers: The roles of parental depression and parental responsiveness. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:1147–1158. doi: 10.1037/a0019659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Pemberton CK, Neiderhiser JM, Leve LD, Natsuaki MN, Shaw DS, Reiss D, Ge X. Influence of parental depressive symptoms on adopted toddler behaviors: An emerging developmental cascade of genetic and environmental effects. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:803–818. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods. 2008;40:879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Priddis LE. Tuned-in parenting and infant sleep patterns. Early Child Development and Care. 2009;179:259–269. doi: 10.1080/03004430600912132. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rhoades KA, Leve LD, Harold GT, Neiderhiser JM, Shaw DS, Reiss D. Longitudinal pathways from marital hostility to child anger during toddlerhood: Genetic susceptibility and indirect effects via harsh parenting. Journal of Family Psychology. 2011;25:282–291. doi: 10.1037/a0022886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rhoades KA, O’Leary SG. Factor structure and validity of the Parenting Scale. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2007;36:137–146. doi: 10.1080/15374410701274157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Sadeh A. Assessment of intervention for infant night waking: parental reports and activity-based home monitoring. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1994;62:63–68. doi: 10.1037//0022-006X.62.1.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sadeh A. A Brief Screening Questionnaire for Infant Sleep Problems: Validation and Findings from an Internet Sample. Pediatrics. 2004;113:e570–e577. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.6.e570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning and behavior problems in school-aged children. Child Development. 2002;73:405–417. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sadeh A, Mindell JA, Luedtke K, Wiegand B. Sleep and sleep ecology in the first three years: A web-based study. Journal of Sleep Research. 2009;18:60–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00699.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sanders MR, Prinz RJ, Shapiro C. Parenting and child maltreatment as public health issues: Implications from the Triple P system of intervention. Clinician’s guide to evidence-based practice series. In: Allen R, editor. Programs and interventions for maltreated children and families at risk. Clinician’s guide to evidence-based practice series. Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 2012. pp. 297–312. [Google Scholar]
  53. Schofield TJ, Conger RD, Martin MJ, Stockdale GD, Conger KJ, Widaman KF. Reciprocity in parenting of adolescents within the context of marital negatively. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45:1708–1722. doi: 10.1037/a0016353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schoppe-Sullivan SJ, Schermerhorn AC, Cummings EM. Marital conflict and children’s adjustment: Evaluation of the parenting process model. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2007;69:1118–1134. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00436.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Selim CA, France KG, Blampied NM, Liberty KA. Treating treatment-resistant infant sleep disturbance with combination pharmacotherapy and behavioural family interventions. Australian Psychologist. 2006;41:193–204. doi: 10.1080/00050060500545044. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Shek DTL. Perceived parental control and parent-child relational qualities in early adolescents in Hong Kong: Parent gender, child gender and grade differences. Sex Roles. 2008;58:666–681. doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9371-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. Simola P, Niskakangas M, Liukkonen K, Virkkula P, Pitkaranta A, Kirjavainen T, Aronen ET. Sleep problems and daytime tiredness in Finnish preschool-aged children-a community survey. Child: Care, Health, and Development. 2010;36:805–811. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2010.01112.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Spruyt K, Gozal D. Pediatric sleep questionnaires as diagnostic or epidemiological tools: A review of currently available instruments. Sleep Medicine Review. 2011;15:19–32. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2010.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Stoléru S, Nottelmann ED, Belmont B, Ronsaville D. Sleep problems in children of affectively ill mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38:831–841. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01601.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Stroud CB, Durbin CE, Wilson S, Mendelsohn KA. Spillover to triadic and dyadic systems in families with young children. Journal of Family Psychology. 2011;25:919–930. doi: 10.1037/a0025443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Teti DM, Kim B, Mayer G, Countermine M. Maternal emotional availability at bedtime predicts infant sleep quality. Journal of Family Psychology. 2010;24:307–315. doi: 10.1037/a0019306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Webster-Stratton CH, Reid MJ. The incredible years program for children from infancy to pre-adolescence: Prevention and treatment of behavior problems. In: Murrihy RC, Kidman AD, Ollendick TH, editors. Clinical handbook of assessing and treating conduct problems in youth. New York, NY, US: Springer Science + Business Media; 2010. pp. 117–138. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES