Abstract
Neuronal plasticity allows an animal to respond to environmental changes by modulating its response to stimuli. In the honey bee (Apis mellifera), the biogenic amine octopamine plays a crucial role in appetitive odor learning, but little is known about how octopamine affects the brain. We investigated its effect in the antennal lobe, the first olfactory center in the brain, using calcium imaging to record background activity and odor responses before and after octopamine application. We show that octopamine increases background activity in olfactory output neurons, while reducing average calcium levels. Odor responses were modulated both upwards and downwards, with more odor response increases in glomeruli with negative or weak odor responses. Importantly, the octopamine effect was variable across glomeruli, odorants, odorant concentrations and animals, suggesting that the octopaminergic network is shaped by plasticity depending on an individual animal’s history and possibly other factors. Using RNA interference, we show that the octopamine receptor AmOA1 (homolog of the Drosophila OAMB receptor) is involved in the octopamine effect. We propose a network model in which octopamine receptors are plastic in their density and located on a subpopulation of inhibitory neurons in a disinhibitory pathway. This would improve odor-coding of behaviorally relevant, previously experienced odors.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00359-013-0805-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Olfaction, Insects, Octopamine, Plasticity, Calcium imaging
Introduction
Forager honey bees must learn to associate floral odors with nectar and pollen rewards that their colony needs for survival. This learning ability has been extensively studied using laboratory procedures that allow for investigation of physiological bases that underlie olfactory learning and memory (Menzel and Giurfa 2001). These studies suggest that the biogenic amine octopamine (OA) represents the occurrence of sucrose in brain networks that process olfactory inputs. Octopaminergic cells extend throughout the brain, including regions important for olfactory learning such as the mushroom bodies (MB), antennal lobes (AL) and subesophageal ganglion (Kreissl et al. 1994; Monastirioti 1999; Sinakevitch et al. 2005). One set of octopaminergic neurons of particular interest is the ventral unpaired medial (VUM) cells. These cells have cell bodies along the ventral midline in the brain’s subesophageal ganglion and receive afferent input from sucrose sensitive taste receptor neurons on the honey bee’s mouthparts (Schröter et al. 2007). Electrophysiological recordings from one particular VUM cell (VUMmx1) have shown that its response to odor changes after that odor has been associated with sucrose reinforcement in a way that produces robust associative conditioning (Hammer 1993, 1997). Moreover, injection of OA into the brain is sufficient to replace sucrose reinforcement (Hammer and Menzel 1998), and disruption of OA pathways via RNA interference or pharmacological treatment blocks associative conditioning (Farooqui et al. 2003). Therefore, OA release is important for the formation of associative memory for an odor associated with sucrose reinforcement. Because of the broad distribution of VUM neuron arborizations (Schröter et al. 2007), it is likely that OA release drives plasticity in several different areas of the brain, such as in the AL (Hammer 1997).
Neural networks in the AL provide the first synaptic contact between afferent sensory inputs from olfactory sensory cells with interneurons in the brain. Axons from sensory cells that express the same receptor, and hence respond to the same odorants, converge in the AL to the same spatial position to form a glomerulus (Galizia and Rössler 2010). Each glomerulus is innervated by dendrites of 3–5 projection neurons (PN), which send axon outputs to represent olfactory information in other areas of the brain (Mobbs 1985). Calcium-imaging studies of odor-driven PN responses have revealed complex spatial and temporal properties that represent the identity and concentration of odorants (Sachse and Galizia 2003; Locatelli et al. 2013). The spatiotemporal patterns that represent an odor in the AL also change when that odor is associated with sucrose reinforcement (Fernandez et al. 2009). Specifically, the patterns for an odor associated with sucrose become significantly more distinct from a different odor explicitly not associated with sucrose (Rath et al. 2011). Therefore, one potential function of plasticity in the AL may be to make odors that are involved in finding floral rewards more detectable and discriminable from less relevant background odors (Smith et al. 2006; Riffell et al. 2013). As a result, each individual honey bee would have a different connectivity network in the AL that reflects its unique set of olfactory experiences. OA release by VUMmx1 may underlie this plasticity because fine OA positive processes from VUM innervate most if not all of the AL glomeruli.
OA acts via binding to different G protein coupled receptors that regulate intracellular levels of cyclic AMP or calcium (Evans and Maqueira 2005). A number of distinct OA receptor subtypes have been cloned and characterized from Drosophila (Han et al. 1998; Balfanz et al. 2005; Maqueira et al. 2005). Drosophila OA receptors cluster into two classes (Evans and Maqueira 2005): One class is the Octα/OAMB receptor class, which consists of one gene with two splice variants: the OAMB-AS/DmOA1A/DmOctα1A receptor and the OAMB-K3/DmOA1B/DmOctα1B receptor. Receptors in this group act to regulate intracellular calcium levels via IP3 activation by PLC (Balfanz et al. 2005; Hoff et al. 2011). The other Drosophila OA receptors make up the Octβ receptor class, which when stimulated act to increase intracellular cAMP levels (Maqueira et al. 2005). To date, only one OA receptor (AmOA1) from the Octα receptor class has been cloned and characterized from honey bee (Grohmann et al. 2003), although the honey bee genome also contains several more OA receptor homologs that are members of the Octβ receptor class (Evans and Maqueira 2005). When expressed in HEK cells, activation of AmOA1 receptors by OA leads to oscillations of intracellular Ca2+ levels and a relatively small increase in cAMP levels (Grohmann et al. 2003). AmOA1 receptors are present throughout the brain, including in the mushroom bodies and ALs (Sinakevitch et al. 2011). Downregulation of AmOA1 via RNA interference significantly reduces olfactory learning (Farooqui et al. 2003), suggesting that AmOA1 receptors are an important part of the OA reinforcement pathway. OA could potentially have direct effects on several different cell types in the AL. In addition to sensory axon terminals and PNs, there are a number of interneurons that differ in morphology with regard to immunoreactivity for different neurotransmitters or peptides (Kreissl et al. 2010). A subset of GABAergic interneurons express AmOA1 receptors and are therefore targets for OA modulation (Sinakevitch et al. 2011).
Here we use calcium imaging of PN activity to perform a network level analysis of the effect of OA on odor representations in the AL with the ultimate objective of understanding how OA may target components of the network. Much of the specific synaptic connectivity between the different cell types within and among glomeruli is unknown. Yet we can predict that the effects of OA treatment on this network will be complex given the number of different cell types and the presence of AmOA1 receptors on the GABAergic interneurons. For example, we find that the calcium response from PNs in a glomerulus may be potentiated by OA in the presence of one odor. The same PNs may be inhibited in the presence of a different odor, which may only weakly excite these PNs, particularly if inhibition from a different glomerulus is potentiated by OA. Furthermore, we find a high variability across individuals, suggesting that OA is indeed involved in the individual learning history of the bee. Data such as these will be necessary for understanding in more detail the circuitry in the AL as well as how OA-mediated plasticity alters the network to adapt to changing contingences among odors. It is interesting to compare these findings to the situation in the mammalian olfactory bulb, in which similar coding and plasticity mechanisms have been described (Hildebrand and Shepherd 1997; Wilson 2008; Leinwand and Chalasani 2011), but where multiple modulators, including acetyl choline, norepinephrine and serotonin may exert related plasticity effects on olfactory networks (Fletcher and Chen 2010).
Materials and methods
Animals
Forager honey bees (Apis mellifera carnica) were collected in the morning at the hive entrance. Pollen and nectar foragers vary in a number of different physiological measures including sucrose sensitivity and learning (Page et al. 1998; Scheiner et al. 2005; Wright et al. 2007; Drezner-Levy et al. 2009), therefore, to reduce variability across animals, only pollen foragers were collected.
Bees were immobilized by cooling and then individually restrained in harnesses. The head was fixed to the harness with soft dental wax (Kerr, Sybron Dental Specialities) such that the bees could move their antennae and proboscis freely. During the experimental procedure, bees were kept at room temperature in plastic boxes with moist tissue and fed to satiation at least two times a day with 1 M sucrose solution.
Projection neuron staining and calcium imaging
We stained PNs of the right antennal lobe (AL) by backfilling them with the calcium-sensitive dye Fura2-dextran (potassium salt, 10,000 MW, Invitrogen) as described elsewhere (Sachse and Galizia 2003; Galizia and Vetter 2004; Locatelli et al. 2013). A window was cut in the head capsule, and medial and lateral mushroom body calyces were exposed by carefully removing glands and tracheae. We inserted the tip of a glass electrode covered with Fura2-dextran into the protocerebrum slightly ventral to the place where medial and lateral calyx meet (Fig. 1a), aiming at axon bundles of the antenno-protocerebral tracts (lAPT and mAPT) that contain axons of uniglomerular projection neurons (Abel et al. 2001; Galizia and Rössler 2010). After the dye bolus dissolved inside the tissue, we removed the glass electrode and rinsed the brain with saline solution to remove excess dye from the brain surface. The head capsule was then closed and sealed with eicosane (Sigma-Aldrich) using the piece of cuticle that had been removed. The dye was allowed to travel along the axons for several hours.
Before imaging, we fixed the antennae at their bases to the head capsule with eicosane. Using fine forceps, the esophagus and supporting chitin structures were carefully pulled outwards through a hole in the cuticle and kept under tension to prevent esophageal movement. Then we reopened the window between the antennae and the ocelli and rinsed the brain with saline. Glands and trachea covering the right AL were pushed aside or, if necessary, removed to get visual access to the AL.
Calcium imaging was done using a CCD camera (image size 130 × 140 pixels; SensiCamQE, T.I.L.L. Photonics, Gräfelfing, Germany) mounted on an upright fluorescence microscope (Olympus BX-50WI, Japan) equipped with a 20 × dip objective, NA = 0.95 (Olympus), 505 nm dichroic mirror, and 515 nm LP filter (T.I.L.L. Photonics). Monochromatic excitation light (PolichromeV, T.I.L.L. Photonics) alternated between 340 and 380 nm. Exposure times differed between 5 and 15 ms for the 380 nm excitation light, depending on how intensely the AL was stained. For 340 nm, the exposure time was set 4 times longer than for 380 nm. Double images were taken at a sampling rate of 4 Hz for background activity and 5 Hz for odor responses. Image acquisition and light exposure were controlled by TILLVision software (T.I.L.L. Photonics).
Odorant stimulation
We recorded the responses to the odorants 1-nonanol, 1-hexanol and 2-heptanone (all odorants from TCI, America). These odorants have been used in several previous studies, and are well characterized both behaviorally and physiologically, in particular with respect to which olfactory glomeruli they activate (Galizia et al. 1999b; Sachse et al. 1999; Guerrieri et al. 2005). Odorants were diluted 1:100 in mineral oil (Sigma-Aldrich). In addition, we recorded a concentration series for 1-nonanol, with concentration steps at 10−4, 10−3 and 10−2 dilution. Thus, 1-nonanol 1:100 was recorded two times in each set. 5 μl of odorant solution was loaded on a 0.5 × 4 cm filter paper strip and placed in a 1 ml glass syringe. The glass syringes were placed in a custom made olfactometer. A charcoal filtered air stream (25 ml/s) continuously flowed through the olfactometer and ventilated the antennae. A three-way valve (LFAA1200118H; The LEE Company) 4 cm upstream from the odorant cartridge controlled the onset of the airflow through the odorant cartridge. Valve opening (stimulus length: 2 s) was synchronized with the optical recordings directly from the TILLVision software. When the valve was open, the odorant-laden air from a cartridge was pushed into the continuous air stream in a mixing chamber. Odorant delivery was targeted at the antenna and positioned 1 cm away from the bee’s head. An exhaust placed 5 cm behind the bee continuously removed air from the arena.
Drugs and solutions
All imaging experiments were performed under saline solution containing (in mM) 130 NaCl, 6 KCl, 4 MgCl2, 5 CaCl2, 160 sucrose, 25 glucose, 10 HEPES; pH 6.7, 500 mOsmol (all chemicals from Sigma-Aldrich). dl-Octopamine HCl (Sigma-Aldrich) was dissolved in saline solution to final concentrations of 1 and 10 mM of octopamine. Osmolarity was adjusted by reducing the amount of sucrose in the saline.
RNA interference
dsRNA was synthesized following the PCR template method (Kennerdell and Carthew 1998) using T7 RNAP promotor linked oligonucleotides. PCR primers for Amoa1 were: TAATACGACTCACTATAGGGAGACCACGAGACGAAGGCGGCGAAGACAC and TAATACGACTCACTATAGGGAGACCACCGTTTGCAGAAGCACTTGACGATG. This sequence produced a 294 bp DNA fragment that was then used as the template for dsRNA production. As a control, dsRNA was also synthesized corresponding to the Drosophila fred gene as disruption of fred has been shown to have significant effects in Drosophila (Chandra et al. 2003). This construct does not contain the level of sequence homology necessary to induce RNA interference in honey bee. PCR Primers used to produce the 822 bp DNA Drosophila fred (Dmfred) template were: TAATACGACTCACTATAGGGAGACCACATGGTGACATTGGAAATACACAG and TAATACGACTCACTATAGGGAGACCACCCTCTTATGCTGTCCAAAGGAT. dsRNA was synthesized in vitro from the PCR templates using the Maxiscript kit (Ambion), ethanol precipitated, resuspended in injection buffer (5 mM KCl; 10 mM NaH2PO4, pH 7.8), quantitated and diluted to 125 ng/μl in injection buffer.
Bees were prepared and their ALs exposed as described above. For brains to be used for calcium imaging, 4 nL of buffer containing 125 ng/μl of either Amoa1 or Dmfred dsRNA was injected into the right AL using a picospritzer (Parker Hannifin Corporation). After injection, bees to be used in calcium imaging had their PNs of the right AL backfilled as described above. In a previous study, a significant reduction in receptor expression had been found 24 h after injection of dsRNA (Farooqui et al. 2003). Therefore, calcium imaging and dissection of ALs for western analysis were done 24 h after the animal had been injected with the dsRNA.
Western analysis
To show that injection of Amoa1 dsRNA, but not control dsRNA, lead to downregulation of the AmOA1 receptor, western analysis was used to quantitate AmOA1 protein levels in the ALs of bees injected with Amoa1 dsRNA, control (Dmfred) dsRNA and bees that had their ALs exposed during surgery, but did not have any dsRNA injected. 24 h after injection with dsRNA or surgery alone, honey bee ALs were homogenized in 2× sample buffer (0.125 M Tris, 4 % SDS, 20 % glycerol, 0.2 mM DTT, pH 6.8), immediately boiled for 3 min, and stored at −70 °C. Homogenate was separated on a 7.5 % acrylamide Tris–glycine gel. Proteins were transferred onto nitrocellulose membrane (Bio-Rad Laboratories, Inc., Hercules, CA, USA) in transfer buffer (25 mM Tris, 192 mM glycine, 15 % methanol) at 0.45 A for 2 h at 4 °C. The membrane was blocked overnight in TBSTw (10 mM Tris, pH 7.5; 30 mM NaCl; 0.1 % Tween-20) plus 10 % low fat powdered milk at 4 °C and incubated in AmOA1 antiserum at 1:2,000 in TBSTw plus 2.5 % milk for 4 h at room temperature. Polyclonal antibodies against AmOA1 were generated against a 15 amino acid peptide (NH2-DFRFAFKSIICKCFC-OH) in the carboxyl terminus of the receptor and have been characterized previously (Farooqui et al. 2003; Sinakevitch et al. 2011). Following four 15 min washes in TBSTw plus 10 % milk, the membrane was incubated in anti-rabbit IgG HRP-conjugated secondary antibodies (Rockland Inc.) at 1:10,000 in TBSTw plus 2.5 % milk for 2 h. The membrane was washed three times in TBSTw with the final wash in TBS with no tween, and then developed using chemiluminescence as described by the manufacturer (Immobilon Western Chemiluminescent HRP Substrate; Millipore Corporation). As a loading control, after blotting with AmOA1 antibodies, the membrane was re-probed with anti-tubulin antibodies (Abcam Inc., Cambridge, MA, USA) at 1:10,000 and processed as above. The tubulin antibody produced a band at approximately 52 kDa. Images for quantification were captured using a ChemiDoc XRS gel imaging system (Bio-Rad Laboratories, Inc., Hercules, CA) and analyzed using ImageJ version 1.41o (Wayne Rasband, National Institutes of Health, USA, http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij).
Data analysis
We identified glomerular borders in calcium-imaging recordings based on their individual temporal dynamics, both in spontaneous background activity and in odor responses (Strauch and Galizia 2008). This approach uses a combination of principal component analysis (PCA) and Independent Component Analysis (ICA). First, we corrected for animal movement by aligning consecutive images to each other. This was achieved by performing a locally restricted cross-correlation on edge-enhanced thumbnail images. We used z-scores to normalize each of the 130 × 140 time series, i.e. we subtracted the mean and divided by the standard deviation. Z-score normalization was performed individually for each individual odorant stimulation or spontaneous activity measurement. Next, we performed dimensionality reduction with a covariance-free PCA algorithm (Papadimitriou et al. 2005), which avoided the construction of huge covariance matrices that would normally arise when applying conventional PCA to imaging datasets. Then, we applied the ICA algorithm fastICA (Hyvarinen and Oja 2000) to a complete recording (all measurements for the animal). The ICA approach resulted in spatially local and contiguous components, i.e. numerous objects consisting of time series that are mutually correlated with each other but uncorrelated to the time series in the other objects. We identified these spatial components as glomeruli when they were globular and within the anatomical boundaries of the AL and/or showed a response to odorant stimulation. Based on anatomical position of the glomeruli and their response dynamics to the tested odorants, we could assign names to glomeruli using the AL standard atlas (Flanagan and Mercer 1989; Galizia et al. 1999a). Glomerulus nomenclature was abbreviated for simplification: glomeruli innervated by the T1-tract were named with an A as prefix (e.g. T1-17 and T1-33 as A17, A33), while glomeruli innervated by the T3-tract were named with a C as prefix (e.g. T3-45 as C45), as done elsewhere (Galizia et al. 1999a). All the above processing (except for the movement correction) was only applied to detect and identify glomeruli. Once glomeruli were identified, we extracted glomerular time series from the movement-corrected but otherwise untreated imaging data. We extracted time series from the center of the identified glomeruli and averaged over a radius of 5 pixels to reduce local photon shot noise. We implemented all the above methods in Java.
Average calcium level is related to the absolute ratio level in FURA recordings: here we took the untreated ratio F 340/F 380. Other time-traces were normalized to pre-stimulus magnitude, and computed as log-fold change: with being the resulting signal measure for each timepoint i, and “prestimulus” being all 30 frames before stimulus, i.e. .
In order to evaluate changes in background activity, we calculated the standard deviation of the fluorescence ratio (F 340/F 380) for each glomerulus in all bees. An increase in standard deviation indicates an increase in number and amplitude of calcium fluctuations, which is typical for an increased background activity.
Odor stimulation can either increase or decrease intracellular calcium concentration, indicated by an increase or decrease in fluorescence ratio. As a measure for response strength, we calculated the response maximum and the mean fluorescence ratio within a 3 s time-interval (for glomeruli that showed calcium decreases to an odor we calculated a negative maximum, i.e. the minimum).
Experimental procedure
Staining intensity, background activity and strength of an odor response can differ across individuals. We therefore followed a within-animal approach: each bee was first challenged with three odorants, and one nonanol concentration series, which were recorded with 5 Hz time resolution. Then, background calcium activity was measured for 125 s with 4 Hz time resolution. Next, and always in the same sequence, the same measurements were repeated with superfusion of 1 mM OA, 10 mM OA, and washing the brain with saline solution (Fig. 1b). Statistical data analysis was done by use of repeated measurements ANOVA, which takes into account that more than one set of recordings was performed on each animal. We used the programs SigmaStat and SigmaPlot (SPSS, IBM, USA) or the statistical language R (http://www.r-project.org/).
Results
Odorant stimulation elicits an odor-specific pattern of glomerular activation in the antennal lobe (AL) of the honey bee (Joerges et al. 1997; Galizia et al. 1999b). We recorded the glomerular response to the odorants 1-hexanol (10−2), 2-heptanone (10−2) and 1-nonanol (concentration series 10−4, 10−3 and 10−2). Upon stimulation with an odorant, a characteristic glomerular response pattern was visible across the AL (Fig. 1c), with some glomeruli increasing, and some decreasing calcium concentration (Fig. 1d). Calcium increases were generally steep, with a slower decay phase after stimulus offset. Odor-response time courses differed for different glomeruli, some with a slow decay, some with a faster decay (Fig. 1c, d). The decrease of intracellular calcium concentration in some glomeruli was most likely due to a closing of calcium channels, while calcium pumps were still active. Given that resting calcium levels, which we measure as average calcium level, are generally low in neurons, the absolute values of calcium decrease were not high, as visible from the small downward deflection in the respective traces (Fig. 1d). When no odorant was given, glomeruli had constantly fluctuating background calcium concentration levels (Fig. 1e). These fluctuations were much smaller than odor responses (compare the ordinate axis in Fig. 1d and e), and the correlation across glomeruli was low, as previously reported (Galan et al. 2006).
Octopamine increases background activity in projection neurons
Superfusing the brain with OA solution led to changes in background activity. Calcium fluctuations increased both with 1 mM OA (Fig. 2b) and with 10 mM OA (Fig. 2c). These fluctuations came back to baseline after OA was washed out. Furthermore, overall calcium levels dropped, in particular with 10 mM OA, as visible in Fig. 2c by the lower level of the curve. Across animals, the increase in calcium fluctuations was significant for 1 mM OA and for 10 mM OA (Fig. 2e), while the drop in overall calcium levels was only significant for 10 mM OA (Fig. 2f). The simultaneous increase in background activity and decrease in calcium levels (e.g. Fig. 2c) indicate that changing background activity in projection neurons is not caused by intrinsic mechanisms of the PNs themselves, but rather by synaptic input from the AL network.
Octopamine modulates the odor response by inducing variable changes in glomerular activity
Odors elicit complex patterns of calcium increase and decrease across glomeruli (Figs. 1c, 3a). The most characteristic property for each glomerulus is the response magnitude. Therefore, we quantified response magnitude across odors and glomeruli, and across identified glomeruli in different animals. We found that applying 1 mM OA modified the response patterns: some glomeruli increased their response, whereas other glomeruli decreased the response. Similarly, response magnitude changed after application of 10 mM OA. For example, glomerulus C45 increased its odor response considerably to 2-heptanone and to 1-hexanol (Fig. 3b, c). This was the strongest glomerulus for these two odors. However, glomerulus A33, which was the strongest glomerulus in the 1-nonanol response pattern, decreased its response to 1-nonanol after application of OA. Thus, the strongest glomeruli increased their responses for some odors, and decreased their responses for other odors. The effect on each glomerulus was odor-specific. For example, responses in glomerulus A17 decreased with OA treatment when 1-nonanol was given, but did not change for 1-hexanol (Fig. 3c, d). Indeed, the OA effect was not only odor-specific, but also concentration-dependent. For example, A17 increased its response to 1-nonanol at an odor concentration of 10−4, but decreased its response to the same odor at 10−2 (suppl. Fig. S1). A two-way ANOVA with the factors treatment and odors (with the different concentrations of 1-nonanol treated as different odors) found significant differences for the levels of treatment (F = 28.4, p < 0.001, i.e. OA had an effect) as well as for the levels of odor (F = 255.1, p < 0.001, i.e. different odors elicit different response patterns). Importantly, however, we found a significant interaction between odor and treatment (F = 4.2, p < 0.001), indicating that octopamine does not affect the response to all odors the same way. With OA superfusion the responses increased to the odors 1-hexanol and 2-heptanone as well as to 1-nonanol at concentrations of 10−3 and 10−4, but the responses to 1-nonanol 10−2 were either not affected (first stimulation) or reduced (second stimulation) in the presence of 10 mM OA (Fig. 4). The reduced effect with the first nonanol stimulation might be caused by the shorter time that octopamine had been present in the bath during this stimulation.
These observations indicate that increase or decrease in projection neuron responses within a glomerulus after OA treatment is not a property of that glomerulus, but rather a property of the AL network. The most prominent effect was that purely negative odor responses only rarely occurred under OA treatment: almost all negative responses reverted to positive ones under different treatment conditions.
The effect of octopamine depends on the strength of the initial odor response
We noted that strong odor responses were less likely to be modulated by OA treatment than weak odor responses. To test this, we created three groups of glomeruli based on their response strength to each particular stimulus: negative, weak, and strong (39 classified responses from 13 bees, N = 13 for each group). We chose 2.5 × mean standard deviation of the background activity, measured during 6 s before odorant onset, as a threshold. Glomeruli with a mean odor response that was below threshold before octopamine application were defined as weak glomeruli, glomeruli with an initial response above threshold as strong glomeruli. Glomeruli responding with a decrease in calcium concentration formed the “negative” group. Across all odors, concentrations and animals, the increased response for weak responses and for intermediate responses was significant, but no significant effect was visible for high responses (Fig. 5a). Specifically, inhibited glomeruli not only showed less negative responses, but generally even positive responses to odorant stimuli when 10 mM of octopamine was applied (e.g. A21 and A37 in Fig. 3b), an effect that was highly significant (Tukey HD following Friedman repeated measures ANOVA, χ2 = 23.7, p < 0.001). The effect was not reversible within 10 min. The observed switch from calcium decrease to calcium increase in the odor responses suggests a strong decrease in inhibitory input to a glomerulus. Weak glomeruli significantly increased their odor responses after application of 10 mM octopamine (Fig. 5a, Tukey HSD following Friedman repeated measures ANOVA, χ2 = 18.5, p < 0.001). In strong glomeruli we observed no clear increase in mean odor response but rather a tendency to decrease the odor response in the presence of OA (Fig. 5a). However, this decrease in mean odor response was not statistically significant (one-way ANOVA, F = 1.277, p = 0.314).
Given that in the control situation, when the entire experiment was done with saline treatment rather than OA treatment, the negative glomerular responses also became positive, albeit after a longer time than in the OA case (Fig. 5b, 23 classified responses from 8 bees), it appears that, at least for the calcium-decrease case, there may be two (or more) overlapping mechanisms: a reduction of negative responses over long measurement times with repeated stimulation and recording (Fig. 5b), and an immediate decrease due to OA treatment (Fig. 5a).
The effect of octopamine is variable across animals
For further analysis, we performed a two-way ANOVA with the factors treatment (with or without OA) and glomerulus (glomerular identity) and found significant differences for both factors, but no interaction between factors (two-way ANOVA, p treatment = 1.1e−08; p glomerulus < 2.2e−16; p interaction = 1). These results again show that octopamine treatment led to a significant difference in mean odor response. Additionally, it shows that the measured glomeruli differed significantly in their mean odor responses, which is not unexpected as all glomeruli have an individual odor-response profile. However, the lack of interaction between factors strengthens the hypothesis that glomerular identity did not determine the response to OA treatment. Moreover, when pooling individual glomeruli across animals, we found significant octopamine-induced changes in mean odor response across odors only in two glomeruli, namely in glomeruli 37 and 49 (Tukey HSD following Friedman repeated measures ANOVA, χ237 = 14.8, p 37 = 0.002 χ249 = 20.6, p 49 < 0.001; to adjust for multiple testing, significance level was corrected by use of Bonferroni-correction; compare with suppl. Fig. S2). Thus, most glomeruli could both increase or decrease activity in the presence of octopamine.
The octopamine effect is a network effect
Next, we investigated whether glomeruli had stereotypical responses to OA treatment. Figure 6 shows bar-plots of odor-response differences for each odor. What is apparent is that the variability is high. Importantly, not even the polarity is uniform. For example, glomerulus A30 showed all ranges of increases and decreases of responses to 1-nonanol after OA treatment, as did glomerulus A18 to 1-hexanol, or glomerulus A33 to 2-heptanone. Weak and strong glomeruli were equally variable (e.g. A33 to 1-nonanol as a strong glomerulus). This variability indicates that OA may act on a network that is not innate, but rather the result of plasticity and/or genetic variability, and thus variable across animals.
OA acts on receptors that increase intracellular calcium concentration. Since we found both response increases and response decreases, it is unlikely that receptors on PNs contribute significantly to the effects shown here. In order to test this explicitly, we superfused the brain with caffeine, which leads to a general increase in intracellular calcium. This treatment led to a general increase in odor responses (at 5 mM caffeine), or a general decrease in odor responses (at 20 mM caffeine), but never to a complex pattern of increases and decreases in different glomeruli (suppl. Fig. S3). Thus, the OA effect reported here does not result from a general intracellular calcium increase, but rather must be a network-specific effect.
The role of AmOA1
All results so far indicated that OA acts as a modulator within the AL. However, other explanations are also possible. In particular, OA could have nonspecific effects on other biogenic amine receptors, e.g. tyramine receptors, which share a high sequence similarity. Furthermore, OA may act in brain areas other than the ALs, and the effects seen here might be mediated by neural feedback connections into the AL. In order to elucidate whether the observed effects indeed originate within the AL, and are caused by OA receptors, we downregulated the expression of the OA receptor AmOA1 using RNA interference. We injected either Amoa1 or control (Dmfred) dsRNA, in addition, a third group of animals either underwent surgery but was not injected (surgery, Fig. 7a–d) or was injected with the injection buffer used to dilute the dsRNA (Fig. 7e). We injected the dsRNA into the right ALs, and 24 h later recorded spontaneous activity and odor responses. At the end of the experiment, ALs were dissected and used for western blot analysis. Injection of Amoa1, but not control dsRNA, led to a significant reduction in AmOA1 receptor protein levels (suppl. Fig. S4).
Background activity increased in control dsRNA animals in the presence of octopamine (Tukey HSD following Friedman repeated measurement ANOVA χ2 = 84.305 p < 0.001, Fig. 7a; AmOA1 dsRNA: 192 glomeruli from 13 bees; surgery control: n = 207 from 13 bees, control dsRNA: n = 169 from 10 bees). However, animals that had been treated with Amoa1 dsRNA did not show an increase in background activity in presence of octopamine, but there was a significant increase in background activity after the washout. Background calcium levels (i.e. mean spontaneous activity) were not affected by injection of either Amoa1 or control dsRNA (Fig. 7b). Sorting glomeruli by response strength (compare with Fig. 5) confirmed a strong increase in responses, in particular for inhibited and weak glomeruli, in the untreated (surgery or injection buffer) and in the control dsRNA treatment (Fig. 7c, d; AmOA1: 39 classified responses, 13 negative, 13 strong, 13 weak from 13 bees; control dsRNA: n = 30 responses from 10 animals; surgery: n = 39 from 13 animals. Same animals for 1 mM OA and for 10 mM OA. Figure 7e: AmOA1: 27 classified responses from 9 bees; control dsRNA: n = 30 responses from 10 animals; surgery: n = 26 from 9 animals). In contrast, after Amoa1 dsRNA injection, there was only a weak and non-significant odor-response increase in inhibited and weak glomeruli. Thus, Amoa1 dsRNA injection almost completely abolished the OA superfusion effect, indicating that AmOA1 receptors within the AL were responsible for the observed modulations provoked by OA.
Discussion
Behavioral studies have shown that octopamine (OA) in the antennal lobe (AL) plays an important role in appetitive odor learning (Hammer and Menzel 1998; Farooqui et al. 2003). OA-like immunoreactivity has been detected in glomeruli and within the coarse central neuropil of the AL (Kreissl et al. 1994; Sinakevitch et al. 2005), and antibodies against the honey bee OA receptor AmOA1 stain different groups of AL neurons including a group of GABAergic local interneurons (Sinakevitch et al. 2011). We show here that the neurotransmitter OA modulates the neural networks in the honey bee AL. Most importantly, we show that the effect of applying OA is not uniform across all olfactory glomeruli. The effect differs across glomeruli and is likely related to network connectivity. The effect is dependent on several factors (most importantly odor, odor concentration, glomerulus), and it has a high variability across animals (Fig. 6). The inter-animal variability in particular suggests that modulation by OA is related to the individual life history of the animal or other factors such as genetic background, and that it plays a role in olfactory memory.
Specifically, we found that applying OA increases background activity of olfactory projection neurons (PNs), i.e. these neurons have a higher level of activity within the network even in the absence of olfactory stimulation (Fig. 2). Background activity in PNs is a network effect and driven, in part, by spontaneous activity in olfactory receptor neurons. The increased activity when applying OA is not due to an increase in the basal level of Ca2+ (Fig. 2f) which would lead to a lowered threshold of the PN itself. Rather, it is likely driven by external factors, e.g. by decreased inhibitory input (see model proposed below). This view is strengthened by our observation that odor responses are also modified by OA, but in a non-uniform way: some glomeruli increase while others decrease their odor response after OA treatment (Fig. 3). Even more intriguingly, some glomeruli increase their response to one odor, but decrease the response to another, or even to the same odor at another concentration (suppl. Fig. S1). Generally, glomeruli most likely to be modulated are those with negative or weak responses (Fig. 5), keeping in mind that not all negative or weak responses are modulated, and some strong glomeruli may be modulated for particular odors.
OA acts on the antennal lobe network
Therefore, while the network effect of OA was reproducible for a given stimulus within a given animal, it was not predictable from one odor to another, or from one animal to another. This observation suggests that OA acts to a large degree at a network level on synaptic contacts, rather than on the excitability of particular cells. The latter was confirmed in a control experiment (suppl. Fig. S3): increasing intracellular calcium levels with caffeine led to a similar increase in background activity as OA, but odor responses were globally increased. This shows that when the increase in background activity is due to a general intracellular threshold shift, the network property revealed by OA is abolished. Similarly, treatments with drugs that inhibit GABAA- or histamine-receptors also lead to a nonspecific increase in background activity in projection neurons (Sachse and Galizia 2002; Sachse et al. 2006).
In our experiments we applied OA to the entire brain, begging the question of where the effect is localized: within the antennae on sensory neurons, as shown in cockroaches (Flecke and Stengl 2009), in some brain areas that have neural projections to the AL, or in the AL itself? When we downregulated AmOA1 receptors via localized injection of dsRNA into the AL, the result was a total abolishment of increased spontaneous activity (Fig. 7a), and a reduced effect in odor-response modulation in the presence of OA (Fig. 7c–e). Thus, at least part of the octopamine effect is likely mediated by AmOA1 receptors within the AL. Nevertheless, other OA receptors may be involved as the bee genome encodes OA receptors of the Octβ receptor class as well as AmOA1 (Evans and Maqueira 2005). Furthermore, feedback connections from other brain areas that need not themselves be octopaminergic may add to the effect, which remains to be tested. Importantly, we show a specific effect of an OA receptor, ruling out a nonspecific cross-reaction of the bath applied OA with other biogenic amine receptors.
OA creates a filter for odor patterns
Since we see both up and downregulation of odor responses, the OA effect onto PNs must be mostly multisynaptic. We propose here that OA acts on a disinhibitory pathway within the AL (Christensen et al. 1993), specifically on a particular subset of GABAergic local neurons (LNs), which themselves act on other GABAergic neurons (Fig. 8a). With increasing OA, the OA target cells become more active, inhibiting their inhibitory synaptic partners, which leads to PNs being disinhibited, increasing their spontaneous activity. In this model, the synaptic efficiency of OA-to-LN synapses is plastic, and depends on previous experience. Thus, odor patterns that have previously been experienced in an appetitive context, that therefore led to an OA release, will be preferentially activated. In the natural environment, such a mechanism would ensure that the AL becomes more sensitive for odor-response patterns that have already successfully indicated the presence of food, and within these patterns the network becomes more selective for the common part of the pattern, i.e. for a consensus odor representation (Smith et al. 2006; Riffell et al. 2013).
This model (Fig. 8a) is based on the observations in this paper, combined with other previously published results. First, immunohistochemical analyses showed that AmOA1 receptors are localized on a subpopulation of GABAergic cells (Sinakevitch et al. 2011), although which specific GABAergic LN subpopulation expresses these receptor genes remains unknown. Also, we cannot exclude that in addition to GABAergic LNs, other neurons (e.g. histaminergic neurons or cholinergic neurons) might also respond to OA. In particular, two AL cell clusters label with antibodies against the AmOA1 receptor that do not co-label with an anti-GABA-antibody (Sinakevitch et al. 2011). Second, immunohistochemical analysis also shows that receptor density varies across glomeruli, and that this diversity is not predictable, i.e. if a glomerulus has a high OA receptor density in one animal, it may have a low density in another animal, supporting our suggestion that OA receptor density is experience dependent (Sinakevitch et al. 2011). Third, activation of AmOA1 receptors expressed in HEK cells leads to Ca2+ fluctuations and increased intracellular Ca2+ concentrations (Grohmann et al. 2003). In this study, we find an increase in Ca2+ fluctuations, but not in Ca2+ concentration, suggesting an indirect effect of OA onto PNs via the network, rather than a direct effect on PNs. Fourth, after appetitive learning, odor-response patterns change in the ALs of honey bees, but when averaging across individuals the effects are not reproducible, i.e. no change is visible (Peele et al. 2006)—again indicating a high network variability that may be experience dependent. Accordingly, when plasticity data is analyzed using multivariate techniques (Fernandez et al. 2009), or when glomeruli are sorted into odor-response classes (Rath et al. 2011), then clear shifts in odor-response profiles can be seen after appetitive olfactory conditioning, showing that network plasticity in the AL depends on previous experience.
Taking these results together, we propose that OA acts on a specific subpopulation of disinhibitory GABAergic LNs in the AL. It should be noted that the schematic in Fig. 8a does not depict the entire network within the AL, but only those elements that are affected by OA (for example, receptor neuron targets are not specified). Indeed, we are confident that further analyses will reveal more OA targets. For example, in several cases we did observe not an odor-response increase, but rather a decrease. The model in Fig. 8b illustrates one that would account for this observation: OA neurons could target both a disinhibitory and an inhibitory pathway onto PNs, and thus plasticity in receptor density would allow both for upregulation and downregulation of a glomerulus in the presence of an expected reward. The degrees of freedom that need to be tested increase in this model. For example, in Fig. 8b OA1 receptor density is approximately proportional for the disinhibitory and the inhibitory pathway—however, this is likely not to be the case, given the heterogeneous expression of OA1 receptors across glomeruli. Furthermore, in Fig. 8b both pathways share the same inhibitory neuron (yellow)—again, not a necessity. Indeed, the inhibitory pathway may not even be GABAergic, but use other inhibitory transmitters, such as histamine. Thus, the scenario of a parallel inhibitory and disinhibitory pathway modulated by OA offers many new hypotheses to be tested. Importantly, however, it offers the possibility that OA modulates glomeruli both up and down, and as a result an odor that activates VUMmx1 (because previously associated with a reward) will lead to a more reliable and stable representation in the brain.
It may well be that octopaminergic networks act in similar ways in other sensory systems in invertebrates as well. In crustaceans, OA either enhances or decreases transmitter release through neuromuscular junctions, and alterations in endogenous OA levels have been suggested to contribute to these variable responses (Djokaj et al. 2001). Daytime-dependent variations in OA levels in the hemolymph have also been suggested to alter the response to exogenously applied OA on pheromone-sensitive neurons in the hawkmoth Manduca sexta (Flecke and Stengl 2009). Finally, apparent or real variations in OA concentration might also cause variable physiological effects: frequency and force of heart contractions in isolated hearts of the honey bee are increased at high concentrations of OA, while low OA concentrations inhibit the heart firing rate (Papaefthimiou and Theophilidis 2011). In our case, these effects may be increased by differential OA receptor expression density.
The network properties underlying these models remain to be elucidated. More localized pharmacological analyses (Girardin et al. 2013) may help to disentangle the detailed network in the honey bee olfactory system. Computer models can provide insight if such a selective, disinhibitory modulatory system including experience-dependent plasticity can indeed create a selective filter for extracting common elements in an environment of fluctuating odorant stimuli, and thus help an animal to efficiently process and recognize odors. Importantly, our study indicates that the results of learning and plasticity may need to be analyzed at the level of each individual, because the network effects average out across individuals. Honey bees are ideal model animals to study these effects, given their robust learning rates and the ease of analyzing individually trained animals. In particular, it will be interesting to compare the effect of octopamine on naive and experienced bees, and thus examine the precise nature of how experience modifies the neural networks involved in olfactory processing.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgments
This research was funded by the NIH NCRR (R01 RR014166 to BHS) and NIH NIDCD (R01 DC011422 BHS co-PI). Grant BMBF 01GQ0771 from the German Research Ministry to CGG, JR, MS.
Conflict of interest
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Abbreviations
- AL
Antennal lobes
- ANOVA
Analysis of variance
- DsRNA
Double stranded RNA
- HEK cells
Human embryonic kidney 293 cells
- LN
Local neuron
- MB
Mushroom bodies
- OA
Octopamine
- PCA
Principal component analysis
- PCR
Polymerase chain reaction
- PN
Projection neuron
- VUM
Ventral unpaired median
Footnotes
B. H. Smith, C. G. Galizia contributed equally to this work (co-PI).
Contributor Information
Brian H. Smith, Email: BrianHSmith@asu.edu
C. Giovanni Galizia, Email: galizia@uni.kn.
References
- Abel R, Rybak J, Menzel R. Structure and response patterns of olfactory interneurons in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol. 2001;437:363–383. doi: 10.1002/cne.1289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balfanz S, Strunker T, Frings S, Baumann A. A family of octopamine receptors that specifically induce cyclic AMP production or Ca2+ release in Drosophila melanogaster. J Neurochem. 2005;93:440–451. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2005.03034.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandra S, Ahmed A, Vaessin H. The Drosophila IgC2 domain protein Friend-of-Echinoid, a paralogue of Echinoid, limits the number of sensory organ precursors in the wing disc and interacts with the Notch signaling pathway. Dev Biol. 2003;256:302–316. doi: 10.1016/S0012-1606(03)00038-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen TA, Waldrop BR, Harrow ID, Hildebrand JG. Local interneurons and information processing in the olfactory glomeruli of the moth Manduca sexta. J Comp Physiol A. 1993;173:385–399. doi: 10.1007/BF00193512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Djokaj S, Cooper RL, Rathmayer W. Presynaptic effects of octopamine, serotonin, and cocktails of the two modulators on neuromuscular transmission in crustaceans. J Comp Physiol A. 2001;187:145–154. doi: 10.1007/s003590100187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drezner-Levy T, Smith BH, Shafir S. The effect of foraging specialization on various learning tasks in the honey bee (Apis mellifera) Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2009;64:135–148. doi: 10.1007/s00265-009-0829-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Evans PD, Maqueira B. Insect octopamine receptors: a new classification scheme based on studies of cloned Drosophila G-protein coupled receptors. Invertebr Neurosci. 2005;5:111–118. doi: 10.1007/s10158-005-0001-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farooqui T, Robinson K, Vaessin H, Smith BH. Modulation of early olfactory processing by an octopaminergic reinforcement pathway in the honeybee. J Neurosci. 2003;23:5370–5380. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-05370.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez PC, Locatelli FF, Person-Rennell N, Deleo G, Smith BH. Associative conditioning tunes transient dynamics of early olfactory processing. J Neurosci. 2009;29:10191–10202. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flanagan D, Mercer AR. An atlas and 3-D reconstruction of the antennal lobes in the worker honey bee, Apis mellifera L. (Hymenoptera: Apidae) Int J Insect Morphol Embryol. 1989;18:145–159. doi: 10.1016/0020-7322(89)90023-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Flecke C, Stengl M. Octopamine and tyramine modulate pheromone-sensitive olfactory sensilla of the hawkmoth Manduca sexta in a time-dependent manner. J Comp Physiol A. 2009;195:529–545. doi: 10.1007/s00359-009-0429-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher ML, Chen WR. Neural correlates of olfactory learning: Critical role of centrifugal neuromodulation. Learn Mem. 2010;17:561–570. doi: 10.1101/lm.941510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galan RF, Weidert M, Menzel R, Herz AV, Galizia CG. Sensory memory for odors is encoded in spontaneous correlated activity between olfactory glomeruli. Neural Comput. 2006;18:10–25. doi: 10.1162/089976606774841558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Rössler W. Parallel olfactory systems in insects: anatomy and function. Annu Rev Entomol. 2010;55:399–420. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-112408-085442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Vetter RS. Optical methods for analyzing odor-evoked activity in the insect brain. In: Christensen TA, editor. Advances in insect sensory neuroscience. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2004. pp. 349–392. [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, McIlwrath SL, Menzel R. A digital three-dimensional atlas of the honeybee antennal lobe based on optical sections acquired by confocal microscopy. Cell Tissue Res. 1999;295:383–394. doi: 10.1007/s004410051245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Sachse S, Rappert A, Menzel R. The glomerular code for odor representation is species specific in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:473–478. doi: 10.1038/8144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girardin CC, Kreissl S, Galizia CG. Inhibitory connections in the honeybee antennal lobe are spatially patchy. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109:332–343. doi: 10.1152/jn.01085.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grohmann L, Blenau W, Erber J, Ebert PR, Strunker T, Baumann A. Molecular and functional characterization of an octopamine receptor from honeybee (Apis mellifera) brain. J Neurochem. 2003;86:725–735. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.01876.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guerrieri F, Lachnit H, Gerber B, Giurfa M. Olfactory blocking and odorant similarity in the honeybee. Learn Mem. 2005;12:86–95. doi: 10.1101/lm.79305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammer M. An identified neuron mediates the unconditioned stimulus in associative olfactory learning in honeybees. Nature. 1993;366:59–63. doi: 10.1038/366059a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammer M. The neural basis of associative reward learning in honeybees. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20:245–252. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)01019-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammer M, Menzel R. Multiple sites of associative odor learning as revealed by local brain microinjections of octopamine in honeybees. Learn Mem. 1998;5:146–156. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han KA, Millar NS, Davis RL. A novel octopamine receptor with preferential expression in Drosophila mushroom bodies. J Neurosci. 1998;18:3650–3658. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03650.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hildebrand JG, Shepherd GM. Mechanisms of olfactory discrimination: converging evidence for common principles across phyla. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1997;20:595–631. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoff M, Balfanz S, Ehling P, Gensch T, Baumann A. A single amino acid residue controls Ca2+ signaling by an octopamine receptor from Drosophila melanogaster. FASEB J. 2011;25:2484–2491. doi: 10.1096/fj.11-180703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyvarinen A, Oja E. Independent component analysis: algorithms and applications. Neural Netw. 2000;13:411–430. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(00)00026-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joerges J, Küttner A, Galizia CG, Menzel R. Representations of odours and odour mixtures visualized in the honeybee brain. Nature. 1997;387:285–288. doi: 10.1038/387285a0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kennerdell JR, Carthew RW. Use of dsRNA-mediated genetic interference to demonstrate that frizzled and frizzled 2 act in the wingless pathway. Cell. 1998;95:1017–1026. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81725-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreissl S, Eichmuller S, Bicker G, Rapus J, Eckert M. Octopamine-like immunoreactivity in the brain and subesophageal ganglion of the honeybee. J Comp Neurol. 1994;348:583–595. doi: 10.1002/cne.903480408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreissl S, Strasser C, Galizia CG. Allatostatin immunoreactivity in the honeybee brain. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518:1391–1417. doi: 10.1002/cne.22343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leinwand SG, Chalasani SH. Olfactory networks: from sensation to perception. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2011;21:806–811. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2011.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locatelli FF, Fernandez PC, Villareal F, Muezzinoglu K, Huerta R, Galizia CG, Smith BH. Nonassociative plasticity alters competitive interactions among mixture components in early olfactory processing. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;37:63–79. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maqueira B, Chatwin H, Evans PD. Identification and characterization of a novel family of Drosophila beta-adrenergic-like octopamine G-protein coupled receptors. J Neurochem. 2005;94:547–560. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2005.03251.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menzel R, Giurfa M. Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:62–71. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01601-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mobbs PG. Brain structure. In: Kerkut GA, Gilbert LI, editors. Comprehensive insect physiology biochemistry and pharmacology 5: nervous system: structure and motor function. Oxford: Pergamon Press; 1985. pp. 299–370. [Google Scholar]
- Monastirioti M. Biogenic amine systems in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Microsc Res Tech. 1999;45:106–121. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0029(19990415)45:2<106::AID-JEMT5>3.0.CO;2-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Page RE, Jr, Erber J, Fondrk MK. The effect of genotype on response thresholds to sucrose and foraging behavior of honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) J Comp Physiol A. 1998;182:489–500. doi: 10.1007/s003590050196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papadimitriou S, Sun J, Faloutsos C (2005) Streaming pattern discovery in multiple time-series. In: Proceedings of the VLDB 2005, pp 697–708
- Papaefthimiou C, Theophilidis G. Octopamine—a single modulator with double action on the heart of two insect species (Apis mellifera macedonica and Bactrocera oleae): Acceleration vs. inhibition. J Insect Physiol. 2011;57:316–325. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2010.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peele P, Ditzen M, Menzel R, Galizia CG. Appetitive odor learning does not change olfactory coding in a subpopulation of honeybee antennal lobe neurons. J Comp Physiol A. 2006;192:1083–1103. doi: 10.1007/s00359-006-0152-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rath L, Galizia CG, Szyszka P. Multiple memory traces after associative learning in the honey bee antennal lobe. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;34:352–360. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07753.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riffell JA, Lei H, Abrell L, Hildebrand JG. Neural basis of a pollinator’s buffet: olfactory specialization and learning in Manduca sexta. Science. 2013;339:200–204. doi: 10.1126/science.1225483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Galizia CG. Role of inhibition for temporal and spatial odor representation in olfactory output neurons: a calcium imaging study. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1106–1117. doi: 10.1152/jn.00325.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Galizia CG. The coding of odour-intensity in the honeybee antennal lobe: local computation optimizes odour representation. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18:2119–2132. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02931.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Rappert A, Galizia CG. The spatial representation of chemical structures in the antennal lobe of honeybees: steps towards the olfactory code. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:3970–3982. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00826.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Peele P, Silbering AF, Guhmann M, Galizia CG. Role of histamine as a putative inhibitory transmitter in the honeybee antennal lobe. Front Zool. 2006;3:22. doi: 10.1186/1742-9994-3-22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheiner R, Kuritz-Kaiser A, Menzel R, Erber J. Sensory responsiveness and the effects of equal subjective rewards on tactile learning and memory of honeybees. Learn Mem. 2005;12:626–635. doi: 10.1101/lm.98105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schröter U, Malun D, Menzel R. Innervation pattern of suboesophageal ventral unpaired median neurones in the honeybee brain. Cell Tissue Res. 2007;327:647–667. doi: 10.1007/s00441-006-0197-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinakevitch I, Niwa M, Strausfeld NJ. Octopamine-like immunoreactivity in the honey bee and cockroach: comparable organization in the brain and subesophageal ganglion. J Comp Neurol. 2005;488:233–254. doi: 10.1002/cne.20572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinakevitch I, Mustard JA, Smith BH. Distribution of the octopamine receptor AmOA1 in the honey bee brain. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e14536. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH, Wright GA, Daly KS (2006) Learning-based recognition and discrimination of floral odors. In: Dudareva N, Pichersky E (eds) The biology and chemistry of floral scents. CRC Press, Boca Raton, pp 263–295
- Strauch M, Galizia CG (2008) Registration to a neuroanatomical reference atlas—identifying glomeruli in optical recordings of the honeybee brain. In: Lecture notes in informatics P-136, pp 85–95
- Wilson RI. Neural and behavioral mechanisms of olfactory perception. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008;18:408–412. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright GA, Mustard JA, Kottcamp SM, Smith BH. Olfactory memory formation and the influence of reward pathway during appetitive learning by honey bees. J Exp Biol. 2007;210:4024–4033. doi: 10.1242/jeb.006585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.